Você está na página 1de 11

The Disciplined Mind: Associations

Between the Kentucky Inventory of


Mindfulness Skills and Attention Control

Brian M. Galla, T. Sigi Hale, Anshu


Shrestha, Sandra K. Loo & Susan
L. Smalley

Mindfulness

ISSN 1868-8527

Mindfulness
DOI 10.1007/s12671-011-0083-0

1 23
Your article is protected by copyright and
all rights are held exclusively by Springer
Science+Business Media, LLC. This e-offprint
is for personal use only and shall not be self-
archived in electronic repositories. If you
wish to self-archive your work, please use the
accepted author’s version for posting to your
own website or your institution’s repository.
You may further deposit the accepted author’s
version on a funder’s repository at a funder’s
request, provided it is not made publicly
available until 12 months after publication.

1 23
Author's personal copy
Mindfulness
DOI 10.1007/s12671-011-0083-0

ORIGINAL PAPER

The Disciplined Mind: Associations Between the Kentucky


Inventory of Mindfulness Skills and Attention Control
Brian M. Galla & T. Sigi Hale & Anshu Shrestha &
Sandra K. Loo & Susan L. Smalley

# Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2011

Abstract In an attempt to replicate and clarify previous Introduction


research, we examined the associations between the
Kentucky Inventory of Mindfulness Skills (KIMS) and Mindfulness refers to a state of consciousness involving a
measures of sustained (Continuous Performance Test; receptive attention to and awareness of present moment
CPT) and executive (Stroop) attention in a community experiences (Analayo 2003; Brown and Ryan 2003). Newly
sample of adults (n=106). After controlling for age, developed self-report measures attempt to capture individ-
gender, education, socio-economic status, IQ, and depres- ual differences in various qualities of mind hypothesized to
sion and anxiety, analyses indicated that the KIMS- underlie such a mindful state of consciousness (Baer et al.
Observe scale predicted enhanced Stroop performance 2004; Baer et al. 2008; Brown and Ryan 2003). Some of
and reduced variability in attentional processing on the the qualities assessed include the perceived ability to notice
CPT. Post hoc analyses also provided evidence that the subtle perceptual events, to act in a deliberate, conscien-
associative strength between KIMS-Observe and reduced tious fashion, and to maintain a non-reactive attitude toward
CPT reaction time variability increased as a function of unpleasant emotional experiences. While these question-
task block, suggestive of a protective effect against naires do appear sensitive to improvements resulting from
attentional lapses due to prolonged exposure to the CPT. mindfulness training (Carmody et al. 2009), a variety of
While the present study failed to replicate previously health-related outcomes (Brown et al. 2007), as well as
reported associations between KIMS and attentional aspects of improved neurobiological functioning (Creswell
functioning, the consistency of current findings to con- et al. 2007), the construct validity of these scales as
ceptualizations of mindfulness suggests that KIMS- measures of “mindfulness” remains unclear (Davidson
Observe taps important attentional processes thought to 2010; Grossman 2008; Grossman and Van Dam 2011;
underlie mindfulness. Rosch 2007). This is partly attributable to the fact that there
is currently no consensus regarding the inventory of skills
Keywords Mindfulness . Self-report . Attention . Cognitive that constitute mindfulness (Baer 2011), and that self-report
control measures of such psychological states are highly vulnerable
to bias and recall error. Ideally, these instruments should be
tested against objective behavioral and physiologic indices
B. M. Galla (*) : T. S. Hale : S. L. Smalley of mindfulness (Robertson et al. 1997; Smallwood et al.
Mindful Awareness Research Center, Semel Institute for 2007); however, it is not yet clear what measures are best
Neuroscience and Human Behavior, University of California,
suited to serve in this capacity (Davidson 2010).
760 Westwood Plaza, Rm 47-444, Box 951759, Los Angeles, CA
90095, USA Because mindfulness is fundamentally about paying
e-mail: gallabrian@gmail.com attention (Analayo 2003), one possible avenue to further
explore the nature of self-report mindfulness scales might
A. Shrestha : S. K. Loo
be to examine their relation to attentional capacities. While
Center for Neurobehavioral Genetics, Semel Institute for
Neuroscience and Human Behavior, University of California, “mindfulness” is not simply reducible to “attention” (Bodhi
Los Angeles, CA, USA 2011; Siegel 2007), it clearly is a component part of
Author's personal copy
Mindfulness

mindfulness, and therefore may usefully serve as a proxy measures were associated with attention performance on the
that is also amenable to quantitative assessment. The fact Stroop task, with each implicating a different scale (e.g.,
that individuals naturally vary in attentional control (Posner Describe, Accept). In order to help clarify and extend this
and Rothbart 2007), coupled with the existence of multiple work, the current study reexamined the relationship
objective measures of attention, greatly facilitates this between the CPT and KIMS subscales, but included all
approach. Indeed, Davidson (2010) proposed that a link standard performance scales; and it reexamined the rela-
between self-reported mindfulness and performance on tionship between KIMS and the Stroop test. Furthermore,
multiple objective measures of attention might help verify whereas the previous three studies relied on relatively small
the self-report measures’ utility as an index of “mindful” student samples (e.g., Josefsson and Broberg 2010;
functioning. As mindfulness seems to be especially reliant Schmertz et al. 2009), the current study was able to utilize
upon both sustained and executive attention (Bishop et al. a larger community sample and control for additional and
2004; Lutz et al. 2008), the degree to which self-report important factors that might influence cognitive func-
measures capture “mindful” functioning should be most tioning, such as age, educational attainment, IQ, socio-
evident in these capacities. To this end, the current study economic status, and depression and anxiety. Controlling
examined the associations between one widely used self- for factors known to be related to cognitive functioning
report mindfulness measure, the Kentucky Inventory of will help reduce Type I errors, and can provide an
Mindfulness Skills (KIMS; Baer et al. 2004), and aspects of additional degree of confidence in any significant
sustained and executive attention to further assess the findings. Of the three studies cited above, only two
nature of this self-report mindfulness scale, and to better controlled for the effects of age (Josefsson and Broberg
understand whether, and how, it maps onto various features 2010; Moore and Malinowski 2009), and none explored
of attention. the effects of other demographic and psychological
As of this writing there is a paucity of studies examining variables. Due to the scarcity and inconsistency of
the relations between the KIMS and attentional processes. previous findings on this topic, we did not make specific
Schmertz et al. (2009) found no evidence for associations predictions regarding the associations between the KIMS
between the KIMS-Act with Awareness scale and indices of and measures of sustained and executive attention. Our
sustained attention (omission errors and reaction time goal was to clarify and extend previous findings in a larger
variability) on the Continuous Performance Test (CPT; and more carefully controlled community sample.
Conners 2000), although they did not report on the other
mindfulness facets assessed by the KIMS. Moore and
Malinowski (2009) found that KIMS-Act with Awareness Method
and Accept scores most strongly related to executive
attention performance on a paper-and-pencil Stroop test Participants and Procedure
(Golden 1978), and sustained attention performance on the
d2-concentration and endurance test (Brickenkamp and Subjects were 106 parents (51% female, Mage =44.7 years,
Zilmer 1988). Finally, Josefsson and Broberg (2010) SD=6.29) drawn from a larger sample of 241 adults (130
explored the relations between the Five Factor Mindfulness families) identified from an ongoing molecular genetics
Questionnaire (FFMQ; Baer et al. 2008)—the second study of ADHD sibling pairs and their families (Smalley et
iteration of the KIMS—and measures of both sustained al. 2000). A previous study using the KIMS included the
(Sustained Attention to Response Test, SART; Robertson et larger sample of 241 (Smalley et al. 2009); however in the
al. 1997) and executive (computerized Stroop test) atten- current study, individuals were excluded from current
tion. Higher FFMQ-Describe scores significantly predicted analyses if they met criteria for a lifetime diagnosis of
fewer errors of sustained attention (FFMQ-Observe and ADHD (n=106, 44%), bringing the total sample to 135.
Non-judge were also marginally significance), and pre- Since the main goal of this study was to replicate, as closely
dicted fewer Stroop errors. as possible, previously reported findings linking KIMS with
Taken together, these studies indicate somewhat incon- cognitive functioning, we limited our analyses to a
sistent and unexpected findings. Schmertz et al. (2009) normative sample of adults. Because ADHD is classified
found no association between the CPT and the Act with as a neurodevelopmental disorder that carries distinct
Awareness scale, but as noted, they did not report any other genetic, neural, and clinical properties (Li et al. 2006),
KIMS scales, namely “Observe”, which is also thought to individuals with a lifetime diagnosis were excluded from all
be attention related (Baer et al. 2004). Furthermore, they analyses. Due to time constraints some participants were
reported on only two of four common CPT performance unable to complete the entire battery of tests, leaving 106
measures. Moreover, the two studies that found significant (out of 135) individuals with complete data. Of the included
effects surprisingly indicated that “non-attentional” KIMS participants, 6.8% met criteria for one or more mood
Author's personal copy
Mindfulness

disorders (major depressive disorder, dysthymic disorder, meaning of the color word (MacLeod 1991). The Golden
mania, hypomania, and/or cyclothymia), and 24.3% met version consists of three blocks (word-naming, color-
criteria for one or more anxiety disorders (generalized anxiety naming, and color–word-naming), although the third block
disorder, separation anxiety disorder, social phobia, simple (color–word-naming) specifically relates to executive atten-
phobia, panic disorder with or without agoraphobia, and/or tion. The total number of items processed during the 45-s
post-traumatic stress disorder). The current sample was time limit on each task block was included as measures of
composed primarily of Caucasians (79%), but also included participants’ performance.
Hispanics (6%), Black/African Americans (5%), Asian/ Sustained attention was assessed using a 14-min Con-
Pacific Islanders (8%), and individuals from mixed/other tinuous Performance Test (Conners 1994). During the task,
ethnic backgrounds (2%). A majority of participants in the subjects monitor a central fixation on a computer screen
current sample had 4-year college or professional degrees while single capital letters are sequentially and centrally
(77%), and were in the middle range of socio-economic status presented during six continuous blocks of 20 trials with
according to the Hollingshead (1957) scale where 1 is the varying interstimulus intervals (ISI). The order of ISI-block
highest and 7 is the lowest (Mses =3.22, SD=1.99). presentation is randomized within subjects. Subjects press
Detailed information regarding informed consent and the the space bar using their dominant hand with every letter
assessments for ADHD and other DSM-IV Axis I disorders presentation except for the letter “X”, which occurs on 10%
has been previously published (Smalley et al. 2000). of the trials within a given ISI-block. Subjects’ sustained
During a single laboratory session, participants completed attention performance was assessed using the following
a battery of self-report measures and standardized neuro- CPT measures: (1) commission errors: a failure to inhibit
psychological tests assessing executive and sustained response when an “X” is presented, (2) omission errors: a
attention (Loo et al. 2008). The CPT paradigm described failure to respond when any letter other than “X” is
in “Measures” was added later in the course of the study, so presented, (3) mean reaction time, and (4) hit reaction time
only a subset of participants (n=82) were able to complete standard deviation: trial-by-trial variability in response
the task. Participants were also administered the Vocabulary times.
and Block Design portions of the Wechsler Adult Intelli-
gence Scale, Third edition (WAIS-III; Wechsler 1997),
allowing for estimation of full IQ for control purposes. Results

Measures Correlations Between KIMS and Attentional Control

Mindfulness was assessed using the Kentucky Inventory Major study variables were first checked for normality and
of Mindfulness Skills (Baer et al. 2004), a 39-item outliers using SPSS (2007). CPT omission errors failed to
questionnaire that taps four mindfulness-related skills: meet assumptions of normality and were log transformed to
(1) Acting with Awareness (e.g., “When I do things, my approximate normal distribution. Furthermore, three out-
mind wanders off and I’m easily distracted”), (2) Observ- liers (scoring more than 3 SDs below the mean) were
ing (e.g., “I notice changes in my body, such as whether identified on the KIMS composite score, and these
my breathing slows down or speeds up”), (3) Describing individuals were removed from subsequent analyses,
(e.g., “I’m good at finding words to describe my feel- leaving a final sample of 103 individuals for analyses.
ings”), and (4) Non-judgmental Acceptance (e.g., “I tell Significant correlations emerged between various
myself that I shouldn’t be feeling the way I’m feeling”). demographic variables and major study variables, and
The scales can also be combined to form a composite were therefore included in all regression analyses
score of mindfulness. The items are rated on a 5-point reported in the next section. Correlations between the
Likert scale, from 1 (never or very rarely true) to 5 (very KIMS scales and attentional control indices are presented
often or always true) with higher scores reflecting greater in Table 1. Positive correlations were noted between
mindfulness. The KIMS-Total score (α=0.85), as well as KIMS-Observe and Describe (r=0.38, p=0.000), KIMS-
the subscales Act with Awareness (α=0.74), Observe (α= Act with Awareness and Accept (r=0.19, p=0.06). Negative
0.84), Describe (α=0.88), and Accept (α=0.83) showed correlations were observed between KIMS-Observe and
high internal consistency. Accept (r=−0.41, p=0.000). KIMS-Describe scale was
Executive attention was assessed using the Golden unrelated to the Acting with Awareness (r=0.10, p=0.31)
version (1978) of the Stroop test. The Stroop test assesses and Accept (r=0.07, p=0.50) scales in our sample.
participants’ ability to overcome habitual responding and Few significant correlations emerged between KIMS scale
activate more subdominant response tendencies by naming scores and attentional control indices. The KIMS-Observe
the font color of a written word rather than the semantic scale was positively correlated with the number of items
Author's personal copy
Mindfulness

Table 1 Descriptive statistics and correlations between major study variables (N=103)

Variable M SD 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9

Age 44.7 6.29 –


Gender 0.51 0.5 0.33** –
SES 3.22 1.99 −0.07 −0.26** –
Education 2.21 1.15 0.07 0.02 −0.25* –
Full-scale IQ 110.04 16.10 0.14 0.05 −0.14 0.37** –
KIMS-Observe 34.54 8.07 0.01 −0.18† −0.05 0.09 0.09 –
KIMS-Describe 29.11 5.95 0.01 −0.06 −0.06 −0.09 0.06 0.38** –
KIMS-Act 32.67 5.50 0.14 0.30** −0.18† −0.02 −0.09 −0.04 0.10 –
KIMS-Accept 32.88 6.22 0.16† 0.16† −0.09 0.02 −0.05 −0.41** 0.07 0.19 –
Stroop items processed (word) 102.36 16.34 −0.19 −0.20* 0.07 0.21* 0.28** 0.11 0.08 0.06 −0.24*
Stroop items processed (color) 72.85 12.07 −0.23* −0.26** 0.09 0.17† 0.26** 0.21* 0.17 −0.04 −0.01
Stroop items processed (color–word) 42.88 8.11 −0.31** −0.03 −0.02 0.19† 0.25* 0.22* 0.14 −0.01 −0.11
CPT omission errorsa 7.20 9.34 0.04 0.21* −0.02 −0.16 −0.06 −0.08 0.01 −0.11 0.11
CPT commission errorsa 10.26 6.36 0.08 0.23* 0.10 −0.11 0.00 −0.09 0.08 −0.03 0.00
CPT mean reaction timea 434.33 60.18 0.03 −0.18 −0.09 −0.10 0.06 −0.08 −0.03 −0.02 0.07
CPT RT variability (SD)a 118.12 40.99 0.09 0.11 −0.19† −0.14 0.12 −0.24* −0.16 −0.09 0.12

CPT Continuous Performance Test, KIMS Kentucky Inventory of Mindfulness Skills


†p<0.10; *p<0.05; **p<0.01
a
N=82

processed on the Stroop color-naming (r=0.21, p=0.04) and (β=−0.23, t=−2.31, p=0.02). No other significant effects
color–word-naming (r=0.22, p=0.03) blocks, and negatively between depression and anxiety and cognitive task perfor-
correlated with hit reaction time (RT) variability (SD) on the mance emerged.
CPT (r=−0.24, p=0.03). Finally, the KIMS-Accept scale
was inversely correlated with the number of items processed Table 2 Regression analyses predicting attention performance from
on the Stroop word-naming block (r=−0.24, p=0.01). KIMS-Observe Score

Model B SE β t
Regression Analyses
Stroop performance (color–word block)
To further explore the significant correlations noted in the (Constant) 41.26 7.46 5.53**
previous section, multiple regressions were conducted with Age −0.52 0.12 −0.40 −4.21**
controls for age, gender, education, socio-economic status, Gender 2.42 1.61 0.15 1.5
and full IQ (results are summarized in Table 2). KIMS- SES 0.25 0.39 0.06 0.64
Observe scores remained a significant predictor for the Education 2.39 1.88 0.13 1.27
number of items processed on the Stroop color–word block Full IQ 0.12 0.05 0.24 2.52*
(β=0.22, t=2.41, p=0.02) and CPT RT variability (β=−0.26, KIMS-Observe 0.22 0.09 0.22 2.41*
t=−2.30, p=0.03). KIMS-Observe scores no longer predicted CPT reaction time variability (SD)a
the number of items processed on the Stroop color-naming
(Constant) 100.23 50.64 1.98*
block (β=0.16, t=1.70, p=0.09). Similarly, KIMS-Accept no
Age 0.53 0.76 0.08 0.7
longer predicted items processed on the Stroop word-naming
Gender −2.62 9.62 −0.03 −0.27
block (β=−0.18, t=1.93, p=0.06).
SES −5.23 2.35 −0.25 −2.22*
To account for other transient factors that might
Education −28.24 11.31 −0.31 −2.50*
influence performance on attention tasks, we conducted
Full IQ 0.71 0.34 0.25 2.10*
two further regression analyses, this time with added
KIMS-Observe −1.25 0.54 −0.26 −2.30*
controls for depression and anxiety. The inclusion of
psychiatric variables did not alter the significant associa- *p<0.05; **p<0.01
tions between KIMS-Observe and performance indices. It CPT Continuous Performance Test, KIMS Kentucky Inventory of
should be noted that anxiety did contribute a negative main Mindfulness Skills
a
effect on items processed on the Stroop color–word block N=82
Author's personal copy
Mindfulness

Post Hoc Analyses: Relations Between KIMS-Observe


and CPT RT Variability as a Function of CPT Task Block

Evidence suggests that the ability to maintain a stable,


sustained attention wanes over time, with attention being
subject to a number of vulnerabilities, including fatigue
(Davies and Parasuraman 1982; Parasuraman 1984) and
mind wandering (Smallwood and Schooler 2006). Increases
in lapses of attention can be seen, in part, through increased
trial-by-trial RT variability over time on the CPT. Given
recent empirical evidence that intensive meditation training
was associated with improved vigilance (reduced attention-
al fatigue) over time (MacLean et al. 2010), we further
examined whether the associations observed between
KIMS-Observe and CPT RT variability were a function of
Fig. 1 Correlation coefficients between KIMS-Observe and Contin-
task block. As shown in Table 3, both the range and mean uous Performance Test Reaction Time Variability (SD) as a function of
levels of RT variability (SD) increased as a function of CPT task block. †p<0.10; *p<0.05
task block, suggestive of an increase in attentional lapses
over time.
Figure 1 shows the correlation coefficients between able to resolve cognitive conflict as measured by the Stroop
KIMS-Observe and CPT RT variability as a function of test, and showed less variability in their attentional
task block. With each successive task block, the relations processing on the CPT. These associations remained
between KIMS-Observe and CPT RT variability became significant even after controlling for age, gender, education,
increasingly stronger and reached marginal significance in socio-economic status, full IQ, and mood and anxiety,
block 5 (r=−0.20, p=0.07) and full significance in block 6 providing a measure of confidence in the results. Post hoc
(r=−0.24, p=0.03). This finding suggests that individuals analyses also provided evidence that the relations between
higher in KIMS-Observe were less vulnerable to attentional KIMS-Observe and CPT RT variability increased as a
lapses over time as indexed by reduced trial-by-trial function of task block.
reaction time variability. Regarding performance on the CPT, our findings were
similar to that of Schmertz et al. (2009) in that we both
found no evidence for an association between KIMS-Act
Discussion with Awareness scores and CPT omission errors or reaction
time variability. However, our findings indicated that other
In the current study, we attempted to replicate, as closely as mindfulness facets assessed by the KIMS which were
possible, and to clarify previously reported findings on the excluded by Schmertz et al. (2009) were associated with
associations between a widely used self-report measure of sustained attention performance. We showed that scores on
mindfulness (KIMS) and tests of sustained (CPT) and KIMS-Observe, the perceived tendency to observe and pay
executive (Stroop) attention. We sought to do this while careful attention to subtle perceptual experiences, were
also addressing several methodological limitations of associated with reduced variability in attentional processing
previous research. Overall, the current results indicated that on a measure of sustained attention. Variability in trial-by-
individuals with high scores on KIMS-Observe were better trial response times on a vigilance task is indicative of
momentary lapses in attention from current processing
Table 3 CPT reaction time variability (SD) across the six task blocks goals (Castellanos et al. 2006), due in part to task-unrelated
processes, like mind wandering (Smallwood et al. 2007).
Variable Minimum Maximum Range Mean SD
Close monitoring of subjective experience includes the
Block 1 52.67 260.08 207.41 111.49 43.04 ability to notice subtle fluctuations or lapses in attention (or
Block 2 42.33 236.48 194.15 101.72 36.77 alertness) from goal-directed processes, and individuals
Block 3 54.19 255.36 201.17 107.07 45.93 who are naturally more attuned to subtle sensory experi-
Block 4 41.59 328.28 286.69 110.04 52.71 ences are likely to be also more sensitive to the location and
Block 5 43.67 374.46 330.79 112.04 55.52
quality of their attention. When current processing goals
Block 6 50.81 490.63 439.82 119.89 70.14
demand the maintenance of a steady, sustained attention
across time, such awareness is likely to facilitate these
CPT Continuous Performance Test efforts. This steady and vivid watchfulness, or monitoring,
Author's personal copy
Mindfulness

of current experience is also part and parcel of mindfulness differentiation between the subscales. Together, these
practice (Bishop et al. 2004; Bodhi 2011; Wallace and differences might have contributed to the discrepant
Shapiro 2006). For example, Lutz et al. (2009) convinc- findings between the current study and Moore and
ingly showed that intensive retreat practice resulted in Malinowski’s (2009) study regarding KIMS-Observe and
reduced trial-by-trial reaction time variability on a dichotic Stroop performance.
listening task compared to a control group (but, interest- The associations Moore and Malinowski (2009) noted
ingly, did not result in group differences in mean reaction between KIMS-Accept and Stroop performance—although
time or increased target detection). not duplicated in our study—is worthy of mention. Baer et
Post hoc analyses also provided evidence that the al. (2004) argued that non-evaluative processing of experi-
associative strength between KIMS-Observe and CPT RT ence, tapped by the Accept scale, might “encourage more
variability increased as a function of task block. This adaptive responding to problematic situations by preventing
finding further supports the argument that close observation automatic, impulsive, maladaptive behaviors” (p. 194).
of sensory experiences, as measured by the KIMS-Observe, While the scale items might not be explicitly related to
can facilitate more efficient and sustained attentional attentional functioning, it is very plausible, given this
functioning in demanding contexts, and may prevent rationale, that Accept scores might track performance on
attentional lapses or fatigue (MacLean et al. 2010). Whether measures of cognitive inhibition and flexibility. As such,
these individuals are actually more resistant to attentional the FFMQ-Non-reactivity subscale, which purportedly taps
fatigue over time, or are more capable of mustering limited the ability to inhibit emotional and/or behavioral reactivity
resources in the midst of fatigue (more resilient) is (Baer et al. 2008), might be even more suited for indexing
unknown. Either way, it does appear that individuals cognitive inhibition/flexibility than Observe or Accept, and
scoring high on KIMS-Observe showed an increased future research would do well to examine this scale for this
capacity for sustained attention as the CPT progressed. It purpose.
should be kept in mind that these results are unique to this We, as well as Moore and Malinowski (2009), were also
study and until more research can confirm these findings, unable to replicate the associations between KIMS-
they should be considered preliminary. Furthermore, it is Describe and reduced Stroop interference showed by
uncertain whether Schmertz et al. (2009) would have found Josefsson and Broberg (2010). These discrepancies might
these results even if they had included the KIMS-Observe be due in large part to the different Stroop administration
scale in their study because the CPT utilized in their study types (paper-and-pencil vs. computer-based) which have
consisted of three blocks of 20 trials compared with six been shown to produce different outcomes in the same
blocks of 20 trials used in our study. Their task might not sample (Salo et al. 2001). In articulating their findings
have been long enough to produce a sufficient amount of however, Josefsson and Broberg (2010) argued that KIMS-
attentional fatigue we reported. Describe might index a tendency to be more conscientious
Regarding performance on the Stroop test, our findings when completing tasks, which in turn might facilitate
were inconsistent with previous research. We were unable performance on the Stroop. While KIMS-Describe does
to replicate the findings of the two previous studies that seem to be related to trait conscientiousness (Baer et al.
examined the associations between the KIMS and the 2004), the Act with Awareness scale is more strongly
Stroop test (Josefsson and Broberg 2010; Moore and associated with trait conscientiousness (Baer et al. 2004)
Malinowski 2009). Furthermore, neither of those studies but was not associated with Stroop performance in
revealed significant associations between KIMS-Observe Josefsson and Broberg’s (2010) study. Further replication
and Stroop performance that we showed in our analyses. studies are needed to authenticate the findings reported by
Moore and Malinowski (2009) did note significant negative Josefsson and Broberg (2010), and it will also be necessary
associations between KIMS-Observe and Stroop errors. We to clarify why exactly Describe should be associated with
were unable to examine this index in our sample due to an attentional control (Grossman and Van Dam 2011).
extremely low frequency and variation in error rate (Stroop While the current findings between KIMS-Observe and
color–word errors: M=1.12, SD=1.40). It is quite possible Stroop do not entirely corroborate previously reported
that the Stroop test implemented in our study was simply findings, they make clear theoretical sense and help support
too short (45 s per block) to reveal meaningful variation in the findings from the CPT. More than anything, mindful-
error rates that might be associated with KIMS-Observe ness is fundamentally about close, repetitive observation of
scores (compared with the 2-min task blocks used in Moore the experiential field (Bodhi 2011), and this is precisely the
and Malinowski’s (2009) study). Also, a unique aspect of quality of mind that KIMS-Observe attempts to capture.
Moore and Malinowski’s (2009) study—that we did not Our results suggest that KIMS-Observe is related to
find in our sample—was the higher than usual correlations performance on the Stroop test, which requires the
between KIMS facets, indicating a possible lack of inhibition of automatic responding. Any prolonged period
Author's personal copy
Mindfulness

of sustained and vivid awareness on present moment mindfulness might attenuate emotional reactivity. In sum,
experience requires monitoring the focus of attention, disen- measures of attention that rely less on speed, or where
gaging from distractions, and redirecting attention back to speed is secondary to another ability (e.g., fatigue resis-
current processing goals. As expected then, the practice of tance) appear more closely aligned with conceptualizations
mindfulness has been shown to reinforce a degree of of mindfulness as a monitoring faculty. Exploiting this
“deautomatization” from automatic tendencies of mind, similarity might advance the field and help to clarify
indexed by improved ability to resolve cognitive interference lingering inconsistencies in the literature.
on the Stroop test (Wenk-Sormaz 2005). Individuals naturally Second, non-reaction-time measures might be less
more sensitive to subtle, low-intensity somatic, and percep- susceptible to other confounding factors, like motivation
tual experiences might maintain a more chronically activated and effort (Flehmig et al. 2007). It is quite possible to
monitoring state (or, meta-awareness; Smallwood and perform well on certain attention tests through shear
Schooler 2006), and may thus be more able to gather, when cognitive will power (Tomporowski and Tinsley 1996),
appropriate, the cognitive resources to resolve the processing and although mindfulness certainly involves attention, it
conflict effectively. likely is not a necessary component to performing well on
Taken together, the consistency of our findings in tests of attention. Jensen et al. (2011) reported that some of
relation to current conceptualizations of mindfulness sug- the results in the meditation literature might be confounded
gests that KIMS-Observe taps important attentional pro- by unaccounted motivational factors. This is a serious
cesses thought to underlie mindfulness. Linking self-report challenge to existing empirical literature, and another
measures of mindfulness to objective measures of attention reason for future research to consider both selecting
appears to represent one promising means to evaluate the attentional measures that are more resistant to effort (e.g.,
nature of these measures (Davidson 2010). However, this attentional blink test; Olivers and Nieuwenhuis 2005) and
approach also carries several methodological and concep- controlling for constructs like motivation.
tual issues that must also be addressed in future research. Finally, we must not assume that attentional measures
First, and most importantly, it is not yet clear which are the only worthwhile objective measures to examine the
measures of attention are most suited to tap underlying nature of self-report measures. While we assessed the
mental processes that support mindfulness. The wide array relations between self-reported mindfulness and active
of attention tasks used, and inconsistent findings in the attentional performance, we do not assume that other facets
literature attest to the current lack of consensus on the do not also measure important qualities of mind related to
subject. One problem might stem from the fact that most mindfulness. There may be other qualities of psychological
objective measures of attention are timed tests that direct functioning related to mindfulness (e.g., curiosity about and
participants to respond to stimuli as quickly as possible. openness to experience, the tendency to approach rather
Mindfulness practice seems to have little in common with than avoid discomfort, stress reactivity, and recovery, etc.)
speed and fast responding, so research that focuses that fell outside the purview of the current study. Future
exclusively on strict reaction time measures may be slightly studies should investigate the associations between “non-
mistargeted. For example, we offered evidence that KIMS- attentional” facets of mindfulness and related psychological
Observe was related to better trial-by-trial attentional processes, although we encourage future research to avoid,
stability (RT SD) and reduced exhaustion, but was where ever possible, strict reliance on self-reports to draw
unrelated to mean reaction time on the CPT. This pattern these linkages.
of findings has been shown in a host of other studies The current study expanded upon the growing litera-
examining the attentional impact of mindfulness practice. ture documenting the associations between self-reported
Zeidan et al. (2010) found meditation practice improved mindfulness and attention functioning in several ways.
the number of consecutive correct responses on a working First, we used a community sample of adults, rather than
memory task, but not processing speed. MacLean et al. relying on a convenience sample of college under-
(2010) showed reduced fatigue following intensive retreat graduates. Secondly, we also controlled for factors
practice, and Semple (2010) found improved target dis- significantly related to attentional control, such as
crimination following regular mindfulness practice. Simi- anxiety (Eysenck et al. 2007), as well as a number of
larly, Lutz et al. (2009) found improved attentional stability other potential demographic variables (e.g., IQ, education,
following intensive meditation retreat, but no improvements SES) that previous studies failed to include. Despite these
in reaction time relative to a control group. The relations strengths, the study also had several limitations. As
between self-reported mindfulness and attention might also mentioned above, our Stroop administration time was
be more effectively assessed in emotionally evocative relatively short and may have prevented us from finding
situations that place a high degree of cognitive load on relations between other KIMS scales and/or error perfor-
participants (Ortner et al. 2007), as higher levels of mance (Moore and Malinowski (2009) used a 2-min
Author's personal copy
Mindfulness

Stroop block administration). Secondly, we included only Flehmig, H. C., Steinborn, M., Langner, R., Anja, S., & Westhoff, K.
(2007). Assessing intraindividual variability in sustained atten-
one measure of mindfulness and cannot speak to the
tion: Reliability, relation to speed and accuracy, and practice
relations other mindfulness measures (e.g., Mindful effects. Psychology Science, 49, 132–149.
Awareness Attention Scale; Brown and Ryan 2003) may Golden, C. J. (1978). Stroop Color and Word Test: A Manual for
exhibit with attention tasks. In conclusion, the results of Clinical and Experimental Uses. Wood Dale: Stoelting.
Grossman, P. (2008). On measuring mindfulness in psychosomatic
the current study offer useful data toward an evolving
and psychological research. Journal of Psychosomatic Research,
cognitive understanding of mindfulness questionnaires. 64(4), 405–408.
Grossman, P., & Van Dam, N. T. (2011). Mindfulness, by any other
Acknowledgments The research reported herein is supported by name…: trials and tribulations of sati in western psychology and
NIMH MH058277 (SLS) and NINDS NS054124 (SKL). We wish to science. Contemporary Buddhism, 12(1), 219–239.
thank all the families who participated in this research. Hollingshead, A. B. (1957). Two Factor Index of Social Position. New
Haven: Yale University Department of Sociology.
Jensen, C. G., Vangklide, S., Frokjaer, V., & Hasselbalch, S. G.
References (2011). Mindfulness training affects attention—Or is it attention-
al effort? Journal of Experimental Psychology: General.
Josefsson, T., & Broberg, A. (2010). Meditators and non-meditators
Analayo. (2003). Satipatthana: The Direct Path to Realization. on sustained and executive attentional performance. Mental
Birmingham: Windhorse. Health, Religion and Culture, 14(3), 291–309.
Baer, R. A. (2011). Measuring mindfulness. Contemporary Buddhism, Li, D., Sham, P. C., Owen, M. J., & He, L. (2006). Meta-analysis
12(1), 241–261. shows significant association between dopamine system genes
Baer, R. A., Smith, G. T., & Allen, K. B. (2004). Assessment of and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (adhd). Human
mindfulness by self-report: The Kentucky inventory of mindful- Molecular Genetics, 15(4), 2276–2284.
ness skills. Assessment, 11(3), 191–206. Loo, S. K., Rich, E. C., Ishii, J., McGough, J., McCracken, J., Nelson,
Baer, R. A., Smith, G. T., Lykins, E. L. B., Button, D., Krietemeyer, S., et al. (2008). Cognitive functioning in affected sibling pairs
J., Sauer, S., et al. (2008). Construct validity of the five facet with ADHD: Familial clustering and dopamine genes. Journal of
mindfulness questionnaire in meditating and nonmeditating Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 49(9), 950–957.
samples. Assessment, 15(3), 329–342. Lutz, A., Slagter, H. A., Dunne, J., & Davidson, R. J. (2008).
Bishop, S. R., Lau, M., Shapiro, S., Carlson, L., Anderson, N. D., Attention regulation and monitoring in meditation. Trends in
Carmody, J., et al. (2004). Mindfulness: A proposed operational Cognitive Sciences, 12(4), 163–169.
definition. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 11(3), Lutz, A., Slagter, H., Rawling, N., Francis, A., Greischar, L. L., &
230–241. Davidson, R. J. (2009). Mental training enhances attentional
Bodhi, B. (2011). What does mindfulness really mean? A canonical stability: Neural and behavioral evidence. Journal of Neurosci-
perspective. Contemporary Buddhism, 12(1), 19–39. ence, 29(42), 13418–13427.
Brickenkamp, R., & Zilmer, E. (1988). D2 Test of Attention: Manual. MacLean, K. A., Ferrer, E., Aichele, S. R., Bridwell, D. A., Zanesco,
Oxford: Hogrefe & Huber. A. P., Jacobs, T. L., et al. (2010). Intensive meditation training
Brown, K. W., & Ryan, R. M. (2003). The benefits of being present: improves perceptual discrimination and sustained attention.
Mindfulness and its role in psychological well-being. Journal of Psychological Science, 21(6), 829–839.
Personality and Social Psychology, 84(4), 822–848. MacLeod, C. M. (1991). Half a century of research on the Stroop
Brown, K. W., Ryan, R. M., & Creswell, J. D. (2007). Mindfulness: effect: An integrative review. Psychological Bulletin, 109(2),
Theoretical foundations and evidence for its salutary effects. 163–203.
Psychological Inquiry, 18(4), 211–237. Moore, A., & Malinowski, P. (2009). Meditation, mindfulness and
Carmody, J., Baer, R. A., Lykins, E. L. B., & Olendzki, N. (2009). An cognitive flexibility. Consciousness and Cognition, 18(1), 176–
empirical study of the mechanisms of mindfulness in a mindfulness- 186.
based reduction program. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 65(6), Olivers, C. N., & Nieuwenhuis, S. (2005). The beneficial effect of
613–626. concurrent task-irrelevant mental activity on temporal attention.
Castellanos, F. X., Sonuga-Barke, E. J., Milham, M. P., & Tannock, R. Psychological Science, 16, 265–269.
(2006). Characterizing cognition in ADHD: Beyond executive Ortner, C. N. M., Kilner, S. J., & Zelazo, P. D. (2007). Mindfulness
dysfunction. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10, 117–123. meditation and reduced emotional interference on a cognitive
Conners, C. K. (1994). The Continuous Performance Test (CPT): Use task. Motivation and Emotion, 31(4), 271–283.
as a Diagnostic Tool and Measure of Treatment Outcome. Los Parasuraman, R. (1984). Sustained attention in detection and discrim-
Angeles: American Psychological Association. ination. In R. Parasuraman & D. R. Davies (Eds.), Varieties of
Conners, C. K. (2000). Continuous Performance Test II Technical Attention (pp. 243–271). Orlando: Academic Press.
Guide and Software Manual. Toronto: Multi-Health Systems. Posner, M. I., & Rothbart, M. K. (2007). Research on attention
Creswell, J. D., Way, B. M., Eisenberger, N. I., & Lieberman, M. D. networks as a model for the integration of psychological science.
(2007). Neural correlates of dispositional mindfulness during Annual Review of Psychology, 58(1), 1–23.
affect labeling. Psychosomatic Medicine, 69(6), 560–565. Robertson, I. H., Manly, T., Andrade, J., Baddeley, B. T., & Yiend, J.
Davidson, R. J. (2010). Empirical explorations of mindfulness: (1997). Oops: Performance correlates of everyday attentional
Conceptual and methodological conundrums. Emotion, 10(1), failures in traumatic brain injured and normal subjects. Neuro-
8–11. psychologia, 35, 747–758.
Davies, D. R., & Parasuraman, R. (1982). The Psychology of Rosch, E. (2007). More than mindfulness: When you have a tiger by
Vigilance. London: Academic Press. the tail, let it eat you. Psychological Inquiry, 18(4), 258–264.
Eysenck, M. W., Derakshan, N., Santos, R., & Calvo, M. G. (2007). Salo, R., Henik, A., & Robertson, L. C. (2001). Interpreting stroop
Anxiety and cognitive performance: Attentional control theory. interference: An analysis of differences between task versions.
Emotion, 7(2), 336–353. Neuropsychology, 15(4), 462–471.
Author's personal copy
Mindfulness

Schmertz, S. K., Anderson, P. L., & Robins, D. L. (2009). The relation Smallwood, J., McSpadden, M., & Schooler, J. W. (2007). The lights
between self-report mindfulness and performance on tasks of are on but no one’s home: Meta-awareness and the decoupling of
sustained attention. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral attention when the mind wanders. Psychonomic Bulletin and
Assessment, 31(1), 60–66. Review, 14(3), 527–533.
Semple, R. J. (2010). Does mindfulness enhance attention? A SPSS. (2007). SPSS for Windows (Version 16.0). New York, NY:
randomized controlled trial. Mindfulness, 1, 121–130. SPSS.
Siegel, D. J. (2007). The Mindful Brain: Reflection and Attunement in Tomporowski, P. D., & Tinsley, V. F. (1996). Effects of memory
the Cultivation of Well-Being (p. 387). New York: W W Norton demand and motivation on sustained attention in young and older
& Co. adults. The American Journal of Psychology, 109, 187–204.
Smalley, S. L., McGough, J. J., Del’Homme, M., NewDelman, J., Wallace, B. A., & Shapiro, S. L. (2006). Mental balance and well-
Gordon, E., Kim, T., et al. (2000). Familial clustering of being: Building bridges between Buddhism and Western psy-
symptoms and disruptive behaviors in multiplex families with chology. American Psychologist, 61(7), 690–701.
attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Journal of the American Wechsler, D. (1997). Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Third Edition
Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 39(9), 1135–1143. (WAIS-3). San Antonio: Harcourt Assessment.
Smalley, S. L., Loo, S. K., Hale, T. S., Shrestha, A., McGough, J., Wenk-Sormaz, H. (2005). Meditation can reduce habitual responding.
Flook, L., et al. (2009). Mindfulness and attention deficit Advances in Mind-Body Medicine, 21(3–4), 33–49.
hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 65(10), Zeidan, F., Johnson, S. K., Diamond, B. J., David, Z., & Goolkasian,
1087–1098. P. (2010). Mindfulness meditation improves cognition: Evidence
Smallwood, J., & Schooler, J. W. (2006). The restless mind. of brief mental training. Consciousness and Cognition, 19(2),
Psychological Bulletin, 132(6), 946–958. 597–605.

Você também pode gostar