Você está na página 1de 10

Carotenoid Access, Nutritional Stress, and the Dewlap Color of Male

Brown Anoles

John E. Steffen1, Geoffrey E. Hill2, and C. Guyer2


Copeia 2010, No. 2, 239–246

Carotenoid Access, Nutritional Stress, and the Dewlap Color of Male


Brown Anoles

John E. Steffen1, Geoffrey E. Hill2, and C. Guyer2


Carotenoids are integumentary colorants that must be ingested by vertebrates to be used as colorants. In many studies
of fish and birds, carotenoid color serves as an indicator of foraging success, nutritional state, or parasite load. The
dewlap of male Brown Anoles (Norops sagrei) contains the carotenoid pigments lutein and zeaxanthin. We performed a
two-factor experiment to determine the effects of nutritional stress and xanthophyll supplementation on male dewlap
color in N. sagrei. We tested the hypotheses that dewlap color is dependent on access to carotenoids and good nutrition.
Contrary to our predictions we found no significant differences among groups of anoles that were either supplemented
or not supplemented with carotenoids. Similarly, post-experimental dewlap spectral variation did not differ
significantly among groups that were provided a standard or reduced diet. These findings demonstrate that dewlap
color of adult male Brown Anoles does not change in response to food access or carotenoid supplementation.

A
NIMALS use a variety of pigments to color their coloration in vertebrates have come from studies of birds
integument (Bagnara and Obika, 1965; Bagnara and and fish. Many species of reptiles also use carotenoid
Hadley, 1973; McGraw, 2006a, 2006b). Carotenoids pigments to color their integument, and although several
have been of interest to biologists because these molecules studies addressing the evolution and signal function of
must ultimately be obtained from the diet (Goodwin, 1984). lizard coloration have been conducted, no experimental
In fish and birds, carotenoid pigmentation acts as an honest study has yet addressed the environmental factors that
signal of an individual’s nutritional state (Frischknecht, might shape coloration in lizards. We conducted an
1993; Hill, 2000; Hill et al., 2002), parasite resistance (Zuk et experimental test of the effects of pigment access and
al., 1990; Houde and Torio, 1992; Thompson et al., 1997; nutrition on the dewlap color of male Brown Anoles (Norops
Brawner et al., 2000), or foraging success (Endler, 1980). The sagrei, following the clade name in Nicholson [2002]).
function and proximate control of carotenoid coloration is Male Brown Anoles possess a large and extendable throat
not nearly so well studied in other vertebrates including fan, known as a dewlap, that derives its color from both
lizards. pterin and carotenoid pigments. The dewlap has a bright red
A central tenet in honest signaling theory is that cheating center with a yellow edge. The yellow along the edge is
must be minimized for a signal to be reliable (Zahavi, 1975, colored primarily by two xanthophyll pigments (lutein and
1977; Andersson, 1982; Kodric-Brown and Brown, 1984; zeaxanthin), while the red center is colored by pterins (e.g.,
Hill, 1994). It has been hypothesized that cheating is especially drosopterin, isoxanthopterin, sepiapterin, pterin,
minimized in those signals that incur a cost (Folstad and and biopterin to a lesser extent; Macedonia et al., 2000;
Carter, 1992). Carotenoids may invoke a cost because they Steffen and McGraw, 2007, 2009). As in most anole species,
must be obtained through foraging, and then once they are male Brown Anoles extend their colorful dewlaps to other
ingested they must be extracted from food, absorbed from males in territorial contests (Jenssen, 1977; McMann, 2000;
the gut, modified (if necessary), transported, and deposited Tokarz, 2002; Tokarz et al., 2003; Paterson and McMann,
2004), to females in courtship interactions (Jenssen, 1970;
(Hill, 2002). If these costs of acquisition and utilization are
Tokarz, 2002; Tokarz et al., 2003), and predators during an
necessary for elaboration of the coloration, then only strong
attack (Leal and Rodriguez-Robles, 1997a, 1997b; Leal,
and healthy males will be highly ornamented (Frischknecht,
1999).
1993).
A common assumption is that the colors within an
Two factors that have been shown to affect carotenoid
individual’s dewlap are stable and constant throughout
coloration are access to sufficient quantities of appropriate
adulthood (Losos, 2009). Here we investigate the impact of
pigments in food and overall nutrition. In both birds and
xanthophyll supplementation and nutritional stress on the
fish, if animals are maintained on diets that do not provide
stability of dewlap coloration of Brown Anoles. Specifically,
abundant carotenoids, carotenoid coloration declines (End-
we assess the effects of xanthophyll supplementation and
ler, 1983; Hill, 2006). Similarly, nutritional stress (in the
nutritional stress on dewlap color to test the hypothesis that
form of reduced food access) has been shown to adversely adult male Brown Anole dewlap coloration is influenced by
affect red feather color in House Finches (Hill, 2000) and access to xanthophyll and nutritional stress.
yellow feather color in American Goldfinches (McGraw et
al., 2005). Evidence suggests that nutritional stress reduces
the caloric intake and energy available for enzyme-driven MATERIALS AND METHODS
biochemical steps involved in carotenoid utilization, and Lizard care.—Adult male lizards were collected on 3 May
reduces an individual’s ability to fully pigment its integu- 2008, from a parking lot 4.8 kilometers SW of Salt Springs,
ment (Hill, 2002; McGraw et al., 2005). Florida, near the Ocala National Forest. Upon return to the
All of the experimental tests to date on the effects of lab two days later, lizards were assigned a unique toe-clip
carotenoid access and nutrition on expression of carotenoid identity (no more than two toes clipped per individual), and
1
Department of Entomology and Plant Pathology, 301 Funchess Hall, Auburn University, Auburn, Alabama 36849; E-mail: steffje@
auburn.edu. Send reprint requests to this address.
2
Department of Biological Sciences, 331 Funchess Hall, Auburn University, Auburn, Alabama 36849.
Submitted: 10 April 2009. Accepted: 23 November 2009. Associate Editor: E. Schultz.
F 2010 by the American Society of Ichthyologists and Herpetologists DOI: 10.1643/CP-09-067
240 Copeia 2010, No. 2

were placed into 37.9-liter terraria, each of which was The experiment was conducted from 5 June 2008 until 5
separated into four compartments so that lizards could be July 2008. We made a final series of spectrometric measure-
separated. Compartment walls were constructed from solid ments on 6 July 2008 as a post-experimental measure of
particle board which prevented lizards from seeing or dewlap color. Lizards were weighed to the nearest 0.001 gram
physically contacting other individuals. Each compartment at the beginning and end of the experiment using an
contained a perch, water dish, and sandy substrate. Lighting electronic balance. We weighed lizards three days after the
strips containing full spectrum fluorescent tubes (Vitalite last meal to minimize the possibility that weights reflected
T8, 32 watt) were suspended 30 cm above each terrarium full stomachs instead of body mass. We performed a two-
top. Terraria, and the lizards in them, were exposed to way ANOVA on mass change, with food access and
natural light via sunlight that entered the holding room xanthophyll supplement level as factors. There was a
through a large window. Water was sprayed into each significant main effect of food access on mass loss in lizards
terrarium daily, and water bowls were refilled three times over the experimental period (F3,36 5 8.56, n 5 40, P 5
per week. The room was maintained on a 14:10 hr light:dark 0.007), indicating that the reduced food access treatment
photoperiod, at an air temperature of 32.2uC, with relative used for this experiment was an effective form of nutritional
humidity ranging between 60 and 80%. stress. Lizards that had reduced access to food lost an
average of 18% of body mass, while lizards that had standard
Experimental protocol, treatments, and controls.—Lizards were access to food maintained average body mass (i.e., they
housed for five weeks before the experiment began, during gained 0.006% body mass).
which they were fed meal worms ad libitum and a minimum
of three crickets per day (dusted with Repta-vite), 3–4 times Spectrometry.—Color of the center and edge dewlap regions
per week. This feeding regiment served as the standard food were measured with an Ocean Optics S2000 UV-visible
provisioning rate for treatment groups assigned to the spectrometer equipped with OOIBase32 software. All reflec-
‘standard food access’ treatment groups. tance data were generated relative to a white reflectance
The reduced food access groups received three crickets per standard and were taken in an unlit room with tightly drawn
feeding, once per week. Crickets for all treatments were blinds covering the window. We placed a small black plastic
three weeks old (Golden Crickets, Alabama; raised on a stopper on the tip of the reflectance probe, creating a 2 mm
protein-rich diet that lacked carotenoids) and were main- gap between the probe tip and the dewlap, ensuring a
tained on a diet of dog food (consisted of lamb, rice, oat, and constant distance between probe and dewlap for all measure-
corn-based meals). Corn-based meal in the dog food may ments. To measure lizard dewlaps with the spec-
contain xanthophylls and therefore be a source of carot- trometer, we placed each lizard ventral side up on a flat black
enoids that crickets ingested, but crickets fed on this diet table and immobilized it with two pieces of athletic tape
were fed to all lizards. Moreover, if this potential source of placed across the belly and lower mandible. The dewlap was
xanthophyll had an effect on dewlap color, then treatment kept maximally extended by attaching the edge to a small,
groups that were assigned to the reduced food provision rate height-adjustable clamp that was attached to a horizontal
should show a decrease in xanthophyll-based color. metal arm on a ring stand. We then placed the spectrometer
Xanthophyll was supplemented in the form of a xantho- probe at a 90u angle, and flush with the exposed skin of the
phyll-nutrient powder designed by Mazuri diet (PMI Nutri- dewlap. We measured spectral reflectance along two ‘regions’
tion, Richmond, IN). The powder was suspended in water by of the dewlap: along the center and the edge. We took seven,
mixing 512 grams of nutrient powder in 1.9 liters of warm non-overlapping spectral measurements per dewlap region
tap water. The powder consisted of 45% protein, 10% fat, for each lizard. These regions are commonly measured in
and 1% fiber to bind the xanthophylls in a fat-soluble other dewlap color studies (Leal and Fleishman, 2002, 2004;
matrix to facilitate micellarization of the pigments and Fleishman et al., 2006; Nicholson et al., 2007).
subsequent absorption by the intestine (Kuksis, 1987; Spectral measurements were gathered as percent reflec-
Parker, 1996; Chavez et al., 1998). For lizards belonging to tance at 1 nm wavelength increments from 300–700 nm
the xanthophyll supplementation group, we fed individuals (which represents the lower range of photon absorption by
3 ml of xanthophyll solution (0.028 mg of xanthophyll per UV-sensitive photoreceptor cones published for anoles
milliliter of water) with a blunt-tipped syringe. As a result, [Fleishman et al., 1993]), and this output was smoothed
each lizard in the xanthophyll treatment group received and processed using CLR v 1.0 (Montgomerie, 2008). For
0.084 mg of xanthophyll per supplementation. This is both the pre- and post-experimental color measurements,
roughly the same xanthophyll quantity used to influence we generated a spectral reflectance curve for every measure-
plumage and bill color of American Goldfinches (Navara ment, and we established a mean reflectance curve for each
and Hill, 2003). The treatment groups that received no dewlap region of every individual lizard by averaging all
xanthophyll solution received warm tap water via a separate seven reflectance curves per dewlap region. We then
blunt-tipped syringe in a manner identical to the xantho- established a pre- and post-experimental mean reflectance
phyll supplemented groups. curve for each dewlap region of each treatment group. Next,
One day before the experiment’s initiation, we measured we calculated two of the ‘tristimulus’ colorimetric variables
lizard dewlap color with a spectrophotometer to serve as a commonly referred to as chroma and brightness. We used
‘pre-experimental’ color measurement (see Spectrometry CLR v 1.0 to determine mean values of chroma, and we used
details below). Lizards were then randomly assigned to one MS Excel, along with formulas described in Montgomerie
of four groups: no xanthophyll supplementation and (2006) to determine brightness. These variables describe
standard food access (the control group), no xanthophyll different aspects of the spectral curve and have been used
supplementation and reduced food access, xanthophyll extensively in the literature (Endler, 1990; Hill, 1998;
supplementation and standard food access, and xanthophyll Montgomerie, 2006). Brightness was calculated as the
supplementation and reduced food access. percent total reflectance in UV (300–400 nm) and yellow
Steffen et al.—Nutrition and dewlap color 241

(550–624 nm) and red wavebands (605–700 nm). UV, priority DV is analyzed as a univariate ANOVA where the
yellow, and red chroma were calculated as proportion of covariates are entered as IVs. The second priority DV is then
light in these wavebands relative to total reflectance across entered into ANCOVA as a DV, while the highest priority
the 300–700 nm spectrum. Hue was not used in the analyses DV is entered as a covariate (along with covariates from
because correlative data on xanthophyll concentration and previous ANOVA). Following a first ANCOVA, a second
its influence on hue are non-significant in the Brown Anole ANCOVA is performed, with the higher priority DV from the
(Steffen and McGraw, unpubl. data). previous ANCOVA being entered as a covariate along with
previous covariates. This succession of ANCOVAs is per-
Statistical analysis.—We studied the effects of xanthophyll formed until the lowest priority DV is entered as a DV, and
supplementation and reduced food access on post-experi- all previous DVs are entered into ANCOVA as covariates
mental dewlap color when pre-experimental dewlap color (along with initial covariates, so that adjustment is made for
was treated as a covariate. By using this covariate approach, the previous DVs as well as the pre-experimental spectral
we could determine if the independent variables (referred to variation (i.e., the covariates). In the study presented here,
as IVs hereafter) had significant effects on the dependent we assigned priority to these DVs in the following order:
variables (referred to as DVs hereafter) after the covariate yellow, red, and UV brightness, then yellow, red, and UV
influence was removed. chroma. This order was chosen because in most UV-sensitive
Before this can be done, however, data need to meet all spectrometric studies of animal coloration (as well as the
assumptions of ANOVA and ANCOVA, multivariate signifi- present study), PC1 typically describes variation in overall
cance needs to be investigated, and covariates need to be brightness (Cuthill et al., 1999). Since yellow brightness
assessed for reliability. If covariates are reliable, then one correlated with xanthophylls in a correlative study investi-
usually wants to determine the ability of specific covariates gating how pigments generate color in Brown Anole
to adjust specific DVs. Only after these steps have been dewlaps (Steffen and McGraw, 2009), we assigned it the
performed can the researcher reliably evaluate the depen- highest priority. Red brightness was assigned second highest
dent variables, and infer statistical significance of the effects priority because xanthophylls also likely influence portions
of factors on DVs (Tabachnik and Fidell, 2001). of the red spectral curve (i.e., yellow and red make orange).
We used JMP to evaluate normality of distributions of By default, UV brightness was assigned third highest
spectral variation. Several variables did not meet assump- priority. PC2 in the present study describes long wavelength
tions of normality; when yellow, red, or UV chroma was (red and yellow) chroma (which were assigned fourth and
non-normally distributed, the values were arc-sin trans- fifth priority), and PC3 describes UV chroma (and was given
formed because the values are expressed as a ratio between 0 lowest priority) because it explained the least amount of
and 1. When yellow, red, or UV brightness was non- variance in the data. Thus, yellow chroma was assigned
normally distributed, the values were log-transformed. The fourth priority, red chroma fifth priority, and UV chroma
transformations were retained and used for the analyses sixth priority.
only if they improved the normality of the variable After performing Roy-Bargmann stepdown analysis, we
(Shapiro-Wilks W test, available in JMP). Otherwise, the used SPSS MANOVA to perform univariate ANCOVA on
original, untransformed variables were used. After appro- each dependent variable, with xanthophyll supplementa-
priate transformations were retained or rejected, the data tion, food access, and the xanthophyll supplementation and
met all assumptions of homogeneity of variance–covariance food access interaction as IVs, and pre-experimental mea-
matrices. sures of yellow, red, and UV chroma and brightness as
The following analyses were all performed in SPSS in covariates. To test for significant effects of food access and
syntax mode (with syntax provided by Tabachnik and Fidell, xanthophyll supplementation on the linear combination of
2001). We used SPSS’s MANOVA command to perform DVs that represent post-experimental dewlap spectral varia-
MANOVA and determine multivariate significance. We also tion, one needs to compare significant differences in
used SPSS MANOVA to assess the reliability of covariates. In spectral variation between univariate ANCOVA and step-
this assessment, we used Wilks’ lambda to test for significant down F tests. According to Tabachnik and Fiddell (2001),
relationships between the linear combinations of DVs dependent variables that show both significant univariate
(representing post-experimental dewlap color) and the and stepdown F statistics are statistically pure tests of
covariates (i.e., pre-experimental dewlap color). significance because they demonstrate that there is unique
We used SPSS MANOVA to investigate the ability of variability shared by the treatments after adjustments for
covariates to adjust specific wavebands of post-experimental differences created by the covariates. We combined F
spectral variation (i.e., the DVs) by performing multiple statistics, and effect and error degrees of freedom from the
regressions for each DV in turn, with the covariates acting as univariate and stepdown analyses, and present them side by
IVs in the model. Regression coefficients (beta coefficients) side in two tables to facilitate comparison and evaluate the
and statistical tests of covariate significance were provided significance of each analysis type. When results show
as output in SPSS. significant univariate but not significant stepdown F
Once the contributions of covariates to adjust the DV statistics, there is not unique treatment variability after
were evaluated, we wanted to see if the DVs were affected by covariate adjustment. Thus, significance is not reliable. If
the treatments once the effect of covariates were removed. there is a significant stepdown F but not a significant
Because many of the dependent variables were significantly univariate F, significant DVs take on importance in the
correlated to each other (i.e., they showed a correlation presence of higher order DVs, and the results may be related
coefficient .0.30 [Tabachnick and Fidell, 2001]), we used to the order in which DVs were entered into the stepdown
SPSS MANOVA to perform Roy-Bargmann step-down analy- analysis. Thus, significance is not reliable in this case, either.
sis in which the DVs are assigned priority based on We controlled for increased likelihood of Type I inflation
theoretical or practical considerations, and the highest error due to multiple comparisons by dividing a typical P
242 Copeia 2010, No. 2

Table 1. Multiple Regression Beta Coefficients (Regression Coefficients) to Assess Ability of Specific Covariates to Adjust DVs in the Dewlap Center
and Edge. YB, RB, UVB = yellow, red, and UV brightness, respectively. YC, RC, UVC = yellow, red, and UV chroma, respectively. * represents beta
coefficient values where P , 0.05, and covariates significantly adjust the DV.

DV
Region Covariate YB RB UVB YC RC UVC
Center YB 20.197 20.323 0.425 0.371 20.001 1.176
RB 0.864 1.922 0.925 23.678* 20.977 20.368
UVB 20.319 21.626 21.629 5.635* 1.818 21.081
YC 0.583* 0.267 0.325 0.372 20.404 20.087
RC 20.646 20.861 20.843 2.030* 1.087 20.116
UVC 0.367 0.939 1.253 23.117* 21.251 0.984
Edge YB 22.356* 22.180* 22.381* 1.317 0.990 21.461
RB 3.852* 3.550* 4.883* 22.455 21.811 3.951*
UVB 2.476 2.105 3.857 21.638 21.340 3.328
YC 23.737 23.346 27.729 4.590 2.852 27.480
RC 21.670 21.266 23.086 1.997 1.584 22.589
UVC 1.834 1.654 4.998 23.092 21.720 5.100

value indicating an experiment-wise error rate of 0.05 by 6 brightness. Post-experimental yellow and UV chroma were
to accommodate the increased probability of making type I not significantly affected by any covariates.
errors among six separate tests (i.e., one separate test for
each DV). Statistical inference.—Results of the univariate and step-down
analyses are combined and summarized by dewlap region in
RESULTS Table 2. In the dewlap center, reduced food access showed a
significant effect on post-experimental red chroma in a
Multivariate significance.—In the dewlap center, post-experi- univariate F test. However, covariates (pre-experimental
mental spectral variation was significantly different between dewlap color) had significant effects on post-experimental
reduced and standard food access (approximate F6,22 5 measures of yellow and red chroma, as well as yellow
2.829, P , 0.05), but not with xanthophyll supplementation brightness. Indeed, post-experimental dewlap spectra did
(approximate F6,22 5 0.834, P 5 0.556). In addition, the not significantly differ with respect to xanthophyll supple-
interaction of xanthophyll and food was not significant mentation, reduced food access, or xanthophyll-by-food
(approximate F6,22 5 0.784, P 5 0.592). interaction after adjusting for covariates (i.e., pre-experi-
Along the dewlap edge, post-experimental experimental mental dewlap spectral variation) via successive stepdown F
spectral variation was not significantly different in response tests and correcting for inflated Type I error due to multiple
to food access (approximate F6,22 5 1.840, P 5 0.137) or comparisons (i.e., Bonferroni corrections).
xanthophyll supplementation (approximate F6,22 5 1.862, P Along the dewlap edge, pre-experimental dewlap color
5 0.133), and the interaction of xanthophyll and food was covaried significantly with measures of post-experimental
not significant (approximate F6,22 5 0.652, P 5 0.688). yellow and UV chroma (Table 2). Reduced food access had a
significant effect on post-experimental red brightness in a
Covariate reliability and assessment.—Although MANOVA univariate ANCOVA. Similar to the dewlap center, however,
showed multivariate significance, pre-experimental dewlap post-experimental dewlap spectra did not significantly differ
color was found to be a reliable covariate of post-experi- with respect to xanthophyll supplementation, reduced food
mental dewlap color in both dewlap regions (dewlap center: access, or xanthophyll-by-food interaction after adjusting
approximate F36,99 5 3.113, P , 0.001; dewlap edge: for differences in pre-experimental spectral variation, and
approximate F36,99 5 2.235, P 5 0.001). Multiple regression via successive stepdown F tests and Bonferroni multiple
showed that several covariates significantly adjusted DVs. In comparisons.
the dewlap center, post-experimental yellow chroma was
significantly adjusted by pre-experimental readings (i.e.,
DISCUSSION
covariates) of UV and red chroma, as well as UV and red
brightness (Table 1). Post-experimental yellow brightness The data presented here demonstrate that dewlap color in
was significantly adjusted by pre-experimental yellow adult Brown Anoles does not change significantly over the
chroma. The remaining post-experimental measures of course of one month in response to carotenoid supplemen-
spectral variation (UV or red chroma, or UV and red tation or food access. These results differ from most other
brightness) were not affected by any covariates. studies of birds and fish, which show that integumentary
Along the dewlap edge, post-experimental UV chroma was carotenoids change in response to both carotenoid supple-
significantly adjusted by pre-experimental readings of UV mentation and food access. For instance, if Trinidadian
brightness (Table 1). Post-experimental yellow, red, and UV Guppies (Poecilia reticulata; Endler, 1983) and Brown Trout
brightness was significantly adjusted by pre-experimental (Salmo trutta; Steven, 1947a, 1947b) are fed a carotenoid-free
readings of yellow brightness. Post-experimental yellow, (or carotenoid-poor) diet, then these colors fade over the
red, and UV brightness as well as red chroma was duration of a few weeks, and if carotenoids are provided
significantly affected by pre-experimental readings red again, the colors become brighter and more intense. In
Steffen et al.—Nutrition and dewlap color 243

Table 2. Univariate and Stepdown F Tests for Assessment of Statistically Significant Differences in Post-experimental Dewlap Color (DVs) between
Factors (Xanthophyll Supplementation, or ‘Xanth’, Food Access, or ‘Food’, and Their Interaction, ‘Xanth by Food’). ‘Center’ refers to results of
Univariate and Stepdown F tests for the dewlap center. ‘Edge’ refers to the results of these F tests for the dewlap edge. Covariates are the pre-
experimental dewlap spectral wavebands (and are the same wavebands described for DVs below). YB, RB, UVB = yellow, red, and UV brightness,
respectively, and YC, RC, UVC = yellow, red, and UV chroma, respectively. a Significance level cannot be evaluated but would reach statistical
significance in univariate context. * P , a (Bonferroni adjusted significance level). Italics represent F values that were statistically significant at P ,
0.05, but which were no longer significant after Bonferroni multiple comparisons test.

Center Edge
Effect DV Univariate F Stepdown F Univariate F Stepdown F df a
Covariates YB 6.046a 6.046* 2.009 2.009 6/27 0.0083
RB 5.408a 5.525* 2.053 0.923 6/26 0.0083
UVB 5.585a 2.564 2.893 8.613** 6/25 0.0083
YC 3.222 3.105 4.775a 3.626 6/24 0.0083
RC 4.039a 1.784 2.342 0.542 6/23 0.0083
UVC 2.419 0.839 6.206a 0.567 6/22 0.0083
Xanth by Food YB 0.000 0.000 0.123 0.123 1/27 0.0083
RB 0.344 1.521 0.219 0.141 1/26 0.0083
UVB 0.002 0.675 0.146 1.403 1/25 0.0083
YC 0.211 0.000 0.059 0.535 1/24 0.0083
RC 0.152 0.230 0.026 1.508 1/23 0.0083
UVC 0.030 2.321 1.041 0.300 1/22 0.0083
Food YB 0.057 0.057 4.138 4.138 1/27 0.0083
RB 2.899 9.641* 7.700a 4.772 1/26 0.0083
UVB 1.011 0.105 0.889 0.106 1/25 0.0083
YC 3.604 5.189 0.592 1.401 1/24 0.0083
RC 8.312a 1.141 0.259 0.306 1/23 0.0083
UVC 1.720 0.136 0.344 0.477 1/22 0.0083
Xanth YB 1.340 1.340 0.415 0.415 1/27 0.0083
RB 3.455 2.903 1.644 3.235 1/26 0.0083
UVB 0.341 0.348 1.071 3.267 1/25 0.0083
YC 0.655 0.040 0.937 0.001 1/24 0.0083
RC 1.007 0.109 0.302 3.831 1/23 0.0083
UVC 3.297 0.683 1.449 0.027 1/22 0.0083

Zebra Finches (Taeniopygia guttata) and American Gold- several streams, where genetic variation in drosopterin
finches (Carduelis tristis) xanthophyll supplementation leads compensated for environmental variation in carotenoids
to an increase in feather and bill saturation (i.e., chroma) (Grether et al., 2005). We did not have the opportunity to
and is condition dependent (McGraw and Ardia, 2003; test the pigment content of dewlaps in our experimental
Navara and Hill, 2003). animals, so this idea cannot be assessed with the data on
Without additional study, there is no way to know why hand. However, Steffen and McGraw (2009) found that
the color of dewlaps of Brown Anoles responded differently drosopterin concentration did not correlate with xantho-
to experimental manipulation of carotenoid and food access phyll concentration in another central Florida population
compared to various species of fish and birds, but a few sampled two years earlier, and this lack of inter-pigmentary
speculations seem in order. First, it is possible that even the correlation suggests an absence of such compensation in
poorest diet—low food and low carotenoids—provided Brown Anoles.
adequate carotenoid pigments for full ornament expression. Third, the coloration of anoles may be fixed by the time
Crickets that formed the base diet for all animals ate corn- they are adult. If carotenoid coloration is fixed by adult-
based foods and may have sequestered sufficient carotenoids hood, then the carotenoids responsible for dewlap color
for lizard color production. This idea could be tested by must be obtained as juveniles or embryos. According to a
feeding anoles a carotenoid-free diet during a one-month dietary analysis of Biminian Norops sagrei (Schoener, 1968),
period and assessing the effect on color. phytophagous insects known to sequester carotenoids
Second, anoles may have used their alternative pigment (Mummery et al., 1975; Rothschild and Mummery, 1985;
system—pterins—to compensate for spectral changes that Czeczuga, 1986, 1990; Rothschild et al., 1986), such as
result from low carotenoid intake or poor nutrition. Grether lepidopteran larvae and adults comprise low volumetric
et al. (2001) considered how drosopterin-to-carotenoid percentages of juvenile stomach volumes compared to
ratios vary in color patches of male Trinidadian Guppies adults. For example, lepidopteran larvae and adults com-
(P. reticulata). In P. reticulata, drosopterin concentration bined comprised only 10.1% of juvenile lizard stomach
correlated positively with carotenoid (tunaxanthin) avail- volumes, and plant matter was non-existent. In contrast,
ability. Moreover, pterin-to-carotenoid ratio was responsible lepidopteran larvae and adults comprised 40.7% and 32.2%
for a particular orange hue that was maintained between of adult male and female stomach volumes, respectively.
244 Copeia 2010, No. 2

Plant matter comprised 16.3% of stomach volume in males Bagnara, J. T., and M. Obika. 1965. Comparative aspects of
and 4.9% of stomach volume in females. The reduced integumental pteridine distribution among amphibians.
volumetric percentage of lepidopteran larvae and adults, as Comparative Biochemistry and Physiology 15:33–49.
well as the lack of whole plant matter in juvenile lizard Brawner, W. R., III, G. E. Hill, and C. A. Sunderman. 2000.
stomachs suggest that juveniles are not going out of their Effects of coccidial and mycoplasmal infection on plum-
way to obtain carotenoids. age pigmentation in male House Finches. Auk 117:
One possibility is that carotenoids used for dewlap 952–963.
spectral variation are obtained during vitellogenesis or Chavez, P. R. G., D. Rengel, R. Gomez, G. Choubert, and
embryogenesis by the mother. Dewlap color obtained from J. C. G. Milicua. 1998. Canthaxanthin saturation of serum
the mother’s diet might indicate something about the lipoproteins from immature Rainbow Trout (Onchorhynchus
mother’s genes or diet, and this maternal source of mykiss). Comparative Biochemistry and Physiology B
environmentally obtained pigment may influence the son’s 121:129–134.
ability to maximally contrast in a given light environment Cuthill, I. C., A. T. D. Bennett, J. C. Partridge, and E. J.
(Leal and Fleishman, 2002, 2004). Maier. 1999. Plumage reflectance and the objective
Whatever the reason that our results differ from previous measurement of avian sexual dichromatism. The Amer-
studies of fish and birds, the lack of color change in anoles ican Naturalist 160:183–200.
in response to manipulation of pigment access and nutrition Czeczuga, B. 1986. The presence of carotenoids in various
has important implications. If they are not used as a signal species of Lepidoptera. Biochemical and Systematic Ecol-
of recent xanthophyll foraging success or nutritional ogy 14:345–351.
condition in adult males, why use carotenoids as integu- Czeczuga, B. 1990. Investigations on carotenoids in insects.
mentary colorants? In the fishes and birds described above, X. Changes in the carotenoids in butterflies (Lepidoptera).
the variation in carotenoid-based color serves as a visual Folia Biologia–Prague 38:5–12.
signal to communicate information to conspecifics about Endler, J. A. 1980. Natural selection on colour patterns in
foraging success (Endler, 1980), parasite resistance (Zuk et Poecilia reticulata. Evolution 34:76–91.
al., 1990; Houde and Torio, 1992; Thompson et al., 1997; Endler, J. A. 1983. Natural and sexual selection on color
Brawner et al., 2000), and nutritional state (Frischknecht, patterns in poeciliid fishes. Environmental Biology of
1993; Hill, 2000; Hill et al., 2002). It is unknown whether Fishes 9:173–190.
xanthophyll-based dewlap color communicates any fitness- Endler, J. A. 1990. On the measurement and classification of
related information in Brown Anoles, but the present study colour in studies of animal colour patterns. Biological
suggests that xanthophylls are not deposited into the Journal of the Linnean Society 41:315–352.
integument and do not communicate information about Fleishman, L. J., M. Leal, and J. Sheehan. 2006. Illumina-
immediate nutritional states or xanthophyll accumulation tion geometry, detector position and the objective
in adult males. determination of animal signal colours in natural light.
Future research should investigate integumentary xantho- Animal Behaviour 71:463–474.
phyll deposition as a maternal effect, and should attempt to Fleishman, L. J., E. R. Loew, and M. Leal. 1993. Ultraviolet
determine physiological benefits of xanthophyll acquisition vision in lizards. Nature 365:397.
and utilization relative to pterin deposition. In union with Folstad, I., and A. J. Karter. 1992. Parasites, bright males,
studies of pterin synthesis and deposition, findings of these and the immunocompetence handicap. American Natur-
sorts would contribute a great deal to the study of dewlap alist 139:603–622.
color evolution because they elucidate the underlying Frischknecht, M. 1993. The breeding coloration of male
physiological causes of dewlap color and help clarify three-spined sticklebacks (Gasterosteus aculeatus) as an
potential uses of dewlap color as a visual signal. indicator of energy investment in vigour. Evolutionary
Ecology 7:439–450.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS Goodwin, T. W. 1984. The Biochemistry of the Carotenoids.
Volume 1. Plants. Chapman and Hall, London.
JES is grateful to L. McBrayer, J. Sergi, and members of the
McBrayer lab for help collecting lizards. Thanks go to L. Grether, G. F., M. E. Cummings, and J. Hudon. 2005.
Siefferman, W. Hood, and members of C. Guyer’s and G. Countergradient variation in the sexual coloration of
Hill’s labs for revision suggestions, B. Tabachnik for reading guppies (Poecilia reticulata): drosopterin synthesis balances
the Roy-Bargmann step down analysis revision and con- carotenoid availability. Evolution 59:175–188.
firming that it was done correctly, and K. Nicholson for Grether, G. F., J. Hudon, and J. A. Endler. 2001. Carotenoid
insightful discussions about dewlap color diversity. We scarcity, synthetic pteridine pigments and the evolution
would also like to thank L. Koutsos of Mazuri diet (PMI of sexual coloration in guppies (Poecilia reticulata). Pro-
Nutrition, Richmond, IN) for designing the xanthophyll ceedings of the Royal Society of London B 268:1245–1253.
diet. All care of lizards followed animal care and use Hill, G. E. 1994. Trait elaboration via adaptive mate choice:
guidelines and IACUC approval (PRN 2006-0961). sexual conflict in the evolution of signals of male quality.
Ethology, Ecology and Evolution 6:351–370.
Hill, G. E. 1998. An easy, inexpensive means to quantify
LITERATURE CITED
plumage coloration. Journal of Field Ornithology 69:353–363.
Andersson, M. 1982. Sexual selection, natural selection and Hill, G. E. 2000. Energetic constraints on expression of
quality advertisement. Biological Journal of the Linnean carotenoid-based plumage coloration. Journal of Avian
Society 17:375–393. Biology 31:559–566.
Bagnara, J. T., and M. E. Hadley. 1973. Chromatophores and Hill, G. E. 2002. Red Bird in a Brown Bag: The Function and
Color Change: The Comparative Physiology of Pigmenta- Evolution of Colorful Plumage in the House Finch. Oxford
tion. Prentice Hall, Englewood Cliffs, New Jersey. University Press, Inc., New York.
Steffen et al.—Nutrition and dewlap color 245

Hill, G. E. 2006. Environmental regulation of ornamental McMann, S. 2000. Effects of residence time on displays
coloration, p. 507–560. In: Bird Coloration. I. Mechanisms during territory establishment in a lizard. Animal Behav-
and Measurements. G. E. Hill and K. J. McGraw (eds.). iour 59:513–522.
Harvard University Press, Cambridge, Massachusetts. Montgomerie, R. 2006. Analyzing colors, p. 90–147. In: Bird
Hill, G. E., C. Y. Inouye, and R. Montgomerie. 2002. Coloration. I. Mechanisms and Measurements. G. E. Hill
Dietary carotenoids predict plumage coloration in wild and K. J. McGraw (eds.). Harvard University Press, Cam-
house finches. Proceedings of the Royal Society of bridge, Massachusetts.
London B 269:1119–1124. Montgomerie, R. 2008. CLR, version 1.0. Queen’s Univer-
Houde, A. E., and A. J. Torio. 1992. Effect of parasitic sity, Kingston, Canada. http://post.queensu.ca/,mont/
infection on male color pattern and female choice in color/analyze.html
guppies. Behavioral Ecology 3:346–351. Mummery, R. S., M. Rothschild, and L. R. Valadon. 1975.
Jenssen, T. A. 1970. Female response to filmed displays of Carotenoids in two silk moths Saturnia pavonia L., and
Anolis nebulosus (Sauria, Iguanidae). Animal Behaviour Actia luna L. Comparative Biochemistry and Physiology B
18:640–647. 50:23–28.
Jenssen, T. A. 1977. Evolution of anoline lizard display Navara, K. J., and G. E. Hill. 2003. Dietary carotenoid
behavior. American Zoologist 17:203–215. pigments and immune function in a songbird with
Kodric-Brown, A., and J. H. Brown. 1984. Truth in extensive carotenoid-based plumage coloration. Behav-
advertising: the kinds of traits favored by sexual selection. ioral Ecology 14:909–916.
American Naturalist 124:309–323. Nicholson, K. E. 2002. Phylogenetic analysis and a test of
Kuksis, A. 1987. Absorption of fat soluble vitamins, p. 65–86. the current infrageneric classification of Norops (beta-
In: Fat Absorption. Volume 2. A. Kuksis (ed.). CRC Press, Anolis). Herpetological Monographs 16:93–120.
Inc., Boca Raton, Florida. Nicholson, K. E., L. J. Harmon, and J. B. Losos. 2007.
Leal, M. 1999. Honest signalling during prey–predator Evolution of Anolis lizard dewlap diversity. PLOS One 2:1–12.
interactions in the lizard Anolis cristatellus. Animal Parker, R. S. 1996. Absorption, metabolism, and transport of
Behaviour 58:521–526. carotenoids. Journal of the Federation of American
Leal, M., and L. J. Fleishman. 2002. Evidence for habitat Societies for Experimental Biology 10:542–551.
partitioning based on adaptation to environmental light Paterson, A. V., and S. McMann. 2004. Differential head-
in a pair of sympatric lizard species. Proceedings of the bob displays toward neighbors and non-neighbors in the
Royal Society of London B 269:351–359. territorial lizard Anolis sagrei. Journal of Herpetology
Leal, M., and L. J. Fleishman. 2004. Differences in visual 38:288–291.
signal design and detectability between allopatric Rothschild, M., and R. Mummery. 1985. Carotenoids and
populations of Anolis lizards. American Naturalist 163: bile pigments in danaid and swallowtail butterflies.
26–39. Biological Journal of the Linnean Society 24:1–14.
Leal, M., and J. A. Rodriguez-Robles. 1997a. Honest Rothschild, M., R. Mummery, and C. Farrell. 1986.
signalling during prey–predator interactions in the lizard Carotenoids of butterfly models and their mimics.
Anolis cristatellus. Animal Behaviour 58:521–526. Biological Journal of the Linnean Society 28:359–372.
Leal, M., and J. A. Rodriguez-Robles. 1997b. Antipredator Schoener, T. W. 1968. The Anolis lizards of Bimini: resource
responses to the Puerto Rican giant anole, Anolis cuvieri partitioning in a complex fauna. Ecology 49:704–726.
(Squamata: Polychrotidae). Biotropica 29:372–375. Steffen, J. E., and K. J. McGraw. 2007. Contributions of
Losos, J. B. 2009. Lizards in an Evolutionary Tree: Ecology pterin and carotenoid pigments to dewlap coloration in
and Adaptive Radiation of Anoles. University of California two anole species. Comparative Biochemistry and
Press, Berkeley. Physiology B 146:42–46.
Macedonia, J. M., S. James, L. W. Wittle, and D. L. Clark. Steffen, J. E., and K. J. McGraw. 2009. How dewlap color
2000. Skin pigments and coloration in the Jamaican reflects its carotenoid and pterin content in male and
radiation of Anolis lizards. Journal of Herpetology 34: female brown anoles (Norops sagrei). Comparative Bio-
99–109. chemistry and Physiology Part B 154:334–340.
McGraw, K. J. 2006a. The mechanics of carotenoid colora- Steven, D. M. 1947a. Astacene in Brown Trout. Nature 160:507.
tion in birds, p. 177–242. In: Bird Coloration. I. Mecha- Steven, D. M. 1947b. Carotenoid pigmentation of trout.
nisms and Measurements. G. E. Hill and K. J. McGraw Nature 160:540.
(eds.). Harvard University Press, Cambridge, Massachu- Tabachnik, B. G., and L. S. Fidell. 2001. Using Multivariate
setts. Statistics. Fourth edition. Allyn and Bacon, Needham
McGraw, K. J. 2006b. The mechanics of uncommon bird Heights, Massachusetts.
colors: pterins, porphyrins, and psittacofulvins, p. 354–398. Thompson, C. W., N. Hillgarth, M. Leu, and H. E.
In: Bird Coloration. I. Mechanisms and Measurements. G. E. McClure. 1997. High parasite load in house finches
Hill and K. J. McGraw (eds.). Harvard University Press, (Carpodacus mexicanus) is correlated with reduced expres-
Cambridge, Massachusetts. sion of a sexually selected trait. American Naturalist
McGraw, K. J., and D. R. Ardia. 2003. Carotenoids, 149:270–294.
immunocompetence, and the information content of Tokarz, R. R. 2002. An experimental test of the importance
sexual colors: an experimental test. American Naturalist of the dewlap in male mating sucess in the lizard Anolis
162:704–712. sagrei. Herpetologica 58:87–94.
McGraw, K. J., G. E. Hill, and R. S. Parker. 2005. The Tokarz, R. R., A. V. Paterson, and S. McMann. 2003.
physiological costs of being colorful: nutritional control Laboratory and field test of the functional significance of
of carotenoid utilization in the American Goldfinch, the male’s dewlap in the lizard Anolis sagrei. Copeia
Carduelis tristis. Animal Behaviour 69:653–660. 2003:502–511.
246 Copeia 2010, No. 2

Vanhooydonck, B., A. Herrel, J. J. Meyers, and D. J. Zahavi, A. 1977. The cost of honesty (further remarks on the
Irschick. 2008. What determines dewlap diversity in handicap principle). Journal of Theoretical Biology
Anolis lizards? An among island comparison. Journal of 67:603–605.
Evolutionary Biology 22:293–305. Zuk, M., R. Thornhill, J. D. Ligon, and K. Johnson. 1990.
Zahavi, A. 1975. Mate selection—a selection for a handicap. Parasites and mate choice in red jungle fowl. American
Journal of Theoretical Biology 53:205–214. Zoologist 30:235–244.

Você também pode gostar