Você está na página 1de 14

J Autism Dev Disord (2008) 38:72–85

DOI 10.1007/s10803-007-0364-6

ORIGINAL PAPER

Asperger Syndrome and Autism: A Comparative Longitudinal


Follow-Up Study More than 5 Years after Original Diagnosis
Mats Cederlund Æ Bibbi Hagberg Æ Eva Billstedt Æ
I. Carina Gillberg Æ Christopher Gillberg

Published online: 6 March 2007


 Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2007

Abstract Prospective follow-up study of 70 males with Hans Asperger (Asperger, 1944), did not appear as
Asperger syndrome (AS), and 70 males with autism a diagnostic entity until the early 1980s after being
more than 5 years after original diagnosis. Instruments reintroduced by Lorna Wing (Wing, 1981). The first set
used at follow-up included overall clinical assessment, of diagnostic criteria were formulated by Gillberg and
the Diagnostic Interview for Social and Communica- Gillberg (1988/1989). The Diagnostic and Statistical
tion Disorders, Wechsler Intelligence Scales, Vineland Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-IV), and the ICD-
Adaptive Behavior Scales, and Global Assessment of 10 classification of Mental and Behavioural Disorders,
Functioning Scale. Specific outcome criteria were used. did not publish diagnostic criteria for AS until well into
Outcome in AS was good in 27% of cases. However, the 1990s (APA, 1994, WHO, 1993). These criteria
26% had a very restricted life, with no occupation/ have been widely criticized (e.g., Leekam, Libby,
activity and no friends. Outcome in the autism group Wing, Gould, & Gillberg, 2000; Miller & Ozonoff,
was significantly worse. Males with AS had worse out- 1997), and there is still no consensus as to how AS
comes than expected given normal to high IQ. How- should best be delineated. The Gillberǵs six criteria for
ever, outcome was considerably better than for the making the diagnosis of AS are based on Hans
comparison group of individuals with autism. Aspergeŕs original publication and require major
problems with social interaction (e.g., impairing diffi-
Keywords Asperger syndrome  Autism  Follow-up  culties making friends and understanding social cues),
Intellectual ability  Outcome  DISCO narrow interests, repetitive routines, speech and lan-
guage (e.g., odd ways of using expressive language, odd
speech, intonation problems, and difficulties under-
Introduction standing others), non-verbal communication problems,
and motor clumsiness. At least 9 symptoms are re-
Asperger syndrome (AS), described as ‘‘autistic psy- quired for a diagnosis of AS. The DSM-IV criteria for
chopathy’’ already in 1944 by the Austrian paediatrician AS specify impairments in social interaction (defined
as for autism), restricted, repetitive, stereotyped
behaviour (defined as for autism), and absence of
M. Cederlund (&)  B. Hagberg  E. Billstedt  clinically significant delay in language or cognitive
I. C. Gillberg  C. Gillberg development, including self-help skills, adaptive
Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Institute of
behaviour and curiosity about the environment, in the
Neuroscience and Physiology, Göteborg University,
GöteborgKungsgatan 12, 411 19, Sweden first 3 years of life (see Appendices). Only 3 symptoms
e-mail: mats.cederlund@vgregion.se are required for a diagnosis of AS.
Over the years, there has been much discussion
M. Cederlund  B. Hagberg  E. Billstedt 
about whether or not AS and so-called High Func-
I. C. Gillberg  C. Gillberg
Queen Silviás Child- and Adolescent Hospital, tioning Autism (HFA) are separate, similar or identi-
Otterhällegatan, 12 A, 411 18 Göteborg, Sweden cal conditions (Gillberg, 1998; Mesibov, Kunce, &

123
J Autism Dev Disord (2008) 38:72–85 73

Schopler, 1998; Wing & Potter, 2002). Although high criteria formulated by Gillberg and Gillberg (1989) and
verbal intelligence—including better verbal than per- Gillberg (1991), and of autistic disorder and atypical
formance skills on the Wechsler scales—and earlier autism as diagnosed according to the DSM-IV. An-
language development have been noted to be more other aim was to estimate the frequency of severe
common in people with AS than in classic autism, no psychiatric disorder (psychosis) in the investigated
research has so far been able to show clear-cut differ- groups, given earlier reports of a high frequency of
ences between people diagnosed with HFA as com- such disorders within the autism spectrum (e.g., Wozniak
pared with those who have been given a diagnosis of et al., 1997; Wolff, 1995). Finally, there has been con-
AS (Eisenmajeret al., 1998; Gilchrist et al., 2001; troversy about the rate of criminal offence within the
Howlin, 2003). AS/HFA group particularly after the publication of
Very few reports on the outcome of AS have been reports documenting a high rate of these diagnoses
published. They have referred to small or highly se- among highly selected samples of offending psychiatric
lected clinical case samples without comparison groups patients (e.g., Siponmaa, Kristiansson, Jonsson, Nydén,
and have reported low levels of employment and social & Gillberg, 2001; Scragg & Shah, 1994), and so our
functioning (Green, Gilchrist, Burton, & Cox, 2000; third aim was to examine the prevalence of such of-
Tantam, 1991; Tsatsanis, 2003; Wing, 1981). In this fence committed by individuals with AS and autism in
study psychosocial functioning includes aspects of early adult life.
employment/studying, relationship to others, indepen- Some individuals with a diagnosis of Asperger syn-
dent living, and absence of psychiatric problems. drome in childhood appear to do well in adult life, and
Intellectual decline over time, as measured by the this has led some people to question the appropriate-
Wechsler scales, was reported in one study of the ness of making the diagnosis at all. Could the diagnosis
intermediate term outcome of AS (Nydén, Billstedt, of Asperger syndrome actually contribute to problems
Hjelmqvist, & Gillberg, 2001), but has not been ob- (such as the supposed stigma of having a diagnosis at
served in later studies. A tendency towards closing of all) in certain cases? We consider that, if diagnoses are
the gap between superior verbal and inferior non-ver- only made in individuals who have clinical impairment,
bal skills over time has been observed (Szatmari, 2003, this would not be an issue. In the present study we used
personal communication). Recent studies of the short- the Global Assessment of Functioning (GAF) scale to
term outcome of AS have suggested a substantially score psychosocial functioning, so as to determine
better outcome than in autism, which may have been whether or not an individual, who fulfils symptom cri-
due to earlier and more effective interventions, or to teria for AS should also be given the diagnosis of AS.
other factors (Starr, Szatmari, Bryson, & Zweigenbaum, The following hypotheses were tested: (i) diagnoses
2003; Szatmari, Bryson, Boyle, Streiner, & Duku, 2003; within the autism spectrum are stable over time, (ii)
Tsatsanis, 2003). AS has a psychosocially better outcome than autism,
Outcome in classic cases of autism has been inves- (iii) better outcome in AS is attributable to higher IQ,
tigated in a number of studies in the past. The rate of (iv) intellectual ability declines over time in these dis-
poor or very poor psychosocial outcome (isolated life orders, (v) individuals with higher verbal IQ have the
with high degree of dependency on others) has been best psychosocial outcome, (vi) those with an earlier
around 50% (e.g., Gillberg & Steffenburg, 1987; diagnosis have fewer problems in early adult life, (vii)
Howlin, Mawhood, & Rutter, 2000; Howlin, Goode, there is a higher frequency of severe psychiatric dis-
Hutton, and Rutter, 2004), and IQ has decreased over orders (psychosis), than in the general population,
time (Billstedt, Gillberg, & Gillberg, 2005). (viii) involvement with the police and the law is at the
The present study is probably the first ever to same rate as in the general population.
present a long-term perspective on the natural out-
come of a reasonably large group of males with AS.
The background and associated factors of the AS Methods
group have been outlined in two previous publications
(Cederlund & Gillberg, 2004; Gillberg & Cederlund, AS group
2005). The AS group was contrasted with a similarly
aged group of males with classic autism/atypical autism One-hundred males with AS, 16–36 years old, diag-
followed up using identical/similar instruments. nosed at the Child Neuropsychiatric Clinic (CNC) in
One aim of the study was to examine the diagnostic Göteborg, Sweden, in the years 1985–1999, were ap-
stability over time within the autism spectrum, in this proached for inclusion in the follow-up study. They
case the stability of AS as diagnosed on the basis of the were born 1967–1988, and had been diagnosed with AS

123
74 J Autism Dev Disord (2008) 38:72–85

at ages 5.5–24.5 years. Mean age at diagnosis was group had been evaluated in childhood with the fol-
11.3 years (SD 3.8 years), which is in keeping with lowing results: Severe Mental Retardation (SMR,
previous findings (Howlin & Ashgarian, 1999). They all IQ < 50) (n = 39), Mild Mental Retardation (MMR,
fulfilled the Gillberg and Gillberg criteria for AS IQ 50–69) (n = 31), Near Average Intelligence (NA,
(Gillberg 1991; Gillberg & Gillberg, 1989), which had IQ 70–84) (n = 10) and Average Intelligence
been in use at the clinic since 1985. (A, IQ 85–114) (n = 4).
Criteria for inclusion in the follow-up study were (i)
normal intelligence (IQ > 70) at diagnosis on the Attrition
Wechsler scales (WISC-R (n = 40), WISC-III (n = 52)
or WAIS-R (n = 8)), (ii) AS diagnosis made ‡5 years AS Group
prior to follow-up, and (iii) age ‡16 years at the census
date 30/06/2004. We included males only, since no There was no mortality in the original AS group of 100
more than 7 females meeting inclusion criteria were males. A letter of information about the AS study was
found. The AS group had been assessed on the sent to all 100 individuals or their parents (in those
Wechsler scales (Wechsler, 1974, 1992a, 1992b) at under 18 years of age). The study was referred to as ‘‘a
original diagnosis, and had had the following mean follow-up study of Asperger syndrome’’. Of the 100
results: Full Scale Intelligence Quotient (FSIQ) 101.4 males with AS and/or their parents targeted for inclu-
(SD 18.3), Verbal Intelligence Quotient (VIQ) 107.2 sion, 24 refused or failed all participation in the follow-
(SD 18.6) and Performance Intelligence Quotient up study. Nineteen of these declined participation over
(PIQ) 94.6 (SD 18.7) (VIQ > PIQ, p < 0.01). the telephone, and three did so in writing. In two cases
Sixty-one individuals in the AS group had had no there was no response in spite of reminders, and so we
further contact with the CNC after the diagnosis had have no clue as to who was ‘‘responsible’’ for the
been made and information about results had been failure to participate. (Of the 24 who refused, five had
shared with the family. A further 8 had had no contact never been informed about their condition. Parents
with the CNC for the past 5 years, but 31 had had one had never told their child about the diagnosis, and they
or several contacts in the recent past (i.e., 2001 or did not want for their sons to find out about it now. In
later). We have argued elsewhere (Cederlund & Gillberg, the other 19 cases, we only have limited information
2004) that the 100 individuals approached are fairly about the reason for refusal/failure to participate).
representative of all males with AS, whose parents There was no significant difference in mean FSIQ at
applied for clinical assessment and help for them in the original diagnosis between the participating (n = 76),
late 1980s and throughout the 1990s. and the non-participating (n = 24) AS groups.

Autism Group Autism Group

A group of 16 to 38-year-old males from a group with In the original autism group of 84 males, three (4%)
autistic disorder (AD) diagnosed with autism (n = 62) had died before follow-up. The causes of death were:
or atypical autism (n = 22, including 2 with disinte- death in an accidental fire (boy with Fragile X syn-
grative disorder) using the DSM-III (APA, 1980), and drome and autism at 11 years of age); death in com-
DSM-III-R (APA, 1987) criteria, before 10 years of plications after heart surgery (boy with trisomy 13 and
age and followed-up with a similar protocol as the AS a severe heart malformation and autism, age at death
group at the CNC 1999–2002 (Billstedt, Gillberg, & unknown); unknown date and cause of death (boy with
Gillberg, 2005; Billstedt, 2006, personal communica- atypical autism, age at death unknown). An additional
tion), were used as a contrast group. These 84 indi- 4 declined participation, leaving 77 for possible inclu-
viduals were all the males from a community-based sion in the Autism study group. Of those who declined
study of autism, who were approached for inclusion in participation two had MMR and two SMR.
a longitudinal prospective follow-up study designed
and carried out by the last three authors. We consider Participants
this autism group to be representative of all individuals
with autism diagnosed in the 1960s, 1970s and 1980s in AS Study Group
the community. The group was strikingly different
from the AS group in terms of IQ, but they still rep- Of the 76 cases participating in the follow-up study,
resented a reasonable contrast group vis-à-vis our AS seventy had had a complete DISCO interview per-
group as regards their social impairment. The Autism formed, and these were included for further study,

123
J Autism Dev Disord (2008) 38:72–85 75

since we intended to match clinical and DISCO-diag- diagnosis except for a small number of cases. About
noses. These 70 cases constituted the ‘‘AS study 50% of all AS cases had also received medical assess-
group’’. ments including one or more of the following; karyo-
Among these 70 cases there were 4 parents who typing, neuroimaging, EEG, auditory brainstem
participated without their son (in one of these cases the response examination, and a variety of urine, blood
son was unaware of his condition), and test information and cerebrospinal fluid examinations. All the follow-up
about intellectual ability at follow-up is missing for assessments were administered, co-ordinated and car-
these four individuals. ried out by the first four authors (authors one and two
in the AS-part, and authors three and four in the aut-
Autism Study Group ism-part). The first author had not been involved at all,
and the second author had only been involved in a few
Of the 77 cases participating in the follow-up study, 75 cases in the original AS diagnostic process. Authors
had had a DISCO-interview completed, and of these, three and four had not been involved in the original
seventy males fell in approximately the same age-range Autism diagnostic process. However, none of the
as those 70 males who were chosen for the ‘‘AS study investigators were completely ‘‘blind’’ to the original
group’’. These 70 autism cases constituted the ‘‘Autism diagnoses, in their respective parts of the study. The
study group’’. fifth author, who is the head of the Child and Ado-
lescent Psychiatric Department at Göteborg Univer-
Mean Age sity, initiated and supervised both studies, but did not
see the individuals at follow-up. All the follow-up
AS Study Group assessments in the AS-part took place at the CNC,
whereas those in the autism part were carried out at
Mean age in the AS study group at follow-up was the CNC or in the homes/workplaces of the partici-
21.5 years (SD 4.4, range 16.0–33.9) years. Forty-eight pants. All four assessors have trained together in the
(69%) individuals in the AS study group, were 22 years use of the DISCO and have been working together in
or younger at follow-up. an autism spectrum disorder/child neuropsychiatric
team at the CNC for several years.
Autism Study Group
Instruments Used in Study Groups at Follow-Up
Mean age in the Autism study group was 24.5 years at age 16–36 Years1
(SD 5.4, 16.1–36.1 years) in the Autism study group. In
the Autism study group 30 males (43%), were 22 years Both groups were followed-up with in-depth examin-
or younger at follow-up. ations performed in 1999–2005. The following instru-
ments were used:
Original Diagnostic Assessments Diagnostic Interview for Social and Communicative
Disorders (DISCO-10: A semi-structured interview
AS and Autism groups intended for interview with a person (often a parent),
who knew the individual well. The DISCO-10 has
Both study groups had been assessed by experts in the excellent inter-rater and test-retest reliability, and is
field of autism/AS, working at the CNC, with ‘‘autism highly valid for assigning diagnoses in the autism
spectrum instruments’’ that were state-of-the-art at the spectrum (Wing et al., 2002). It also includes sections
time of the diagnostic evaluations, e.g., in-depth clini- on common associated problems in autism such as
cal interview, the Handicaps, Behaviours, and Skills psychiatric disorder, including ‘‘psychosis’’, has a
Schedule (Wing 1980), the Childhood Autism Rating developmental perspective, and is designed for use
Scale (Schopler, Reichler, DeVellis, & Daly, 1980), the throughout the lifespan (Wing et al., 2002). The DIS-
Autism Behaviour Checklist (Krug, Arick, & Almond, CO was chosen in favour of the ADI, (LeCouteur
1980) and the Asperger Syndrome Diagnostic Inter- et al., 1989) because the latter was designed for use in
view (ASDI) (Gillberg, Gillberg, Rastam, & Wentz, the diagnosis of classic autism, whereas the DISCO
2001). The Autism Diagnostic Interview (ADI) includes a range of items intended to detect milder
(LeCouteur et al., 1989), and the Diagnostic Interview forms of autism spectrum disorders. In addition, the
for Social and Communication Disorders (DISCO)
(Wing, Leekam, Libby, Gould, & Larcombe, 2002), 1
A number of other instruments were used, but results from
were not available in Swedish at the time of original these will be presented in separate papers.

123
76 J Autism Dev Disord (2008) 38:72–85

DISCO has a developmental perspective and is information, except the information generated at
designed for use from early childhood into adult life DISCO-10 interview—using the Gillberg (1991) cri-
(Wing et al., 2002). teria for AS (except the motor clumsiness criterion,
Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Third edition which was not universally fulfilled) or DSM-IV/ICD-10
(WAIS-III) (Wechsler, 2003): This well-established criteria for autistic disorder/atypical autism. In the
IQ-test, including Full Scale IQ (FSIQ), and subtests Autism study group a clinical autism spectrum diag-
for Verbal IQ (VIQ) and Performance IQ (PIQ), was nosis was made using the same criteria as mentioned
used with all participants in the AS study group. In the above for the AS part of the study.
Autism study group only a minority could be tested on
the Wechsler scales ((Wechsler Adult Intelligence DISCO-10 diagnosis
Scale-Revised (WAIS-R) (Wechsler, 1981) or
(Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-Third Edi- Research diagnoses of autism spectrum disorders were
tion (WISC-III) (Wechsler, 1999)), and the majority also made according to the algorithm of the DISCO-10
were categorized in terms of IQ/DQ/SQ-band using (DISCO-algorithm Gillberg criteria for AS and the
the Vineland Adaptive Behaviour Scales (see below). DISCO-algorithm DSM-IV/ICD-10 criteria for autism/
Global Assessment of Functioning scale (GAF) atypical autism). Diagnoses were generated by com-
(APA, 1994): GAF yields scores from 0 to 100, where a puter on the basis of the results obtained at the
score of ‡70 indicates good or only mildly abnormal DISCO-interview.
psychosocial functioning. GAF was scored conjointly
by the first/second, and the third/fourth authors in all
cases in the respective studies. Most studies performed Criteria for Outcome in Both Study Groups
on the GAF have found it to be a reliable and useful
instrument in measuring a persons psychosocial func- The criteria used for the classification of outcomes,
tioning, requiring only minor pre-scoring information were similar to those employed in an earlier study of
(e.g., Billstedt et al., 2005; Hilsenroth et al., 2000; autism in our centre (Gillberg & Steffenburg, 1987),
Startup, Jackson, & Bendix, 2002). which was based on the outcome criteria published by
Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales (VABS) (Spar- Lotter (Lotter, 1978). Reliability studies—to our
row, Balla, & Cicchetti, 1984): a semi-structured knowledge—have not been performed on the use of
interview with a parent/caregiver that offers a com- these criteria. The classifications were based on all
prehensive assessment of adaptive behaviour. Cases available information (including the DISCO) at the
were categorized in DQ/SQ bands (Developmental time of examination.
Quotient/Social Quotient) on the basis of the results on
the VABS. The VABS has been reported to be a valid The Outcome Criteria Were
instrument in establishing the cognitive level for an
individual functioning at an IQ-level below 70–75 Good outcome: (a) being employed or in ‘‘higher’’ (age
(APA, 1994; Luckasson et al., 1992). The VABS has and IQ-appropriate (‘‘normal’’)) education or voca-
also been widely used to map the overall functioning of tional training, and, (b) if 23 years of age or older,
an individual in socialization, communication, and living independently, or if 22 years or younger, having
daily living skills, regardless of IQ in order to be used two or more friends/a steady relationship;
as a prognostic and intervention tool for habilitation Fair outcome: either (a) or (b), but not both, under
(Balboni, Pedrabissi, Molteni, & Villa, 2001; Gilotti, good outcome;
Kenworthy, Sirian, Black, & Wagner, 2002; Rhea Restricted outcome: neither (a) nor (b) under good
et al., 2004). outcome, and not meeting criteria for a major psychi-
A structured neuropsychiatric assessment, performed atric disorder other than autistic disorder or another
by the first and fourth authors respectively. autism ASD. This category refers to a group of people
with the characteristics of poor outcome, but who have
Autism Spectrum Diagnoses at Follow-Up been accepted by a group of peers or personnel to such
an extent that their handicaps are not so readily obvi-
Clinical Diagnosis ous;
Poor outcome: Obvious severe handicap, with either
A clinical diagnosis of autistic disorder, atypical of, no independent social progress or presence of a
autism, or AS, at follow-up of the AS study group major psychiatric disorder, but with some clear verbal
was made systematically—including all available or non-verbal communicative skills;

123
J Autism Dev Disord (2008) 38:72–85 77

Very poor outcome: Obvious very severe handicap, Table 1 Clinical and DISCO-10 autism spectrum diagnoses
unable to lead any kind of independent existence, no at follow-up
clear verbal or non-verbal communication. Clinical/DISCO diagnoses AS (%) Autism (%)

Statistical Methods Used Clinical diagnosis 52 (75%) 0 (5)** (0%) (7%)


of AS
Clinical diagnosis of 7* (10%) 58 (83%)
Chi-square tests (with Yates’s correction whenever autistic disorder
appropriate) were employed in comparison of group Clinical diagnosis of 3 (4%) 11 (16%)
frequencies. atypical autism
No clinical diagnosis of 8 (11%) 1 (1%)
any autism spectrum
disorder
Results DISCO algorithm 59 11
diagnosis of AS
DISCO algorithm 55 56
Diagnosis at Follow-Up and Diagnostic Stability diagnosis of autistic
Over Time disorder
DISCO algorithm 10 14
AS Study Group diagnosis of atypical
autism
No DISCO algorithm 1 0
Fifty-nine individuals in the AS study group (84%) still diagnosis of an autism
met clinical (Gillberg, 1991) diagnostic criteria for AS, spectrum disorder
three (4%) met criteria for atypical autism, and * 7 cases meeting both Gillberg and Gillberg criteria for AS and
8 (12%) no longer met criteria for a clinical diagnosis DSM-IV criteria for autistic disorder, but clinically better fitting
in the autism spectrum. Seven (10%) of the males autistic disorder
meeting clinical criteria for AS also met clinical criteria ** 5 cases meeting Gillberg & Gillberg criteria for AS, but
clinically better fitting autistic disorder
for autistic disorder (DSM-IV) and were clinically
In the DISCO part of this table more than one diagnosis was
judged to better fit the latter diagnosis. possible, since for AS the Gillberg & Gillberg DISCO algorithm
The DISCO-classification (Gillberg, DISCO-algo- criteria was used and for Autistic Disorder and Atypical autism
rithm criteria) concurred with that of the clinical the DSM-IV/ICD-10 DISCO algorithm criteria was used
assessment in 55 of the 59 (93%) cases with a clinical
diagnosis of AS at follow-up. the same diagnosis according to DISCO-10 algorithm,
Of the eight males who did not meet full clinical and the other two had an autism diagnosis according
criteria for an ASD diagnosis at follow-up, six had a to the same algorithm. Five males in the Autism
diagnosis of atypical autism (DSM-IV/ICD-10), one study group fulfilled criteria for an AS diagnosis
had a diagnosis of AS (Gillberg, 1991), and one did (Gillberg, 1991), but were clinically judged better to
not have any autism spectrum diagnosis (DSM-IV/ fit a diagnosis of autism. The only male in the Autism
ICD-10) according to the DISCO-10 algorithm study group without a clinical diagnosis at follow-up
(Table 1). had an atypical autism diagnosis on the DISCO-10
(Table 1).
Autism Study Group

In the Autism study group, 43 out of 53 (81%) origi- Intellectual Functioning at Follow-Up
nally diagnosed AD still met clinical criteria for AD, 9
(17%) had a clinical diagnosis of atypical autism, and AS Study Group
one individual did not have a clinical autism spectrum
diagnosis at follow-up. All individuals seen face-to-face at follow-up (n = 66)
Only 2 out of 17 individuals with atypical autism at were able to take a complete WAIS-III test. Mean
original diagnosis fulfilled criteria for the same diag- age at testing was 21.6 years (SD 4.5). Average FSIQ
nosis at follow-up. All remaining 15 individuals ful- was 103.0 (SD 14.8, range 66–143). Two individuals
filled criteria for AD at follow-up. Fifty-four out of scored above IQ 130, and one below IQ 70. Mean
58 (93%) with a clinical AD diagnosis at follow-up VIQ was 104.0 (SD 15.7) and PIQ 101.3 (SD 15.7)
also had a DISCO-10 diagnosis of AD and the other (Table 2). Compared to at original diagnosis the
four had atypical autism. Nine out of eleven indi- FSIQ was stable for the group as a whole, although
viduals with a clinical diagnosis of atypical autism had there were significant differences in FSIQ on an

123
78 J Autism Dev Disord (2008) 38:72–85

Table 2 IQ distribution in AS and autism groups at follow-up compared to at original diagnosis


IQ/DQ/SQ-band AS original diagnosis AS follow-up Autism original diagnosis Autism follow-up
(n = 70) (%) (n = 66) (%) (n = 70) (%) (n = 70) (%)

£49 0 (0%) 0 (0%) 33 (47%) 50 (72%)


50–69 0 (0%) 1 (2%) 24 (35%) 15 (21%)
70–84 15 (21%) 4 (6%) 10 (14%) 2 (3%)
85–114 41 (59%) 45 (68%) 3 (4%) 3 (4%)
115–129 10 (14%) 14 (21%) 0 (0%) 0 (0%)
‡130 4 (6%) 2 (3%) 0 (0%) 0 (0%)
p < 0.001 for IQ-level comparing AS group at original diagnosis (and at follow-up) versus autism group at original diagnosis and at
follow-up. The change in the AS group is without trend. In the autism group there is a downward shift in intellectual capacity that was
significant (p < 0.001)

individual basis between evaluation at original diag- Overall Outcome


nosis and at follow-up. There was a significant
VIQ > PIQ difference (‡15 points) in 13 (19%) at AS study Group
follow-up, compared to 31 (45%) at original diag-
nosis (p < 0.01). The gap between VIQ and PIQ for Of the 70 males in the AS study group, 19 (27%) had
the whole group had decreased from 11 IQ-points good outcome. Seven of these 19 no longer met clinical
at original diagnosis to less than 3 IQ-points at fol- criteria for AS diagnosis. Thirty-three (47%) had fair
low-up. The PIQ had gone up and the VIQ had outcome and, 16 (23%) had restricted outcome. Two
dropped somewhat, but changes were not statistically individuals (3%) in the AS group had poor outcome,
significant. The subtests included in PIQ at original but no one had very poor outcome (Table 3). Lower
diagnosis were not all identical to those used at fol- FSIQ contributed to poorer outcomes. Analysis of
low-up. Matrix reasoning (MR) was included in this covariance found age-difference to be non-significant
score at follow-up instead of Object Assembly (OA). when overall outcome was related both to FSIQ and
The males scored much better on MR at follow-up VIQ. However, there was a significant difference be-
than at OA at original diagnosis. However, the result tween Good and Poor outcome (FSIQ & VIQ)
on OA at follow-up was also better than OA at (p < 0.05), and between Good-Fair and Restricted-
original diagnosis, and the results on OA and MR at Poor ooutcome (FSIQ & VIQ) (p < 0.05) (Table 4).
follow-up were similar. The individuals who no When participants in the AS study group were di-
longer had a diagnosis in the autism spectrum did not vided into groups according to age at original diagno-
differ in intellectual capacity from the group who did sis, good outcome was seen in 9/26 (35%) in the
(FSIQ 101.9 (SD 12.3) compared to 103.1 (SD 15.2)). youngest group (5.5–9.5 years at diagnosis), in 9/35
(26%) of the ‘‘in-between-group’’ (10.0–15.5 years at
Autism Study Group diagnosis), and in 2/9 (22%) of the oldest group (16.0–
24.5 years at diagnosis) (n.s.).
In the Autism study group, Wechsler scale testing
(WAIS-R or WISC-III) was only possible in 16 Table 3 Overall outcome categories
individuals. The mean FSIQ in this small tested
group was 59.6 (SD 17.9, n = 16), VIQ 63.2 (SD 19.8, Outcome categories/Independent AS Autism
living
n = 15) and PIQ 58.9 (SD 12.3, n = 15). The results
from the overall intellectual assessment of the Aut- Good outcome 19 (27%) 0 (0%)
ism study group (including those tested on the Fair outcome 33 (47%) 5 (7%)
Restricted outcome 16 (23%) 12 (17%)
Wechsler scales) are presented in Table 2. Those Poor outcome 2 (3%) 14 (20%)
individuals who could not be tested on the Wechsler Very poor outcome 0 (0%) 39 (56%)
scales were categorized according to results on the Independent living (23 years of 14/22 (64%) 3/40 (8%)
VABS. The results in the Autism study group were age or older)
significantly lower than at original evaluation, and p < 0.0001 for outcome AS group versus Autism group (Wilcoxon
contrasted to those of the AS group, in which mean rank sum)
FSIQ had not changed over time (Table 2). p < 0.001 for independence AS group versus Autism group

123
J Autism Dev Disord (2008) 38:72–85 79

Table 4 Outcome related to FSIQ and age in AS study group


Outcome AS FSIQ (n = 66) VIQ (n = 66) Age at follow-up
(SD) years

Good 19 (27%) 107.8 (SD 14.3) 109.3 (SD 17.5) 21.5 (SD 3.7)
Fair 33 (47%) 105.0 (SD 14.8) * 104.3 (SD 14.2) * 20.7 (SD 4.5)
Restricted 16 (23%) 97.4 (SD 13.0) 98.6 (SD 15.1) 23.5 (SD 4.8)
Poor 2 (3%) 82.0 (SD 4.2) 92.0 (SD 12.7) 18.5 (SD 2.4)
Very poor 0 (0%)
* n = 29 (four males in this group did not participate in the follow-up)

Table 5 Outcome related to intelligence level and age in autism study group
Outcome Autism Intellectual level Mean age at follow-up
(SD) years
A NA MMR SMR

Good n = 0
Fair n = 5 (7%) 1 2 2 0 26.1 (5.9)
Restricted n = 12 (17%) 2 0 7 3 25.9 (7.1)
Poor n = 14 (20%) 0 0 5 9 25.5 (5.1)
Very poor n = 39 (56%) 0 0 1 38* 23.5 (4.9)
* p < 0.001, intellectual level versus outcome (Chi square)

Autism Study Group learning disabilities. Three males with AS were in


special schools away from home, where their training
Of the 70 males in the Autism group no one had good also included training in social and daily living skills.
outcome. Five (7%) had fair outcome and, 12 (17%) Two males had finished school after nine years of
had restricted outcome. Fourteen individuals (20%) compulsory school.
had poor outcome, and 39 individuals (56%) had very
poor outcome (Table 3). Lower intellectual level con-
Autism Study Group
tributed to poorer outcome (Table 5).
None of the males in the Autism group did, or had
Education
done, university studies. Six males in the Autism study
group were in, or had currently finished, high school (4
AS Study Group
of whom had IQ >70). One of the males in the autism
group was studying at a Folk High School. Twenty-
Eight of the AS males did university studies, and a
three males with autism were (or had been) in a school
further two had a university degree (computer sci-
for adolescents with mild learning disabilities. An
ence, civil engineering). Thirty-three males in the AS
additional 40 males had or had had their training in
study group were in, or had currently finished, high
special training schools.
school. Only 21 of these 33 (64%) were, or had
been, following ordinary study programs. Three
males in the AS group were in special AS class- Occupation
rooms, four had special individually developed
study-programs, and five had some kind of special AS Study Group
education regularly. Two AS males were studying at
a Folk High School.2 Eight males with AS were (or Seven men in the AS group held ordinary jobs, and a
had been) in a school for adolescents with mild further six individuals had ‘‘daily occupational activi-
ties’’ in a group centre. Twelve males with AS (17%)
2
Folk High School, is a Swedish form of school, with different had no organized daily activity at all and were
educational levels ranging from Swedish High School equivalent
to post High School education in specific fields mainly to acquire
dependent on social services and/or the Swedish
specific ‘‘non-academic’’ skills. insurance system for their welfare.

123
80 J Autism Dev Disord (2008) 38:72–85

Autism Study Group normal or near normal functioning (Table 6). How-
ever, six of these 12 men no longer met criteria for an
In the Autism group one man held an ordinary job, and autism spectrum diagnosis. Of the males, who were still
four individuals had ‘‘daily occupational activities’’ in a regarded clinically to have sufficient impairment from
group centre. Thirty-three males in the Autism study their symptoms to warrant a clinical diagnosis of AS,
group had regular individually tailored daily activities. five had a GAF-score of 70, and one had a GAF-score
Thirteen males with (19%) had no organized daily of 72.
activity at all and were dependent on social services There was no difference at follow-up in GAF-scores
and/or the Swedish insurance system for their welfare. between the AS-age-at-diagnosis-groups, 5–9 years
59.5 (SD 7.1, n = 26), 10–15 years 58.4 (SD 9.7, n = 35)
Independent living and 16+ years 59.0 (SD 14.3, n = 9). The individuals
with AS, who had been followed-up at the CNC on a
AS Study Group fairly regular basis after diagnosis (n = 29) had a mean
GAF score of 57.6 (SD 6.8), similar to those who had
In the AS study group 14/22 (64%) of those who were not been followed there (n = 41) 59.8 (SD 10.8).
‡23 years, were living independently. In addition,
5 males with AS £ 22 years of age were living inde- Autism Study Group
pendently. Although living away from their parents,
they were all dependent upon them for support. Three The mean GAF-score was 22.2 (SD 16.5 range 4–67,
males with AS were living in a long-term relationship, p < 0.001) in the Autism study group. No individual in
and a further 10 had had relationships for varying the Autism study group had a GAF score of 70 or
periods of time in the past. above (Table 6).

Autism Study Group Involvement with the Police and the Law

Of the males who were ‡23 years in the Autism group AS Study Group
(n = 40), only 3 males (8%), one each with IQ-level A,
NA and MMR), were living independently. Although The vast majority in the AS study group were consid-
living independently, they were all in need of their ered very law–abiding. However, according to parent
parents for support. One male with Autism was living report, seven males (10%) with AS had been involved
in a long-term relationship, and yet another one had with police and the law for different reasons, (fraud
recently had a relationship for a longer duration. (1), harassment of police officer (1), harassment of
young woman (1), stealing (1), assault (1), sexual abuse
GAF-Scores (1), and unknown in 1 case).

AS Study Group Autism Study Group

The mean GAF-score for the AS study group was 58.9 In the Autism study group there were no reports by
(SD 9.4 range 35–82). Twelve males (17%) in the AS parents or other informants concerning involvement
group, had a GAF score of 70 or above, indicating with police or the law.

Table 6 GAF-scores in relation to intellectual ability


GAF-scores AS FSIQ (SD) Autism Intellectual ability

Mean GAF-score (SD) 58.9 (9.4) 22.2 (16.5)


GAF-score 70- 12 109.7 (15.2) 0 –
GAF-score 50–69 51 102.7 (14.2) * 8 A (3) NA (2) MMR (3)
GAF-score 31–49 7 91.9 (13.7) 13 MMR (5) SMR (8)
GAF-score -30 0 – 49 MMR (4) SMR (45)
* n = 47
p < 0.001 for mean GAF-scores AS versus Autism group
p < 0.001 for intellectual ability related to GAF score

123
J Autism Dev Disord (2008) 38:72–85 81

Psychotic Disorder criteria for AD at follow-up. All the remaining males


but one had a clinical diagnosis of atypical autism. Of
AS Study Group the 17 individuals with atypical autism at original
diagnosis, 15 fulfilled criteria for AD at follow-up.
Three individuals in the AS study group had been The DISCO-10 identified the vast majority of indi-
diagnosed as suffering from psychosis by independent viduals in both study groups in the same categories as
psychiatrists. One of these males had received a the clinicians.
diagnosis of bipolar disorder, and there was suspicion ‘‘Restricted’’ and ‘‘poor’’ outcome affected more
of such disorder in at least one further case. No indi- AS individuals (26%) than expected, considering the
vidual had been diagnosed with schizophrenia in the average intellectual level for this group as a whole, and
AS group. All three individuals with a psychosis diag- the outcome criteria used. However, the Autism study
nosis in the AS group were on current anti-psychotic group had a much worse outcome, with the vast
medication. In terms of intellectual ability two of them majority of individuals (76%) belonging in the poor or
(the third one was not assessed) had had significant very poor outcome groups. The intellectual level was
drop (‡20 IQ-points) in their FSIQ as measured on the much lower in the Autism study group, where only five
WAIS-III. However, all 3 individuals with psychosis (7%) individuals had a normal intellectual ability at
were reported to have shown a significant drop in follow-up.
intellectual ability between original diagnosis and fol- There was a tendency for FSIQ, and particularly,
low-up. as hypothesized, for VIQ to be correlated with better
outcome within the AS group, even though those
Autism Study Group with better outcomes had not been followed up for
as long as those with restricted outcome making
In the Autism study group four individuals had been definite conclusions about ultimate prognosis some-
diagnosed as suffering from psychosis, and in none of what less certain. In contrast, in the Autism study
those cases had a diagnosis of bipolar disorder been group the individuals in the ‘‘very poor’’ outcome
made. No individual had been diagnosed with schizo- group were the youngest (i.e. mean age was lowest in
phrenia in the Autism group. Two out of four indi- this group).
viduals with a psychosis diagnosis in the Autism group There was no decline in FSIQ in the AS group over
were on current anti-psychotic medication. time. However, the difference between verbal and non-
verbal ability of 15 IQ-points or more that applied in
45% at original diagnosis was now present only in 19%
Discussion of the individuals. Performance results tended to im-
prove over time and the mean VIQ > PIQ difference
This, to our knowledge, is the largest prospective of 11 IQ-points or more that was present at original
comparative follow-up study ever published of a cohort diagnosis was no longer at hand at follow-up. This
of individuals with AS, and autism, followed over a might reflect an improvement in visual-spatial ability
period of more than 5 years. We believe that the over time, but the subtests included in the Perceptual
individuals included are representative of AS, and Organization index had changed over time, meaning
autism/atypical autism, as diagnosed 10 years ago or that definite conclusions in this respect will have to
more. The results are of particular interest, given that await further studies. The stable overall IQ in the AS
most previous studies have related to smaller, and/or group contrasted with the decline of intellectual ability
possibly highly selected samples. in the Autism study group, where there had been a
Were our hypotheses supported by the findings? considerable drop in intellectual ability over the years.
The diagnosis of AS was still clinically valid in the A small, but not insignificant, group of individuals in
vast majority of AS cases (84%), but 11 individuals both the AS and Autism study groups had been diag-
did not meet clinical criteria for this particular diag- nosed by independent psychiatrists as having ‘‘psy-
nosis at follow-up. Three of these 11 had a clinical chosis’’. ‘‘Schizophrenia’’ had not been diagnosed in a
diagnosis of atypical autism, but eight did not have single individual, and even though cases of this condi-
sufficient clinical impairment to warrant a clinical tion might well appear with time, it seems clear that
diagnosis of an autism spectrum disorder. However, neither AS, nor autism is associated with a much in-
this was not equivalent to lack of impairment from creased risk for schizophrenia in early adult life. At
autistic type problems. In the Autism study group least two of the three males in the AS group, who had
81% originally diagnosed with AD still met clinical been diagnosed with psychosis had had severe decline

123
82 J Autism Dev Disord (2008) 38:72–85

in intellectual functioning between original diagnosis to recruit an Autism study group matched for IQ,
and follow-up (the third male was not tested at follow- particularly given speculation (e.g., Gillberg, 1998) that
up). They all fulfilled criteria for AS at follow-up. the main difference across cases clinically diagnosed as
There was a (non-significant) young age at diagnosis AS and autism is the much higher IQ in the former
by better outcome trend in the AS group. Given that group, and that there are no clinically diagnosed cases
young age at diagnosis probably also indicates a more of autism who have IQ in the superior range. We be-
difficult child, this trend would seem to indicate that an lieve that, as regards the social deficits, the two groups
early diagnosis in itself possibly would contribute to a were roughly comparable in childhood and/or adoles-
better outcome. However no definite conclusions can cence, and that they therefore constituted reasonable
be drawn in this respect on the basis of the current contrast groups for the purpose of follow-up of psy-
results. chosocial adjustment and general outcome in adult life.
Seven individuals with AS (10%) were reported to
have been involved with the police and the law for
various acts of crime. In this age group in Sweden, a Conclusions
rate of 10% criminality is not surprisingly high (Na-
tional Council for Crime Prevention, Sweden, 2005). Males with AS diagnosed in childhood–young adult
Nevertheless, the nature of the acts of crime performed age have outcomes that are very much better than
by the young men with AS was rather ‘‘severe’’ in those found in males diagnosed in childhood as suf-
several cases, and visualized difficulties with perspec- fering from autism/atypical autism. Nevertheless, given
tive-taking, and difficulties in appreciating the conse- their good intellectual capacity, the outcomes must be
quences of their actions, which was also reported in an regarded as sub-optimal. Medical, social and occupa-
earlier study (Murphy, 2003). tional services must find ways to achieve more indi-
In summary, we found a diagnosis in the autism vidually adjusted solutions so as to be more successful
spectrum to be stable over time in the vast majority of in meeting the needs of individuals with autism spec-
cases. Outcome in the AS study group was worse than trum disorders.
expected, taken the level of intelligence and outcome
criteria used into account. However, it was dramati- Acknowledgments This study was supported by the Linnéa &
Josef Carlsson Foundation, the Wilhelm and Martina Lundgren
cally better than in the Autism study group. The better
Foundation, the Söderström-Königska Foundation, the Swedish
outcome could probably be attributed to the much Autism Foundation, the Göteborg Medical Society, the Petter
higher FSIQ in the former group. We also found FSIQ Silverskiöld Memorial Foundation, grants from the State under
to be stable over time in the AS study group, which was the ALF (LUA) agreement, and by a grant from the Swedish
Scientific Council (MRC grant: 2003–4581) for professor
in contrast to the Autism study group where there was
Gillberg. Both studies were approved by the Medical Ethical
an intellectual decline over time. In a small AS sub- Committee of Göteborg University.
group there was psychotic disorder which, in turn, was
associated with decline in intellectual ability, and
hence in outcome. Criminal acts were not reported at Appendix A: Diagnostic Criteria for Asperger
very high rates, but the acts of crime were sometimes Syndrome
odd and reflecting the lack of common sense that is one
of the key issues in AS. Gillberg and Gillberg (1989) diagnostic criteria elabo-
rated (Gillberg 1991)

Limitations
1. Social impairment (extreme egocentricity) (at least
two of the following)
Of the 100 males approached for inclusion in the AS
follow-up study, only 76 participated, and 70 of these (a) inability to interact with peers
were included in the present study. We know that the (b) lack of desire to interact with peers
non-participants did not differ from the participants in (c) lack of appreciation of social cues
terms of overall intelligence and there are no obvious (d) socially and emotionally inappropriate behaviour.
indications that the examined group differs in any 2. Narrow interest (at least one of the following)
major way from the larger group originally targeted.
The autism contrast group may not be regarded as (a) exclusion of other activities
ideal because of its much lower IQ. Nevertheless, it (b) repetitive adherence
would be unrealistic (and, clinically, probably impossible) (c) more rote than meaning.

123
J Autism Dev Disord (2008) 38:72–85 83

3. Repetitive routines (at least one of the following) (b) apparently inflexible adherence to specific, non-
functional routines or rituals.
(a) on self, in aspects
(c) stereotyped and repetitive motor-mannerisms
(b) on others.
(hand- or finger-flapping or twisting or complex
4. Speech and language peculiarities (at least three of whole-body movements)
the following) (d) persistent preoccupation with parts of objects
(a) delayed development The disturbance causes clinically significant impair-
(b) superficially perfect expressive language ment in social, occupational, or other important areas
(c) formal pedantic language of functioning:
(d) odd prosody, peculiar voice characteristics The is no clinically significant general delay in lan-
(e) impairment of comprehension, including misin- guage (e.g., single words used by age 2 years, com-
terpretations of literal/implied meanings. municative phrases used by age 3 years).
There is no clinically significant delay in cognitive
5. Non-verbal communication problems (at least one
development or in the development of age-appropriate
of the following):
self-help skills, adaptive behaviour (other than in social
(a) limited use of gestures interaction), and curiosity about the environment in
(b) clumsy/gauche body language childhood.
(c) limited facial expression Criteria are not met for another Pervasive Devel-
(d) inappropriate expression opmental Disorder or Schizophrenia.
(e) peculiar, stiff gaze.
6. Motor clumsiness
References
(a) poor performance on neuro-developmental
examination. American Psychiatric Association (1980). Diagnostic and statis-
tical manual of mental disorders, 3rd edn: DSM-III.
Washington, DC: Author.
American Psychiatric Association (1987). Diagnostic and statis-
Appendix B: DSM-IV (APA, 1994) Criteria tical manual of mental disorders, 3rd revised edition: DSM-
III-R. Washington, DC: Author.
for Autistic Disorder American Psychiatric Association (1994). Diagnostic and statis-
tical manual of mental disorders, 4th edn: DSM-IV. Wash-
Qualitative impairment in social interaction, as mani- ington, DC: Author.
fested by at least two of the following: Asperger, H. (1944). Die autistischen Psychopathen im Kindes-
alter. Archiv für Psychiatrie und Nervenkrankheiten, 117,
76–136 [In German].
Balboni, G., Pedrabissi, L., Molteni., M., & Villa, S. (2001).
(a) marked impairment in the use of multiple non-
Discriminant validity of the Vineland scales: Score profiles
verbal behaviours such as eye-to-eye gaze, facial of individuals with mental retardation and a specific
expression, body postures, and gesture to regulate disorder. American Journal of Mental Retardation, 106,
social interaction 162–172.
Billstedt, E., Gillberg, I. C., & Gillberg, C. (2005). Autism after
(b) failure to develop peer relationships appropriate
adolescence. Population based 13–22-year follow-up study
for developmental level of 120 individuals with autism diagnosed in childhood.
(c) a lack of spontaneous seeking to share enjoyment, Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 35, 351–
interests, or achievements with other people (e.g., 360.
Cederlund, M., & Gillberg, C. (2004). One hundred males with
by lack of showing, bringing, or pointing out ob-
Asperger syndrome. A clinical study of background and
jects of interests to other people) associated factors. Developmental Medicine and Child Neu-
(d) lack of social or emotional reciprocity rology, 46, 652–660.
Eisenmajer, R., Prior, M., Leekam, S., Wing, L., Ong, B., Gould,
Restricted repetitive and stereotyped patterns of J., & Welham M. (1998). Delayed language onset as a
behaviour, interests, and activities, as manifested by at predictor of clinical symptoms in pervasive developmental
least one of the following: disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders
28, 527–533.
Gilchrist, A., Green, J., Cox, A., Burton, D., Rutter, M., & Le
Couteur A. (2001). Development and current functioning
(a) encompassing preoccupation with one or more
in adolescents with Asperger syndrome: A comparative
stereotyped and restricted patterns of interest study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 42,
that is abnormal either in intensity or focus 227–240.

123
84 J Autism Dev Disord (2008) 38:72–85

Gillberg, C. (1991). Clinical and neurobiological aspects of Luckasson, R., Coulter, D. L., Polloway, E. A., Reiss, S.,
Asperger syndrome in six family studies. In U. Frith (ed.), Schalock, R. L., Snell, M. E., Spitalnik, D. M., & Stark, J. A.
Autism and Asperger syndrome. Cambridge: Cambridge (1992). Mental retardation. Definition, classification, and
University Press. systems of supports. Washington, DC: American Association
Gillberg, C., & Steffenburg, S. (1987). Outcome and prognostic on Mental Retardation.
factors in infantile autism and similar conditions: A popu- Mesibov, G. B., Kunce, L., & Schopler, E. (1998). Asperger
lation-based study of 46 cases followed through puberty. syndrome or high functioning autism? In series: Current
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 17, 273– issues in autism. New York: Plenum Press.
288. Miller, J. N., & Ozonoff, S. (1997). Did Asperger’s cases have
Gillberg, C., & Gillberg, I. C. (1989). Asperger syndrome—some Asperger disorder? A research note. Journal of Child
epidemiological considerations: a research note. Journal of Psychology and Psychiatry, 38, 247–251.
Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 30, 631–638. Murphy, D. (2003). Admission and cognitive details of male
Gillberg, C. (1998). Asperger syndrome and high-functioning patients diagnosed with Asperger’s Syndrome detained in a
autism. British Journal of Psychiatry, 172, 200–209. Special Hospital: comparison with a schizophrenia and
Gillberg, C., Gillberg, I. C., Rastam, M., & Wentz, E. (2001). The personality disorder sample. The Journal of Forensic Psy-
Asperger Syndrome (and High Functioning Autism) Diag- chiatry and Psychology, 14, 506–524.
nostic Interview (ASDI): A preliminary study of a new National Board for Crime Prevention (2005). Stockholm.
structured clinical interview. Autism, 5, 57–66. Nydén, A., Billstedt, E., Hjelmquist, E., & Gillberg, C. (2001).
Gillberg, C., & Cederlund, M. (2005). Asperger syndrome: Neurocognitive stability in Asperger syndrome, ADHD,
Familial and pre- and perinatal factors. Journal of Autism and reading and writing disorder: a pilot study. Develop-
and Developmental Disorders, 35, 159–166. mental Medicine and Child Neurology, 43, 165–171.
Gilotty, L., Kenworthy, L., Sirian, L., Black, D. O., & Wagner, Rhea, P., Miles, S., Cicchetti, D., Sparrow, S., Klin, A., Volkmar,
A. E. (2002). Adaptive skills and executive function in F., Coflin, M., & Booker, S. (2004). Adaptive behaviour in
autism spectrum disorders. Child Neuropsychology, 8, 241– autism and pervasive developmental disorder-not otherwise
248. specified: Microanalysis of scores on the vineland adaptive
Green, J., Gilchrist, A., Burton, D., & Cox, A. (2000). Social and behavior scales. Journal of Autism and Developmental
psychiatric functioning in adolescents with Asperger syn- Disorders, 34, 223–228 .
drome compared with conduct disorder. Journal of Autism Schopler, E., Reichler, R. J., DeVellis, R. F., & Daly, K. (1980).
and Developmental Disorders, 30, 279–293. Toward objective classification of childhood autism: Child-
Hilsenroth, M., Ackerman, S., Blagys, M., Baumann, B., Baity, M., hood Autism Rating Scale (CARS). Journal of Autism and
Smith, S., Price, J., Smith, C., Heindselman, T., Mount, M., & Developmental Disorders, 10, 91–103.
Holdwick, D. Jr. (2000). Reliability and validity of DSM-IV Scragg, P., & Shah, A. (1994). Prevalence of Aspergeŕs syndrome
Axis V. American Journal of Psychiatry, 157, 1858–1863. in a secure hospital. British Journal of Psychiatry, 165, 679–
Howlin, P., & Asgharian, A. (1999). The diagnosis of autism 682.
and Asperger syndrome: findings from a survey of 770 Siponmaa, L., Kristiansson, M., Jonsson, C., Nydén, A., &
families. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, Gillberg, C. (2001). Juvenile and young adult mentally
41, 834–839. disordered offenders: the role of child neuropsychiatric
Howlin, P., Mawhood, L. M., & Rutter M. (2000). Autism and disorders. Journal of the American Academy of Psychiatry
developmental receptive language disorder: A follow-up and the Law, 29, 420–426.
comparison in early adult life: Social, behavioural and Sparrow, S., Balla, D., Cicchetti, D. (1984). Vineland adaptive
psychiatric outcomes. Journal of Child Psychology and behavior scales. Expanded form manual. Circle Pines, MN:
Psychiatry, 41, 561–578. American Guidance Service.
Howlin P. (2003). Outcome in high-functioning adults with Starr, E., Szatmari, P., Bryson, S., & Zweigenbaum, L. (2003).
autism with and without early language delays: Implications Stability and change among High-Functioning Children with
for the differentiation between autism and Asperger syn- pervasive developmental disorders: A 2-year outcome study.
drome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 33, Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 33, 15–22.
3–13. Startup, M., Jackson, M., & Bendix, S. (2002). Brief report: The
Howlin, P., Goode, S., Hutton, J., & Rutter, M. (2004). Adult concurrent validity of the Global Assessment of Functioning
outcome for children with autism. Journal of Child Psy- (GAF). British Journal of Clinical Psycholog, 41, 417–422.
chology and Psychiatry, 45, 212–229. Szatmari, P., Bryson, S. E., Boyle, M. H., Streiner, D. L., &
Krug, D. A., Arick, J., & Almond, P. (1980). Behaviour checklist Duku, E. (2003). Predictors of outcome among high
for identifying severely handicapped individuals with high functioning children with autism and Asperger syndrome.
levels of autistic behaviours. Journal of Child Psychology Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 44, 520–528.
and Psychiatry, 21, 221–229. Tantam, D. (1991). Asperger’s syndrome in adulthood. In Frith
LeCouteur, A., Rutter, M., Lord, C., Rios, P., Robertson, S., U (Ed.), Autism and Asperger syndrome (pp. 147–183).
Holdgrafer, M., & Mc Lennan, J. (1989). Autism Diagnostic Cambridge: Cambridge University.
Interview: A standardized investigator-based instrument. Tsatsanis, K. D. (2003). Outcome research in Asperger syndrome
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 19, 363– and autism. Child Adolescent Psychiatric Clinics of North
387. America, 12, 47–63.
Leekam, S., Libby, S., Wing, L., Gould, J., & Gillberg, C. (2000). Wechsler, D. (1974). Wechsler intelligence scale for children-
Comparison of ICD-10 and Gillberg’s criteria for Asperger revised. New York: The psychological Corporation Limited.
syndrome. Autism, 4, 11–28. Wechsler, D. (1981). Wechsler adult intelligence scale-revised.
Lotter, V. (1978). Follow-up studies. In M. Rutter, & E. Schopler San Antonio. TX: The Psychological Corporation Limited.
(Eds.), Autism. A reappraisal of concepts and treatment. Wechsler, D. (1992a). Wechsler intelligence scale for children, 3rd
New York: Plenum Press. edn. New York: The Psychological Corporation Limited.

123
J Autism Dev Disord (2008) 38:72–85 85

Wechsler, D. (1992b). Wechsler adult intelligence scale-revised. Wing, L., Leekam, S. R., Libby, S. J., Gould, J., & Larcombe, M.
[Swedish version]. Stockholm: Psykologiförlaget. (2002). The diagnostic interview for social and communica-
Wechsler, D. (1999). Wechsler intelligence scale for children, 3rd tion disorders: Background, inter-rater reliability and clinical
edn. [Swedish version]. Stockholm: Psykologiförlaget. use. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry , 43, 307–325.
Wechsler, D. (2003). Wechsler adult intelligence scale-3rd edn. Wolff, S. (1995). Loners. The life path of unusual children.
[Swedish version]. Stockholm: Psykologiförlaget. Routledge, London. New York.
Wing, L. (1981). Asperger’s syndrome: A clinical account. World Health Organisation (1993). International Classification
Psychological Medicine, 11, 115–129. of Diseases (Classification of Mental and Behavioural
Wing, L. (1980). Handicap, behaviours and skills schedule. Disorders (ICD-10)) Geneva: Author.
London: MRC Social Psychiatry Unit. Wozniak, J., Biederman, J., Faraone, S. V., Frazier, J., Kim, J.,
Wing, L., & Potter, D. (2002). The epidemiology of autistic Millstein, R., Gershon, J., Thornell, A., Cha, K., & Snyder, JB.
spectrum disorders: is the prevalence rising? Mental retar- (1997). Mania in children with pervasive developmental
dation and Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews, 8, disorder revisited. Journal of American Child and Adolescent
151–161. Psychiatry, 36, 1552–1559.

123

Você também pode gostar