Você está na página 1de 12

This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier.

The attached
copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research
and education use, including for instruction at the authors institution
and sharing with colleagues.
Other uses, including reproduction and distribution, or selling or
licensing copies, or posting to personal, institutional or third party
websites are prohibited.
In most cases authors are permitted to post their version of the
article (e.g. in Word or Tex form) to their personal website or
institutional repository. Authors requiring further information
regarding Elsevier’s archiving and manuscript policies are
encouraged to visit:
http://www.elsevier.com/authorsrights
Author's personal copy

Clinical Neurophysiology 125 (2014) 287–297

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Clinical Neurophysiology
journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/clinph

Insight into the relationship between brain/behavioral speed


and variability in patients with minimal hepatic encephalopathy
S. Schiff a,b,c,⇑, C. D’Avanzo d, G. Cona e, A. Goljahani d, S. Montagnese a,b, C. Volpato c, A. Gatta a,b,
G. Sparacino b,d, P. Amodio a,b,1, P. Bisiacchi b,e,1
a
Department of Medicine, University of Padua, Italy
b
C.I.R.M.A.ME.C., University of Padua, Italy
c
IRCCS San Camillo, Lido di Venice, Italy
d
Department of Information Engineering, University of Padua, Italy
e
Department of General Psychology, University of Padua, Italy

a r t i c l e i n f o h i g h l i g h t s

Article history:
 Single-trial P300 latency increases and amplitude decreases along reaction times (RTs) distribution.
Accepted 8 August 2013
 The relationship between P300 and RTs disappears in patients with Minimal hepatic encephalopathy
Available online 10 September 2013
(MHE).
 Temporal overlap between stimulus and response selection is related to both RTs speed and
Keywords:
Intra-individual variability of RTs variability.
Single-trial P300
Minimal hepatic encephalopathy
Simon task a b s t r a c t
Objective: Intra-individual variability (IIV) of response reaction times (RTs) and psychomotor slowing
were proposed as markers of brain dysfunction in patients with minimal hepatic encephalopathy
(MHE), a subclinical disorder of the central nervous system frequently detectable in patients with liver
cirrhosis. However, behavioral measures alone do not enable investigations into the neural correlates
of these phenomena. The aim of this study was to investigate the electrophysiological correlates of psy-
chomotor slowing and increased IIV of RTs in patients with MHE.
Methods: Event-related potentials (ERPs), evoked by a stimulus–response (S–R) conflict task, were
recorded from a sample of patients with liver cirrhosis, with and without MHE, and a group of healthy con-
trols. A recently presented Bayesian approach was used to estimate single-trial P300 parameters.
Results: Patients with MHE, with both psychomotor slowing and higher IIV of RTs, showed higher P300
latency jittering and lower single-trial P300 amplitude compared to healthy controls. In healthy controls,
distribution analysis revealed that single-trial P300 latency increased and amplitude decreased as RTs
became longer; however, in patients with MHE the linkage between P300 and RTs was weaker or even
absent.
Conclusions: These findings suggest that in patients with MHE, the loss of the relationship between P300
parameters and RTs is related to both higher IIV of RTs and psychomotor slowing.
Significance: This study highlights the utility of investigating the relationship between single-trial ERPs
parameters along with RT distributions to explore brain functioning in normal or pathological conditions.
Ó 2013 International Federation of Clinical Neurophysiology. Published by Elsevier Ireland Ltd. All rights
reserved.

1. Introduction common observation in most cognitive and neuropsychological


studies is that results based on subject samples cannot afford indi-
The study of within-person behavioral variability is an emerging vidual predictions. This is because data on variability within the
topic in cognitive neuroscience (MacDonald et al., 2006). Indeed, a same person are largely overshadowed by conventional measures
of central tendency. Increased intra-individual variability (IIV) of re-
⇑ Corresponding author. Address: Department of Medicine – DIMED, University sponse speed is detectable in many clinical conditions and could be
of Padova, Via Giustiniani, 2, 35128 Padova, Italy. Tel.: +39 049 8218675; fax: +39
considered as a marker of brain dysfunction (Hultsch et al., 2000).
049 8754179.
E-mail address: sami.schiff@unipd.it (S. Schiff).
For example, increased IIV of reaction times (RTs) was previously
1
PA and PB joint senior authorship. observed: (i) in dementia (Hultsch et al., 2000; Murtha et al.,

1388-2457/$36.00 Ó 2013 International Federation of Clinical Neurophysiology. Published by Elsevier Ireland Ltd. All rights reserved.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.clinph.2013.08.004
Author's personal copy

288 S. Schiff et al. / Clinical Neurophysiology 125 (2014) 287–297

2002; Christensen et al., 2005), (ii) in traumatic brain injury (Burton Usually, P300 amplitude decreases and P300 latency increases
et al., 2002; Stuss et al., 1994, 2003), (iii) in attention-deficit with increasing RTs (Holm et al., 2006; Li et al., 2008); however,
hyperactivity disorder (ADHD; Leth-Steensen et al., 2000; if latency jittering of an ERP component increases, the amplitude
Castellanos and Tannock, 2002; Castellanos et al., 2006), and (iv) of the wave obtained with the conventional averaging technique
in schizophrenia (Kaiser et al., 2008). (i.e., the usually adopted technique) decreases, and its latency is
Minimal hepatic encephalopathy (MHE) refers to the initial no longer equivalent to the mean latency recorded in each trial
subclinical phase of hepatic encephalopathy which is a condition (see e.g., Mouraux and Iannetti, 2008; D’Avanzo et al., 2013 for
characterised by quantifiable neurophysiological (Amodio et al., clinical and methodological remarks). Recently, single-trial analy-
1999a,b, 2001; Marchetti et al., 2011) and neuropsychological sis of ERPs was used to determine the brain activity expressed in
abnormalities including executive/attentional dysfunction and each trial during a cognitive task, providing newer information
psychomotor slowing and reduced vigilance (Amodio et al., about the IIV in both normal subjects (Walhovd et al., 2008; Fjell
2005a, 2010; Schiff et al., 2005a) related to liver cirrhosis and/or et al., 2009; Li et al., 2009; D’Avanzo et al., 2011; Saville et al.,
porto-systemic blood shunting (Del Piccolo et al., 2003). 2011) and clinical populations (Roth et al., 2007; Fell, 2007;
Two studies have previously investigated IIV of RTs in patients De Lucia et al., 2010; Hu et al., 2010). For example, (Lorenzo-Lopez
with cirrhosis on a behavioral basis (Elsass et al., 1985; Schiff et al., et al., 2007) showed that low-performing older adults manifest
2006). Specifically, Elsass et al., (1985) demonstrated that patients lower P300 amplitude and increased variability of P300 latency
with cirrhosis exhibited a higher IIV of RTs during an auditory compared to both younger adults and age-matched high-perform-
simple reaction time task not only compared with healthy individ- ing older adults. Nevertheless, Walhovd et al., (2008) showed that
uals, but also with patients with traumatic brain injury. In the even if older adults manifest higher variability of P300 latency, the
second study, Schiff et al., (2006) used a visual choice reaction time negative correlation between age and average-based P300 ampli-
task and observed both psychomotor slowing and higher IIV of RT tude persists after the correction of P300 latency jittering. Like-
in patients with cirrhosis, both with and without MHE. However, wise, Saville et al., (2011) showed that healthy individuals with
the neural correlates of psychomotor slowing and increased IIV high IIV of RTs manifest both higher P300 latency jittering and re-
of RTs have never been investigated in patients with cirrhosis duced P300 amplitude also after adequate single-trial correction.
so far. In patients with cirrhosis, studies adopting the classic oddball
Thus, the scope of the present work is to explore the neural paradigm showed that P300 is frequently prolonged in latency
substrate of psychomotor slowing and IIV of response speed in and reduced in amplitude (Klüger, 1996; Saxena et al., 2002; Amo-
patients with MHE. Event-related brain potentials (ERPs) have dio et al., 2005b). Thus, since both patients with and without MHE
higher temporal resolution (i.e. milliseconds) than some other exhibit increased variability of RTs and psychomotor slowing, the
neuroimaging methods (e.g., function MRI) and are preferred when single-trial approach seems to be the most adequate method for
the time course of mental operations is under study, in contrast to proper investigation of the neural correlates of these phenomena
the precise neural location. In particular, a specific ERP component in patients with cirrhosis. In addition, single-trial analysis allows
that seems to be particularly suited for investigating the neural investigation of whether the increased variability of single-trial
locus of RT variability and the relationship between RTs and brain P300 latencies, or the reduction of P300 amplitude in each sin-
activity is the P300 component (Sutton et al., 1965). P300 is gener- gle-trial response, contribute to the reduction of average-based
ally assumed to be a reliable neurophysiological correlate of the P300 amplitude observed in patients with cirrhosis (e.g., Amodio
stimulus evaluation process (Magliero et al., 1984; Rugg and Coles, et al., 2005b).
1995; Polich, 2007). However, it was recently suggested that P300 In the present study, a Bayesian technique (D’Avanzo et al.,
component is not only related to stimulus evaluation but also to 2011) was adopted to estimate single-trial P300 parameters (i.e.,
response selection, or at least to some initial decisions on stimu- latency and amplitude) evoked by the Simon task, a choice reaction
lus–response (S–R) association (Verleger, 1997; Verleger et al., times task widely used to investigate spatial S–R compatibility,
2005). In agreement with this view, Nieuwenhuis et al. (2005) sug- from cirrhotic patients with and without MHE. In this task, partic-
gested a key role of the coeruleus–noradrenergic system in the ipants are asked to respond using spatially arranged keys to a non-
decision process and in the modulation of cortical P300 responses. spatial stimulus feature (i.e., color) of lateralised targets (Simon
In addition, a link between catecholamines and the IIV of response and Rudell, 1967; Simon, 2011). The Simon task was previously
speed was recently suggested (Castellanos et al., 2005). Indeed, used to study the temporal dynamic of S–R compatibility in normal
catecholamines seem to modulate the signal-to-noise ratio (SNR) individuals (Vallesi et al., 2005) and in patients with cirrhosis
of neural information processing when an organism is engaged in (Schiff et al., 2006). The Simon effect refers to the finding that re-
tasks requiring attention and it contributes towards sustaining sponse choices are usually faster and more accurate when the
internally generated decision processes (Aston-Jones and Cohen, stimulus and the response-hand positions correspond spatially
2005). Thus, the relationship between signal modulation at the compared to when they do not, even if stimulus position is task-
neural level and P300 suggests a possible link between P300 and irrelevant.
IIV of response latency and psychomotor slowing. Interestingly, The standard deviation (SD) is usually used to assess within-
changes in P300 amplitude and/or latency are frequently observed subjects IIV of RTs. A more sophisticated approach exploits distri-
in populations that also exhibit prolongation and increased IIV of butional analysis providing information on the shape and the
RTs. For example, IIV of RTs changes throughout the lifespan from skewness of RTs distribution, which offers additional insight into
childhood to old age. Indeed, IIV decreased from childhood to IIV (Ratcliff, 1979; Castellanos et al., 2006). RT distributions are
adulthood and subsequently increased in older subjects (Hultsch well described by an ex-Gaussian distribution: a Gaussian-like dis-
et al., 2002; Bunce et al., 2004; Williams et al., 2005; MacDonald tribution with a longer right-sided tail. Balota and Yap (2011) sug-
et al., 2006). On the other hand, together with increased IIV of gested that a difference in the slopes of RTs distribution is a
RTs, older adults show longer RTs, delayed P300 latency and re- measure of a difference in s a parameter that defines the exponen-
duced P300 amplitude when compared to younger adults (Wal- tial component of the ex-Gaussian distribution, or in other terms, a
hovd et al., 2008; Schiff et al., 2008; Fjell et al., 2009). Segalowitz difference in the skewness of the two distributions. The trial-by-
and colleagues (1997) studied patients with head injury and found trial association between behavioral responses and P300 parame-
increased IIV of RTs was well explained by P300 amplitude ters provides a clear description of the neural correlates of RT
reduction. speed and variability along RTs distribution. Since temporal over-
Author's personal copy

S. Schiff et al. / Clinical Neurophysiology 125 (2014) 287–297 289

lap between early decision processes on S–R association and age-and education-adjusted validated paper and pencil psycho-
response selection may contribute to fast motor responses metric tests, including the Trail-Making test (TMT part A and B),
(Kornblum et al., 1990), it can be hypothesised that, using the dis- Symbol-Digit test, and the computerised SCAN test (Amodio
tribution analysis, a relationship between P300 parameters and et al., 1998, 2008). Patients were considered to have MHE if at least
RTs may be detected in healthy individuals: i.e., amplitude one measure (psychometric tests and/or EEG) was abnormal
decreases and latency increases with increasing RTs. In contrast, (Ferenci et al., 2002). On the basis of this criterion, 14 patients were
in patients with psychomotor slowing and higher IIV variability classified as having MHE.
of RTs, this relationship would be weaker due to a reduced overlap
between the S–R association processes. 2.3. Simon task
In summary, in the present paper we will specifically assess: (i)
if P300 amplitude reduction that has been observed in patients The experiment took place in a dimly light and sound attenu-
with cirrhosis and MHE is explained, at least in part, by an ated room. Each participant (control or patient) was presented
increased variability in single-trial P300 latency, and (ii) if P300 with a red or green target checkerboard on the right or the left side
is related to RTs and to their distribution in both patients with of the computer screen while a ‘neutral’ black and white checker-
cirrhosis and in healthy individuals. board always appeared on the other side. Checkerboards were pre-
sented for 176 ms and the inter-trial interval ranged from 800 to
2. Methods 1200 ms. The distance between the eyes and the screen was fixed
at 80 cm. A total of 300 experimental trials, balanced for stimulus
2.1. Participants color (red or green) and position (left or right), were presented to
the participants in a completely randomised sequence. Before
A total of 43 participants were enrolled in the study: 29 patients starting the experimental task, 40 trials were presented for prac-
with liver cirrhosis (age 51 ± 9 years, mean ± SD; males 66%; tise. The participant was asked to press, as fast and accurately as
education level 9 ± 4 years), of whom 14 had MHE and 15 did possible, one of the two keys corresponding to the color of the
not, and 14 age-matched healthy controls (age 49 ± 10 years, males checkerboard. Half of the participants were told that button ‘M’
57%, education 13 ± 5 years). Patients and healthy controls did not (right side of the keyboard) corresponds to the red checkerboard
differ in age or education level. and button ‘Z’ (left side of the keyboard) corresponds to the green
None of the participants had a history of neurological or neuro- checkerboard. The other half of the participants received the oppo-
psychiatric disorders, or significant cardiovascular, respiratory or site S–R association. Speed and accuracy were equally emphasised
renal impairment; none had taken psychotropic medication or when providing the instructions.
had an uncorrectable impairment of visual acuity or was color
blind. One of the patients had a history of alcohol misuse, however 2.4. Electrophysiological recordings
he was abstinent for at least 6 months. The diagnosis of cirrhosis
was made on the basis of historical, clinical, laboratory, endoscopic The electroencephalogram (EEG) was continuously recorded
and radiological findings. The aetiology of liver cirrhosis, the (Micromed System Plus, Mogliano Veneto, Italy) by 30 Ag/AgCl elec-
degree of hepatic failure according to Child–Pugh (Pugh et al., trodes mounted on an elastic cap and positioned according to the
1973) and the laboratory variables are reported in Table 1. None international 10/20 system. The Fpz was used as the ground and
of the patients had overt hepatic encephalopathy and all of them the reference was provided by the linked right and left mastoid elec-
showed disorientation for time or space at the time of the study trodes. Two electrodes were placed on the external canthus and un-
(Bajaj et al., 2011). All the participants provided informed consent der the left eye to monitor horizontal and vertical eye movements
to take part in the study which was approved by the local Hospital (EOG), respectively. Each channel had its own analogue-to-digital
University ethical committee. converter. The EEG and EOG signals were digitalised on-line with a
frequency rate of 512 Hz and a conversion resolution of 0.19 lV/di-
git. Impedance was kept lower than 5 kX. Signals were band-pass
2.2. Detection of MHE
filtered between 0.03 and 30 Hz. The EEG epochs were extracted
using a time-window analysis time of 2300 ms (800 ms pre-stimu-
MHE was diagnosed based on spectral electroencephalographic
lus and 1500 post-stimulus) and baseline corrected using a 200 ms
(EEG) features (Amodio et al., 1999a,b, 2001) and performance in
pre-stimulus time interval. Epochs with excessive drift or containing
eye movements/blinks were corrected using an independent com-
Table 1 ponent analysis algorithm (Jung et al., 2001; Delorme and Makeig,
Clinical characteristics of the patients. 2004; Makeig et al., 2004). A visual inspection was then performed
Clinical characteristics
in order to reject epochs with significant artefacts and only adequate
epochs were subsequently considered for the analysis.
Aetiology (%) Hepatitis B 24.1
Average-based P300 parameters (amplitude and latency) were
Hepatitis C 48.3
Alcohol 3.5 calculated by identifying the maximum, in the time-window be-
Other 24.1 tween 250 and 600 ms after stimulus onset, of the ERP profile ob-
Child–Pugh classification (%) A 16.7 tained by conventional averaging.
B 55.6
C 27.8 2.5. Estimation of single-trial P300 responses
Biochemical variables Median (SE)
Total Bilirubin (mmol/L) 48.4 (6.9) During the last few years, different mathematical approaches
Albumin (g/L) 46.6 (14.6)
have been proposed in order to derive ERP parameters, such as
Protrombin time [PT] (%) 60.6 (3.6)
Venous ammonia (mmol/L) 59.7 (13.5)
amplitude and latency, from single-trial EEG responses (Jaskowski
Na (mmol/L) 139.4 (0.6) and Verleger, 1999, 2000; Jung et al., 2001; Li et al., 2009; Rushby
Urea (mmol/L) 6.7 (8.9) and Barry, 2009; D’Avanzo et al., 2013).
Creatinine (mmol/L) 87.6 (4.4) Here, a single-trial P300 estimate was made using a Bayesian
Glucose (mmol/L) 17.8 (8.1)
approach originally proposed by Sparacino et al. (2002) and further
Author's personal copy

290 S. Schiff et al. / Clinical Neurophysiology 125 (2014) 287–297

developed by D’Avanzo et al. (2011), to which we refer the reader the mean single-trial P300 amplitude as predictors of the average-
for further details. Briefly, the method is formulated in two stages. based P300 amplitude.
In the first stage, each of the available raw sweeps is processed by The mean of single-trial P300 parameters (latency and ampli-
an individual ‘optimal’ filter, determined using statistical informa- tude) was calculated for each RTs quintile to evaluate the relation-
tion on the background EEG and on the unknown ERP obtained ship between behavioral and electrophysiological responses along
from pre-stimulus and post-stimulus data, respectively. This RTs distribution. Two ANOVAs were computed with the group as
avoids critical assumptions regarding EEG and ERP stationarity. between – subjects factor and task condition, quintile and deriva-
Then, a mean ERP is determined as the weighted average of the fil- tion as within – subjects factors, to investigate the relationship be-
tered sweeps, where each weight is proportional to the expected tween P300 parameters (latency and amplitude) and RTs
reliability of the sweep. In the second stage, single-sweep estima- distribution. Post-hoc analyses were carried out using Bonferroni
tion is dealt within the same framework by using the average ERP correction for multiple comparisons.
estimated in the previous stage as an a priori expected response.
The P300 latency and amplitude were determined for each sin- 3. Results
gle-trial by identifying the maximum of the ERP profile in the
time-window between 250 and 600 ms after stimulus onset. The 3.1. Behavioral analysis
assessment of the method made in D’Avanzo et al. (2011) on syn-
thetic data demonstrated its ability in estimating the latency of the 3.1.1. RTs
P300 component and its variability (e.g., the correlation coefficient RTs analysis highlighted the main effects of the task condition
between the estimated and the true single-trial latency was 0.74 [F(1, 41) = 33.39, p < 0.001; g2p = 0.45] and of the group [F(2,
when the SNR was ‘‘high’’ and 0.47 when the SNR was ‘‘low’’; see
40) = 10.89; p < 0.001; g2p = 0.35]. Post-hoc comparisons revealed
the cited paper above for details). However, detecting latency
slower RTs in non-corresponding than corresponding condition
and amplitude of the P300 component remains a challenging task:
(i.e., Simon effect) and that patients with MHE were slower than
occasional trials with estimated P300 latency resulting at the
both healthy controls and patients without MHE (all post hoc
boundary of the 250–600 ms time-window (e.g., because of mono-
p < 0.05). No interaction was observed between group and task
tonic increasing or decreasing behaviour of the estimated ERP)
condition [F(1, 41) = 1.42; p = 0.2; g2p = 0.03] (see Table 2).
were considered not sufficiently robust and were excluded from
the analysis. Notably, these trials occurred in less than 5% of the to-
tal for each subject, except for one subject who had 10% of the tri- 3.1.2. Response accuracy
als excluded from the analysis. Finally, the mean of P300 latencies The same main effect of task condition and group [F(2, 40)=5.64,
of all of the trials performed by each participant was computed as p < 0.01; g2p = 0.2] were found when response accuracy was consid-
well as the SD, which was used as an index of IIV in P300. Mean la- ered [F(1, 41) = 9.09, p < 0.005; g2p = 0.18]. Post-hoc tests showed
tency and amplitude of P300 from each RTs quintile were analysed lower accuracy in the non-corresponding compared to correspond-
in order to determine the relationship between physiological mea- ing condition and that patients with MHE were less accurate than
sures and the RTs distribution. healthy controls (all p < 0.05). Also, in this case, the interaction be-
tween group and task condition was not significant [F(1,
41) = 0.49; p = 0.6; g2p = 0.02].
2.6. Statistics
3.1.3. IIV in RTs
A series of analyses of variance (ANOVA) for repeated-measures The IIV of RTs was greater in the corresponding compared to
were run with the group (patients with MHE, patients without the non-corresponding condition [F(1, 40) = 23.96, p < 0.0001;
MHE and healthy controls) as a between-subjects factor and the g2p = 0.34] (Table 2). Patients with MHE showed higher IIV of RTs
task condition (Corresponding vs. non-Corresponding) as a with- than both healthy controls and patients without MHE [F(2,
in-subjects factor. For behavioral analyses, mean RTs, percentage 40) = 10.96, p < 0.001; g2p = 0.22, post hoc p’s < 0.05]. No difference
of correct response and RTs SDs were considered as dependent in IIV of RTs was observed between healthy controls and patients
variables. Individual RTs were also ordered from the fastest to without MHE (Fig. 1 left panel). No interaction was observed
the slowest and were divided into five quintiles (i.e., bins) in order between group and task condition [F(1,41) = 0.22; p < 0.64;
to study RTs distribution. In this case, the ANOVA also included the g2p = 0.005].
quintile factor as within-factor variable.
For the study of both average-based and single-trial P300 3.1.4. Distribution analysis of RTs
amplitudes and latencies, the ANOVA also included the factor der- A dynamic image of IIV was provided by the study of RT distri-
ivation (Pz, Cz, Fz) as a further within-subject factor. Pearson’s cor- bution via quintile analysis. Together with the effect of group
relations were computed to evaluate the goodness of fit of the P300 [F(2,40) = 10.86, p < 0.001; g2p = 0.35], quintile [F(4, 164) = 304.00,
estimates given by the Bayesian single-trial method and with those p < 0.0001; g2p = 0.87] and task condition [F(1, 41) = 39.4,
obtained using conventional averaging. Linear multiple regression p < 0.00001; g2p = 0.47], the interaction between task condition
analysis was run to assess the role of P300 latency jittering and of and quintile [F(4, 160) = 22.29, p < 0.00001; g2p = 0.33] was signfi-

Table 2
Behavioral measures obtained from the Simon task in controls and patients. Mean (standard deviation).

Controls N = 14 All Patients N = 29 Patients without MHE N = 13 Patients with MHE N = 16


Mean RT corresponding (ms) 487 (63) 602 (119) 553 (103) 641 (119)
Mean RT non-corresponding (ms) 510 (67) 637 (117) 582 (105) 681 (109)
IIV. RT corresponding (ms) 110 (25) 144 (41) 130 (45) 156 (35)
IIV. RT non-corresponding (ms) 90 (18) 128 (37) 107 (30) 144 (35)
Response accuracy corresponding (%) 97 (2) 90 (12) 95 (6) 87 (15)
Response accuracy non-corresponding (%) 95 (4) 87 (12) 91 (8) 84 (13)
Author's personal copy

S. Schiff et al. / Clinical Neurophysiology 125 (2014) 287–297 291

Fig. 1. Left panel shows IIV of RTs of the three groups of subjects. The IIV of RTs progressively increases from healthy controls to patients with MHE (Bonferroni post hoc test,
p < 0.05). Right panel shows the IIV of single-trial P300 latencies of the three groups of subjects. Patients with MHE showed a higher variability in P300 latency than healthy
controls (post hoc test p < 0.05).

Fig. 2. Distribution analysis of RTs showed that the Simon effect (difference
between non-corresponding and corresponding conditions) decreased with slower
reaction times (solid line). In contrast, as concerning P300 latency, the magnitude of
Fig. 3. The distribution analysis of the reaction times (RTs) shows an interaction
the Simon effect did not change with longer RTs (dashed line).
between group and quintile. A larger variability in RTs, from faster to slower
quintiles, can be seen in individuals with MHE (squares) compared to healthy
controls (diamonds). Asterisks denote significant differences between patients with
cant, revealing – as expected – that the Simon effect decreased MHE and healthy controls (Bonferroni post-hoc test, p’s < 0.05).
with slower RTs (Fig. 2 continuous line). The quintile per task con-
dition effect interaction was observed in all groups whereas no count both task condition [F(1, 41) = 1.51, p = 0.17; g2p = 0.04] and
interaction was observed between group, task condition and quin- derivation [F(1, 41) = 0.11; p = 0.9; g2p = 0.003].
tile [F(8, 160) = 0.50, p = 0.85; g2p = 0.02]. A more relevant group per P300 amplitude was lower in the non-corresponding compared
quintile interaction was found [F(8, 160) = 5.61, p < 0.00001; to the corresponding condition [F(1, 41) = 18.77, p < 0.0001;
g2p = 0.21] and post hoc comparisons revealed that the RTs were g2p = 0.32]; furthermore, an effect of derivation was also found
more scattered in patients with MHE compared to healthy controls [F(2, 82) = 3.32, p < 0.05; g2p = 0.07], showing that P300 was greater
(all p’s < 0.05) from the 2nd to the 5th quintile (Fig. 3). in the Pz compared to the Fz site (p < 0.05). The interaction be-
tween task condition and derivation [F(2, 82) = 3.20, p = 0.05;
3.2. P300. analysis g2p = 0.07] showed that the difference between the corresponding
and non-corresponding conditions was greater in the Pz compared
3.2.1. Average-based P300 measures to the Fz site (p < 0.05) (Table 3). A main effect of group was de-
Overall, a trend of shorter latency of P300 in the corresponding tected [F(2, 40) = 8.19; p < 0.005; g2p = 0.33] revealing that P300
compared to the non-corresponding condition was found [F(1, was smaller in patients with MHE compared to healthy controls
41) = 3.71, p = 0.06; g2p = 0.8] (Table 3). No difference was observed but not compared to patients without MHE (post hoc p < 0.05)
in P300 latency between healthy controls and patients with and (Figs. 4 and 5 left panel). No interaction of task condition or deriva-
without MHE [F(1, 41) = 0.09, p = 0.74; g2p = 0.003], taking into ac- tion with the factor group was found (Table 3).
Author's personal copy

292 S. Schiff et al. / Clinical Neurophysiology 125 (2014) 287–297

Table 3
Average-based and single-trial estimated P300 latencies and amplitudes in Pz, Cz, Fz Mean (standard deviation).

Group Controls All patients Patients Patients Controls All patients Patients Patients
Derivation without MHE with MHE without MHE with MHE
Latency (ms) Amplitude (lV)
AVG-P300 Pz 346 (56) 361 (71) 352 (52) 369 (69) 9.8 (4.6) 6.3 (3.2) 7 (3.1) 5.8 (3.3)
corresponding Cz 359 (30) 374 (79) 354 (55) 390 (93) 9.7 (4.3) 5.5 (3.1) 7 (3.2) 4.5 (2.6)
Fz 364 (20) 379 (74) 362 (42) 393 (92) 9.0 (3.4) 6.0 (3.0) 7 (3.0) 4.5 (1.9)
AVG-P300 Pz 360 (65) 365 (103) 347 (104) 378 (102) 9.0 (3.5) 5.4 (2.8) 6.0 (3.0) 4.9 (2.6)
non-corresponding Cz 388 (37) 377 (76) 366 (41) 386 (95) 8.8 (3.7) 4.7 (2.9) 5.8 (3.0) 3.9 (2.7)
Fz 373 (32) 379 (86) 367 (40) 388 (111) 7.9 (2.9) 5.1 (2.8) 6.4 (2.9) 4.0 (2.3)
Single-trial P300 Pz 365 (31) 377 (36) 370 (37) 383 (36) 12.6 (4.2) 9.6 (3.4) 9.8 (3.4) 9.4 (3.4)
corresponding Cz 375 (27) 387 (32) 377 (27) 394 (37) 12.7 (4.3) 8.9 (2.9) 9.6 (3.3) 8.0 (2)
Fz 366 (20) 382 (34) 370 (25) 392 (38) 11.4 (3.6) 8.9 (2.6) 9.7 (3.3) 8.3 (1.7)
Single-trial P300 Pz 385 (40) 391 (43) 388 (53) 397 (34) 12.0 (3.4) 9.1 (3) 9.4 (3.2) 8.8 (3.0)
non-corresponding Cz 393 (34) 405 (34) 396 (29) 412 (37) 12.4 (3.8) 8.7 (3) 9.4 (3.3) 8.1 (2.6)
Fz 384 (26) 394 (34) 383 (31) 403 (33) 11.3 (3.3) 8.8 (2.7) 9.0 (3.2) 8.2 (2.2)

Fig. 4. Event-related potentials in Pz, Cz and Fz are plotted separately for corresponding (on the left) and non-corresponding condition (on the right) and group (healthy
controls, pts without MHE, pts with MHE). P300 was larger in amplitude in healthy controls (solid line) compared to patients with MHE (dotted line) but not compared to
patients without MHE (dashed line).

3.2.2. Single-trial-based P300 measures agreement with the results observed using the averaged-based
Contrary to average based analysis, single-trial P300 latency P300 analysis, single-trial analysis also revealed a significant effect
was clearly shorter in the corresponding compared to the non-cor- of derivation [F(2, 41) = 3.57, p < 0.05; g2p = 0.08], showing that
responding condition [F(1, 41) = 76.54, p < 0.00001; g2p = 0.65]. In P300 latency was higher in the Pz compared to the Fz site (post
Author's personal copy

S. Schiff et al. / Clinical Neurophysiology 125 (2014) 287–297 293

Fig. 5. Left panel: P300 scalp voltage distributions in the three groups are shown for the corresponding and non-corresponding trials. Right panel: Colour plots of single-trial
P300 responses in Pz are depicted in order of RTs speed (black curves), from the fastest to the slowest, in the three groups. Patients with MHE show an increased P300 latency
jittering, a reduction in P300 amplitude (both in the corresponding and non-corresponding conditions), and a loss of the relationship between P300 responses and RTs.

hoc p < 0.05). Also in this case, P300 latency did not differ between determined by using the ERP waveform obtained with the conven-
healthy controls and the patients with cirrhosis, with or without tional averaging. In fact, the obtained correlation coefficient was
MHE [F(2, 82)=1.29, p = 0.26; g2p = 0.03] (Table 3) and no interaction not surprising since, in the second stage of the method described
was found between task condition or derivation with the factor in Section 2.5, single-trial estimates were obtained by exploiting
group. an ‘‘average’’ obtained in stage 1.
Overall, P300 amplitude was lower in the non-corresponding The relationship between IIV of single-trial P300 latency and
condition compared to the corresponding conditions [F(1, average-based P300 amplitude was deducible by the inverse corre-
41) = 5.57, p < 0.05; g2p = 0.12). A significant derivation per task lation between these two measures (r = 0.73, p < 0.0001), show-
condition interaction was found [F(2, 82) = 7.51, p < 0.005; ing that the higher the IIV P300 latency, the lower the average-
g2p = 0.16], showing that the difference between corresponding based P300 amplitude. However, on closer inspection, multivariate
and non-corresponding conditions was greater in Pz compared to regression analysis showed that the averaged-based P300 ampli-
both Fz and Cz (all p’s < 0.001) (Table 3). The main effect of group tude was predicted by both the IIV of single-trial P300 latency
[F(2, 40) = 5.07, p < 0.05; g2p = 0.19] revealed that patients with (b = 0.25 ± 0.04 (SE); p < 0.0001) and single-trial P300 amplitude
MHE had lower P300 amplitude compared to healthy controls (b = 0.78 ± 0.04; p < 0.0001). This was also true considering
(p < 0.05) (Table 3). No interaction was found between task condi- healthy controls and patients with cirrhosis separately: averaged-
tion or derivation and the factor group. based P300 amplitude vs. IIV of single-trial P300 latency (patients
with cirrhosis and healthy controls: b = 0.33 ± 0.06, p < 0.0001
3.2.3. Relationship between average-based and single-trial P300 and b = 0.16 ± 0.06, p < 0.05, respectively), averaged-based P300
measures amplitude vs. single-trial P300 amplitude (patients with cirrhosis
In Pz, both average-based latency and amplitude correlated and healthy controls b = 0.73 ± 0.06, p < 0.0001, and
with the mean of their estimated single-trials counterparts b = 0.85 ± 0.07, p < 0.0001, respectively). Notably, the ratio be-
(r = 0.72, p < 0.0001 and r = 0.90, p < 0.0001, respectively). These tween the Beta’s and their SE (i.e., a measure of the strength of
results confirm that the estimated single-trial P300 latencies were the association between the dependent variable and its predictor),
distributed around a value that was very close to the P300 latency was twice as high in patients with cirrhosis compared to healthy
Author's personal copy

294 S. Schiff et al. / Clinical Neurophysiology 125 (2014) 287–297

Table 4
IIV of P300 latency obtained by single trial estimate in the derivations Pz, Cz, Fz. Mean (standard deviation).

Electrode Controls All patients Patients without MHE Patients with MHE
Latency (ms)
IIV P300 corresponding Pz 77 (23) 94 (20) 87 (19) 99 (20)
IIV P300 non-corresponding Pz 83 (21) 106 (22) 97 (23) 112 (19)
IIV P300 corresponding Cz 79 (18) 98 (19) 89 (17) 104 (18)
IIV P300 non-corresponding Cz 82 (16) 104 (20) 97 (17) 110 (21)
IIV P300 corresponding Fz 80 (17) 93 (20) 84 (18) 101 (20)
IIV P300 non-corresponding Fz 85 (15) 100 (20) 92 (15) 106 (22)

Fig. 6. Left panel: The relationship between P300 latency and RTs shows an interaction between group and quintile, proving that in healthy controls (diamonds), P300 latency
increased with slower RTs. This relationship can also be observed in patients without MHE (circles) but only with faster RTs (i.e. 1st, 2nd and 3rd). In contrast, the relationship
between P300 latency and RTs disappears in patients with MHE (squares). Right panel: The relationship between P300 amplitude and RTs shows an interaction between
group and quintile proving that P300 amplitude decreased with increasing RTs in healthy controls and in patients without MHE, but not in those with MHE.

controls: 5 and 2.3, respectively. This result showed that the con- 3.2.5. Relationship between single-trial P300 and RT distribution
tribution of P300 latency jittering to average-based P300 ampli- Regarding single-trial P300 latency, the main effects of task
tude was higher in patients with cirrhosis compared to healthy condition [F(1, 41) = 75.78, p < 0.00001; g2p = 0.65] and quintile
controls. Thus, not only IIV of RTs but also increased P300 latency [F(4, 164) = 8.49, p < 0.005; g2p = 0.17] were significant. The effect
jittering should be considered as pathophysiological markers of of quintile revealed that P300 latency increased with longer RTs
brain dysfunction in patients with cirrhosis. (1st vs. 2nd, 3rd, 4th quintiles; all p’s < 0.05). Unlike with RTs, no
interaction was found between task condition and quintile [F(4,
164) = 0.97, p = 0.4; g2p = 0.02]; see Fig. 2 (dashed line). A relevant
3.2.4. IIV of P300 group per quintile interaction was found [F(8, 160) = 5.14,
The IIV in P300 latency was higher in the non-corresponding p < 0.00001; g2p = 0.20], showing that the relationship between
compared to the corresponding condition [F(1, 41) = 11.23, P300 latency and RTs was closer and steeper in healthy controls
p < 0.005; g2p = 0.21]. Furthermore, a derivation per task condition than in patients with cirrhosis (Fig. 6 right). Post-hoc tests revealed
interaction was found [F(2, 82) = 5.45, p < 0.01 g2p = 0.12], showing that P300 latency increased along RT distribution (1st vs. 3rd, 4th
that the difference in P300 latency jittering between the corre- and 5th quintiles; all p’s < 0.05) in healthy controls, but not in
sponding and non-corresponding trials was greater in Pz site (post patients with cirrhosis, either with or without MHE. Indeed, in
hoc p < 0.05). The main effect of group [F(2, 39) = 7.8721, p = 0.005; patients with cirrhosis the relationship between RTs and P300
g2p = 0.28] revealed higher IIV in P300 latency in patients with MHE latency was completely lost (see Fig. 6 left panel).
compared to healthy controls, (p < 0.05; see Table 4 and Fig. 1 right The relationship between single-trial P300 amplitude and
panel); in contrast, a post hoc test between healthy controls and RTs distribution highlighted the main effects of task condition
patients without MHE did not reach statistical significance [F(1, 41) = 4.47, p < 0.05; g2p = 0.11], quintile [F(4, 164) = 37.92,
(p = 0.08). No interaction was found between task condition and p < 0.0001; g2p = 0.48], and derivation [F(2, 82) = 4.98, p < 0.01;
group [F(1, 41) = 1.1, p = 0.3; g2p = 0.02]. g2p = 0.48]. The effect of quintile showed that the P300 amplitude
Considering IIV in P300 amplitude, a significant main effect of decreased with increasing RTs (1st vs. 2nd, 3rd, 4th and 5th; 2nd
site was found [F(2, 82) = 7.95, p < 0.001; g2p = 0.16]. Post-hoc vs. 3rd, 4th and 5th; and 3rd vs. 5th quintiles; all p’s < 0.05). A site
comparisons revealed that IIV of P300 amplitude was higher in per quintile interaction was also found, showing that the decrease
Cz compared to both Pz and Fz sites (all p’s < 0.05). The ANOVA in amplitude with slower RTs was greater for the Pz and Cz sites
did not reveal any other significant effects of IIV in P300 amplitude compared to the Fz site (all p’s < 0.05). Furthermore, the significant
concerning the factors task condition [F(1, 41) = 0.37, p = 0.85; effect of group [F(1, 41) = 8.71, p < 0.01; g2p = 0.18] and the signifi-
g2p = 0.001] and group [F(1, 41) = 0.35, p = 0.54; g2p = 0.01]. cant interaction between group and quintile [F(8, 160) = 2.7,
Author's personal copy

S. Schiff et al. / Clinical Neurophysiology 125 (2014) 287–297 295

p < 0.01; g2p = 0.12], revealed that the P300 amplitude was smaller and astrocytes convert it into glutamine (Gln) before it is returned
in patients with MHE and decreased along RTs distribution in to the neurons and recycled. From a neurobiological point of view,
healthy controls (1st vs. 3rd, 4th and 5th quintiles) and in patients astrocyte swelling with the accumulation of Gln is considered to be
without MHE (1st and 2nd vs. 4th and 5th; 2nd vs. 5th quintiles; all a key mechanism related to hepatic encephalopathy. It can be
p’s < 0.05); such a relationship was lost in patients with MHE (1st hypothesised that this metabolic dysfunction is implicated in
vs. 4th quintile; p < 0.05; see Figs. 5 and 6 right panels). increasing variability in the synchrony of the firing rate of large
neuronal populations, as a consequence increasing the variability
of post-synaptic neural responses, P300 and behavioral response.
4. Discussion Interestingly, a similar mechanism linking neurones and astrocyte
functioning was recently suggested in order to explain increased
In the present study, a Bayesian approach was used to estimate IIV in behavioral speed and RTs slowness in subjects with ADHD
single-trial P300 parameters evoked by a conflict S–R Simon task in (Russell et al., 2006). Following a different perspective on brain
patients with and without minimal metabolic brain dysfunction functioning, fMRI studies showed an incomplete deactivation of
due to liver cirrhosis and in a group of healthy controls. In line with the default mode network during task execution, which was corre-
our hypothesis, patients with liver cirrhosis, especially those with lated with increased variability in behavioral speed (Kelly et al.,
MHE, showed longer RTs and reduced response accuracy as com- 2008). Within this framework, it is important to note that in indi-
pared with healthy controls. Furthermore, IIV in RTs was greater viduals with ADHD, a lack of default network suppression was
in patients with cirrhosis, both with and without MHE, than in found and linked to increased response speed variability (Fassb-
healthy controls. This latter result was confirmed by distribution ender et al., 2009). Similarly, an abnormal activation of the default
analysis of RTs. Patients with cirrhosis had a greater difference be- network was also found in patients with cirrhosis when engaged in
tween slower and faster quintiles when compared to healthy con- a cognitive task (Zhang et al., 2007).
trols. Therefore, our data confirm previous observations regarding Another observation derived from the present study concerns
increased variability in reaction speed and changes in the slope of the functional meaning of P300. Using a single-trial approach, we
RTs distribution in patients with cirrhosis compared to healthy have evaluated the dynamics of the relationship between P300 la-
controls (Elsass et al., 1985; Schiff et al., 2006). tency and S–R correspondence along the RTs distribution. Usually,
The first aim of the present study was to investigate whether the Simon effect is explained in terms of competition between a di-
patients with or without MHE exhibit increased variability in sin- rect visuo-motor transmission route carrying the response linked
gle-trial P300 parameters and to evaluate the relationship between to the spatial position of the stimulus, and an indirect route hold-
P300 latency jittering and average-based P300 amplitude. The pat- ing task demands. The activation/suppression model suggests that,
tern of results showed that patients with cirrhosis had smaller in the non-corresponding condition, the early activation of the
P300 amplitude compared to healthy controls; in contrast, P300 la- incorrect response spatially corresponding with stimulus position
tency did not differ between groups. Our data revealed a greater IIV is proactively suppressed, at a response selection stage, by a late
of P300 latency in patients with cirrhosis, mainly those with MHE, inhibitory mechanism. This mechanism needs time to be effective
compared to healthy controls. Furthermore, even if average-based and, for this reason, it affects the magnitude of the Simon effect,
P300 amplitude was related to both single-trial P300 amplitude mainly for slower RTs (Ridderinkhof, 2002). In agreement with this
and P300 latency jittering, the contribution of IIV of P300 latency model, the Simon effect usually decreases with longer RTs.
was double in patients compared to controls. These results suggest In the present study, the Simon effect (i.e., slower RTs and lower
a specific link between P300 amplitude, IIV of P300 latency, and accuracy in the non-corresponding condition than in the corre-
the metabolic brain dysfunction that characterises patients with sponding condition) was detected both in healthy controls and pa-
cirrhosis. tients with cirrhosis. Distribution analysis of RTs showed that the
The second aim of the study was to evaluate the relationship Simon effect decreased in magnitude as RTs increased, both in
between RTs distribution and P300 parameters. Our analysis re- healthy controls and patients with cirrhosis, regardless of the pres-
vealed that in healthy participants, P300 latency increased and ence of psychomotor slowing. Electrophysiological results confirm
amplitude decreased with increasing RTs. In contrast, the relation- that P300, obtained during the Simon task, is sensitive to S–R cor-
ships between RTs and P300 parameters were weaker in patients respondence (Valle-Inclan, 1996; Wascher and Wauschkuhn,
without MHE and absent in patients with MHE. These results sug- 1996). A longer P300 latency and a smaller P300 amplitude in
gest that, in normal conditions, the temporal overlap between the non-corresponding condition compared to the corresponding
early decision processes on S–R associations and response-related condition were observed both in healthy controls and in patients
processes leads to faster RTs and is well described by the changes with cirrhosis; however, in contrast to behavioral data, when
in within-subjects P300 parameters along the RTs distribution: P300 latency was considered, a suppression of the Simon effect
lower P300 amplitude and delayed P300 latency for slower com- with longer RTs was not observed. These results suggest that
pared to faster RTs. In contrast, in patients with cirrhosis both psy- P300 is related to S–R association but not to late response
chomotor slowing and increase of IIV of RTs might be explained by inhibition.
the loss of the temporal overlap between the early decision on S–R Indeed, P300-like responses have been recorded intra-cortically
association and late response selection processes. from several brain sites (Halgren et al., 1995a,b). However, parietal
From a pathophysiological point of view, the results described P300 (P3b) is plausibly generated mainly in the temporal-parietal
above suggest that the increased variability in RTs is related to junction (Verleger, 1997). Several pieces of evidence suggest that
the altered brain functioning and neurotransmission observed in these regions of the brain are functionally involved in the integra-
patients with cirrhosis (Zafiris et al., 2004; Shah et al., 2008; Pove- tion of visual information and action plans (Rushworth et al., 2001;
da et al., 2010). The metabolic brain dysfunction in patients with Schiff et al., 2011).
cirrhosis seems to depend, at least in part, on increased blood/cere- On the other hand, Forstmann et al., (2008a,b) showed, using
bral ammonia levels. These lead to changes in cerebral energy model-based fMRI, that in the Simon task the early activation of
metabolism and neurotransmission, in particular within the gluta- the prepotent incorrect response correlates with activity of the
matergic system (Wilkinson et al., 2010). At the synaptic level, anterior cingulate cortex and the pre-supplementary motor area.
astrocytes play an important role in removing glutamate (Glu) In contrast, the late inhibition of the activated incorrect response
from synapses. Glutamate is released by pre-synaptic neurons was associated with the activity of the inferior frontal cortex. These
Author's personal copy

296 S. Schiff et al. / Clinical Neurophysiology 125 (2014) 287–297

results suggest that inhibition can be considered a two-stage Bajaj JS, Cordoba J, Mullen KD, Amodio P, Shawcross DL, Butterworth RF, et al.
Review article: the design of clinical trials in hepatic encephalopathy – An
process acting on both the early phase of response activation and
international society for hepatic encephalopathy and nitrogen metabolism
on the late inhibition of an incorrect response (Bar et al., 2006). (ISHEN) consensus statement. Aliment Pharmacol. Ther. 2011;33:739.
In agreement with this view, Wylie et al., (2010) showed that, in Balota DA, Yap MJ. Moving beyond the mean in studies of mental chronometry: the
patients with Parkinson’s disease, deep brain stimulation of the power of response time distributional analyses. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci.
2011;20:160–6.
sub-thalamic nucleus affects both the early activation and late Bar M, Kassam KS, Ghuman AS, Boshyan J, Schmi AM, Dale AM, et al. Top–down
inhibition of a prepotent incorrect response. Thus, in agreement facilitation of visual recognition. PNAS 2006;103:449–54.
with Verleger et al., (2005), our results suggest that P300 is sensi- Bunce D, MacDonald SW, Hultsch DF. Inconsistency in serial choice decision and
motor reaction times dissociate in younger and older adults. Brain Cogn.
tive to early coding of the S–R association but independent of 2004;56:320–7.
inhibitory processes acting on later response-selection stage. Burton CL, Hultsch DF, Strauss E, Hunter MA. Intraindividual variability in physical
In conclusion, the present study: (i) supports the convincement and emotional functioning: comparison of adults with traumatic brain injuries
and healthy adults. Clin. Neuropsychol. 2002;16:264–79.
that behavioral and electrophysiological measures of within- Castellanos FX, Sonuga-Barke EJ, Milham MP, Tannock R. Characterizing cognition in
subject variability are useful indicators of brain dysfunctions and ADHD: beyond executive dysfunction. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2006;10:117–23.
single-trial ERP analysis is a powerful technique both in clinical Castellanos FX, Sonuga-Barke EJ, Scheres A, Di MA, Hyde C, Walters JR. Varieties of
attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder-related intra-individual variability.
studies and basic research especially when within-subject brain re- Biol. Psychiatry. 2005;57:1416–23.
sponses are investigated together with RTs distribution, (ii) proves Castellanos FX, Tannock R. Neuroscience of attentiondeficit/hyperactivity disorder:
that in patients with MHE, the higher IIV of RTs is associated with the search for endophenotypes. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2002;2002(3):617–28.
Christensen H, Dear KB, Anstey KJ, Parslow RA, Sachdev P, Jorm AF. Within-occasion
both a reduction in P300 amplitude and an increase in P300 latency
intraindividual variability and preclinical diagnostic status: is intraindividual
jittering, and (iii) shows that the loss of a temporal overlap variability an indicator of mild cognitive impairment? Neuropsychology
between P300 and behavioral responses in patients with MHE is 2005;19:309–17.
associated with psychomotor slowing, but is independent of late D’Avanzo C, Schiff S, Amodio P, Sparacino G. A Bayesian method to estimate single-
trial event-related potentials with application to the study of the P300
response inhibition. The present study further suggests that new variability. J. Neurosci. Methods 2011;198:114–24.
technological approaches to EEG signal analysis, such as single- D’Avanzo C, Goljahani A, Pillonetto G, De Nicolao G, Sparacino G. A multi-task
trial ERPs estimation or automatic detection of individual alpha learning approach for the extraction of single-trial evoked potentials. Comput.
Methods Programs Biomed. 2013;110:125–36.
frequency (Goljahani et al., 2012), may reveal further electrophys- Del Piccolo F, Sacerdoti D, Amodio P, Bombonato G, Bolognesi M, Mapelli D, et al.
iological peculiarities uniquely related to the MHE condition. These Central nervous system alterations in liver cirrhosis: the role of portal-systemic
peculiarities may allow development of non-invasive reliable shunt and portal hypoperfusion. Metab. Brain Dis. 2003;18:51–62.
Delorme A, Makeig S. EEGLAB: an open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial
diagnostic tools. EEG dynamics including independent component analysis. J. Neurosci. Methods
2004;134:9–21.
Acknowledgment De Lucia M, Michel CM, Murray MM. Comparing ICA-based and single-trial
topographic ERP analyses. Brain Topogr. 2010;23:119–27.
Elsass P, Christensen SE, Mortensen EL, Vilstrup H. Discrimination between organic
The study was partially supported by the Bial Foundation Grant and hepatic encephalopathy by means of continuous reaction times. Liver
No. 146/2008 to P.B., by the Regione Veneto FSE Grants ‘‘Develop- 1985;5:29–34.
Fassbender C, Zhang H, Buzy WM, Cortes CR, Mizuiri D, Beckett L, et al. A lack of
ment of a system for the analysis of the intraindividual EEG default network suppression is linked to increased distractibility in ADHD. Brain
variability for the early identification of cognitive deficiencies’’ Res. 2009;1273:114–28.
and ‘‘Implementation of methodologies for the quantification of Fell J. Cognitive neurophysiology: beyond averaging. Neuroimage
2007;37:1069–72.
electrophysiological signals in mild cognitive disorders’’ to C.D.A. Ferenci P, Lockwood A, Mullen K, Tarter R, Weissenborn K, Blei AT. Hepatic
and A.G., and by the University of Padova Grant ‘‘Quantitative encephalopathy–definition, nomenclature, diagnosis, and quantification: final
understanding of the human brain functioning through advanced report of the working party at the 11th World congresses of gastroenterology,
Vienna, 1998. Hepatol 2002;35:716–21.
EEG and NIRS signal processing’’ to G.S.
Fjell AM, Rosquist H, Walhovd KB. Instability in the latency of P3a/P3b brain
potentials and cognitive function in aging. Neurobiol. Aging 2009;30:2065–79.
References Forstmann BU, Jahfari S, Scholte HS, Wolfensteller U, van den Wildenberg WPM,
Ridderinkhof KR. Function and structure of the right inferior frontal cortex
predict individual differences in response inhibition: a model-based approach. J.
Amodio P, Ridola L, Schiff S, Montagnese S, Pasquale C, Nardelli S, et al. Improving
Neurosci. 2008a;28:9790–6.
detection of minimal hepatic encephalopathy using the inhibitory control task.
Forstmann BU, van den Wildenberg WPM, Ridderinkhof KR. Neural Mechanisms,
Gastroenterol 2010;139:510–8.
Temporal Dynamics, and Individual Differences in Interference Control. J. Cog.
Amodio P, Campagna F, Olianas S, Iannizzi P, Mapelli D, Penzo M, et al. Detection of
Neurosci. 2008b;20:1854–65.
minimal hepatic encephalopathy: normalization and optimization of the
Goljahani A, D’Avanzo C, Schiff S, Amodio P, Bisiacchi P, Sparacino G. A novel
psychometric hepatic encephalopathy score. A neuropsychological and
method for the determination of the EEG individual alpha frequency.
quantified EEG study. J. Hepatol. 2008;49:346–53.
Neuroimage 2012;60:774–86.
Amodio P, Schiff S, Del Piccolo F, Mapelli D, Gatta A, Umilta C. Attention dysfunction
Halgren E, Baudena P, Clarke JM, Heit G, Liégeois C, Chauvel P, et al. Intracerebral
in cirrhotic patients: an inquiry on the role of executive control, attention
potentials to rare target and distractor auditory and visual stimuli. I. Superior
orienting and focusing. Metab. Brain. Dis. 2005a;20:115–27.
temporal plane and parietal lobe. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol.
Amodio P, Valenti P, Del Piccolo F, Pellegrini A, Schiff S, Angeli P, et al. P300 latency
1995a;94:191–220.
for the diagnosis of minimal hepatic encephalopathy: evidence that spectral
Halgren E, Baudena P, Clarke JM, Heit G, Marinkovic K, Devaux B, et al. Intracerebral
EEG analysis and psychometric tests are enough. Dig. Liver Dis.
potentials to rare target and distractor auditory and visual stimuli. II. Medial,
2005b;37:861–8.
lateral and posterior temporal lobe. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol.
Amodio P, Del Piccolo F, Petteno E, Mapelli D, Angeli P, Iemmolo R, et al. Prevalence
1995b;94:229–50.
and prognostic value of quantified electroencephalogram (EEG) alterations in
Holm A, Ranta-aho PO, Sallinen M, Karjalainen PA, Müller K. Relationship of P300
cirrhotic patients. J. Hepatol. 2001;35:37–45.
single-trial responses with reaction time and preceding stimulus sequence. Int.
Amodio P, Del Piccolo F, Marchetti P, Angeli P, Iemmolo R, Caregaro L, et al. Clinical
J. Psychophysiol. 2006;61:244–52.
features and survivial of cirrhotic patients with subclinical cognitive alterations
Hu L, Mouraux A, Hu Y. Iannetti G.D. A novel approach for enhancing the signal-to-
detected by the number connection test and computerized psychometric tests.
noise ratio and detecting automatically event-related potentials (ERPs) in single
Hepatol 1999a;29:1662–7.
trials. Neuroimage 2010;50:99–111.
Amodio P, Marchetti P, Del Piccolo F, de Tourtchaninoff M, Varghese P, Zuliani C,
Hultsch DF, MacDonald SW, Dixon RA. Variability in reaction time performance of
et al. Spectral versus visual EEG analysis in mild hepatic encephalopathy. Clin.
younger and older adults. J. Gerontol. B Psychol. Sci. Soc. Sci. 2002;57:101–15.
Neurophysiol. 1999b;110:1334–44.
Hultsch DF, MacDonald SW, Hunter MA, Levy-Bencheton J, Strauss E.
Amodio P, Marchetti P, Del Piccolo F, Rizzo C, Iemmolo RM, Caregaro L, et al. Study
Intraindividual variability in cognitive performance in older adults:
on the sternberg paradigm in cirrhotic patients without overt hepatic
comparison of adults with mild dementia, adults with arthritis, and healthy
encephalopathy. Metab. Brain Dis. 1998;13:59–72.
adults. Neuropsychology 2000;14:588–98.
Aston-Jones G, Cohen JD. An integrative theory of locus coeruleus-norepinephrine
Jaskowski P, Verleger R. Amplitudes and latencies of single-trial ERP’s estimated by
function: adaptive gain and optimal performance. Annu. Rev. Neurosci.
a maximum-likelihood method. IEEE. Trans. Biomed. Eng. 1999;46:987–93.
2005;28:403–50.
Author's personal copy

S. Schiff et al. / Clinical Neurophysiology 125 (2014) 287–297 297

Jaskowski P, Verleger R. An evaluation of methods for single-trial estimation of P3 Saville CWN, Dean RO, Daley D, Intriligator J, Boehm S, Feige B, Klein C.
latency. Psychophysiology 2000;37:153–62. Electrocortical correlates of intra-subject variability in reaction times:
Jung TP, Makeig S, Westerfield M, Townsend J, Courchesne E, Sejnowski TJ. Analysis Average and single-trial analyses. Biol. Psychol. 2011;87:74–83.
and visualization of single-trial event-related potentials. Hum. Brain. Mapp. Saxena N, Bhatia M, Joshi YK, Garg PK, Dwivedi SN, Tandon RK. Electrophysiological
2001;14:166–85. and neuropsychological tests for the diagnosis of subclinical hepatic
Kaiser S, Roth A, Rentrop M, Friederich HC, Bender S, Weisbrod M. Intra-individual encephalopathy and prediction of overt encephalopathy. Liver 2002;22:190–7.
reaction time variability in schizophrenia, depression and borderline Schiff S, Bardi L, Basso D, Mapelli D. Timing spatial conflict within the posterior
personality disorder. Brain Cogn. 2008;66:73–82. parietal cortex. A TMS study. J. Cog. Neurosci. 2011;23:3998–4007.
Kelly AM, Uddin LQ, Biswal BB, Castellanos FX, Milham MP. Competition between Schiff S, Valenti P, Pellegrini A, Lot M, Bisiacchi P, Gatta A, et al. The effect of aging
functional brain networks mediates behavioral variability. Neuroimage on auditory components of event-related brain potentials. Clin. Neurophysiol.
2008;39:527–37. 2008;119:1795–802.
Kornblum S, Hasbroucq T, Osman A. Dimensional overlap: cognitive basis for Schiff S, Mapelli D, Vallesi A, Orsato R, Gatta A, Umilta C, et al. Top–down and
stimulus–response compatibility: a model and taxonomy. Psychol. Rev. bottom-up processes in the extrastriate cortex of cirrhotic patients: an ERP
1990;1990(97):253–70. study. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2006;117:1728–36.
Leth-Steensen C, Elbaz ZK, Douglas VI. Mean response times, variability, and skew in Schiff S, Vallesi A, Mapelli D, Orsato R, Pellegrini A, Umilta C, et al. Impairment of
the responding of ADHD children: a response time distributional approach. response inhibition precedes motor alteration in the early stage of liver
Acta. Psychol. 2000;104:167–90. cirrhosis: a behavioral and electrophysiological study. Metab. Brain. Dis.
Li R, Keil A, Principe JC. Single-trial P300 estimation with a spatiotemporal filtering 2005;20:381–92.
method. J. Neurosci. Methods 2009;177:488–96. Shah NJ, Neeb H, Kircheis G, Engels P, Haussinger D, Zilles K. Quantitative cerebral
Lorenzo-Lopez L, Amenedo E, Pazo-Alvarez P, Cadaveira F. Visual target processing water content mapping in hepatic encephalopathy. Neuroimage
in high- and low-performing older subjects indexed by P3 component. 2008;41:706–17.
Neurophysiolo. Clin/Clin. Neurophysiol. 2007;37:53–61. Simon JR. The simon effect: a potent behavioral mechanism. Acta. Psychol.
MacDonald SW, Nyberg L, Backman L. Intra-individual variability in behavior: links 2011;136:181.
to brain structure, neurotransmission and neuronal activity. Trends Neurosci. Simon JR, Rudell AP. Auditory S–R compatibility: the effect of an irrelevant cue on
2006;29:474–80. information processing. J. Appl. Psychol. 1967;51:300–4.
Magliero A, Bashore TR, Coles MG, Donchin E. On the dependence of P300 latency on Sparacino G, Milani S, Arslan E, Cobelli C. A Bayesian approach to estimate evoked
stimulus evaluation processes. Psychophysiology 1984;21:171–86. potentials. Comput. Methods Programs Biomed. 2002;68:233–48.
Makeig S, Debener S, Onton J, Delorme A. Mining event-related brain dynamics. Stuss DT, Pogue J, Buckle L, Bondar J. Characterization of stability of performance in
Trends Cogn. Sci. 2004;8:204–10. patients with traumatic brain injury: variability and consistency on reaction
Marchetti P, D’Avanzo C, Orsato R, Montagnese S, Schiff S, Kaplan PW, et al. time tests. Neuropsychology 1994;8:316–24.
Electroencephalography in patients with cirrhosis. Gastroenterology Stuss DT, Murphy KJ, Binns MA, Alexander MP. Staying on the job: the frontal lobes
2011;141:1680–9. control individual performance variability. Brain 2003;126:2363–80.
Mouraux A, Iannetti GD. Across-trial averaging of event-related EEG responses and Sutton S, Braren M, Zubin J, John ER. Evoked-potential correlates of stimulus
beyond. Magn. Reson. Imaging 2008;26:1041–54. uncertainty. Science 1965;150:1187–8.
Murtha S, Cismaru R, Waechter R, Chertkow H. Increased variability accompanies Valle-Inclan F. The locus of interference in the Simon effect: an ERP study. Biol.
frontal lobe damage in dementia. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 2002;8:360–72. Psychol. 1996;43:147–62.
Nieuwenhuis S, Aston-Jones G, Cohen JD. Decision making, the P3, and the locus Vallesi A, Mapelli D, Schiff S, Amodio P, Umilta C. Horizontal and vertical simon
coeruleus–norepinephrine system. Psycho. Bull 2005;4:510–32. effect: different underlying mechanisms? Cognition 2005;96:B33–43.
Polich J. Updating P300: an integrative theory of P3a and P3b. Clin. Neurophysiol. Verleger R, Jaskowski P, Wascher E. Evidence for an integrative role of P3b in linking
2007;118:2128–48. reaction times to perception. J. Psychophysiol. 2005;19:165–81.
Poveda MJ, Bernabeu A, Concepcion L, Roa E, ME De, Zapater P, Perez-Mateo M, et al. Verleger R. On the utility of P3 latency as an index of mental chronometry.
Brain edema dynamics in patients with overt hepatic encephalopathy a Psychophysiology 1997;34:131–56.
magnetic resonance imaging study. Neuroimage 2010;52:481–7. Walhovd KB, Rosquist H, Fjell AM. P300 amplitude age reductions are not caused by
Pugh RN, Murray-Lyon IM, Dawson JL, Pietroni MC, Williams R. Transection of the latency jitter. Psychophysiology 2008;45:545–53.
oesophagus for bleeding oesophageal varices. Br. J. Surg. 1973;60:646–9. Wascher E, Wauschkuhn B. The interaction of stimulus- and response-related
Ratcliff R. Group reaction time distributions and an analysis of distribution processes measured by event-related lateralizations of the EEG.
statistics. Psychol. Bull 1979;86:446–61. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 1996;99:149–62.
Ridderinkhof KR. Micro- and macro-adjustments of task set: activation and Wilkinson DJ, Smeeton NJ, Watt PW. Ammonia metabolism, the brain and fatigue;
suppression in conflict tasks. Psychol. Res. 2002;66:312–23. revisiting the link. Prog. Neurobiol. 2010;91:200–19.
Roth A, Roesch-Ely D, Bender S, Weisbrod M, Kaiser S. Increased event-related Williams BR, Hultsch DF, Strauss EH, Hunter MA, Tannock R. Inconsistency in
potential latency and amplitude variability in schizophrenia detected through reaction time across the life span. Neuropsychology 2005;19:88–96.
wavelet-based single trial analysis. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 2007;66:244–54. Wylie SA, Ridderinkhof KR, Elias WJ, Frysinger RC, Bashore TR, Downs KE, et al.
Rugg MD, Coles MGH. Electrophysiol. Mind. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 1995. Subthalamic nucleus stimulation influences expression and suppression of
Rushby JA, Barry RJ. Single-trial event-related potentials to significant stimuli. Int. J. impulsive behaviour in Parkinson’s disease. Brain 2010;133:3611–24.
Psychophysiol. 2009;74:120–31. Zafiris O, Kircheis G, Rood HA, Boers F, Haussinger D, Zilles K. Neural mechanism
Rushworth MF, Ellison A, Walsh V. Complementary localization and lateralization of underlying impaired visual judgement in the dysmetabolic brain: an fMRI
orienting and motor attention. Nat. Neurosci. 2001;4:656–61. study. Neuroimage 2004;22:541–52.
Russell VA, Oades RD, Tannock R, Killeen PR, Auerbach JG, Johansen EB, et al. Zhang LJ, Yang G, Yin J, Liu Y, Qi J. Abnormal default-mode network activation in
Response variability in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a neuronal and cirrhotic patients: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Acta. Radiol.
glial energetics hypothesis. Behav. Brain. Funct. 2006;2:30. 2007;48:781–7.

Você também pode gostar