Você está na página 1de 6

Nonerbal Communication, Social Psychology of

In the future with the help of computer technology, the typical manifestations of aggressiveness change
research will more often address how multiple chan- from childhood to adulthood, interpersonal differ-
nels of communication combine to convey meaning, ences seem to remain relatively stable over time. This
the nature of nonverbal behavior between interacting is particularly due to a subgroup of early starters who
partners or in groups, and nonverbal behavior as it are life-course-persistent in their antisocial behavior
changes over time within interactions. and frequently engage in serious and violent offending
(Moffitt 1993).
See also: Communication and Social Psychology; Prospective longitudinal studies have revealed well-
Communication: Philosophical Aspects; Emotion and replicated causes and risks for such a development
Expression; Gesture in Linguistics; Personality and (Loeber and Farrington 1998; Loeber and Stout-
Marriage; Pragmatics: Linguistic; Verbal Communi- hamer-Loeber 1998). However, even the best pre-
cation, Psychology of dictions explain only a moderate amount of variance.
Many children who are exposed to high risk do not
develop serious aggressive behavior. Others who seem
Bibliography
to enter the early-starting pathway leave it again after
Darwin C 1965 The Expression of the Emotions in Man and some time. Although these processes of abstinence and
Animals. University of Chicago Press, Chicago, (originally desistance are very important for explanation and
published in 1872) prevention, far less research has been directed toward
DePaulo B M 1992 Nonverbal behavior and self-presentation. the issue of nonviolence in high-risk youngsters than
Psychology Bulletin 111: 203–43
DePaulo B M, Friedman H S 1998 Nonverbal communi-
vice versa (Lo$ sel and Bender 2001). These studies are
cation. In: D T Gilbert, S T Fiske, G Lindzey (eds.) The part of the more general research on resilience, that is
Handbook of Social Psychology, 4th edn. McGraw-Hill, a process of positive adaptation in spite of significant
Boston, pp. 3–40 adversity (Luthar et al. 2000). However, research in
Eibl-Eibesfeldt I 1975 Loe and Hate. Schocken, New York this field is subject to various conceptual and meth-
Ekman P (ed.) 1982 Emotion in the Human Face, 2nd edn. odological problems.
Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, UK
Ekman P, Friesen W V 1969 The repertoire of nonverbal
behavior: Categories, origins, usage, and coding. Semiotica 1: 1. Conceptual and Methodological Issues
49–98
Ekman P, Friesen W V 1978 The Facial Action Coding System:
Major problems are the definition and operationaliza-
A Technique for the Measurement of Facial Moement. tion of risk, the ambiguity of protective factors, and
Consulting Psychologists Press, Palo Alto, CA the multidimensionality of behavioral outcomes. Re-
Goffman E 1959 The Presentation of Self in Eeryday Life. silience is not a static personality characteristic. The
Doubleday\Anchor, Garden City, NY positive outcome of nonviolence may vary over time
Hall J A 1984 Nonerbal Sex Differences: Communication and developmental circumstances depending on the
Accuracy and Expressie Style. The Johns Hopkins University presence or absence of risk and protective factors.
Press, Baltimore, MD However, these risk and protective factors are not
Hall E T 1966 The Hidden Dimension. Doubleday, Garden City, different classes of variables but often the negative vs.
NY
Knapp M L, Hall J A 1997 Nonerbal Communication in Human
positive pole of the same variable (e.g., low vs. good
Interaction, 4th edn. Harcourt Brace, Fort Worth, TX intelligence). Risk can be defined by objective variables
Krauss R M, Chen Y, Chawla P 1996 Nonverbal behavior and (e.g., low SES) or subjective indicators (e.g., violence-
nonverbal communication: What do conversational hand prone attitudes). In the latter case, protective mech-
gestures tell us? Adances in Experimental Social Psychology anisms may be partially confounded with both risk
28: 389–449 and behavioral outcome. Similarly important is the
Rosenthal R, Hall J A, DiMatteo M R, Rogers P L, Archer D degree of risk; without a substantial risk, a good
1979 Sensitiity to Nonerbal Communication: The PONS outcome may be misinterpreted as a result of a
Test. The Johns Hopkins University Press, Baltimore, MD protective mechanism. In contrast, the more risks are
Russell J A, Fernandez-Dols J M (eds.) 1997 The Psychology of
Facial Expression. Cambridge University Press, Paris
present, the less variance is left over for protective
Scherer K R, Ekman P (eds.) 1982 Handbook of Nonerbal effects.
Behaior Research. Cambridge University Press, New York Protective factors are variables that differ between
groups of comparable high risk who show positive vs.
J. A. Hall negative behavioral outcome (Rutter 1985). A good
example of research is the Kauai Study that investigat-
ed resilient and deviant children from early childhood
to adulthood (Werner and Smith 1992). Other authors
Nonviolence: Protective Factors suggest that a factor should be labeled protective only
if it reduces the problem behavior when risk is high but
Violent behavior is the outcome of complex transac- has no impact when risk is low (Stattin et al. 1997). In
tions between biological, psychological, and social both concepts, the search for protective mechanisms
factors under specific situational conditions. Although refers to interaction effects (for other methodological

10706
Noniolence: Protectie Factors

concepts, see Luthar et al. 2000; Masten et al. 1990). Similarly, biological variables can contribute to
However, the theoretical interpretation of such effects protective mechanisms in the context of given social
may be difficult when multiple risks or protective risks. For example, autonomic arousal as indicated by
variables are combined. This is often the case because high HRL or enhanced skin conductance (SC) reduce
single variables show only small correlations with later the risk of criminality for males with a criminal father,
violence. for boys from a low SES background, or for boys from
Given a specific risk constellation, even the same families with poor parent–child relationships. It was
pole of a variable may have both risk and protective also found that boys who desisted from antisocial
effects depending on the kind of outcome. For ex- behavior had a higher HRL, higher SC arousal, and
ample, shyness in childhood may be a protective factor better SC conditioning than persistent criminals.
against violence but be a risk for anxiety and de- These results can be interpreted within neuropsycholo-
pression. Therefore, risk and protective factors should gical theories on imbalances between the behavior
not be conceptualized too broadly, but it must always activation and inhibition system or on prefrontal
be asked: ‘risk for and protection against what type of dysfunctions in aggressive individuals. Perhaps, the
problem behavior?’ Sometimes, a positive behavioral stress hormone cortisol may have a similar inhibiting
outcome is defined not only by the absence of effect on both the testosterone level and aggressive
psychopathology but also by indicators of particularly behavior. Enhanced salivary cortisol accompanies
good adaptation and competence. In the present shyness, anxiety, depression, and high arousal in some
paper, positive outcome is defined by the absence of studies. Thus, biological protective mechanisms may
violence or other serious offending in spite of various reflect an inhibited temperament that is associated
risk constellations. This contains both processes of with a reduced risk of antisociality. However, there are
general abstinence and desistance after a period of also comorbidities between externalizing and inter-
violence. nalizing problems and specific forms of overcontrolled
aggression.

2. Examples of Protectie Mechanisms


2.2 Temperament Characteristics
The development of persistently antisocial behavior
can be conceptualized as the outcome of a chain Impulsivity, hyperactivity, negative emotionality
reaction of multilevel, biopsychosocial influences from (neuroticism), sensation seeking, and risk taking are
childhood to adolescence (see Fig. 1). personality dispositions for violence. These and re-
The relations do not represent a necessary causal lated characteristics describe a child with a ‘difficult’
sequence, and most of the constructs reveal only low temperament. In contrast, an ‘easy’ temperament
correlations with each other. Although the cumulation seems to have a protective function (e.g., Werner and
of factors increases the probability of violence, even Smith 1982). Children who did not develop aggressive
multiple risks may not lead to serious problem and other antisocial behavior in spite of social and
behavior when one or more links in the chain are individual risks had advantageous dispositions such as
‘broken’ because of positive personal or social positive mood, low impulsivity, flexibility in adap-
resources. The following factors may exert such a tation, sociability, and regularity of biological func-
protective function. tions. According to Moffitt et al. (1996), boys who
behaved untypically well in both childhood and
adolescence showed the least difficult temperament at
ages 3 and 5 years. However, positive temperament
2.1 Biological Factors
traits should not be interpreted as innate and stable
Biological risks for aggressive behavior have been resiliency because those boys who recovered from
found on various levels, for example: behavior gen- early antisocial behavior improved in temperament.
etics, autonomic underarousal, functional deficits in Children with a difficult temperament are also more
the prefrontal brain, hormonal influences such as high frequently victims of parental hostility, neglect, and
testosterone, neurotransmitter processes, prenatal fac- physical abuse. This, in turn, increases the risk of later
tors such as maternal smoking, or perinatal complica- aggressive behavior. In contrast, an easy temperament
tions (Raine et al. 1997). However, we can rarely may reduce or avoid such negative family influences.
assume simple causal relationships. Complex interac- The child’s personality can also help them to cope
tions between biological and social factors seem to be successfully with unfavorable social experiences. Both
more the rule than an exception. For example, low perspectives are suggested by studies on disadvantaged
heart rate level (HRL) is primarily a risk for aggression and maltreated children. For example, Cicchetti and
in children from relatively normal family back- Rogosch (1997) used Block’s personality concepts of
grounds. Pregnancy and birth complications are par- ego overcontrol (e.g., rigid impulse containment),
ticularly related to violence when combined with undercontrol (e.g., impulsivity), and ego resiliency
maternal rejection, low SES, or family instability. (dynamic capacity to modify the level of ego control

10707
Noniolence: Protectie Factors

Figure 1
A simple model of cumulating risks in development of persistent antisocial behavior

according to environmental demands). These authors Individuals with good cognitive competencies may
found that maltreated children who were more ego not only avoid aggression in concrete situations but
resilient or more overcontrolled adapted better and also experience more positive reactions in a variety of
were less a target of continued abuse. However, social contexts. Thus they are able to construct a more
inhibited children may sometimes ‘pay the price’ for favorable environment. For example, high-risk indivi-
better protection against aggressive behavior by ex- duals who planned their lives more carefully had
posure to a higher risk for internalizing problems. better school experiences and chose better partners
than others from similar backgrounds (Quinton et al.
1993).

2.3 Cognitie Competencies


2.4 Childrearing and Attachment
Whereas low intelligence is a risk factor for persistently
violent and delinquent behavior, above-average or Parenting behaviors such as aggression, rejection,
high intelligence can have a protective function (Wer- inconsistency, or laxness are well-replicated risk fac-
ner and Smith 1982). This is both a main effect and a tors for serious and violent offending in youngsters. In
buffer against risks such as a criminal father, a contrast, parental involvement, positive emotionality,
multiproblem milieu, or earlier problem behavior. A firm supervision, as well as good and reflective
protective effect of cognitive competencies is in line communication may help children to adapt relatively
with theories on deficits in neuropsychological func- well in spite of risk factors for antisocial development
tioning, social information processing, and self-con- (Stouthamer-Loeber et al. 1993, Werner and Smith
trol in aggressive youngsters. Intelligent children are 1992). Safety and stability in the family also seem to
more able to plan their behavior, anticipate conse- moderate negative environmental influences such as
quences, solve conflicts verbally, and develop alterna- neighborhood violence (Richters and Martinez 1993).
tives to aggressive reactions (Rutter et al. 1998). However, results on interactions between family vari-
Probably, it is not primarily general intellectual ables and community violence are mixed.
capacity but more competencies in planning, com- On the one hand, protective functions of parenting
munication, and interpersonal problem solving that behavior operate through reinforcement and the mod-
help to cope with social risks of antisocial behavior. eling of nonaggressive behavior. On the other hand,

10708
Noniolence: Protectie Factors

emotionally responsive and supporting parental be- against various emotional and behavioral problems.
havior increases the probability of secure attachment However, in the case of aggressive behavior, this
as an internal working model for the future. At- general relation needs to be differentiated. Whereas
tachment to a competent caregiver promotes a struc- support from nondeviant family members, relatives,
tured, predictable, and regulated environment that is teachers, educators, or friends may function in a
relevant for general neuronal functioning and the protective way (Werner and Smith 1992), belonging to
regulation of aggressive behavior. Although there is a delinquent peer group is a strong predictor of violent
some controversy about the stability and generality of offending and gang membership (Thornberry 1998).
attachment types, secure bonding proves to be a Whether the peer network has a risk or a protective
protective factor against various risks for behavioral effect depends on the characteristics of the individuals
problems. These include, for example, family conflicts, involved. Having nondelinquent peers protects against
child abuse, multiple life burdens, a mentally ill parent, the onset or persistence of antisocial behavior, whereas
parental divorce, or a deprived lower-class milieu delinquent friends promote the opposite (Stouthamer-
(Lo$ sel and Bender 2001). Loeber et al. 1993, Moffitt et al. 1996). Because
Protective functions of an emotionally positive and individuals join groups according to their own inter-
accepting as well as norm-oriented and adequately ests and attitudes, deviant peers may be both a
demanding upbringing are not restricted to the family precursor and a result of one’s own antisociality.
context. Such an ‘authoritative’ climate in school or in Children with early onset, undersocialized-aggressive
residential care institutions can also have a positive behavior seem to be rejected by ‘normal’ youngsters
effect on children at risk for aggression (Lo$ sel and and thus more likely to join groups with antisocial
Bliesener 1994). This is important because these problems.
youngsters often come from families in which pro- How far the peer group exerts long-term positive or
tective childrearing features are lacking. negative influences on the development of antisocial
behavior depends not least on the quality of relations
to family and school. Satisfaction with the social
network also seems to play a role. Both antisocial and
2.5 School Achieement and Bonding
nondeviant adolescents continue in their respective
Low academic achievement and deficits in school behavior when they are satisfied with their network
bonding are replicated predictors of violent offending (Bender and Lo$ sel 1997). However, not being em-
(Hawkins et al. 1998). It is not clear how far school bedded in a social network has a protective effect for
problems reflect a cause, an effect, or merely a correlate those with previous antisociality, but a risk effect for
of antisocial behavior. However, academic achieve- those who were nondeviant before. Similarly, het-
ment and good school bonding can have a protective erosexual relations may have a risk or a protective
function when risks for antisociality are present. effect depending on the individual age, gender, and
Prospective studies have shown that success at school, social context.
regular attendance, achievement orientation, parental
interest in a school career, and higher education are
related to nonviolence or a recovery from antisocial
2.7 Self-Related and Social Cognitions
behavior in childhood (Moffitt et al. 1996, Werner and
Smith 1992). Good school achievement and school The literature on resilience emphasizes a positive self-
bonding still seem to exercise a protective function concept and self-efficacy as a relatively general and
against antisocial behavior beyond the effects of strong protective factor. Although this view is support-
intelligence. ed partially by results on aggressive behavior (e.g.,
Positive experiences in school are a source of self- Werner and Smith 1992), self-esteem plays a more
affirmation that helps to compensate for negative ambiguous role in this field. Subgroups of violent
experiences in the family. A good school career is also individuals do not have a negative but an inflated self-
an indicator for bonds to society and increases the esteem (Baumeister et al. 1996). This is not just the
chance for nondeviant opportunities in life. Similar case in adult psychopaths. Already aggressive children
effects are found for good work behavior, motivation have more idealized ratings of their own competence
for vocational improvement, and job stability. Such and relationship quality than nonaggressive children.
characteristics lead to positive turning points of Such youngsters may show aggression more readily in
previous delinquents in late adolescence or young response to perceived negative feedback from others.
adulthood (Sampson and Laub 1993). Idealized and defensive self-schemata contribute to
forms of social information processing that trigger
and reinforce aggressive behavior (e.g., hostility bias,
egocentric goal setting, positive evaluation of ag-
2.6 Peer Groups and Social Networks
gressive reactions (Crick and Dodge 1994).
Resilience research suggests that social support and That specific self-related and social cognitions have
involvement in social networks have a protective effect a protective effect against aggressive behavior is

10709
Noniolence: Protectie Factors

supported by findings on offender treatment. Here, natural history of violent and nonviolent behavior.
cognitive–behavioral approaches show particularly Protective functions against violence and other serious
positive results. Such programs address social per- antisocial behavior may emerge on and between
ception, self-critical thinking, anger management, biological, psychological, and social levels at various
social perspective taking, victim awareness, creative ages. Although relatively few studies are designed
thinking, interpersonal problem solving, social skills specifically to detect protective mechanisms against
and attitudes. Ethical and religious beliefs can also violence, the following factors seem to contribute to
have a protective function against antisocial behavior. nonviolence in spite of serious risks:
(a) Genetic dispositions and high autonomic arousal
(b) An easy or inhibited temperament
(c) Personality dispositions of ego resiliency
2.8 Neighborhood and Community Factors (d) Above-average intelligence and good planning
(e) Emotionally secure bonds to the mother or other
Although it is methodologically difficult to separate
caregivers in or outside the family
individual and family characteristics from neighbor-
(f) Affiliation to nonaggressive peers and (for
hood and community characteristics, the latter seem
specific risk groups) some social isolation
to harbor a specific risk for antisocial behavior. In
(g) Supportive relations to nondeviant friends or
analogy to this, the protective model assumes that
heterosexual partners
positive features of the broader social context, such as
(h) Warmth, consistency, and supervision in child-
low poverty, low violence, low unemployment, many
rearing in the family, at school, or in other institutions
intact families, and high cohesion, may buffer negative
(i) Adult role models of prosocial behavior and
influences on the individual and microsocial level. For
resilience under difficult developmental circumstances
example, Kupersmidt et al. (1995) found no main
(j) Bonding to school and experiences of success at
effect of neighborhood factors, such as income, eth-
school
nicity, and family structure, on childhood aggression.
(k) Vocational achievement and employment stab-
However, there were various interactions suggesting
ility
that the same neighborhood may be protective for one
(l) Experiences of self-efficacy in nonaggressive
family type while functioning as a risk or neutral
activities and a good but not inflated self-esteem
factor for others. Middle-class neighborhoods, for
(m) Cognitive schemata, beliefs, and information
example, had a protective effect only on the aggressive
processing that are not aggression-prone
behavior of black children from low-income, single-
(n) Experiences of meaning and structure in one’s
parent homes. Wikstro$ m and Loeber (2000) only
life
found a negative effect for disadvantaged neigbor-
(o) Neighborhoods that do not concentrate too
hoods on older children with low individual or family
many high-risk families, are nonviolent, and socially
risks.
integrated in a nondeviant mode.
Neighborhoods with low violence may have a
Probably, none of these factors are protective under
protective effect because they provide fewer negative
all kinds and degrees of risk. Some counteract violence
role models and reinforcements for aggressive be-
only under specific circumstances. The protective
havior (Thornberry 1998). However, subjective experi-
function of others may depend on their dosage, age
ences of the local climate have also to be considered.
periods, gender, or co-occurrence with other indi-
For example, Seidman et al. (1998) reported more
vidual or social resources. More research is needed to
adolescent antisocial behavior in neighborhoods with
understand the interaction of various levels of risk and
moderate poverty and violence than in neighborhoods
protection.
that ranked high on such structural features. This
Further levels of influence need also to be integrated.
relation held primarily for middle (not young) adoles-
For example, low exposure to violence in the media,
cents who perceived their neighborhoods as more
effective fire-arm control, and generally low violence
hassling and less cohesive. There was also a subgroup
in the society may function in a protective mode.
with low antisociality who experienced themselves as
Target hardening, victim assertiveness, informal
disconnected to their neighborhood. This may indicate
neighborhood control, and other forms of situational
a protective effect of the family’s actively reduced
crime prevention can also counteract the behavioral
involvement in a disadvantaged and violent neighbor-
manifestation of dispositions to aggressiveness.
hood.
Last but not least, it is necessary to integrate
experimental research on prevention and intervention
more strongly with the correlational-longitudinal re-
search on protective factors (Farrington and Coid
2.9 Conclusions and Perspecties
2001). Although successful programs seem to address
Research on protective factors and mechanisms is not similar factors as those that are protective in the
the counterpart of traditional risk research but an natural environment, there is not yet much exchange
integrated part of differentiated explanations of the between the two areas of research.

10710
Normatie Aspects of Social and Behaioral Science

See also: Aggression in Adulthood, Psychology of; Quinton D, Pickles A, Maughan B, Rutter M 1993 Partners,
Attachment Theory: Psychological; Networks: Social; peers and pathways: Assortative pairing and continuities in
conduct disorder. Deelopment and Psychopathology 5: 763–83
Self-regulation in Adulthood; Self-regulation in Child-
Raine A, Farrington D P, Brennan P, Mednick S A (eds.) 1997
hood; Violence and Media; Violence, History of; Biosocial Bases of Violence. Plenum, New York
Violence in Anthropology; Violence: Public Richters J E, Martinez P E 1993 Violent communities, family
choices, and children’s chances: An algorithm for improving
the odds. Deelopment and Psychopathology 5: 609–27
Rutter M 1985 Resilience in the face of adversity. Protective
Bibliography factors and resistance to psychiatric disorder. British Journal
of Psychiatry 147: 598–611
Baumeister R F, Smart L, Boden J M 1996 Relation of threat-
Rutter M, Giller H, Hagell A 1998 Antisocial Behaior by Young
ened egotism to violence and aggression: the dark side of high
People. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, UK
self-esteem. Psychological Bulletin 103: 5–33
Sampson R J, Laub J H 1993 Crime in the Making: Pathways
Bender D, Lo$ sel F 1997 Protective and risk effects of peer
and Turning Points through Life. Harvard University Press,
relations and social support on antisocial behaviour in
Cambridge, MA
adolescents from multi-problem milieus. Journal of Ado-
Seidman E, Yoshikawa H, Roberts A, Chesir-Teran D, Allen L,
lescence 20: 661–78
Friedman J L, Aber J L 1998 Structural and experiental
Cicchetti D, Rogosch F A 1997 The role of self-organization in
neighborhood contexts, developmental stage, and antisocial
the promotion of resilience in maltreated children. Deel-
behavior among urban adolescents. Deelopment and Psy-
opment and Psychopathology 9: 797–815
chopathology 10: 259–81
Crick N R, Dodge K A 1994 A review and reformulation of
Stattin H, Romelsjo$ A, Stenbacka M 1997 Personal resources as
social information-processing mechanisms in children’s social
modifiers of the risk for future criminality. British Journal of
adjustment. Psychological Bulletin 115: 74–101
Criminology 37: 198–223
Farrington D P, Coid J (eds.) 2001 Preention of Adult Antisocial
Stouthamer-Loeber M, Loeber R, Farrington D P, Zhang Q,
Behaior. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, UK (in
van Kammen W, Maguin E 1993 The double edge of
press)
protective and risk factors for delinquency: Interrelations and
Hawkins J D, Herrenkohl T, Farrington D P, Brewer D,
developmental patterns. Deelopment and Psychopathology 5:
Catalano R F, Harachi T W 1998 A review of predictors of
683–701
youth violence. In: Loeber R, Farrington D P (eds.) Serious &
Thornberry T P 1998 Membership in youth gangs and in-
Violent Juenile Offenders: Risk Factors and Successful Inter-
volvement in serious and violent offending. In: Loeber R,
entions. Sage, Thousand Oaks, CA
Farrington D P (eds.) Serious & Violent Juenile Offenders:
Kapersmidt J B, Griesler J C, De Rosier M E, Patterson C J,
Risk Factors and Successful Interentions. Sage, Thousand
Davis P W 1995 Childhood aggression and peer relations in
Oaks, CA
the context of family and neighborhood factors. Child
Werner E E, Smith R S 1982 Vulnerable but Inincible. McGraw-
Deelopment 66: 360–75
Hill, New York
Loeber R, Farrington D P (eds.) 1998 Serious and Violent
Werner E E, Smith R S 1992 Oercoming the Odds. Cornell
Juenile Offenders: Risk Factors and Successful Interentions.
University Press, Ithaca, NY
Sage, Thousand Oaks, CA
Wikstro$ m P-O, Loeber R 2000 Do disadvantaged neighborhoods
Loeber R, Stouthamer-Loeber M 1998 Development of juvenile
cause well-adjusted children to become adolescent delin-
aggression and violence: Some common misconceptions and
quents? Criminology 38: 1109–42
controversies. American Psychologist 53: 242–59
Lo$ sel F, Bender D 2001 Protective factors and resilience. In:
Farrington D P, Coid J (eds.) Preention of Adult Antisocial F. Lo$ sel
Behaior. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, UK (in
press)
Copyright # 2001 Elsevier Science Ltd.
Lo$ sel F, Bliesener T 1994 Some high-risk adolescents do not All rights reserved.
develop conduct problems: A study of protective factors.
International Journal of Behaioral Deelopment 17: 753–77 Normative Aspects of Social and
Luthar S S, Cicchetti D, Becker B 2000 The construct of
resilience: A critical evaluation and guidelines for future work.
Behavioral Science
Child Deelopment 71: 543–62
Masten A S, Best K M, Garmezy N 1990 Resilience and Since their very beginning, genuine concern about the
development: Contributions from the study of children who scientific character of social and behavioral sciences
overcome adversity. Deelopment and Psychopathology 2: has continuously stimulated the development of soph-
425–44 isticated methodologies for this group of disciplines,
McCord J, Tremblay R (eds.) 1995 Preenting Antisocial but also shifted interest away from the variety of
Behaior: Interention From Birth Through Adolescence. Guil- normative aspects ineluctably connected with them.
ford Press, London Most important among these widely differing aspects
Moffitt T E 1993 Adolescence-limited and life-course-persistent
are (1) object and objective of the social sciences and
antisocial behavior: A developmental taxonomy. Psycho-
logical Reiew 100: 674–701 their bearing on methods and possible results, (2) the
Moffitt T E, Caspi A, Dickson N, Silva P, Stanton W 1996 role of value judgments in social and behavioral
Childhood-onset versus adolescent-onset antisocial conduct science, and (3) the feasibility of rational argumen-
problems in males: Natural history from ages 3 to 18 years. tation about norms. In the sequel, the concept of norm
Deelopment and Psychopathology 8: 399–424 will be used in a wide sense to cover norms followed

10711

International Encyclopedia of the Social & Behavioral Sciences ISBN: 0-08-043076-7

Você também pode gostar