Você está na página 1de 6

ARTICLE IN PRESS

Journal of Environmental Psychology 28 (2008) 203–208


www.elsevier.com/locate/jep

Moral reasoning and concern for the environment


Christie P. Karpiak, Galen L. Baril
Department of Psychology, University of Scranton, 206 Alumni Memorial Hall, Scranton, PA 18510-4596, USA
Available online 27 December 2007

Abstract

The relation between Kohlberg’s cognitive moral reasoning and concern for the environment was measured in 158 college students.
Rest’s Defining Issues Test and Thompson and Barton’s measure of environmental attitudes were administered. Principled moral
reasoning, the weighted ranking of responses at the most advanced level of moral development, correlated positively with ecocentrism
(belief in the intrinsic importance of nature), negatively with environmental apathy, and was unrelated to anthropocentrism (belief that
nature is important because it is central to human wellbeing). Ecocentrism, the only attitude that has been found in previous research to
correspond with environmentally friendly behavior, was predicted by principled moral reasoning, gender, and college major.
r 2007 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction However, a pro-environmental attitude may result from


different motivations that could have very different
1.1. Moral reasoning and concern for the environment implications for behavior and cognitive process. For
example, Stern, Dietz, and Kalof (1993) and Schultz
Philosophical, moral, and religious considerations of the (2000) proposed the value–belief–norm (VBN) theory that
relationship between humans and the rest of nature date identifies three types of environmental concerns: egoistic,
back thousands of years to the major eastern religions as social-altruistic, and biospheric. Related to this concept,
well as tribal religions world wide (Nash, 1989). The Thompson and Barton (1994) developed a scale to measure
history of scientific research on these issues, embodied in ecocentrism (the importance of the environment for its own
the areas of environmentalism, conservation, and sustain- sake, similar to biospheric concerns), anthropocentrism
ability, is much shorter; it is nevertheless quite extensive. (the importance of the environment for use by humans,
For example, Vining and Ebreo’s (2002) review of the which is similar to some combination of social-altruistic
range of theories that have been applied to this field and egoistic values), and finally apathy toward environ-
contains 165 references and at least 26 distinct theories. mental issues. They found ecocentrism to be positively
Much of the research in this area has involved the related to conserving behaviors and membership in
measurement of attitudes and opinions about the environ- environmental organizations and negatively related to
ment and conservation. Initially, these were somewhat environmental apathy. Anthropocentrism is positively
unsophisticated measures consisting of one or a few face related to environmental apathy and negatively related to
valid items. Dunlap and Van Liere (1978) developed and conserving behaviors and membership in environmental
later revised (Dunlap, Van Liere, Mertig, & Jones, 2000) organizations.
one of the first psychometrically and conceptually sophis- Schultz and Zelezny (1999) found that ecocentrism is
ticated instruments to assess positivity toward the environ- also negatively associated with the values of power and
ment: the New Ecological Paradigm (NEP) scale, now traditionalism while anthropocentrism is positively asso-
consisting of 15 items that generate a single score ciated with the values of power, tradition, security and, like
representing attitude toward the environment. ecocentrism, the value of conformity. They also found the
NEP is positively related with ecocentrism, as might be
Corresponding author. Tel.: +1 570 941 5886; fax: +1 570 941 7899. expected since the NEP showed exactly the same pattern of
E-mail address: karpiakc2@scranton.edu (C.P. Karpiak). relationships found for ecocentrism.

0272-4944/$ - see front matter r 2007 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.jenvp.2007.12.001
ARTICLE IN PRESS
204 C.P. Karpiak, G.L. Baril / Journal of Environmental Psychology 28 (2008) 203–208

All of these measures and much of the associated research 2. Method


have an explicit or implicit foundation in moral/ethical
considerations. In fact, Kahn (2006) looks at the extent to 2.1. Participants
which explicit moral concepts and theories apply to
environmental/conservation research in general. His litera- One hundred sixty-four students (60% female) enrolled
ture review is set in a broad context that includes ‘‘the in various sections of the introductory psychology course
traditional moral-developmental view, as circumscribed by at a comprehensive Jesuit University in Pennsylvania
Piaget and Kohlberg, that focuses on justice, rights and received credit for participating in this study. Of these,
obligations’’ (p. 461). However, there is no other reference to six turned in incomplete measures and were not included in
either Piaget or Kohlberg in the article except for a mention the current analyses. A wide array of academic majors were
of ‘‘Kohlberg’s just community’’ (p. 464). In addition, represented, including biological sciences (17%), arts and
Vining and Ebreo’s (2002) review of theories that have been humanities (16%), social sciences (16%), nursing (13%),
applied to conservation research has no reference whatso- occupational and physical therapy (11%), communication
ever to Kohlberg or Piaget. Finally, our extensive literature (10%), business (8%), education (6%), and undecided/
search did not uncover any research at all on environ- other (2%). Students were predominantly white, Catholic
mental, conservation or sustainability issues that involved (67%), and of middle (48%) and upper-middle (38%)
Kohlberg’s (1984) theory of moral development or deriva- socioeconomic class.
tives of it, e.g. Rest’s (1993) Defining Issues Test (DIT).
Kohlberg’s theory postulates the development of moral 2.2. Measures and procedure
reasoning based on the concepts of justice, fairness, rights,
and obligations. Such reasoning develops with age, We administered a packet of tests and questionnaires to
education, and experience from relatively primitive, self- groups of students. Demographic information included
serving levels (Preconventional Stages 1 and 2) focused on academic major, gender, socioeconomic status (rated low,
punishment and rewards, to reasoning that focuses on mid-low, middle, mid-high, and high) and religious
personal relationships (Conventional Stage 3) and societal affiliation.
rules and laws (Conventional Stage 4), and finally to The three-scenario version of Rest’s (1993) DIT was used
reasoning that takes into account the broader moral to measure participants’ cognitive moral reasoning. Parti-
principles of justice and fairness that underlie these rules cipants read the Heinz, Escaped Prisoner, and Newspaper
and laws (Postconventional Stages 5 and 6) (Kohlberg, dilemmas. For example, the Heinz dilemma is about a
1984; Thoma, 2006). For a critique of Kohlberg’s theory, husband whose wife is dying and needs a drug for which
see Lapsley (2006). the husband does not have enough money to pay. Since the
It seems obvious that the development of moral reason- druggist will not reduce the price, the husband is
ing should be related to views about whether and why (e.g., considering stealing the drug.
ecocentrism versus anthropocentrism reasons) conserva- Participants ranked their top four considerations in
tion and sustainability are important. Anthropocentrism reaching a solution for each dilemma from a list of 12
seems to be in part related to the pure self-serving concerns considerations per scenario. These rankings were converted
of Kohlberg’s Preconventional Level because of the into scores for Stages 2 through 6 based on the weighted
egocentric component of anthropocentrism. However, rankings of chosen considerations from each stage (Rest,
there is also the social-altruistic component, which is likely 1993). A P-score (principled moral reasoning) was then
to be related to higher levels of moral reasoning. generated from a combination of Stages 5 and 6 scores
Ecocentrism would seem to be the most advanced level of (Postconventional reasoning). For instance, one P-score
environmental concern and would therefore be associated item (Stage 5) is ‘‘Would stealing in such a case bring about
with the highest level of moral reasoning: the Postconven- more total good for the whole society or not?’’ Ranking
tional Level. Support can be found for this proposed this as one of the top four considerations in making the
connection between Kohlberg’s justice reasoning and decision to steal the drug or not would contribute to a high
sustainability attitudes in the research using Schwartz’s P-score. ‘‘Whether a community’s laws are going to be
Value Survey (Schultz et al., 2005; Schultz & Zelezny, 1999; upheld’’ is an example of the Stage 4 item, and ‘‘Whether
Schwartz, 1994), showing that the dimension of Univers- Heinz is stealing for himself or doing this solely to help
alism, which contains social justice and equality as someone else’’ typifies Stage 3. Finally, ‘‘Is Heinz willing to
components, is positively related to ecocentrism. risk getting shoty’’ is Stage 1/2. Following Rest’s (1993)
This study is a test of the relationship between recommendations, only the P-score from this 3-scenario
Kohlbergian moral reasoning and environmental opinions. ‘short form’ version of the DIT was used in subsequent
Specifically, we predicted that ecocentered attitudes would analyses.
be positively related to Postconventional (justice) moral We measured attitudes toward the environment using
reasoning and that anthropocentric views and environ- the instrument developed by Thompson and Barton (1994)
mental apathy would be negatively or unrelated to discussed in the introduction. This measure consists of 30
Postconventional reasoning. statements about the environment, each rated from 1
ARTICLE IN PRESS
C.P. Karpiak, G.L. Baril / Journal of Environmental Psychology 28 (2008) 203–208 205

(strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree), and results in (90% or more in one category). Correlations between the
scores on three scales. The ecocentrism scale provides variables from our a priori hypotheses are reported in
information about an individual’s degree of belief that the Table 1, along with significant demographic variables.
environment, including animals and plants, is valuable in Ecocentrism correlated positively with principled moral
its own right, separate from its utility for humans. The reasoning. Anthropocentrism was unrelated to principled
anthropocentrism scale indexes a person’s concern about moral reasoning, and apathy toward the environment
the environment for its importance to human survival and correlated negatively with principled moral reasoning. The
industry. The apathy scale is a measure of general lack of lack of relation between principled moral reasoning and
concern about the environment and environmental issues. anthropocentric reasons for concern about the environ-
Information about the reliability and validity of this ment is interesting and perhaps counterintuitive, but is
measure may be found in Thompson and Barton (1994). consistent with reports from Schultz and Zelezny (1999)
regarding related values.
3. Results With regard to demographic and background factors,
only gender and major were related to the variables of
Means and standard deviations are presented in Table 1 interest. Women were higher in ecocentrism and lower in
for the three scales of the environmental attitudes measure apathy than men. Consistent with much of the literature,
and for the principled moral reasoning score (the P-score) there was no relation between gender and principled moral
from the DIT. Means and standard deviations for each reasoning. Majors in biological sciences evidenced higher
environmental scale were similar to average results from principled moral reasoning and ecocentrism, and lower
the samples presented by Thompson and Barton (1994), anthropocentrism and apathy, than other majors.
and data from the anthropocentrism and ecocentrism A conservative statistical approach, standard multiple
scales presented for a large sample of the United States regression with simultaneous entry of all independent
college students by Schultz and Zelezny (1999). Internal variables, was employed to examine the relative importance
consistency was fully consistent with reports by Thompson of these correlates in predicting environmental attitudes.
and Barton (1994), with Cronbach’s a ¼ .83 for ecocentr- Regression analyses were conducted separately for each of
ism, .70 for anthropocentrism, and .81 for apathy. The the three environmental scales, with P-score, gender, and
apathy scores evidenced moderate negative skew and a biology major as predictors. Multiple R, R2, and adjusted
square root transformation was conducted prior to the R2 for each dependent variable are presented in Table 2, as
linear analysis. The transformed variable was used in all are standardized regression coefficients (b) for each
linear analyses involving the apathy scale. independent variable.
Pearson correlations were run between the three These analyses indicated that principled moral reasoning
environmental scales, the principled moral reasoning score, contributes to the prediction of ecocentrism and of apathy.
and the demographic and background variables of gender, Ecocentrism was predicted by the model, F(3, 154) ¼ 6.48,
major, socioeconomic group (SES), and religion. Major, po.001, and each of the three predictor variables
SES, and religion were dummy-coded into specific con- contributed significantly to its prediction. Apathy was also
trasts (e.g., upper-middle SES versus all other SES) prior to predicted by the model, F(3, 154) ¼ 8.57, po.001, but only
analysis for all groups that contained at least 16 gender and P-score were significant contributors. In both
participants, based on Tabachnick and Fidell’s (2001)
warning regarding problems posed for linear analyses by
Table 2
markedly uneven distributions of dichotomous variables
Standard multiple regression analyses for variables predicting environ-
mental attitudes
Table 1
Means, standard deviations, and correlations for primary variables Variable R R2 Adjusted R2 b

Variable M SD 1 2 3 4 5 Ecocentrism .34 .11 .10


P-score .18*
DIT moral reasoning Gender .22**
1. P-score 26.75 14.93 Biological science .17*

Thompson and Barton environmental attitudes Apathy .38 .14 .13


2. Ecocentrism 3.85 .60 .21** P-score .26**
3. Anthropocentrism 3.17 .55 .11 .00 Gender .22**
4. Apathy 2.02 .61 .29** .55** .23** Biological science .14

Gender and relevant academic majors Anthropocentrism .22 .05 .03


5. Gendera .01 .20* .06 .20* P-score .07
6. Biological science .22** .18* .21** .17* .12 Gender .04
Biological science .19*
*po.05.
**po.01. *po.05.
a
Females ¼ 0 and males ¼ 1. **po.01.
ARTICLE IN PRESS
206 C.P. Karpiak, G.L. Baril / Journal of Environmental Psychology 28 (2008) 203–208

cases the contribution was small, with the P-score attitude toward the intrinsic importance of the environ-
independently accounting for approximately the same ment (e.g., ‘‘The worst thing about the loss of the rain
amount of variability in these attitudes as gender. forest is that it will restrict the development of new
Anthropocentrism was also predicted by the model, medicines’’) that makes it surprising that ecocentrism and
F(3, 154) ¼ 2.71, p ¼ .047. However, as expected based on anthropocentrism are not negatively related. Interestingly,
the zero-order correlations, the actual amount predicted in our sample, students majoring in the biological sciences
was very small (adjusted R2 ¼ .03), and biological science evidenced less anthropocentric attitudes than students in
major was the only significant predictor. other majors. Self-selection into the field of biology
probably contributes to this relationship, but it is also
4. Discussion likely that the study of biology decreases anthropocentrism
through enhanced understanding of nonhuman life.
The relationships we observed between the three scales Biology majors also evidenced higher ecocentrism than
of the environmental measure are fully consistent with other majors.
those reported by Thompson and Barton (1994). Ecocentr- As predicted, apathy was negatively related to principled
ism is strongly negatively correlated with apathy, while moral reasoning. People who are relatively low on
anthropocentrism is unrelated with ecocentrism and principled moral reasoning evidence higher levels of apathy
positively correlated with apathy. Individuals who assign about the environment.
intrinsic value to the environment are less likely to be Finally, we predicted and found that ecocentrism is
apathetic, while those who value the environment for its positively related to Kohlberg’s principled moral reason-
utility to humans are more likely to be apathetic. These ing. The basis of our prediction was that we thought that
results regarding anthropocentric motivations are consis- justice-oriented morality would extrapolate from humans
tent with analyses by Schultz and Zelezny (1999), and with to nature. However, most of the ecocentrism items seem to
the findings of Axelrod (1994), who used ecological reflect an emotional affinity toward nature (e.g., ‘‘Some-
dilemmas to test hypotheses about value orientations and times it makes me sad to see forests cleared for
found that socially oriented individuals ‘‘appear to place agriculture’’) rather than the extrapolation from humans
the needs of many before the needs of any one individual or to nature of an impartial and universal justice orientation
the natural environment (p. 101).’’ A better understanding (Postconventional reasoning). In fact, a strong philosophi-
of these intriguing patterns is potentially important in cal, even religious, aspect of the environmental movement
promoting environmentally friendly behavior, since is the importance of an affectively based realization of the
Thompson and Barton (1994) found that ecocentric ‘‘oneness’’ of everything (see Suzuki, 2002). Schultz’s
attitudes are predictive of such behavior while anthropo- (2000) study of the relationship between ‘‘perspective
centric attitudes are not. taking’’ and biospheric concerns is congruent with this
We found women to be higher on ecocentrism and lower conceptualization. Given these considerations, perhaps it is
on apathy than men. These findings are consistent with counterintuitive that the cognitive justice orientation, the
popular conceptions about varying levels of concern for product of rational thought, is positively related to
others in women and men, with the environmental ecocentrism.
literature (see Stern & Dietz, 1994), and with recent Why, then, are Stages 5/6 related to ecocentrism?
meta-analyses (Zelezny, Chua, & Aldrich, 2000). None of Because perspective taking is absolutely essential for the
the publications that include the full scales of the application of fairness and justice to moral dilemmas
Thompson and Barton measure report their results for involving competing interests and rights, the core concern
gender, and Zelezny et al.’s (2000) results for a subset of of Kohlberg’s theory. In his chapter on ‘‘Justice as
items of the ecocentrism scale were generally supportive of Reversibility’’ (Kohlberg, 1981, pp. 190–230) and elsewhere
higher scores for women, but did not evidence significant (Kohlberg, 1986), Kohlberg describes in detail how ‘‘ideal
differences between women and men in the United States. role taking’’ and a ‘‘‘second-order’ use of the Golden Rule’’
Gender in our sample accounted for approximately 5% of (p. 203) are necessary to fairly resolve moral dilemmas.
the variability in both ecocentric and apathetic attitudes This perspective taking goes well beyond what occurs at
toward the environment, an amount that is fully consistent Stage 3 (where the basic Golden Rule is applied in the
with the large-scale data reported by Zelezny et al. (2000). context of a concern over the evaluation of significant
One of our primary predictions was that anthropocentr- others). It involves a complex multiple reversing of
ism would be negatively or unrelated to principled moral perspectives and the weighing of individual rights based
reasoning. We found that it was unrelated. Our reasoning on these perspectives: for example, the perspectives of the
was that anthropocentrism is thought to be composed of dying wife, the druggist, and the husband in the Heinz
not only a concern for human kind generally but also an dilemma. To extrapolate the rights of humans to the
egocentric concern (see Thompson & Barton, 1994; Schultz ‘‘rights of nature’’ requires both empathizing with nature
& Zelezny, 1999) which is unlikely to be congruent with the (Nash, 1989) and a clear understanding of ‘‘rights’’ in
Postconventional level of moral reasoning. In fact, some of general. Also, Walker (2006) finds that those at the higher
the anthropocentrism items reflect a particularly callous moral Stages are ‘‘more likely to be split in their moral
ARTICLE IN PRESS
C.P. Karpiak, G.L. Baril / Journal of Environmental Psychology 28 (2008) 203–208 207

orientation, that is, to evidence substantial amounts of anything. Also, the strongest effects are found at the adult
both care and justice reasoning’’ (p. 105). levels.
One limitation of this study is that our sample is They also found that moral reasoning educational
demographically restricted in two ways. First, no attempt programs that focus on dilemma discussion were effective,
was made to proportionally sample, much less randomly while general academic courses on ethics were not. This is
sample, demographic categories. Our sample came solely of particular interest since Axelrod (1994) developed
from introductory psychology classes, although a wide ‘‘ecological’’ dilemmas to study the relationship between
variety of majors are represented. In addition, the sample value orientation, economic and social conditions reflected
and the university in general are predominately white, in the dilemmas, and decision making. Such dilemmas and
middle and upper-middle class, and Catholic. However, as similar ones could easily be incorporated into a section of a
reported in the results section, the means of our measures course to develop environmental reasoning.
were comparable to the results of a broad, international These reviews also showed that personality and social
sample of college students obtained by Schultz and Zelezny development programs such as interventions that empha-
(1999). The amount of variability in environmental sized empathy training, communication skill training,
attitudes accounted for by gender in our sample also cooperation simulation games, and volunteer service work,
matches well with reports from the same broad, interna- improved moral reasoning. Even though causal sequence is
tional sample (Zelezny et al., 2000). In addition, our sample not established by our study, our results suggest the
shows wide variation for all of the relevant variables. possibility that improving moral reasoning with regard to
Nonetheless, caution is warranted, and further studies are humans might generalize to nature. If so, it is not such a
needed with more diverse samples. leap to entertain the intriguing possibility that increasing
Another concern is that the relationships we found, sensitivity to humans might increase sensitivity to the
although statistically significant, are small in terms of effect environment.
sizes. This is typical of the current literature on demo-
graphic and psychological predictors of environmental
attitudes and behaviors, and we believe it probably reflects References
real complexity in the development of these attitudes and
behaviors. A recent follow-up to our study (Irace, Cannon, Axelrod, L. J. (1994). Balancing personal needs with environmental
Karpiak, Baril, & Melone, 2007) replicated our basic preservation: Identifying the values that guide decisions in ecological
results with another, similar sample of university students, dilemmas. Journal of Social Issues, 50, 85–104.
Bebeau, M. J. (2002). The defining issues test and the four component
adding strength to our assertions about the relations model: Contributions to professional education. Journal of Moral
between moral reasoning and environmental attitudes. Education, 31, 271–295.
Still, further replication and extension to more diverse Dunlap, R. E., & Van Liere, K. (1978). The new environmental paradigm:
samples are clearly needed. Finally, like most of the A proposed measuring instrument and preliminary results. Journal of
Environmental Education, 9, 10–19.
relevant literature, our study is neither experimental nor
Dunlap, R. E., Van Liere, K. D., Mertig, A. G., & Jones, R. E. (2000).
longitudinal and thus cannot provide clear indication of Measuring endorsement of the New Ecological Paradigm: A revised
causal sequence. NEP scale. Journal of Social Issues, 56, 425–442.
How then can this study contribute to the understanding Irace, C., Cannon, J. T., Karpiak, C. P., Baril, G. L., & Melone, A. N.
and development of environmentally responsible attitudes (2007). Environmental attitudes: Relationships with right wing
and behaviors? First, it provides an empirical connection to authoritarianism, social dominance orientation, political affiliation,
and moral reasoning. Poster presented at the annual meeting of the
a well-developed area of theory and research: moral Eastern Psychological Association, Philadelphia, PA.
reasoning and behavior. Although such a connection has Kahn, P. H. (2006). Nature and moral development. In M. Killen, &
been alluded to by Kahn (2006) as mentioned above, it has J. Smetana (Eds.), Handbook of moral development (pp. 461–480).
never been established. Our discussion above would be an Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
King, P. M., & Mayhew, M. J. (2002). Moral judgement development in
initial step in developing the theoretical links between the
higher education: Insights from the defining issues test. Journal of
two lines of research and an expansion of our under- Moral Education, 31, 248–270.
standing environmentalism. Kohlberg, L. (1981). The philosophy of moral development: Moral stages
In addition, new directions for increasing sustainable and the idea of justice. New York: Harper and Row.
attitudes and behavior might be suggested by successful Kohlberg, L. (1984). The psychology of moral development: The nature and
education programs used to increase moral reasoning. For validity of moral stages. New York: Harper and Row.
Kohlberg, L. (1986). A current statement on some theoretical issues. In
example, Rest and Thoma (1986) reviewed 55 studies of S. Modgil, & E. Modgil (Eds.), Lawrence Kohlberg: Consensus and
moral reasoning interventions for college students. This controversy. Philadelphia: Falmer Press.
review was updated by King and Mayhew (2002) and Lapsley, D. K. (2006). Moral stage theory. In M. Killen, & J. Smetana
extended to programs for professionals by Rest and (Eds.), Handbook of moral development (pp. 37–66). Mahwah, NJ:
Narvaez (1994) and Bebeau (2002). These reviews showed, Lawrence Erlbaum.
Nash, R. F. (1989). The rights of nature: A history of environmental ethics.
for example, that the ideal duration of such programs for Madison, WI: University of Wisconsin Press.
college students is between 4 and 12 weeks; shorter Rest, J. (1993). Guide for the defining issues test. Minneapolis, MN: Center
durations are not effective and longer ones do not add for the Study of Ethical Development.
ARTICLE IN PRESS
208 C.P. Karpiak, G.L. Baril / Journal of Environmental Psychology 28 (2008) 203–208

Rest, J., & Narvaez, D. (Eds.). (1994). Moral development in the Stern, P. C., Dietz, T., & Kalof, L. (1993). Value orientations, gender, and
professions: Psychology and applied ethics. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence environmental concern. Environment and Behavior, 25, 322–348.
Erlbaum. Suzuki, D. (2002). The sacred balance: Rediscovering our place in nature.
Rest, J., & Thoma, S. J. (1986). Educational programs and interventions. Vancouver, CA: Greystone Books.
In J. Rest (Ed.), Moral development: Advances in research and theory Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (2001). Using multivariate statistics (4th
(pp. 59–88). New York: Praeger Publishers. ed.). Boston, MA: Allyn and Bacon.
Schultz, P. W. (2000). Empathizing with nature: The effects of perspective Thoma, S. J. (2006). Research on the defining issues test. In M. Killen, &
taking on concern for environmental issues. Journal of Social Issues, J. Smetana (Eds.), Handbook of moral development (pp. 67–91).
56, 391–406. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Schultz, P. W., Gouveia, V. V., Cameron, L. D., Tankha, G., Schmuck, Thompson, S. C., & Barton, M. (1994). Ecocentric and anthropocentric
P., & Franek, M. (2005). Values and their relationship to environ- attitudes toward the environment. Journal of Environmental Psychol-
mental concern and conservation behavior. Journal of Cross-Cultural ogy, 14, 149–157.
Psychology, 36, 457–475. Vining, J., & Ebreo, A. (2002). Emerging theoretical and methodological
Schultz, P. W., & Zelezny, L. (1999). Values as predictors of environ- perspectives on conservation behavior. In R. Bechtel, & A. Churchman
mental attitudes: Evidence for consistency across 14 countries. Journal (Eds.), New handbook of environmental psychology (pp. 541–558). New
of Environmental Psychology, 28, 255–265. York: Wiley.
Schwartz, S. H. (1994). Are there universal aspects in the structure Walker, L. J. (2006). Gender and morality. In M. Killen, & J. Smetana
and contents of human values? Journal of Social Issues, 50, (Eds.), Handbook of moral development (pp. 461–480). Mahwah, NJ:
19–45. Lawrence Erlbaum.
Stern, P. C., & Dietz, T. (1994). The value basis of environmental concern. Zelezny, L. C., Chua, P., & Aldrich, C. (2000). Elaborating on gender
Journal of Social Issues, 50, 65–84. differences in environmentalism. Journal of Social Issues, 56, 443–457.

Você também pode gostar