Você está na página 1de 18

Journal of Family Psychology

1999, Vol. 13, No. 4, 580-597


Copyright 1999 by the American Psychological Association, Inc.
0893-3200/99/S3.00
Attachment and Marital Functioning: Comparison of
Spouses With Continuous-Secure, Earned-Secure,
Dismissing, and Preoccupied Attachment Stances
Blair Paley
Neuropsychiatric Institute and Hospital,
University of California, Los Angeles
Martha J. Cox and Margaret R. Burchinal
University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill
C. Chris Payne
University of North Carolina at Greensboro
In a sample of 138 couples, the present study examined whether individuals' marital
functioning related to both their own and their partner's attachment stance. Earned-
secure wives managed their affect as well as continuous-secure wives during
problem-solving discussions and better than preoccupied or dismissing wives.
However, preoccupied and dismissing wives did not exhibit markedly different
patterns of affect regulation in their marriages. Regarding individuals' marital
functioning and partners' attachment stance, neither husbands' behavior nor percep-
tions related to their wives' attachment stance. However, wives of continuous-secure
husbands exhibited more positive marital behavior than wives of dismissing and
earned-secure husbands. Findings are discussed in terms of how attachment work-
ing models may account for both continuities and discontinuities between earlier
caregiving experiences and functioning in adult relationships.
Previous studies suggest there may be consid-
erable continuity between adverse caregiving
experiences in childhood and marital difficulties
later in life (Caspi & Elder, 1988; Eysenck &
Wakefield, 1981; Quinton, Rutter, & Liddle,
1984; Snyder, 1979). However, there is also
evidence that many individuals who experience
adverse caregiving experiences are nonetheless
able to establish strong and sustaining marriages
(Rutter & Quinton, 1984). How can researchers
understand when adverse caregiving experi-
ences do and do not lead to difficulty in close
adult relationships? That is, what mechanisms
might account for continuities and discontinui-
ties between earlier parent-child relationships
and marital functioning later in life?
One promising approach is suggested by John
Bowlby's (1969,1973,1980) attachment theory.
Bowlby (1973) proposed that "confidence in the
availability of attachment figures, or lack of it, is
built up slowly during the years of immaturity
infancy, childhood, and adolescenceand that
whatever expectations are developed during
those years tend to persist relatively unchanged
throughout the rest of life" (p. 202). Bowlby
(1969) proposed that these expectations, or
Blair Paley, Neuropsychatric Institute and Hospital,
University of California, Los Angeles; Martha J. Cox
and Margaret R. Burchinal, Frank Porter Graham
Child Development Center, University of North Caro-
lina at Chapel Hill; C. Chris Payne, Department of
Human Development and Family Studies, University
of North Carolina at Greensboro.
This research was supported by National Institute of
Mental Health (NIMH) Grant R01MN44763. Partial
support for the preparation of this study was provided
by NIMH Grant MH19734 (Multisite Research Train-
ing in Family Risk and Resilience).
Portions of these data were reported at the biennial
meeting of the Society for Research in Child
Development, March 1995, Indianapolis, Indiana. We
gratefully acknowledge Sami Hanks for her contin-
ued efforts on this study.
Correspondence concerning this article should be
addressed to Blair Paley, University of California,
Los Angeles, Neurospsychiatric Institute and Hospi-
tal, 760 Westwood Plaza, #58-239A, Los Angeles,
California 90024. Electronic mail may be sent to
bpaley@mednet.ucla.edu.
580
T
h
i
s

d
o
c
u
m
e
n
t

i
s

c
o
p
y
r
i
g
h
t
e
d

b
y

t
h
e

A
m
e
r
i
c
a
n

P
s
y
c
h
o
l
o
g
i
c
a
l

A
s
s
o
c
i
a
t
i
o
n

o
r

o
n
e

o
f

i
t
s

a
l
l
i
e
d

p
u
b
l
i
s
h
e
r
s
.


T
h
i
s

a
r
t
i
c
l
e

i
s

i
n
t
e
n
d
e
d

s
o
l
e
l
y

f
o
r

t
h
e

p
e
r
s
o
n
a
l

u
s
e

o
f

t
h
e

i
n
d
i
v
i
d
u
a
l

u
s
e
r

a
n
d

i
s

n
o
t

t
o

b
e

d
i
s
s
e
m
i
n
a
t
e
d

b
r
o
a
d
l
y
.
ATTACHMENT AND MARITAL FUNCTIONING 581
"working models," of attachment relationships
reflect beliefs about how reliably and sensitively
others will respond to one's needs and how
worthy the self is of such responses. Consistent,
sensitive parenting is thought to lead to secure
working models, wherein others are viewed as
dependable and supportive, and the self as
worthy of that support. Alternatively, adverse
caregiving experiences give rise to insecure
models, wherein others are expected to be
inconsistently available or overtly rejecting, and
the self is seen as unworthy. Such models are
thought to guide interpersonal appraisals and
behavior throughout life, leading individuals to
"recreate aspects of relationship systems previ-
ously experienced" (Sroufe & Fleeson, 1986, p.
53). Thus, qualities of one's working models
with regard to attachment may be associated
with functioning in adult intimate relationships,
such as in marriages. The extent to which
models of childhood attachment relationships
are related to actual behavior in marital
interaction has been rarely considered and is the
subject of the current investigation.
Adult Attachment Style
and Intimate Relationships
A large body of research has provided
evidence for a link between adult attachment
style and functioning in romantic relationships
(e.g., Collins & Read, 1990; Feeney, Noller, &
Callan, 1994; Hazan & Shaver, 1987; Kirkpa-
trick & Davis, 1994; Scharfe & Bartholomew,
1995; Simpson, Rholes, & Phillips, 1996).
Although these studies have advanced our
efforts to understand continuity between attach-
ment experiences and adult relationship function-
ing, they have focused primarily on models of
adult intimate relationships, and researchers
have recognized the need for studies that more
closely examine working models of childhood
attachment relationships and how such models
relate to marital functioning (Cohn, Silver,
Cowan, Cowan, & Pearson, 1992). We propose
that such an avenue of research is a necessary
complement to those studies that focus on adult
attachment style and would enrich the explora-
tion of Bowlby's (1969, 1973, 1980) notion that
the quality of earlier attachment relationships
will have profound import for interpersonal
functioning across the life span.
The Adult Attachment Interview
The Adult Attachment Interview (AAI),
devised by George, Kaplan, and Main (1984),
focuses on an individual's constructions of their
childhood caregiving relationships, rather than
of current intimate relationships. Two recent
studies using the AAI found that more secure
models of childhood relationships were related
to better marital functioning (Cohn et al., 1992;
Eiden, Teti, & Corns, 1995). In the current
investigation, we used the AAI to focus on two
main issues that remain relatively unexplored
with regards to the link between models of
childhood attachment relationships and marital
functioning. First, we examined the relationship
between an individual's attachment models and
his or her marital behavior and perceptions and,
in particular, looked beyond global secure and
insecure distinctions and considered more spe-
cific attachment patterns. Previous studies have
not investigated whether specific attachment
stances (i.e., continuous secure, earned secure,
dismissing, and preoccupied) may relate to
particular strategies for regulating affect in
marital relationships. Second, there has been
little exploration of the association between an
individual's marital functioning and his or her
partner's models of childhood attachment rela-
tionships, and previous research examining this
issue has been somewhat limited by a small
sample size (e.g., Cohn et al., 1992).
The use of the AAI in the present study
allowed us to further examine Bowlby's (1969,
1973,1980) notion that the working models that
an individual develops of earlier caregiving
relationships will have a significant and endur-
ing impact on the quality of other close
relationships throughout life. The AAI calls for
individuals to describe in a narrative fashion
their relationships with their parents and various
caregiving experiences during childhood. Indi-
viduals are classified as having secure or
insecure working models, on the basis of the
coherency of their accounts, rather than on the
basis of whether such accounts are positive or
negative (Main & Goldwyn, 1994). That is,
individuals may be classified as secure either
because they have provided convincing, consis-
tent accounts of generally positive caregiving
experiences or because they have acknowledged
negative caregiving experiences, but have done
so in a coherent and relatively objective manner.
Conversely, individuals who provide incoherent
T
h
i
s

d
o
c
u
m
e
n
t

i
s

c
o
p
y
r
i
g
h
t
e
d

b
y

t
h
e

A
m
e
r
i
c
a
n

P
s
y
c
h
o
l
o
g
i
c
a
l

A
s
s
o
c
i
a
t
i
o
n

o
r

o
n
e

o
f

i
t
s

a
l
l
i
e
d

p
u
b
l
i
s
h
e
r
s
.


T
h
i
s

a
r
t
i
c
l
e

i
s

i
n
t
e
n
d
e
d

s
o
l
e
l
y

f
o
r

t
h
e

p
e
r
s
o
n
a
l

u
s
e

o
f

t
h
e

i
n
d
i
v
i
d
u
a
l

u
s
e
r

a
n
d

i
s

n
o
t

t
o

b
e

d
i
s
s
e
m
i
n
a
t
e
d

b
r
o
a
d
l
y
.
582 PALEY, COX, BURCHINAL, AND PAYNE
narratives, in the form of contradictory, minimiz-
ing, excessively angry, or vague accounts of
attachment experiences, receive insecure classi-
fications. Thus, AAI classifications reflect an
individual's current working models or state of
mind with regards to childhood attachment
relationships, and it is that current state of mind,
rather than the reported positivity or negativity
of earlier attachment experiences, that we
expected to be associated with functioning in
marital relationships.
The adult attachment classifications yielded
by the AAI
1
secure, dismissing, and preoccu-
piedare considered to parallel the infant
attachment patterns of secure, avoidant, and
ambivalent, respectively, as identified by Ains-
worth, Blehar, Waters, and Wall (1978) in their
seminal work using the Strange Situation
paradigm. Within the insecure category, adults
classified as dismissing devalue the importance
of attachment relationships, whereas adults
classified as preoccupied remain overly in-
volved in past attachment experiences. Individu-
als classified as having a secure attachment
stance provide coherent, balanced, and rela-
tively objective accounts of positive and/or
negative childhood attachment experiences and
value the importance of attachment relation-
ships. More recently, researchers (e.g., Main &
Goldwyn, 1994; Pearson, Cohn, Cowan, &
Cowan, 1994) have distinguished between two
groups of secure individuals: those who convinc-
ingly describe positive childhood experiences
(continuous secure), and those who recount
difficult childhoods, but do so without either
discounting the potential negative impact of
such experiences or remaining entangled in such
experiences (earned secure).
Relationship of Attachment Working
Models to Marital Behavior
and Perceptions
Dismissing and Preoccupied
Attachment Stances
Individuals with insecure attachment models
have presumably experienced rejecting or incon-
sistent caregiving and, moreover, have not come
to terms with those earlier negative experiences.
Thus, one may expect some continuity between
their earlier caregiving experiences and their
behavior in marital interactions, as well as then-
perceptions about their marriage. Kobak, Ruck-
deschel, and Hazan (1994) have noted that
insecure individuals may be more likely to
anticipate negative responses from their marital
partners and have difficulty managing negative
emotion in marital interactions because of their
experiences of negative, rejecting, or inconsis-
tent caregiving. It is hypothesized, however, that
although individuals with a dismissing attach-
ment stance and those with a preoccupied stance
have both experienced less-than-optimal parent-
ing, they have developed different attachment
stances because of variations in their particular
early caregiving experiences (Ainsworth, 1982;
Cassidy, 1994; Main & Goldwyn, 1994). That is,
attachment researchers (Cassidy, 1994; Kobak
& Duemmler, 1994; Main, Kaplan, & Cassidy,
1985) have proposed that the particular nature of
an insecure individual's attachment experiences
may give rise to different constellations of
working models about relationships and, corre-
spondingly, different strategies for managing
affect in close relationships. Cassidy (1994) has
suggested that an avoidant attachment (analo-
gous to a dismissing stance in adulthood) arises
because the child's signals of negative affect
have likely been rejected by his or her caregiver
and he or she comes to expect that expressions
of emotional distress will drive others away.
Thus, the avoidant child cultivates a strategy of
minimizing displays of distress to maintain
proximity to an attachment figure. Conversely,
the child who has experienced inconsistent
caregiving develops an ambivalent attachment
(analogous to a preoccupied stance in adult-
hood), adopting a strategy of heightening
affective expressions to elicit a more reliable
response from an attachment figure. Cassidy had
noted that "the negative emotionality of the
insecure/ambivalent child may be exaggerated
and chronic because he recognizes that to relax
and be soothed by the presence of the attach-
ment figure is to run the risk of losing contact
with the inconsistently available parent" (p.
241).
Indeed, work on adult attachment style and
affect regulation has suggested that different
1
More recently, a third insecure category
unresolved/disorganizedhas been assigned to indi-
viduals who exhibit significant disorganization in
their thinking or discourse when discussing experi-
ences of loss or trauma. Such individuals are assigned
to a secondary best-fitting category of secure,
dismissing, or preoccupied, but are considered to
have an overall classification of insecure.
T
h
i
s

d
o
c
u
m
e
n
t

i
s

c
o
p
y
r
i
g
h
t
e
d

b
y

t
h
e

A
m
e
r
i
c
a
n

P
s
y
c
h
o
l
o
g
i
c
a
l

A
s
s
o
c
i
a
t
i
o
n

o
r

o
n
e

o
f

i
t
s

a
l
l
i
e
d

p
u
b
l
i
s
h
e
r
s
.


T
h
i
s

a
r
t
i
c
l
e

i
s

i
n
t
e
n
d
e
d

s
o
l
e
l
y

f
o
r

t
h
e

p
e
r
s
o
n
a
l

u
s
e

o
f

t
h
e

i
n
d
i
v
i
d
u
a
l

u
s
e
r

a
n
d

i
s

n
o
t

t
o

b
e

d
i
s
s
e
m
i
n
a
t
e
d

b
r
o
a
d
l
y
.
ATTACHMENT AND MARITAL FUNCTIONING
583
patterns of insecurity may lead to different
strategies for managing distress in close relation-
ships (Feeney et al., 1994; Fraley & Shaver,
1997; Simpson, Rholes, & Nelligan, 1992;
Simpson et al., 1996). Our goal was to explore
whether similar links would be found between
specific attachment stances that are based on
childhood working models and particular pat-
terns of affect regulation in marriage. We
expected that in terms of marital behavior,
dismissing spouses would exhibit less negative
affect than preoccupied spouses during marital
interactions, analogous to the "minimizing"
strategy of avoidant children. We also hypoth-
esized dismissing spouses to be more likely to
withdraw during conflict as a way of managing
any negative affect that threatened to emerge.
Conversely, we expected preoccupied spouses
to express less positive and more negative affect
than dismissing spouses, so as not to appear
"soothed" and "run the risk of losing contact"
with their partner. In exploring how dismissing
and preoccupied spouses might differ in their
marital perceptions, we focused on three vari-
ables: the extent to which spouses felt their
partners were responsive to their emotional
needs, the extent to which they expressed a
strong commitment to their marriage, and the
extent to which they expressed strong feelings
of love and attachment to their partner. In line
with the notion that dismissing spouses are
likely to minimize their emotional needs,
whereas preoccupied spouses are likely to
emphasize their emotional needs, we expected
that dismissing spouses, relative to preoccupied
spouses, would rate their partners as more
responsive to their needs (to do otherwise would
acknowledge their dependence on attachment
relationships) and express less commitment to
their relationship, and less intense feelings of
love and attachment.
Earned-Secure and Continuous-Secure
Attachment Stances
Individuals with secure working models are
presumed to have some confidence that they will
be responded to sensitively and supportively
and, thus, are expected to be more comfortable
with expressing their emotions to their romantic
partners (Kobak et al., 1994). As noted earlier,
researchers (e.g., Main & Goldwyn, 1994;
Pearson et al., 1994) have begun distinguishing
between two groups of secure individuals:
continuous-secure individuals, who provide
coherent accounts of generally positive child-
hood experiences, and earned-secure individu-
als, who recount difficult childhoods, but do so
in a thoughtful, reflective manner and neither
discount the potential negative impact of such
experiences nor remain entangled in those
experiences. Pearson et al. found that earned-
secure parents exhibited as much warmth in
their parenting as continuous-secure parents and
significantly more warmth than insecure parents
despite reporting more depression. More re-
cently, Phelps, Belsky, and Crnic (1998) demon-
strated that both earned-secure and continuous-
secure mothers exhibited more positive parenting
than insecure mothers under high-stress condi-
tions, although no such differences were de-
tected under low levels of stress. Such findings
led us to question how earned-secure individuals
would function in marital relationships. The
identification of individuals who, in spite of
apparently negative attachment experiences
earlier in life, may go on to function compe-
tently in close relationships in adulthood (i.e., as
parents and perhaps as spouses), would be an
important step in understanding mechanisms
that allow for departures from earlier negative
developmental trajectories.
The departure that earned-secure individuals
may make from earlier adversity is consistent
with Belsky and Nezworski's (1988) notion that
"if the nature and quality of care provided to the
child is changed and/or the child's or adult's
working model of self and of relationships is
modified, . . . one might anticipate change in
developmental trajectories and thus appropri-
ately speak of 'lawful discontinuity' " (p. 14).
Thus, we expected that despite their adverse
caregiving experiences, the coherent, secure
stance of earned-secure spouses would be the
important determinant of their marital function-
ing. That is, we hypothesized that earned-secure
spouses would not differ from continuous-
secure spouses in their expression of positive or
negative affect or tendency to withdraw during
marital interactions and that they would per-
ceive their marriages in generally positive ways,
similar to those of continuous-secure spouses.
Differences Between Secure
and Insecure Subgroups
Another question that arises is how each
secure subgroup might compare with each
T
h
i
s

d
o
c
u
m
e
n
t

i
s

c
o
p
y
r
i
g
h
t
e
d

b
y

t
h
e

A
m
e
r
i
c
a
n

P
s
y
c
h
o
l
o
g
i
c
a
l

A
s
s
o
c
i
a
t
i
o
n

o
r

o
n
e

o
f

i
t
s

a
l
l
i
e
d

p
u
b
l
i
s
h
e
r
s
.


T
h
i
s

a
r
t
i
c
l
e

i
s

i
n
t
e
n
d
e
d

s
o
l
e
l
y

f
o
r

t
h
e

p
e
r
s
o
n
a
l

u
s
e

o
f

t
h
e

i
n
d
i
v
i
d
u
a
l

u
s
e
r

a
n
d

i
s

n
o
t

t
o

b
e

d
i
s
s
e
m
i
n
a
t
e
d

b
r
o
a
d
l
y
.
584
PALEY, COX, BURCHINAL, AND PAYNE
insecure subgroup in terms of marital function-
ing. On the one hand, in line with the notion of
lawful discontinuity (Belsky & Nezworski,
1988) we anticipated that, in addition to
continuous-secure spouses, earned-secure
spouses would also differ from dismissing and
preoccupied individuals in their marital interac-
tions and perceptions, despite their similarly
adverse childhood caregiving experiences. How-
ever, because of the strategies that dismissing
and preoccupied individuals may use to mini-
mize or emphasize their distress, respectively,
predictions regarding differences between spe-
cific secure and insecure subgroups become less
straightforward. For example, although dismiss-
ing spouses' strategy of minimizing distress
might lead them to exhibit more positive and
less negative affect than preoccupied spouses, it
is unclear how they might compare with either
secure group. That is, would dismissing spouses'
minimizing strategy be so "effective" that they
might express as much or more positive affect
and as little or less negative affect than one or
both secure groups? Given the lack of empirical
findings in this area, we limited ourselves to
predicting that earned-secure and continuous-
secure spouses would express more positive and
less negative affect than preoccupied spouses
and engage in less withdrawal than dismissing
spouses. Regarding self-report measures, we
hypothesized that preoccupied spouses would
rate their partners as less emotionally responsive
than earned-secure or continuous-secure spouses,
whereas dismissing spouses would express less
commitment and love than either secure group.
Influence of Partner' s Attachment Stance
In addition to considering the relation be-
tween an individual's marital functioning and
their own working models of childhood relation-
ships, researchers have increasingly under-
scored the need to consider how a partner's
attachment models may relate to an individual's
marital behavior and perceptions as well.
Berman, Marcus, and Berman (1994) have
proposed that "for any couple, the nature of the
attachment . . . internal working model . . . of
each member of the couple both shapes and
responds to the behaviors of the partner, in a
complex interplay between overt behaviors and
the meaning each person ascribes to these
behaviors" (p. 227). Thus, if one spouse with an
insecure attachment stance anticipates indiffer-
ence or hostility from the other spouse and
consequently engages in more negative marital
behavior, the other spouse may be more likely to
view him or her more negatively and react with
their own negative behavior as well.
Again, although some studies have found
links between one partner's satisfaction or
behavior in the relationship and the other
partner's models of adult intimate relationships
(Collins & Read, 1990; Kobak & Hazan, 1991;
Mikulincer & Erev, 1991; Senchak & Leonard,
1992), there are limited data examining the link
between an individual's marital functioning and
his or her partner's working models of child-
hood relationships. Owens et al. (1995) found
that partners of individuals with secure child-
hood models were more likely to have coherent
models of their current romantic relationship
than partners of individuals with insecure
childhood models. Cohn et al. (1992) found that
couples consisting of insecure wives and secure
husbands exhibited better marital functioning
than couples consisting of insecure wives and
insecure husbands, although because marital
behavior was coded at the dyadic level, it is
unclear whether these differences were due to
husbands' behavior, wives' behavior, or both.
However, to our knowledge no studies have yet
explored differences among the various secure
(continuous and earned) and insecure (dismiss-
ing and preoccupied) attachment stances with
regard to their impact on the other spouse's
marital behavior and perceptions. Thus, in the
present study, we considered these to be an
exploratory set of analyses and did not make
specific predictions regarding differences among
secure and insecure subgroups.
In examining how an individual's marital
functioning might be related to his or her
partner's attachment stance, we also considered
whether these links might be different for secure
or insecure spouses. Thus, for example, might
secure spouses function well in their marriages
regardless of whether their partner had a secure
or insecure attachment stance, whereas insecure
spouses might be more likely compromised if
paired with an insecure partner rather than a
secure partner? Results from a study by Senchak
and Leonard (1992) actually suggest the con-
trary, as their findings indicated mat the marital
functioning of secure individuals was compro-
mised by being paired with insecure partners,
whereas insecure individuals, although in gen-
T
h
i
s

d
o
c
u
m
e
n
t

i
s

c
o
p
y
r
i
g
h
t
e
d

b
y

t
h
e

A
m
e
r
i
c
a
n

P
s
y
c
h
o
l
o
g
i
c
a
l

A
s
s
o
c
i
a
t
i
o
n

o
r

o
n
e

o
f

i
t
s

a
l
l
i
e
d

p
u
b
l
i
s
h
e
r
s
.


T
h
i
s

a
r
t
i
c
l
e

i
s

i
n
t
e
n
d
e
d

s
o
l
e
l
y

f
o
r

t
h
e

p
e
r
s
o
n
a
l

u
s
e

o
f

t
h
e

i
n
d
i
v
i
d
u
a
l

u
s
e
r

a
n
d

i
s

n
o
t

t
o

b
e

d
i
s
s
e
m
i
n
a
t
e
d

b
r
o
a
d
l
y
.
ATTACHMENT AND MARITAL FUNCTIONING 585
eral did not function as well in their marriages as
secure individuals, fared no worse when paired
with an insecure partner than when paired with a
secure partner. Thus, in the present study we
also explored whether an interaction effect
would be found between individuals' and their
partners' attachment stances. To address this
question, it was necessary to recombine the
more specific secure (continuous-secure and
earned-secure) and insecure (dismissing and
preoccupied) subgroups into overall secure and
insecure groups. Although it would have been
ideal to maintain the subgroup distinctions to
explore this final question, maintaining the finer
distinctions would yield extremely small num-
bers within certain group cells given the relatively
small proportion of preoccupied individuals in
our study, as is typical for nonclinical samples.
To summarize, two major questions were
addressed in the current study. First, are specific
attachment stances associated with particular
strategies for regulating affect in close relation-
ships, as evidenced in individuals' marital
behavior and perceptions? Specifically, would
preoccupied individuals exhibit less positive
affect, more negative affect, and less withdrawal
than would dismissing individuals during mari-
tal interactions? Regarding self-report mea-
sures, we expected that dismissing spouses,
relative to preoccupied spouses, would rate their
partners as more responsive to their needs and
express less commitment to their relationship
and less intense feelings of love and attachment.
We expected earned-secure individuals to func-
tion as well in their marriages as continuous-
secure individuals, as assessed both during
marital interactions and with self-report mea-
sures. We also expected both secure groups
would exhibit less withdrawal than dismissing
individuals and would express more positive
and less negative affect than preoccupied
individuals. Additionally, we hypothesized that
dismissing individuals would express less com-
mitment and less intense feelings of love,
whereas preoccupied individuals would per-
ceive their partners to be less emotionally
supportive than either continuous- or earned-
secure individuals on self-report measures of
marital quality. Finally, we explored whether
individuals' marital behavior and perceptions
related not only to their own working models of
childhood attachment relationships, but to their
partners' attachment models as well.
In examining these issues, we controlled for
three different factors in predicting marital
functioning. First, as a broad demographic
control, we considered the role of individuals'
education level in predicting marital behavior
and perceptions. Second, because there is ample
evidence that couples' marital satisfaction de-
clines over time (Cowan, Cowan, Heming, &
Miller, 1991), we controlled for the length of
time that couples had been married. Finally, as
numerous studies have documented the links
between attachment and depressive symptoms
(Armsden, McCauley, Greenberg, Burke, &
Mitchell, 1990; Kobak, Sudler, & Gamble,
1991) and depression and marital discord
(Barnett & Gotlib, 1988; Beach, Sandeen, &
O'Leary, 1990), we controlled for individuals'
level of depression to examine the possibility
that the relationship between attachment and
marital functioning might be largely artifactual.
Method
Sample
The sample for the present study was part of a
larger sample of 138 couples recruited prior to the
birth of their first child for a larger investigation of
couples' transition to parenthood. Couples were
drawn from prenatal classes in four counties of a
largely rural area of the southeastern United States.
Of those couples contacted, 72% agreed to participate
in the study; participating and nonparticipating
couples did not differ on demographic variables. The
mean age was 28.3 years (range = 1941) for
husbands and 27.2 years (range = 18-35) for wives.
In this sample, 97% of the couples were Caucasian,
with the remaining 3% being African American, as is
representative of this rural area. Husbands' average
years of education was 13.9 years (range = 9-22),
and wives' average years of education was 13.8 years
(range = 8-18). The average monthly income for
husbands was $1,465 (range = $652-2,501) and for
wives was $917 (range = $250-2,501). Couples had
been married an average of 41.3 months (range = 3 -
204 months), and this was the first marriage for 88%
of the husbands and 85% of the wives.
Procedures
Couples were interviewed at their homes prior to
the birth of their first child. Husbands and wives were
administered the AAI separately and by different
interviewers. Each spouse then completed a battery of
self-report questionnaires, which included several
measures of marital satisfaction and perceptions of
T
h
i
s

d
o
c
u
m
e
n
t

i
s

c
o
p
y
r
i
g
h
t
e
d

b
y

t
h
e

A
m
e
r
i
c
a
n

P
s
y
c
h
o
l
o
g
i
c
a
l

A
s
s
o
c
i
a
t
i
o
n

o
r

o
n
e

o
f

i
t
s

a
l
l
i
e
d

p
u
b
l
i
s
h
e
r
s
.


T
h
i
s

a
r
t
i
c
l
e

i
s

i
n
t
e
n
d
e
d

s
o
l
e
l
y

f
o
r

t
h
e

p
e
r
s
o
n
a
l

u
s
e

o
f

t
h
e

i
n
d
i
v
i
d
u
a
l

u
s
e
r

a
n
d

i
s

n
o
t

t
o

b
e

d
i
s
s
e
m
i
n
a
t
e
d

b
r
o
a
d
l
y
.
586 PALEY, COX, BURCHINAL, AND PAYNE
one's partner, as well as a measure of depressive
symptoms. Finally, couples participated in a marital
problem-solving task, in which they were asked to
discuss and to arrive at some resolution regarding a
major area of disagreement in their relationship.
Sample sizes may vary slightly by analysis because of
incomplete data for a small number of participants.
Measures
AAI. The AAI is a methodology designed to
assess an individual's current stance toward attach-
ment (Main & Goldwyn, 1994). Participants are
asked to describe their childhood relationships with
each of their parents (and/or other relevant attachment
figures) and to provide examples that support those
descriptions; they are also asked to recount any
experiences of rejection, loss, or separation and to
describe how their parents responded when they were
upset or sick. In addition, participants are asked to
consider the causes of their parents' behavior and the
impact that their relationships with their parents had
on their development.
Interviews are audiotaped and transcribed and then
coded in three phases using the written transcripts.
First, ratings are assigned regarding the participant's
probable experiences with each parent, including the
degree to which a parent was loving, rejecting,
neglecting, and role reversing. It is important to note
that these ratings reflect the coder's best estimation of
how the participant was treated by each parent during
childhood, and coders do not necessarily assume
participants' descriptions of their parents are veridi-
cal. For example, an individual who initially states
that their parent was never rejecting in any way, but
then provides several examples to the contrary, would
receive at least a moderate (if not high) score on the
rejecting scale. Second, ratings are made regarding
various dimensions of the participant's current state
of mind with respect to attachment including
preoccupying anger, idealization, and derogation of
attachment. Finally, on the basis of their current state
of mind toward attachment and the coherency with
which they recount past experiences, individuals are
classified as secure, dismissing, preoccupied, unre-
solved with respect to loss or trauma, or cannot
classify.
According to the classification system for the AAI
(Main & Goldwyn, 1994), secure individuals value
attachment relationships and are able to reflect on the
importance of attachment-related experiences but
remain coherent and relatively objective when
considering such relationships. Dismissing individu-
als minimize the importance of attachment relation-
ships, often either denying the occurrence of any
negative childhood experiences or disavowing the
impact of any negative experiences that are acknowl-
edged. Preoccupied individuals remain overly in-
volved in attachment relationships, manifested in
either excessive anger toward attachment figures or a
continuing sense of wanting to please attachment
figures. An unresolved classification is assigned to
individuals who exhibit significant disorganization or
disorientation in their thinking or discourse when
discussing experiences of loss or trauma. In rare
cases, individuals are assigned to a cannot classify
category when they do not exhibit any single
consistent mental state or strategy when discussing
attachment-related experiences (Main & Hesse, 1994,
as cited by Main & Goldwyn, 1994). AAI classifica-
tions have been found to be unrelated to measures of
nonattachment-related autobiographical memory, in-
telligence, and social desirability (Bakermans-
Kranenburg & van IJzendoorn, 1993; Sagi et al.,
1994).
Interviews were coded by an experienced rater who
received extensive training in the AAI scoring
procedures from Mary Main (the first author of the
classification system for the AAI). This rater demon-
strated good reliability with an expert coder and
collaborator of Main's (E. Hesse) on a separate
sample of interviews (coded at the same time as
interviews for the current investigation) for both
overall secure versus insecure classifications (93%
agreement) and for the five more specific classifica-
tions (87% agreement).
For the purposes of our study, secure individuals
were further categorized into continuous-secure
(individuals who convincingly describe what seem to
have been genuinely positive childhood attachment
relationships) and earned-secure (individuals who
recount difficult childhood attachment relationships,
but do so in a coherent manner) subgroups, on the
basis of a revision of the procedure suggested by
Pearson et al. (1994). Those individuals classified as
F3A
2
or F4A were considered to be continuous-
2
According to Main and Goldwyn's (1994) Adult
Attachment Scoring and Classification system, secure
classifications are designated with an F. Individuals
classified as F3A are considered to be secure/
autonomous and are described as having primarily
supportive caregiving experiences during childhood.
Individuals classified as F4A are described as having
generally supportive caregiving experiences, al-
though there may have been some difficulty in their
relationship with one or both parents. Other F
classifications may include individuals who have
some tendency toward dismissal of or preoccupation
with attachment, but who nonetheless consider
attachment relationships important in their lives and
are able to remain fairly objective with regards to any
specific attachment-related experiences.
T
h
i
s

d
o
c
u
m
e
n
t

i
s

c
o
p
y
r
i
g
h
t
e
d

b
y

t
h
e

A
m
e
r
i
c
a
n

P
s
y
c
h
o
l
o
g
i
c
a
l

A
s
s
o
c
i
a
t
i
o
n

o
r

o
n
e

o
f

i
t
s

a
l
l
i
e
d

p
u
b
l
i
s
h
e
r
s
.


T
h
i
s

a
r
t
i
c
l
e

i
s

i
n
t
e
n
d
e
d

s
o
l
e
l
y

f
o
r

t
h
e

p
e
r
s
o
n
a
l

u
s
e

o
f

t
h
e

i
n
d
i
v
i
d
u
a
l

u
s
e
r

a
n
d

i
s

n
o
t

t
o

b
e

d
i
s
s
e
m
i
n
a
t
e
d

b
r
o
a
d
l
y
.
ATTACHMENT AND MARITAL FUNCTIONING 587
secure; individuals classified into any of the other F
subcategories but who had ratings of 5 or greater on
the Loving scale and less than 5 on the Rejecting,
Neglecting, and Role-Reversing scales for both
mothers and fathers were also considered to be
continuous secure. Individuals classified into any of
the F categories other than F3A or F4A and who had
ratings of less than 5 on the Loving scale or 5 or
greater on the Rejecting, Neglecting, or Role-
Reversing scales for either mother or father were
considered to be earned secure.
To check the validity of this procedure for
differentiating these two subgroups, individuals
identified as earned secure were compared with
continuous-secure, preoccupied, and dismissing indi-
viduals with regards to the ratings they were assigned
on the Loving, Rejecting, Neglecting, and Role-
Reversal experience scales of the AAI. Analyses of
variance (ANOVAs) were conducted separately for
husbands and wives, followed by planned contrasts
between the following groups: (a) earned secure
versus continuous secure, (b) earned secure versus
dismissing, and (c) earned secure versus preoccupied.
It was expected that earned-secure individuals would
be rated as having had childhood experiences with
their mothers and fathers that were more negative
than continuous-secure individuals but comparable
with those of preoccupied and dismissing individuals.
Results of these analyses, displayed in Table I,
indicated there were significant differences in the
predicted direction between earned-secure and con-
tinuous-secure individuals on the AAI Childhood
Experience scales. In addition, the few differences
detected between earned-secure and either dismissing
or preoccupied individuals were generally in the
direction of earned-secure individuals being rated as
having had more negative experiences than either of
the insecure groups. Such results support the validity
of our procedure to distinguish between continuous-
secure and earned-secure subgroups within the larger
Table 1
Mean Scores on Adult Attachment Interview (AAI) Experience Scales by AAI Classification
AAI
Experience
Scale
Mother
Loving
M
SD
Rejecting
M
SD
Neglecting
M
SD
Role Rev.
M
SD
Father
Loving
M
SD
Rejecting
M
SD
Neglecting
M
SD
Role Rev.
M
SD
C-S
(n = 41)
7.20
a
1.08
2.17.
1.00
1.12.
0.40
1.98
a
,
b
1.08
6.46
a
1.38
2.73
a
1.03
2.05
a
1.24
1.44,
0.84
Husbands' AAI
E-S
(n = 20)
4.89
b
1.63
4.29
b
1.72
3.16b
2.61
2.68^
1.63
3.89
b
1.79
5.50^
1.54
3.68
b
,
c
2.03
1.16
a
0.37
Dism.
(n = 47)
4.79
b
1.16
3.60,,
1.48
1.47.
1.08
1.94b
1.29
3.79b
1.63
4.68
b
1.90
2.81.*
2.05
1.32.
0.81
Pre.
(it = 10)
4.60,,
1.66
4.00b
2.16
2.70b
2.41
3.70
c
2.00
2.80b
1.81
6.40
c
2.63
4.89
C
3.62
2.30,,
1.89
C-S
(n = 48)
7.17.
1.14
2.27
a
0.92
1.19
a
0.45
2.29
a
1.18
6.88
a
1.27
2.54
a
1.18
1.92
a
0.96
1.58,,,
0.82
Wives'
E-S
( = 30)
5.41
b
2.06
3.21
b
2.01
2.45
b
2.16
3.79b
1.88
4.07b
2.31
4.50b
1.99
4.86
b
2.88
2.38
b
1.50
AAI
Dism.
( = 28)
4.61
C
1.45
3.56
b
2.01
2.18
b
2.14
2.30
a
1.44
4.33
b
1.57
4.15b
2.38
3.33
C
2.29
1.70,,,
0.82
Pre.
( = 9)
5.33
b
,
c
1.32
3.44b
2.13
2.00
ab
2.00
4.11
b
1.83
4.25
b
1.75
4.75
b
2.92
1.78.
0.97
2.33
b
,
c
1.66
Note. Means with different subscripts are significantly different atp < .05. C-S = Continuous Secure;
Earned Secure; Dism. = Dismissing; Pre. = Preoccupied; Role Rev. = Role Reversal.
E-S =
T
h
i
s

d
o
c
u
m
e
n
t

i
s

c
o
p
y
r
i
g
h
t
e
d

b
y

t
h
e

A
m
e
r
i
c
a
n

P
s
y
c
h
o
l
o
g
i
c
a
l

A
s
s
o
c
i
a
t
i
o
n

o
r

o
n
e

o
f

i
t
s

a
l
l
i
e
d

p
u
b
l
i
s
h
e
r
s
.


T
h
i
s

a
r
t
i
c
l
e

i
s

i
n
t
e
n
d
e
d

s
o
l
e
l
y

f
o
r

t
h
e

p
e
r
s
o
n
a
l

u
s
e

o
f

t
h
e

i
n
d
i
v
i
d
u
a
l

u
s
e
r

a
n
d

i
s

n
o
t

t
o

b
e

d
i
s
s
e
m
i
n
a
t
e
d

b
r
o
a
d
l
y
.
588 PALEY, COX, BURCHINAL, AND PAYNE
secure classification.
3
These results are also largely
consistent with those obtained by Phelps et al. (1998).
On the basis of these classification procedures, the
distribution of AAI classifications for husbands and
wives was as follows: for husbandscontinuous
secure = 41 (30%), earned secure = 20 (15%),
dismissing = 47 (34%), preoccupied = 10 (7%),
unresolved = 1 7 (12%), and cannot classify = 3
(2%); for wivescontinuous secure = 48 (35%),
earned secure = 30 (22%), dismissing = 28 (20%),
preoccupied = 9 (7%), unresolved = 20 (14%), and
cannot classify = 3 (2%). Although the distribution of
AAI classification for the wives in our sample was
comparable with the distributions found in other
nonclinical samples, the percentage of dismissing
husbands in our sample was somewhat higher than
what has been found in other studies reviewed by van
IJzendoorn and Bakermans-Kranenburg (1996) in a
recent meta-analysis. However, this same meta-
analysis also found that among studies focusing on
lower socioeconomic status samples, the dismissing
category was overrepresented. Although many of
these studies focused exclusively on women, Critten-
den, Partridge, and Claussen (1991) found that 30%
of the men in their study were classified as
dismissing, a proportion very similar to that obtained
in our sample. It may be that for individuals faced
with social and economic adversity, it is adaptive to
not spend a great deal of time reflecting on
attachment-related experiences, but rather to assume
a fairly stoic, unemotional manner, and in our sample,
this may have been a strategy especially favored by
men.
Spouses who were assigned a primary classifica-
tion of either unresolved or cannot classify were not
included in subsequent analyses. Currently, there is
little known about the implications of these two
attachment classifications for functioning in close
adult relationships, and thus it was not possible to
make clear predictions regarding patterns of marital
behavior and perceptions that might be associated
with such attachment stances.
Because previous research (van IJzendoorn &
Bakersmans-Kranenburg, 1996) has suggested some
evidence for assortative mating with regard to adult
attachment stance (e.g., secure individuals are more
likely to pair with secure mates and insecure
individuals are more likely to pair with insecure
mates) and because assortative mating could influ-
ence study results, we examined whether there was
any evidence for such a pattern among the four AAI
groups of interest in this study. Chi-square analyses
revealed that there were no differences among the
four AAI groups in terms of their likelihood of
marrying a secure or insecure spouse, either for
wives, x
2
d2, N= 115) = 11.53, p = .48, or for
husbands, x
2
(12,iV = 118) = 9.91, p = .62.
We also examined whether there were any
differences among the four attachment groups with
regards to several demographic variables, including
education level, occupational level, income, length of
marriage, and depression. ANOVAs revealed a
significant main effect only for husband's depression,
F(3, 112) = 4.72, p < .01. Planned comparisons
indicated the preoccupied husbands reported signifi-
cantly more depressed symptoms than did continuous-
secure husbands, F(l , 112) = 12.10, p < .01;
earned-secure husbands, F(l , 112) = 9.90, p < .01;
and dismissing husbands, F(\, 112) = 13.41,/? < .01.
Perceptions of marital relationship. Husbands
and wives completed all self-report measures sepa-
rately. Two subscales of the Four-Factor Scale of
Intimate Relations (Braiker & Kelley, 1976) were
administered to assess spouses' commitment to their
relationship (Ambivalence) and feelings of positive
regard for their partner and their relationship (Love).
The Intimacy subscale of the Personal Assessment of
Intimate Relationships (PAIR; Schaefer & Olson,
1981) assesses the degree to which spouses view their
partners as emotionally sensitive and supportive.
Marital interaction. Couples were videotaped at
home during a 15-min problem-solving task in which
they were asked to discuss and attempt to resolve a
major source of disagreement in their marriage. Prior
to the interaction, each spouse completed the
Relationship Problem Inventory (Knox, 1971), rating
how much each of several possible areas of
disagreement was a problem in their relationship.
Spouses were then asked to review their lists of
problem areas together and to select a topic they
considered to be a primary source of disagreement in
their relationship. After a couple had selected a topic
together, their problem list was removed, and they
were instructed to discuss the problem area they had
chosen and to attempt to come to a mutually
satisfying resolution to the problem.
These videotaped interactions were rated by
trained observers, who were unaware of any other
information about the couples, using a subset of codes
from the Interactional Dimensions Coding System
(Julien, Markman, & Lindahl, 1989). Lindahl and
Markman (1990) have noted how difficulties in affect
regulation may be reflected in the way emotions are
expressed in family interactions: "Communication
behaviors may be used to regulate one's own and
3
Main and Goldwyn (1994) have suggested a more
stringent procedure for identifying subgroups within
the larger secure category, which results in the
exclusion of some individuals from either a continu-
ous-secure or earned-secure subgroup. Analyses
conducted using Main and Goldwyn's procedure
yielded results similar to those obtained with the
procedure outlined above. For the purposes of this
article, we present results that are based on the more
inclusive procedure, so as to represent all secure
participants in the sample.
T
h
i
s

d
o
c
u
m
e
n
t

i
s

c
o
p
y
r
i
g
h
t
e
d

b
y

t
h
e

A
m
e
r
i
c
a
n

P
s
y
c
h
o
l
o
g
i
c
a
l

A
s
s
o
c
i
a
t
i
o
n

o
r

o
n
e

o
f

i
t
s

a
l
l
i
e
d

p
u
b
l
i
s
h
e
r
s
.


T
h
i
s

a
r
t
i
c
l
e

i
s

i
n
t
e
n
d
e
d

s
o
l
e
l
y

f
o
r

t
h
e

p
e
r
s
o
n
a
l

u
s
e

o
f

t
h
e

i
n
d
i
v
i
d
u
a
l

u
s
e
r

a
n
d

i
s

n
o
t

t
o

b
e

d
i
s
s
e
m
i
n
a
t
e
d

b
r
o
a
d
l
y
.
ATTACHMENT AND MARITAL FUNCTIONING 589
other family member's affect, as well as be signs or
barometers of affect regulation processes.... In
distressed families, the presence of dysfunctional
processes of affect regulation will be evidenced in
communication patterns and in modes of emotional
expression" (pp. 99-100). Husbands and wives were
rated separately on the degree of positive and
negative affect they expressed and the amount of
withdrawal they exhibited during the problem-
solving discussion. Interrater reliability was calcu-
lated for 20% of the interactions and found to be
acceptable (positive affect: r = .67; negative affect:
r - .88; withdrawal: r = .90).
Resul t s
Relationship Between Individuals' Marital
Functioning and Their Own
Attachment Stance
Our first set of analyses was designed to
examine differences within and between secure
(earned secure and continuous secure) and
insecure subgroups (dismissing and preoccupied
spouses) in terms of their marital behavior and
perceptions. Analyses were conducted in the
following manner. First, we conducted multivar-
iate analyses of covariance (MANCOVAs)
separately for wives and husbands to look at the
relationship between an individual's attachment
stance and his or her own marital behavior
(positive affect, negative affect, and with-
drawal), while controlling for the individual's
education level, length of the couple's marriage,
and the individual's depressive symptoms. A
similar set of MANCOVAs was conducted to
look at husbands' and wives' marital perceptions
(Ambivalence, Love, and Intimacy) as they
related to their own attachment stance. Planned
comparisons were then conducted to examine
differences within (earned secure vs. continuous
secure; dismissing vs. preoccupied) and be-
tween (earned secure vs. dismissing; earned
secure vs. preoccupied; continuous secure vs.
dismissing; continuous secure vs. preoccupied)
secure and insecure subgroups.
A MANCOVA revealed significant associa-
tions between wives' attachment stance and
their behavior during marital interactions, F(9,
253) = 2.56, p < .01. Follow-up univariate
analyses of covariance (ANCOVAs) revealed
significant effects for wives' positive affect,
4
F(3,106) = 3.79, p < .02, and withdrawal, F(3,
106) = 2.77, p < .05. Planned comparisons (see
Table 2 for group means) indicated that
preoccupied wives expressed less positive affect
than both continuous-secure wives, F(l , 106) =
6.55, p < .02, and earned-secure wives, F(l ,
106) = 10.56, p < .01. Preoccupied wives also
showed a trend toward expressing less positive
affect than dismissing wives, F(l , 106) = 3.70,
p < .06. Dismissing wives exhibited a trend
towards expressing less positive affect than
earned-secure wives, F(l , 106) = 3.55,p < .07.
Dismissing wives also engaged in more with-
drawal than both continuous-secure wives, F(l ,
106) = 5.39, p < .03, and earned-secure wives,
F(l , 106) = 6.59, p<.02.
5
A separate MANCOVA indicated that there
was no relation between wives' marital percep-
tions and their own attachment stance, although
wives who reported higher levels of depressive
symptoms perceived their husbands as less
emotionally responsive (Intimacy), F(l , 104) =
9.30, p<. 003.
For husbands, there was no relationship
between their own attachment stance and either
their marital behavior or perceptions, although
husbands' with higher levels of education
exhibited more positive affect, F(l , 107) =
20.42, p < .001; less negative affect,
F(l , 107) = 5.63, p < .02; less withdrawal in
their marital interaction, F(l , 107) = 11.34,/? <
.01; reported feeling greater commitment (Am-
bivalence) to their marriages, F(l , 107) = 9.88,
p < .01; and perceived their wives as more
emotionally responsive (Intimacy), F(l , 107) =
13.66, p < .001, than men with lower levels of
4
To examine the possibility that secure and
insecure subgroups might differ with regard to the
balance between positive affect and negative affect,
we conducted additional analyses using the ratio
between the amount of positive and negative affect
the individual exhibited during marital interactions.
However, analyses revealed no significant differences
between or among secure and insecure subgroups
with regard to the ratio between positive and negative
affect.
5
Although there was no evidence of an overall
pattern of assortative mating for the four attachment
groups, we considered more specifically the possibil-
ity that eamed-secure wives might be exhibiting more
positive affect and less withdrawal than dismissing
wives because they were more likely to be married to
secure husbands than were dismissing wives. A
chi-square analysis in fact revealed there was a trend
suggesting that earned-secure women were more
likely to be paired with secure husbands than were
dismissing women, x
2
(l> N ~ 58) = 3.52,/) < .07.
T
h
i
s

d
o
c
u
m
e
n
t

i
s

c
o
p
y
r
i
g
h
t
e
d

b
y

t
h
e

A
m
e
r
i
c
a
n

P
s
y
c
h
o
l
o
g
i
c
a
l

A
s
s
o
c
i
a
t
i
o
n

o
r

o
n
e

o
f

i
t
s

a
l
l
i
e
d

p
u
b
l
i
s
h
e
r
s
.


T
h
i
s

a
r
t
i
c
l
e

i
s

i
n
t
e
n
d
e
d

s
o
l
e
l
y

f
o
r

t
h
e

p
e
r
s
o
n
a
l

u
s
e

o
f

t
h
e

i
n
d
i
v
i
d
u
a
l

u
s
e
r

a
n
d

i
s

n
o
t

t
o

b
e

d
i
s
s
e
m
i
n
a
t
e
d

b
r
o
a
d
l
y
.
590
PALEY, COX, BURCHINAL, AND PAYNE
Table 2
Relation of Wives'Marital Behavior to Their Own Adult Attachment Stance
Wives'
marital
behavior
Positive affect
Withdrawal
Continuous-
secure
(Group 1,
n = 48)
M SD
5.73 1.59
2.33 1.49
Wives' attachment stance
Earned-secure
(Group 2,
n = 29)
M SD
6.18 1.36
2.21 0.99
Dismissing
(Group 3,
n = 28)
M SD
5.43 1.73
3.11 1.75
Preoccupied
(Group 4,
= 9)
M SD
4.22 1.64
3.00 1.41
Planned contrasts:
ANCOVA
Groups
1 > 4
2 > 3
2 > 4
3 > 4
1 <3
2 < 3
6.55
3.55
10.56
3.70
5.39
6.59
P
<m
<.07
<.OO2
<.06
<.03
<.02
Note. ANCOVA = analysis of covariance.
a
dfo for all planned comparisons are (1,106).
education. Additionally, for husbands who
reported higher levels of depressive symptoms,
there were trends for these husbands to report
feeling less commitment to their marriages,
F(l , 107) = 3.82, p < .06, and to perceive their
wives as less emotionally supportive, F(l ,
107) = 3.35, p<. 07.
Relationship Between Individuals' Marital
Functioning and Partners'
Attachment Stance
We conducted MANCOVAs separately for
wives and husbands to look at the relationship
between partners' attachment stance and indi-
viduals' marital behavior and perceptions, while
controlling for both husbands' and wives'
education levels, length of couple's marriage,
and both husbands' and wives' depressive
symptoms.
For wives' marital behavior, a MANCOVA
revealed that the association between wives'
behavior during marital interactions and their
husbands' attachment stance fell short of signifi-
cance, F(9, 251) = 1.63, p = .11. Although the
MANCOVA was not statistically significant,
exploratory univariate analyses were pursued, as
we felt there was heuristic merit in examining
whether husbands' attachment stance might
relate to specific marital behaviors of wives.
Follow-up univariate ANCOVAs revealed sig-
nificant main effects of husbands' attachment
stance for wives' positive affect, F(3, 105) =
3.18, p < .03, and negative affect, F(3, 105) =
3.12, p < .03. Planned comparisons (see Table 3
for group means) indicated that wives of
dismissing husbands expressed less positive
affect, F(l , 105) = 7.81, p < .01, and more
negative affect, F(l , 105) = 9.27, p < .01, than
wives of continuous-secure husbands. Addition-
ally, wives of earned-secure husbands also
expressed less positive affect than wives of
continuous-secure husbands, F(l , 105) = 4.60,
p< .04.
A MANCOVA revealed significant associa-
tions between husbands' attachment stance and
their wives marital perceptions, F(9, 248) =
2.06, p < .04. Follow-up ANCOVAs revealed a
significant main effect of husbands' attachment
stance for wives' commitment (Ambivalence),
F(3,104) = 4.44, p < .01. Planned comparisons
indicated that wives of dismissing husbands
expressed less commitment to their relationship
than did wives of continuous-secure husbands,
F(l , 104) = 12.75,/? < .01. Additionally, there
was a trend for wives of preoccupied husbands,
F(l , 104) = 3.19, p < .08, and wives of
earned-secure husbands, F(l , 104) = 2.85, p <
.10, to express less commitment than wives of
continuous-secure husbands. For husbands, there
were no effects of wives' attachment stance for
husbands' marital behavior or perceptions.
Individuals' Marital Behavior and
Perceptions in Relation to the Interaction
Between Their Own and Their
Partners'Attachment Stance
We conducted a final set of analyses to
determine whether the interaction between an
individual's attachment stance and their part-
ner's attachment stance related to their marital
T
h
i
s

d
o
c
u
m
e
n
t

i
s

c
o
p
y
r
i
g
h
t
e
d

b
y

t
h
e

A
m
e
r
i
c
a
n

P
s
y
c
h
o
l
o
g
i
c
a
l

A
s
s
o
c
i
a
t
i
o
n

o
r

o
n
e

o
f

i
t
s

a
l
l
i
e
d

p
u
b
l
i
s
h
e
r
s
.


T
h
i
s

a
r
t
i
c
l
e

i
s

i
n
t
e
n
d
e
d

s
o
l
e
l
y

f
o
r

t
h
e

p
e
r
s
o
n
a
l

u
s
e

o
f

t
h
e

i
n
d
i
v
i
d
u
a
l

u
s
e
r

a
n
d

i
s

n
o
t

t
o

b
e

d
i
s
s
e
m
i
n
a
t
e
d

b
r
o
a
d
l
y
.
ATTACHMENT AND MARITAL FUNCTIONING 591
Table 3
Relation of Wives' Marital Behavior and Perceptions to Husbands 'Attachment Stance
Wives' variable
Wives' marital behavior
1
Positive affect
Negative affect
Wives' marital perceptions'
1
Ambivalence
Husbands'
Continuous-secure
(Group 1,
n =
M
6.10
1.98
1.33
41)
SD
1.45
1.04
0.38
attachment stance
Earned-secure
(Group 2,
n =
M
5.16
2.42
1.78
:
19)
SD
1.42
0.96
1.10
Dismissing
(Group 3,
n = 45)
M SD
5.31 1.68
2.82 1.59
1.95 1.10
Preoccupied
(Group 4,
n = 9)
M SD
5.44 1.33
2.44 1.24
2.00 0.75
Planned contrasts:
ANCOVA
Groups
1 > 2
1 >3
1 < 3
1 <2
1 < 3
1 <4
F p
4.60 <.O4
7.81 <.01
9.27 <.01
2.85 <.10
12.75 <.01
3.19 <.08
Note. ANCOVA = analysis of covariance.
*df& for planned comparisons are (1,105).
b
dfa for planned comparisons are (1,104).
behavior or perceptions. As noted earlier, it was
necessary to combine attachment subgroups into
overall secure and insecure groups to have suf-
ficient cell sizes for these analyses. MANCOVAs
revealed no effects for the interaction be-
tween own and partner's attachment stance on
either wives' or husbands' marital behavior or
perceptions.
Discussion
The link between marital distress and difficul-
ties in managing affect has been well docu-
mented (Gottman, 1994; Markman & Kraft,
1989). Gottman has noted that compared with
satisfied couples, dissatisfied couples exhibit
more negative affect and less positive affect. In
the attachment literature, several lines of re-
search suggest that earlier attachment experi-
ences play a significant role in shaping how
individuals come to manage their emotional
experiences in peer (Cohn, 1990; Kobak &
Sceery, 1988), parent-child (Kobak, Cole,
Ferenz-Gillies, & Fleming, 1993), and romantic
(Simpson et al., 1996) relationships. These
findings from both the marital and attachment
literature suggest that an exploration of differ-
ences in the ability to regulate emotions as a
function of particular attachment experiences
may illuminate one possible pathway between
earlier parent-child difficulties and problems in
later intimate relationships. Thus, the present
study was aimed at expanding on previous
research by examining the link between individu-
als' ability to regulate their affect in the context
of their marital relationship and both their own
and their partners' stance toward attachment, as
reflected by particular constellations of working
models of childhood relationships.
Although in analyses comparing specific
attachment subgroups we found no association
between attachment stance and the ability to
regulate affect during marital interactions for
husbands, wives' attachment stance was some-
what related to their marital behavior. As
predicted, earned-secure wives regulated their
affect as well as continuous-secure wives in
their marital problem-solving interactions. More-
over, both earned-secure and continuous-secure
wives expressed more positive affect than
preoccupied wives and less withdrawal than
dismissing wives. Previous studies using the
AAI have generally included earned-secure
individuals along with those who are judged to
have had genuinely positive caregiving experi-
ences (continuous secure) in one overall secure
category. Consequently, the notion that it is the
coherency, rather than the valence, of individu-
als' accounts' of earlier caregiving experiences
that is predictive of functioning in other close
relationships has remained largely untested. Our
findings that earned-secure wives regulated their
affect as well as continuous-secure wives and
more competently than dismissing and preoccu-
pied wives provide partial support for this
notion.
Our findings for earned-secure wives are
consistent with both Pearson et al.'s (1994)
finding that earned-secure individuals exhibit
more competent parenting than insecure indi-
T
h
i
s

d
o
c
u
m
e
n
t

i
s

c
o
p
y
r
i
g
h
t
e
d

b
y

t
h
e

A
m
e
r
i
c
a
n

P
s
y
c
h
o
l
o
g
i
c
a
l

A
s
s
o
c
i
a
t
i
o
n

o
r

o
n
e

o
f

i
t
s

a
l
l
i
e
d

p
u
b
l
i
s
h
e
r
s
.


T
h
i
s

a
r
t
i
c
l
e

i
s

i
n
t
e
n
d
e
d

s
o
l
e
l
y

f
o
r

t
h
e

p
e
r
s
o
n
a
l

u
s
e

o
f

t
h
e

i
n
d
i
v
i
d
u
a
l

u
s
e
r

a
n
d

i
s

n
o
t

t
o

b
e

d
i
s
s
e
m
i
n
a
t
e
d

b
r
o
a
d
l
y
.
592
PALEY, COX, BURCHINAL, AND PAYNE
viduals and with previous research (e.g., Rutter
& Quinton, 1984) that suggests that although
there is a significant association between
disturbed parent-child relationships earlier in
life and marital difficulties in adulthood, there
are clearly discontinuities in this association as
well. One question that arises concerns the
process or processes that might allow some
individuals with difficult childhoods to develop
a secure attachment stance. In describing the
notion of lawful discontinuity, Belsky and
Nezworski (1988) have noted that "even if
some developmental trajectories pose risks to
the developing child, these risks can be re-
duced" (p. 14). One way such risks might be
reduced is through participation in other healthier
relationships (Belsky & Cassidy, 1994) that
provide evidence that close relationships are not
necessarily and uniformly painful or disappoint-
ing, and, thus, lead to the revision of working
models in a more positive direction. Sroufe
(1988) has emphasized that "the idea of
working models means both that such models
are active constructions forged over time and
they are subject to change" (p. 22). Earned-
secure individuals may be a particularly impor-
tant group for further study, allowing research-
ers to examine some of the processes by which
the intergenerational transmission of discordant
family relationships might be disrupted.
Regarding our findings for dismissing and
preoccupied spouses, our prediction that these
two groups would exhibit different patterns of
affect regulation in their marital behavior did not
receive a great deal of support. Although
preoccupied wives exhibited a trend toward
expressing less positive affect than dismissing
wives, these two groups did not differ in their
levels of negative affect or withdrawal from
their spouses. Our finding that preoccupied
wives expressed less positive affect than dismiss-
ing wives is consistent with the notion that
because of earlier attachment experiences,
preoccupied individuals may come to expect
that expressing positive emotions is not an
effective way of maintaining the attention of an
attachment figure. However, the lack of more
consistent differences between dismissing and
preoccupied wives in terms of their marital
behavior raises the question of how meaningful
these subgroup distinctions are with regards to
how insecure individuals function in marital
relationships. Alternatively, it may be that these
two groups did not differ in their expression of
negative affect or withdrawal because they used
similar strategies in the service of different
goals. For example, dismissing wives may
express negative affect in hopes of pushing their
partner away, thus abbreviating an emotionally
arousing discussion, whereas preoccupied wives
may express negative affect to provoke a
response from their partner and to maintain their
partner's engagement in the interaction. Another
possibility is that dismissing and preoccupied
individuals may differ with regards to the
particular kinds of negative affect they exhibit
during marital conflicts. That is, dismissing
individuals, by definition, regard attachment
relationships in a dismissive manner, and some
may even describe attachment relationships in a
ridiculing or derisive manner (Main & Gold-
wyn, 1994). Such behavior may be related to
Gottman's (1994) dimension of contempt, a
behavior that appears to be particularly damag-
ing to marital relationships. Although preoccu-
pied individuals might also exhibit higher levels
of negative affect, their expressions of negative
affect might entail more explicit expressions of
anxiety or fear. As there are little other data on
differences between dismissing and preoccu-
pied individuals' in terms of their marital
functioning and, particularly, with regards to the
specific forms of negative affect they may
exhibit, this area of research clearly merits
further investigation.
Regarding the link between individuals'
marital functioning and their partners' attach-
ment stance, although there were no associa-
tions between husbands' marital behavior or
perceptions and their wives' attachment stance,
significant findings were obtained for wives'
marital behavior as a function of their husbands'
attachment stance. Wives of continuous-secure
husbands were more positive than wives of
dismissing or earned-secure husbands and less
negative than wives of dismissing husbands.
These findings are notable in that they suggest
that although earned-secure wives appear to
function as well as continuous-secure wives in
their marriage, there may be importance differ-
ences between earned-secure and continuous-
secure husbands that were undetected by our
marital interactions but may nonetheless have
implications for their wives' positive marital
behavior. However, it is also important to note
T
h
i
s

d
o
c
u
m
e
n
t

i
s

c
o
p
y
r
i
g
h
t
e
d

b
y

t
h
e

A
m
e
r
i
c
a
n

P
s
y
c
h
o
l
o
g
i
c
a
l

A
s
s
o
c
i
a
t
i
o
n

o
r

o
n
e

o
f

i
t
s

a
l
l
i
e
d

p
u
b
l
i
s
h
e
r
s
.


T
h
i
s

a
r
t
i
c
l
e

i
s

i
n
t
e
n
d
e
d

s
o
l
e
l
y

f
o
r

t
h
e

p
e
r
s
o
n
a
l

u
s
e

o
f

t
h
e

i
n
d
i
v
i
d
u
a
l

u
s
e
r

a
n
d

i
s

n
o
t

t
o

b
e

d
i
s
s
e
m
i
n
a
t
e
d

b
r
o
a
d
l
y
.
ATTACHMENT AND MARITAL FUNCTIONING 593
that wives of earned-secure husbands did not
differ from wives of continuous-secure hus-
bands in their expressions of negative affect, as
did wives of dismissing husbands. Given that
negative affect appears to be the stronger
predictor of marital dissolution, marriages with
dismissing husbands may be at greater risk for
negative outcomes than marriages with earned-
secure husbands. More generally, it is impor-
tant to note that these findings should be viewed
cautiously as they were found in the context of
a nonsignificant overall MANCOVA effect.
Nonetheless, they do suggest some intrigu-
ing possibilities that seem worthy of further
investigation.
One general question that arises with regard
to our findings is why wives' behavior, but not
husbands' behavior, was related to their part-
ners' attachment stance. One possible explana-
tion may relate to differences in men's and
women's responses to different attachment-
related behaviors exhibited by their partners.
That is, there is some evidence that women may
find a partner's discomfort with closeness the
most distressing, whereas men may be more
bothered by a partner's anxiety or fears of
abandonment (Collins & Read, 1990). Discom-
fort with closeness is most characteristic of a
dismissing adult attachment stance, whereas
anxiety about abandonment is most characteris-
tic of a preoccupied stance, so we might predict
that the most distress would be evident among
wives of dismissing husbands and husbands of
preoccupied wives. In our sample, we did find
the wives of dismissing husbands exhibited less
positive affect and more negative affect than
wives of continuous-secure husbands. Although
we did not find significant differences between
husbands of preoccupied wives and any of the
other groups of husbands,
6
this may have been
due to the small number of husbands of
preoccupied wives {n = 9). Clearly, one goal for
future research will be to obtain sufficiently
large samples to examine the different effects of
an individual's particular secure (i.e., earned
secure or continuous secure) or insecure (i.e.,
dismissing or preoccupied) attachment stance
on their partner's marital behavior.
One notable aspect of the current findings is
that a link between attachment stance and
marital behavior was found for wives but not for
husbands. Caspi and Elder (1988) have specu-
lated that women may be more likely than men
to carry forward the remnants of earlier
attachment relationships into adult intimate
relationships, proposing that "while socializa-
tion into masculinity involves socialization into
separateness outside the home and beyond the
family, girls' socialization is embedded in their
own ongoing relationships within the family and
stresses affective relationships to others" (p.
219). However, given our findings that wives'
marital behavior was also somewhat related to
their husbands' attachment stance, it is also
possible that there is something about the
behavior of at least dismissing and earned-
secure men that is associated with less positive
marital behavior from their wives, but simply
was not captured in our marital interactions. One
possible explanation is that although the link
between men's attachment stance and the ability
to regulate their affect might not be evident in
problem-solving discussions, such an associa-
tion might be found in the context of marital
interactions that do not directly involve conflict
with one's partner. Because previous research
(see Gottman, 1994) suggests that conflict is
more physiologically stressful for men in
general than it is for women, it is possible that
physiological factors may exert a stronger
influence than earlier attachment experiences on
men's ability to regulate affect, especially
during conflict. However, in marital interactions
that involve expressions of distress regarding
issues unrelated to conflict between spouses
(e.g., problems at work), husbands' attachment
stance may be more strongly related to their
ability to manage both their own affect, as well
as respond to their partner's affect. Some
support is provided for this notion by Simpson
et al. (1992), who demonstrated that men with a
more secure attachment style provided more
support to their dating partners during a stressful
situation than men with a more insecure-
avoidant attachment style. Thus, it may be that
in our study, the less positive marital behavior of
wives of dismissing and earned-secure husbands
6
Interestingly, we did find in post hoc analyses that
when education level was removed from the model
(as husbands' education level had accounted for such
a large portion of the variance), husbands of
preoccupied wives did in fact express significantly
less positive affect than husbands of continuous-
secure, earned-secure, and dismissing wives.
T
h
i
s

d
o
c
u
m
e
n
t

i
s

c
o
p
y
r
i
g
h
t
e
d

b
y

t
h
e

A
m
e
r
i
c
a
n

P
s
y
c
h
o
l
o
g
i
c
a
l

A
s
s
o
c
i
a
t
i
o
n

o
r

o
n
e

o
f

i
t
s

a
l
l
i
e
d

p
u
b
l
i
s
h
e
r
s
.


T
h
i
s

a
r
t
i
c
l
e

i
s

i
n
t
e
n
d
e
d

s
o
l
e
l
y

f
o
r

t
h
e

p
e
r
s
o
n
a
l

u
s
e

o
f

t
h
e

i
n
d
i
v
i
d
u
a
l

u
s
e
r

a
n
d

i
s

n
o
t

t
o

b
e

d
i
s
s
e
m
i
n
a
t
e
d

b
r
o
a
d
l
y
.
594
PALEY, COX, BURCHINAL, AND PAYNE
was due to behavior these husbands manifested
during other types of marital interactions, such
as when those wives sought comfort from their
husbands about a problem unrelated to their
marriage. A recent study by Pasch and Bradbury
(1998) suggests that provision of support during
couples' discussions of nonmarital difficulties is
an important predictor of marital outcome. It
will be important for future studies to explore
the implications of spouses' attachment stances
for how couples interact in a variety of contexts,
including not only those that involve direct
conflict between spouses, but also those in
which spouses seek support from one another.
Although our findings revealed some associa-
tions between wives' marital behavior and both
their own and their husbands' attachment stance,
both wives' and husbands' marital perceptions
were generally unrelated to either their own or
their partners' attachment stance. Our results
were fairly consistent with those of Cohn et al.
(1992), who found that husbands' attachment
stance was related to couples' marital behavior,
but found no link between either spouse's
attachment stance and their marital perceptions.
We did find, however, an association between
marital perceptions and depressive symptoms.
Wives who reported higher levels of depressive
symptoms perceived their husbands as less
emotionally responsive, whereas husbands who
reported greater depressive symptoms reported
less commitment to their marriages and per-
ceived their wives as less emotionally respon-
sive. These findings suggest that although
working models of childhood attachment rela-
tionships may relate to actual behavior in
marital interactions, other factors, such as
spouses' level of psychological distress, may
have a greater impact on marital perceptions.
Limitations and Future Directions
First, and perhaps most important, as the
results from the present study are fairly modest,
we must acknowledge the possibility that the
connections between working models of child-
hood attachment relationships, as assessed by
the AAI, and marital quality are not as strong as
we and other researchers have presumed. An
alternative possibility is that in light of what the
AAI was designed to assess, there may be other
aspects of marital functioning that would be
more strongly predicted from AAI attachment
classifications. That is, attachment researchers
(Crowell, Fraley, & Shaver, 1999) have high-
lighted the point that the AAI was developed to
assess how a parent might function as a
caregiver or "to maximize the prediction of an
adult parent's infant's classification in the
Strange Situation" (pp. 452-^53). Thus, as we
noted earlier, it would be important to examine
linkages between adult attachment stance and
marital functioning in other contexts, such as
when spouses are seeking emotional support
from or providing emotional support to one
another, as opposed to engaging in marital
conflict. It may be that these contexts would tap
more of the caregiving behavior that would be
predicted from the AAI, and, thus, we would
expect stronger links between the various adult
attachment classifications and marital function-
ing in such contexts.
Second, the concurrent assessment of attach-
ment stance and marital functioning requires us
to be cautious in making assumptions about
directions of influences. The very notion that an
individual can "earn" a secure attachment
stance is based on the premise that working
models may be revised with new experiences,
and marriage may lead to modifications of one's
working models of earlier attachment relation-
ships. For example, an insecure individual who
marries a supportive partner may not only
become more adept at regulating their emotions,
they may also begin to revise their attachment
models in a more secure direction. That is,
although an especially supportive partner cannot
erase rejections or disappointments that one
experienced in childhood, they may provide an
atmosphere where such experiences can be
safely discussed and expectations of new
rejections are disconfirmed. In noting that it is
likely that working models and experiences in
ongoing relationships mutually shape one an-
other, Kobak (1994) has emphasized the need
for longitudinal studies that assess working
models prior to the establishment of new
relationships. Such studies would not only
enhance researchers understanding of how such
models might influence interactions in current
relationships but also the extent to which these
models may be revised to accommodate new
experiences.
Finally, the present study did not find effects
of the interaction between wives' and husbands'
attachment stance for either marital behavior or
perceptions. However, as these findings were
T
h
i
s

d
o
c
u
m
e
n
t

i
s

c
o
p
y
r
i
g
h
t
e
d

b
y

t
h
e

A
m
e
r
i
c
a
n

P
s
y
c
h
o
l
o
g
i
c
a
l

A
s
s
o
c
i
a
t
i
o
n

o
r

o
n
e

o
f

i
t
s

a
l
l
i
e
d

p
u
b
l
i
s
h
e
r
s
.


T
h
i
s

a
r
t
i
c
l
e

i
s

i
n
t
e
n
d
e
d

s
o
l
e
l
y

f
o
r

t
h
e

p
e
r
s
o
n
a
l

u
s
e

o
f

t
h
e

i
n
d
i
v
i
d
u
a
l

u
s
e
r

a
n
d

i
s

n
o
t

t
o

b
e

d
i
s
s
e
m
i
n
a
t
e
d

b
r
o
a
d
l
y
.
ATTACHMENT AND MARITAL FUNCTIONING 595
based on combining more specific attachment
groups into overall secure and insecure groups,
the need remains for a more refined examination
of how the ability to manage affect in the marital
relationship may relate to both one's own and to
one's partners' particular stance toward attach-
ment. For example, we might expect a preoccu-
pied spouse to react to a dismissing partner in a
different fashion than would a secure spouse (or
for that matter, another dismissing spouse).
Clearly, predictions about the nature of a
couple's relationship become increasingly com-
plex when considering both spouses' stances
toward attachment, particularly if one moves
beyond global secure and insecure distinctions
and considers more specific variations within
these larger classifications. However, our under-
standing of both the continuities and discontinui-
ties between earlier attachment experiences and
functioning in adult intimate relationships would
likely be enriched by jointly considering the
particular strategies each partner may possess
for navigating the emotional landscape of their
relationship.
Implications for Application
and Public Policy
Findings from the present study suggest that
one's current state of mind with regards to
attachment relationships may have significant
import for how emotional experiences are
managed in the context of one's marital
relationships. Thus, it may be important for
mental health professionals working with indi-
viduals or couples on relationship issues to not
only explore early family relationships but also
to examine how individuals have to come to
understand and regard such attachment experi-
ences. Our results for earned-secure wives
suggest that difficult childhood experiences
alone may not confer a greater vulnerability for
future relationship problems but rather whether
such experiences are either denied or mini-
mized, or alternately, remain overwhelming and
preoccupying. Moreover, our findings add to a
small but growing body of literature (e.g.,
Pearson et al., 1994; Phelps et al., 1998) that
suggests that earned-secure individuals may be a
particularly important group for further study in
terms of enhancing prevention and intervention
efforts. That is, understanding the processes that
have allowed earned-secure individuals to appar-
ently depart from earlier negative developmen-
tal trajectories may greatly assist in directing the
appropriate resources (e.g., perhaps the availabil-
ity of an alternate attachment figure) toward
children and adolescents already identified to be
at-risk because of less-than-optimal attachment
relationships.
References
Ainsworth, M. D. S. (1982). Attachment: Retrospect
and prospect. In C. M. Parkes & J. Stevenson-Hinde
(Eds.), The place of attachment in human behavior
(pp. 3-30). New York: Basic Books.
Ainsworth, M. D. S., Blehar, M. C, Waters, E., &
Wall, S. (1978). Patterns of attachment: A psycho-
logical study of the strange situation. Hillsdale, NJ:
Erlbaum.
Armsden, G. C, McCauley, E., Greenberg, M. T.,
Burke, P. M., & Mitchell, J. R. (1990). Parent and
peer attachment in early adolescent depression.
Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 18, 683-
697.
Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., & van IJzendoorn,
M. H. (1993). A psychometric study of the Adult
Attachment Interview: Reliability and discriminant
validity. Developmental Psychology, 29, 870-879.
Barnett, P. A., & Gotlib, I. H. (1988). Psychosocial
functioning and depression: Distinguishing among
antecedents, concomitants, and consequences. Psy-
chological Bulletin, 104, 97-126.
Beach, S. R., Sandeen, E. E., & O'Leary, K. D.
(1990). Depression in marriage. New York: Guil-
ford Press.
Belsky, J., & Cassidy, J. (1994). Attachment: Theory
and evidence. In M. Rutter & D. F. Hay (Eds.),
Development through life: A handbook for clini-
cians (pp. 373^102). London: Blackwell.
Belsky, J., & Nezworski, T. (1988). Clinical implica-
tions of attachment. In J. Belsky & T. Nezworski
(Eds.), Clinical implications of attachment (pp.
3-17). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Berman, W. H., Marcus, L., Berman, E. R. (1994).
Attachment in marital relations. In M. B. Sperling &
W. H. Berman (Eds.), Attachment in adults: Clinical
and developmental perspectives (pp. 204-231).
New York: Guilford Press.
Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment and loss: Vol. 1.
Attachment. New York: Basic Books.
Bowlby, J. (1973). Attachment and loss: Vol. 2.
Separation: Anxiety and anger. New York: Basic
Books.
Bowlby, J. (1980). Attachment and loss: Vol. 3. Loss,
sadness, and depression. New York: Basic Books.
Braiker, H., & Kelley, H. (1976). Conflict in the
development of close relationships. In R. Burgess &
T. Huston (Eds.), Social exchange and developing
relationships (pp. 79-102). New York: Academic
Press.
T
h
i
s

d
o
c
u
m
e
n
t

i
s

c
o
p
y
r
i
g
h
t
e
d

b
y

t
h
e

A
m
e
r
i
c
a
n

P
s
y
c
h
o
l
o
g
i
c
a
l

A
s
s
o
c
i
a
t
i
o
n

o
r

o
n
e

o
f

i
t
s

a
l
l
i
e
d

p
u
b
l
i
s
h
e
r
s
.


T
h
i
s

a
r
t
i
c
l
e

i
s

i
n
t
e
n
d
e
d

s
o
l
e
l
y

f
o
r

t
h
e

p
e
r
s
o
n
a
l

u
s
e

o
f

t
h
e

i
n
d
i
v
i
d
u
a
l

u
s
e
r

a
n
d

i
s

n
o
t

t
o

b
e

d
i
s
s
e
m
i
n
a
t
e
d

b
r
o
a
d
l
y
.
596
PALEY, COX, BURCHINAL, AND PAYNE
Caspi, A., & Elder, G. H. (1988). Developmental
history, personality, and family relationships: To-
ward an emergent family system. In R. Hinde & J.
Stevenson-Hinde (Eds.), Relationships within fami-
lies: Mutual Influences (pp. 218-240). Oxford,
England: Oxford University Press.
Cassidy, J. (1994). Emotion regulation: Influences of
attachment relationships. In N. Fox (Ed.), Biologi-
cal and behavioral foundations of emotion regula-
tion. Monographs of the Society for Research in
Child Development, 59, 228-249.
Conn, D. A. (1990). Child-mother attachment of
six-year-olds and social competence at school.
Child Development, 61, 152-162.
Conn, D. A., Silver, D. H., Cowan, C. P., Cowan,
P. A., & Pearson, J. (1992). Working models of
childhood attachment and couple relationships.
Journal of Family Issues, 13, 432-449.
Collins, N. J., & Read, S. J. (1990). Adult attachment,
working models, and relationship quality in dating
couples. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol-
ogy, 58, 644-663.
Cowan, C. P., Cowan, P. A., Heming, G., & Miller,
N. B. (1991). Becoming a family: Marriage,
parenting, and child development. In P. A. Cowan &
M. Hetherington (Eds.), Family transitions (pp.
79-109). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Crittenden, P., Partridge, M., & Claussen, A. (1991).
Family patterns of relationships in normative and
dysfunctional families. Development and Psychopa-
thology, 3, 491-512.
Crowell, J. A., Fraley, R. C, & Shaver, P. R. (1999).
Measurement of individual differences in adoles-
cent and adult attachment. In J. Cassidy & P. R.
Shaver (Eds.), Handbook of attachment: Theory,
research, and clinical applications (pp. 434-465).
New York: Guilford Press.
Eiden, R. D., Teti, D. M., & Corns, K. M. (1995).
Maternal working models of attachment, marital
adjustment, and the parent-child relationship. Child
Development, 66, 1504-1518.
Eysenck, H. J., & Wakefield, J. A. (1981). Psychologi-
cal factors as predictors of marital satisfaction.
Advances in Behaviour Research and Therapy, 3,
151-192.
Feeney, J. A., Noller, P., & Callan, V. J. (1994).
Attachment style, communication, and satisfaction
in the early years of marriage. In K. Bartholomew &
D. Perlman (Eds.), Advances in personal relation-
ships: Vol. 5. Attachment processes in adulthood
(pp. 269-308). London: Jessica-Kingsley.
Fraley, R. C, & Shaver, P. R. (1997). Adult
attachment and the suppression of unwanted
thoughts. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol-
ogy, 73, 1080-1091.
George, C, Kaplan, N., & Main, M. (1984). The
attachment interview for adults. Unpublished manu-
script, University of California, Berkeley.
Gottman, J. M. (1994). What predicts divorce: The
relationship between marital processes and marital
outcomes. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Hazan, C, & Shaver, P. (1987). Romantic love
conceptualized as an attachment process. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 511-524.
Julien, D., Markman, H. J., & Lindahl, K. M. (1989).
A comparison of a global and microanalytic coding
system: Implications for future trends in studying
interactions. Behavioral Assessment, 11, 81-100.
Kirkpatrick, L. A., & Davis, K. E. (1994). Attachment
style, gender, and relationship stability: A longitudi-
nal analysis. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 66, 502-512.
Knox, D. (1971). Marriage happiness. Champaign,
IL: Research Press.
Kobak, R. R. (1994). Adult attachment: A personality
or relationship construct. Psychological Inquiry, 5,
42-44.
Kobak, R. R., Cole, H. E., Ferenz-Gillies, R., &
Fleming, W. S. (1993). Attachment and emotion
regulation during mother-teen problem solving: A
control theory analysis. Child Development, 64,
231-245.
Kobak, R., & Duemmler, S. (1994). Attachment and
conversation: Toward a discourse analysis of
adolescent and social security. In K. Bartholomew
& D. Perlman (Eds.), Advances in personal
relationships: Vol. 5. Attachment processes in
adulthood (pp. 121-149). London: Jessica-Kings-
ley.
Kobak, R. R., & Hazan, C. (1991). Attachment in
marriage: Effects of security and accuracy of
working models. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 60, 861-869.
Kobak, R. R., Ruckdeschel, K., & Hazan, C. (1994).
From symptom to signal: An attachment view of
emotion in marital therapy. In S. M. Johnson & L. S.
Greenberg (Eds.), The heart of the matter: Perspec-
tives on emotion in marital therapy (pp. 46-71).
New York: Brunner/Mazel.
Kobak, R. R., & Sceery, A. (1988). Attachment in late
adolescence: Working models, affect regulation,
and representations of self and others. Child
Development, 59, 135-146.
Kobak, R. R., Sudler, N., & Gamble, W. (1991).
Attachment and depressive symptoms during adoles-
cence: A developmental pathways analysis. Develop-
ment and Psychopathology, 3, 461474.
Lindahl, K. M., & Markman, H. J. (1990). Communi-
cation and negative affect regulation in the family.
In E. A. Blechman (Ed.), Emotions and the family:
For better or for worse (pp. 99-115). Hillsdale, NJ:
Erlbaum.
Main, M., & Goldwyn, R. (1994). Adult attachment
scoring and classification systems. Unpublished
manuscript, University of California, Berkeley.
Main, M., Kaplan, N., & Cassidy, J. (1985). Security
in infancy, childhood, and adulthood: A move to the
level of representation. In I. Bretherton & E. Waters
T
h
i
s

d
o
c
u
m
e
n
t

i
s

c
o
p
y
r
i
g
h
t
e
d

b
y

t
h
e

A
m
e
r
i
c
a
n

P
s
y
c
h
o
l
o
g
i
c
a
l

A
s
s
o
c
i
a
t
i
o
n

o
r

o
n
e

o
f

i
t
s

a
l
l
i
e
d

p
u
b
l
i
s
h
e
r
s
.


T
h
i
s

a
r
t
i
c
l
e

i
s

i
n
t
e
n
d
e
d

s
o
l
e
l
y

f
o
r

t
h
e

p
e
r
s
o
n
a
l

u
s
e

o
f

t
h
e

i
n
d
i
v
i
d
u
a
l

u
s
e
r

a
n
d

i
s

n
o
t

t
o

b
e

d
i
s
s
e
m
i
n
a
t
e
d

b
r
o
a
d
l
y
.
ATTACHMENT AND MARITAL FUNCTIONING 597
(Eds.), Growing points in attachment theory and
research (pp. 66-104). Monographs of the Society
for Research in Child Development, 50(1-2, Serial
No. 209).
Markman, H. J., & Kraft, S. A. (1989). Men and
women in marriage: Dealing with gender differ-
ences in marital therapy. The Behavior Therapist,
12,51-56.
Mikulincer, M., & Erev, I. (1991). Attachment style
and the structure of romantic love. British Journal
of Social Psychology, 30, 273-291.
Owens, G., Crowell, J. A., Pan, H., Treboux, D.,
O'Connor, E., & Waters, E. (1995). The prototype
hypothesis and the origins of attachment working
models: Adult relationships with parents and
romantic partners. In E. Waters, B. E. Vaughn, G.
Posada, & K. Kondo-Ikemura (Eds.), Caregiving,
cultural, and cognitive perspectives on secure-base
behavior and working models: New growing points
of attachment theory and research (pp. 216-233).
Monographs of the Society for Research in Child
Development, 60(2-3, Serial No. 244).
Pasch, L. A., & Bradbury, T. N. (1998). Social
support, conflict, and the development of marital
dysfunction. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
Psychology, 66, 219-230.
Pearson, J. L., Cohn, D. A., Cowan, P. A., & Cowan,
C. P. (1994). Earned- and continuous-security in
adult attachment: Relation to depressive symptom-
atology and parenting style. Development and
Psychopathology, 6, 359-373.
Phelps, J. L., Belsky, J., & Crnic, K. (1998). Earned
security, daily stress, and parenting: A comparison
of five alternative models. Development and Psycho-
pathology, 10, 21-38.
Quinton, D., Rutter, M., & Liddle, C. (1984). Institutional
rearing, parenting difficulties and marital support.
Psychological Medicine, 14, 107-124.
Rutter, M., & Quinton, D. (1984). Long-term
follow-up of women institutionalized in childhood".
Factors promoting good functioning in adult life.
British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 2,
191-204.
Sagi, A., van IJzendoorn, M. H., Scharf, M.,
Koren-Karie, N., Joels, T., & Mayseless, O. (1994).
Stability and discriminant validity of the Adult
Attachment Interview: A psychometric study in
young Israeli adults. Developmental Psychology,
30, 771-777.
Schaefer, M. T, & Olson, D. H. (1981). Assessing
intimacy: The PAIR inventory. Journal of Marital
and Family Therapy, 7, 47-60.
Scharfe, E., & Bartholomew, K. (1995). Accommoda-
tion and attachment representations in young
couples. Journal of Personal and Social Relation-
ships, 12, 389-401.
Senchak, M., & Leonard, K. E. (1992). Attachment
styles and marital adjustment among newlywed
couples. Journal of Social and Personal Relation-
ships, 9, 51-64.
Simpson, J. A., Rholes, W. S., & Nelligan, J. S.
(1992). Support seeking and support giving within
couples in an anxiety-provoking situation: The role
of attachment styles. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 62, 434446.
Simpson, J. A., Rholes, W. S., & Phillips, D. (1996).
Conflict in close relationships: An attachment
perspective. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 71, 899-914.
Snyder, D. K. (1979). Multidimensional assessment
of marital satisfaction. Journal of Marriage and the
Family, 41, 813-823.
Sroufe, L. A. (1988). The role of infant-caregiver
attachment in development. In J. Belsky & T.
Nezworski (Eds.), Clinical implications of attach-
ment (pp. 18-40). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Sroufe, L. A., & Fleeson, J. (1986). Attachment and
the construction of relationships. In W. Hartup & Z.
Rubin (Eds.), Relationships and development (pp.
51-71). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
van IJzendoorn, M. H., & Bakermans-Kranenburg,
M. J. (1996). Attachment representations in moth-
ers, fathers, adolescents, and clinical groups: A
meta-analytic search for normative data. Journal of
Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 64, 8-21.
Received May 15, 1998
Revision received June 21, 1999
Accepted July 21, 1999
T
h
i
s

d
o
c
u
m
e
n
t

i
s

c
o
p
y
r
i
g
h
t
e
d

b
y

t
h
e

A
m
e
r
i
c
a
n

P
s
y
c
h
o
l
o
g
i
c
a
l

A
s
s
o
c
i
a
t
i
o
n

o
r

o
n
e

o
f

i
t
s

a
l
l
i
e
d

p
u
b
l
i
s
h
e
r
s
.


T
h
i
s

a
r
t
i
c
l
e

i
s

i
n
t
e
n
d
e
d

s
o
l
e
l
y

f
o
r

t
h
e

p
e
r
s
o
n
a
l

u
s
e

o
f

t
h
e

i
n
d
i
v
i
d
u
a
l

u
s
e
r

a
n
d

i
s

n
o
t

t
o

b
e

d
i
s
s
e
m
i
n
a
t
e
d

b
r
o
a
d
l
y
.

Você também pode gostar