Você está na página 1de 11

Application of Descriptive Phenomenological Research Method

to the Field of Clinical Research


Barbro M. Giorgi, Ph.D.
Saybrook University
United States

Abstract
The literature on the psychotherapeutic practices reveals a high level of discord. On one
hand there is a clamor for evidence-based research and on the other hand there are many
serious criticisms about the nature of empirical research on the therapeutic practices. On
one side there are the logical rules for conducting allegedly sound research and on the other
hand there are the experiences of therapeutic practitioners which seem to be at odds with the
accepted rules. It is suggested that this chasm can be bridged by phenomenological research
strategies that are experientially congruent and based upon the discovery of lived meanings
derived from the concrete descriptions of the participants, whether therapists or clients.

As I embarked on my studies in psychology, I was frustrated by my experience which could


be simplistically summarized by "but its not like that". All the theories I read in the textbooks
held together beautifully as long as they remained on the page but the minute I tried to take them
into "life" and tried to use them in order to understand myself or to understand others around me
and our relationship to one another, the beautiful logic of the theory would collapse to one degree
or another with me saying to myself "but it doesnt work that way".
This frustration continued all through graduate school and, I think culminated in the
literature review for my dissertation. From the very beginning of my education I experienced a
gap between what I was being taught about psychology, and life as I experienced it. When I
started my literature review about the therapeutic process I then also came face to face with the
gap between clinical practice "the way it really works", and the research results that are supposed
to inform this clinical practice.
There was an overwhelming amount of research on the topic but almost every article I read
complained about the general lack of relevance of research findings to clinical praxis. Some
complaints concerned the lack of ecological validity. Research is being conducted under
circumstances that are too different from the way therapy is being practiced, dealing with issues
in ways that are too simplistic and approaching clients in reductionistic ways. Throughout the
literature there was a sense of crisis. First, a sense that research is not sufficiently relevant to

Les Collectifs du Cirp


Volume 1 (dition spciale), pp. 119-129.
2010 Cirp (Cercle interdisciplinaire de recherches phnomnologiques)
ISBN 978-0-9866654-1-7

120

Application of Descriptive Phenomenological Research Method to the Field of Clinical Practice

practitioners for them to even read the literature and secondly, a frustration over the lack of
influence research was having on clinical training and the education of psychologists and, thirdly,
a sense of urgency to show some clear and positive results in order to justify therapy, an urgency
fueled by new economic and political pressures from managed care and third party payers. The
literature was full of calls for new and different approaches to doing clinical research in order to
overcome these issues. Some called for small variations, tweaking our methods a little and others
were calling for a fundamental and radical paradigm shift.
The objections contained in the literature, and their sources, are summarized in Tables 1 and
2. Table 1 lists the types of objections being made about clinical research strategies, and Table 2
specifies the type of complaints being voiced concerning the lack of relevance of therapeutic
research for therapeutic practice in general. I shall not dwell on these objections because most
experienced clinicians are well aware of them, and besides, I want to concentrate on a key
positive aspect that the phenomenological approach can bring to these issues. The key inference
to be drawn from these two tables is that research into the therapeutic process is problematic, and
because of this problematic status, this subfield of psychology should be open to novel strategies
of research.
Table 1
The types of objections made about psychotherapeutic research over the last three decades by therapists/clinicians
who conduct the research and their sources. Each source cited has at least one such objection, many have several,
and a few contain them all.

Objections Concerning Strategies

Sources

Overemphasis on methodology in empiricist


tradition

Barlow, 1996; Bergin & Strupp, 1972; Budge,


1983; Elliott, 1983; Frank, 1979; Gelso &
Fassinger, 1990; Goldfreid & Wolfe, 1996;
Gottman & Markman, 1978; Greenberg, 1986;
Horowitz, 1982; Hoshmond, 1989; Howard,
Moras, Brill, Maritinovich, & Lutz, 1996; Meehl,
1978; Orlinsky & Howard, 1978; Persons, 1991;
Rice & Greenberg, 1984; Rorer & Widiger,
1983; Sechrest, McKnight, & McKnight, 1996;
Seligman, 1995; 1996; Sher & Truell, 1996;
Singer, 1980; Wiggins & Pincus, 1992.

Questioning of empiricist strategies and


assumptions
Disaffection with verification mode of research
as only mode
Therapeutic research should open up to other
paradigms
Major strategy shifts are required in therapeutic
research
Analogue therapeutic research is inadequate
Therapeutic research has yielded noncumulative
findings

Les Collectifs du Cirp


Volume 1 (dition spciale), pp. 119-129.
2010 Cirp (Cercle interdisciplinaire de recherches phnomnologiques)
ISBN 978-0-9866654-1-7

Barbro M. Giorgi

121

Table 2
The types of objections made about psychotherapeutic research over the last three decades by therapists/clinicians
who conduct the research and their sources. Each source cited has at least one such objection, many have several,
and a few contain them all.

Objections Concerning Clinical Relevance


Scientific rigor and clinical relevance are at odds
Research conditions and practical clinical settings
are at odds
There are tensions in scientific-practitioner model
Clinical research and clinical training are at odds
There is mismatch between level of
conceptualization of phenomena and level of
typical measurement
Decontextualized isolated variable studies are not
ecologically related to clinical settings

Sources
Anderson & Strupp, 1996; Elliott, 1983, 1995;
Emmelkamp, 1981; Fishman, 1981; Gelso &
Fassinger, 1990; Goldfried & Wolfe, 1996;
Greenberg, 1986; Hayes & Nelson, 1981; Heller,
1990; Hollon, 1996; Howard, Orlinsky, &
Lueger, 1995; Hugdahl & st, 1981; Levy, 1981;
Magnusson & Trestad, 1993; Mahrer, 1997;
Maletzky, 1981; McCullough, 1984; Mintz,
Drake, & Crits-Cristoph, 1996; Norcross, 1997;
Persons, 1991; Sechrest, McKnight, &
McKnight, 1996; Seligman, 1996; Shapiro &
Shapiro, 1983; Strupp, 1996; 1997; Tedeschi,
Heister, & Gahagan, 1969.

My own frustration increased as I reviewed more and more of the traditional literature. Not
only were there no answers to my own clinical questions, but it became quite clear that we do not
really know what happens to our clients in therapy or how come therapy works. I had to conclude
that the state of affairs of clinical research is indeed problematic. Research results show, without
a doubt, that therapy does work, but, there are no significant differences to be found either in
terms of schools of thought or in terms of implemented tools or techniques. Much research has
been conducted, but no theory or type of practice can be shown to be better than any other. And,
while there is no discernible difference between different theories or techniques, there is,
moreover, no difference between professionals, paraprofessionals or lay persons nor are there
significant differences related to level of experience among professionals. In other words,
research shows that therapy works but it makes no significant difference what theory or technique
you use nor how much experience you have. This is a problem both from a research perspective
as well as from the perspective of training and education.
As I proceeded in my review it also became increasingly difficult to arrive at some kind of
understanding of what the various aspects of therapy are in traditional terms; i.e., what are the
variables? The literature is full of arguments for various aspects of therapy for its success. The
list is long and contains concepts such as "being heard", "safety", "correcting erroneous
cognitions", "corrective emotional experiences", "cathartic experiences", "dealing with
unfinished business", plus many other lists of critical factors. A more complete list of these
factors is presented in Table 3.

Les Collectifs du Cirp


Volume 1 (dition spciale), pp. 119-129.
2010 Cirp (Cercle interdisciplinaire de recherches phnomnologiques)
ISBN 978-0-9866654-1-7

122

Application of Descriptive Phenomenological Research Method to the Field of Clinical Practice

Table 3
Summary of factors designated as helpful in psychotherapeutic research.
(Table derived on the basis of data from Mahrer and Nadler, 1986).

Criteria Used To Establish


Good Moments

Outcome Criteria

Normal Ways of Being and


Behaving

Theories of Therapeutic
Practice

Helpful good moments


-Voice quality was active, vivid and rich, energetic and
expressive.
-Client involvement and active engagement.
-Willingness to communicate.
-Commitment to change.
-Trust in therapist.
-Recognition of one's own responsibility, owning
feelings and behaviors.
-Clients who engage in warm accepting dialogue.
-Support-seeking relationship with their therapist.
-Mature behavior.
-Reduction of defensive behavior.
-Focusing on immediate experience.
-Higher levels of cooperativeness; but also some
disagreement and disapproval.
-Emotional expression of feelings.
-Depth of personal self-exploration.
-Shift in self-concept.

-Free and realistic expression of basic impulses.


-Heightened awareness of interpersonal consequences.
-Heightened positive attitude toward self and others.
-Search for felt meaning and inner understanding.
-Heightened awareness of higher order personal
relative to opening up of life options.
-Open and direct expression of feeling and emotions.
-Engagement in internal encounters with deeper
personality processes.
-Experiential sampling of concrete new ways of being
and behaving.
-Cooperative attitudes with therapist
-Hard work on problems.
-Achieving emotional insight.
-Being in a state of emotional arousal and ready to
apprehend new ways of seeing selves and others.
-Accessing cognitions, feelings and impulses
previously unavailable.
-Sense of completion, i.e. feeling that something had
shifted and that a problem ceased to exist.

Les Collectifs du Cirp


Volume 1 (dition spciale), pp. 119-129.
2010 Cirp (Cercle interdisciplinaire de recherches phnomnologiques)
ISBN 978-0-9866654-1-7

Barbro M. Giorgi

Post-Session Evaluation

123

-Moments of heightened insight and understanding.


-Moments of generalized good feelings such as trust,
optimism, security, relaxation, warmth, acceptance
and support.
-Experiences of most intimate personal relationships,
past or present.
-Moments of personal insight, problem clarification
and solution.
-Moments of personal contact, reassurance and
involvement with therapist.
-Moments of lowered resistance in analytic
relationship.
-Decreased description of problems and increased
insight into clients problems.
-Expression of thoughts and feelings and discussion of
their meanings.

Upon review, it can be seen that much of the literature emphasized a few particular aspects
and suggested that this or that aspect was the most critical one. Some researchers indicated that
the therapeutic relationship is the cornerstone of therapy while others would downplay the
significance of the individual therapist and suggest that specific techniques account for success.
Then, of course, there is the issue of what is successful therapy. Research can be broadly divided
into process versus outcome research and this is linked to the issue of what constitutes successful
therapy. A positive outcome such as a reduction of symptoms or symptom relief is one
possibility, and so is increased functioning, increased insight and self awareness. More maturity
and personal growth or a sense of deepened meaning of life and so on, are other possibilities.
There is by no means any consensus as to what a positive result actually means or a consensus as
to what should be examined. It seems that the philosophical and theoretical perspective of the
researcher determines how and what is to be researched as opposed to a preexisting body of
research findings or knowledge upon which to build further understanding. Some research
emphasizes learning or modeling, intellectual insight, cognitive clarification and correction while
others emphasize a human encounter in a "here and now" context or "in the moment" experience
and still others take a spiritual approach and/or an embodied approach borrowing much from
Eastern philosophy and its practices. Should the issue be symptoms, diagnoses and subsequent
interventions, as in the medical model, or a more humanistic approach with a foundation in
existential philosophy, or a biological, neurological perspective looking for genetic
predispositions and biochemical solutions?
Yet another issue of how to approach clinical research is in terms of what time span to
include. In the studies found in the literature the researchers have chosen a specific time period to
examine such as a certain number of sessions, critical parts of single sessions, some of those
decisions based on a numeric amount such as number of words as a cut-off point or number of
minutes within a session, while others are based on content in the session such as emotional
intensity, nature of the dialog between client and therapist and so on. Some studies take a
somewhat longitudinal approach while others look at critical instances.

Les Collectifs du Cirp


Volume 1 (dition spciale), pp. 119-129.
2010 Cirp (Cercle interdisciplinaire de recherches phnomnologiques)
ISBN 978-0-9866654-1-7

124

Application of Descriptive Phenomenological Research Method to the Field of Clinical Practice

As a result of the literature review I felt more confused than ever and I was glad to have a
phenomenological method as a research method at my disposal for a number of reasons and in a
number of ways. [For details about the method, see Giorgi and Giorgi, 2003a; 2003b.] I will not
be able to give full justice to the method here but I will highlight what I found to be some of the
most important benefits of the very approach of the method in my study (von Knorring-Giorgi,
1998). First, let me describe briefly the data collection process and how this is relevant to the
previous discussion. Phenomenology deals with concrete descriptions of experiences that
participants themselves have lived through or experienced. This is critical because it saves the
participants from having to know or understand why or how something works. In my study, I
asked the participants to simply describe as concretely as possible their experience of a successful
and lasting change that occurred within the therapeutic context for them. This did not require
them to actually understand it but simply to describe it the way they had experienced it. This also
meant, perhaps even more importantly, that I, as the researcher, did not have to know what to
look for. I did not have to make decisions about whether it should be a certain number of
sessions, whether to look at the therapeutic relationship alone or include the clients lives outside
of therapy or whether to include other significant people in the clients lives. In a
phenomenological study the researcher does not have to nor should he or she make such
decisions. The research participants make those decisions and inform you about the various
aspects that arc important to them and how they relate to the issue in meaningful ways. When a
person goes about describing an experience or event to another, he or she will spontaneously give
the listener all the necessary background information so that the experience can be understood in
its context. Not only did this save me from having to design "the right kind of study" in terms of
time frame and what and who to include, what questions to ask, but results also provided a much
more integrated picture of the process than what I had found in the literature in general.
Interestingly enough, all participants started with childhood, not in great detail and not in
terms of traumatic experiences but each participant found it necessary to describe their familial
relations and how they had perceived themselves and the world entering into therapy in order for
me to fully appreciate what the experience of change within therapy had meant to them. This was
interesting in the sense that none of the current research on psychotherapy had incorporated this
into their inquiries. And in the literature in general, when childhood experience is included in
research it is often approached in terms of specific problems or trauma. In any case, in the clinical
research I reviewed, childhood was almost completely excluded. This is, of course, quite natural.
If the decision of what time frame to limit the study to is to be chosen by the researcher, it would
seem to most researchers a daunting task to have to start at the very beginning and include
everything in the participant's life that could be connected to a therapeutic change. Of course,
there is certainly the awareness that humans are complex beings and that in every moment of our
lives we also incorporate all of our past history and experience as well as all our future goals,
hopes, fears and aspirations, but this cannot be easily accommodated in a research project and so
it is usually set aside from the inquiry. Phenomenology, in contrast, can to some extent
accommodate this aspect of humans. This is important from a phenomenological perspective
because it espouses to be faithful to the phenomenon of inquiry such that if this is indeed "how
human beings are" then they must be studied in such a manner. What makes this, at least to some
degree, possible is that in a phenomenological study the researcher does not have to ask about
everything in the participant's life that might be of importance, but rather the participant will
Les Collectifs du Cirp
Volume 1 (dition spciale), pp. 119-129.
2010 Cirp (Cercle interdisciplinaire de recherches phnomnologiques)
ISBN 978-0-9866654-1-7

Barbro M. Giorgi

125

make the decisions about where to start and what to include based on his or her own experience
and include what is important to him or her and exclude what is not. This has the additional
benefit of allowing for individual differences. In other words, what is important to one participant
may not be to the next and most importantly, the researcher does not have to know this in
advance. Another aspect that is critical here is the fact that the description is not about how the
participants understood their own experiences but simply a description of "what happened" from
their perspective. This means that the participant does not need to have a psychological insight
into the processes involved. This is an important aspect of doing phenomenological research
because it capitalizes on the human phenomenon of people generally being very good at "living
life" or at "doing things" while much weaker at being able to "know" what we do or understand
ourselves. Thus, in a description of a concrete lived experience so much more is revealed than
would be possible if an explanation or an understanding of the experience was required. It is
through the analyses of this concrete data that an understanding of the psychological meanings
emerges.
Another concrete benefit of the phenomenological approach was that a particular important
aspect of idiosyncrasy was not lost in the study. I will take a small but illustrative example from
the findings in my study to demonstrate this point. In the literature the issue of safety in the
therapeutic relationship was a common theme. Safety was also shown to be important in my
doctoral research. The difference between how safety was discussed in the literature and what
emerged from my study has to do with idiosyncrasy. The literature found safety to be an
important aspect and then proceeded to outline what safety is for a client and how to achieve it in
therapy. This study, however showed that this is not necessarily the best approach to the idea of
safety in therapy. Safety was equally important to each of the participants, but what safety was
and how it was achieved differed drastically from one person to the next. What makes this critical
is that, in effect, what made one person safe would actually have threatened another. The
implication of this is that we cannot, as therapists, attempt to understand in a generic manner,
what makes a client safe and then proceed to provide the client with that generic sense of safety.
Instead we must listen to each individual client and discern what exactly makes them feel safe
and from there attempt to provide a tailor-made safety for each client.
This became especially evident in looking at two of the participants, both women, and
recognizing that one of them needed to be nurtured and met with gentleness and warmth to feel
safe, while the other woman felt safe because the therapist was confrontational and allowed her to
also be confrontational. What is critical here is the psychological meaning of nurturing and of
confrontation that each woman experienced. The woman who needed warmth and compassion
was very capable and strong while feeling generally unloved and therefore felt neither cared for
nor safe. The other woman instead was treated by significant others with warmth but was not
allowed to stand up for herself and acknowledge her own desires and opinions, especially if these
were in any way confrontational. The fact that confrontation was allowed in therapy and that she
was not rejected or punished for being confrontational made her feel safe. Thus, we cannot
answer the question of safety with a prescription such as warmth and understanding or with being
confrontational and straightforward, but must look at the meaning that it holds for each client at
the most private and idiosyncratic level. A phenomenological study seeks to uncover the specific
meanings of concrete lived experiences and in this case the psychological meanings that
essentially describe the therapeutic process. The risk of ending up with a "one size fits all" model
Les Collectifs du Cirp
Volume 1 (dition spciale), pp. 119-129.
2010 Cirp (Cercle interdisciplinaire de recherches phnomnologiques)
ISBN 978-0-9866654-1-7

126

Application of Descriptive Phenomenological Research Method to the Field of Clinical Practice

that actually does not fit anybody all that well is therefore somewhat less of a risk in a
phenomenological study than may be the case in general.
In more general terms the phenomenological method offers the possibility to address issues
that are not easily addressed with traditional research methods; i.e., those phenomena that are
both more subtle, more intangible and more complex. Phenomenology allows for research in
areas where it may be impossible to know what the critical variables are in advance both in terms
of the time period to examine and in terms of what aspects of the phenomenon to look at and
what players or individuals or relationships to include, where it is (as it is in most human
contexts) impossible to control for variables and isolate specific aspects from their lived context
and still get ecologically valid results. Phenomenology is discovery-oriented rather than
hypothesis-testing oriented and the researcher can therefore pose research questions where not
many prior answers exist.
Perhaps the discovery aspect of phenomenological research should be emphasized more.
The "hypothesis-testing" method is surely a tried and true method in science. It certainly works
well where there is solid background knowledge and certain specifics have to be tested where
true independence of variables exists. But a more open-ended, discovery oriented approach seems
more adequate for phenomena that contain many individual differences and where genuine
independence among the factors (variables) do not exist. In a "hypothesis-testing" method, one
has to know, or presume to know, quite a bit before the role of a specific factor can be
determined. The discovery approach does not assume specific a priori knowing. Rather, it more
generally asks, "How is the phenomenon operating? What is going on that makes it work, when it
does?" Such questions are far more important where interdependency among the so-called
variables exist. The traditional experimental method assumes genuine independence of variables
so that each variable can be modified without simultaneously affecting other variables. But that
condition does not exist with experiential phenomena, where strong inter-dependency is the
norm. As with all gestalts, if one modifies one factor, all of the others are spontaneously modified
because of internal connections among them. Consequently, all experiential changes have to be
understood holistically and, initially, idiosyncratically. In such an approach, the specific
experiences of therapeutic change are directly discriminable because they are contextually
understood. Generalizations have to be made relationally and contextually, not in terms of the
effects of isolated variables.
An important part of the strength of this research approach as applied to clinical research is
that results have ecological validity. What perhaps was most personally satisfying for me
conducting this study was the response from other clinicians to the findings rather than meeting
the skepticism so prevalent among clinicians vis--vis research my study has been greeted with
enthusiasm from colleagues. I have also been pleased to find myself informed by the findings in
my own clinical work and this is indeed the very reason for my passion for clinical research.
Finally, a concrete practical implication of my study was that the results indicated most of
the same findings already in the literature, but more importantly, it also gave some indication of
how the findings related to each other and the context in which therapeutic change occurs. The
study was holistic, contextual and concrete, and yet generalizations were possible. To have
conducted such a comprehensive study using traditional research methods would have been
impossible given the scope of the study but because the participants gave a comprehensive
picture of the event as it related to their entire life while also restricting themselves only to what
Les Collectifs du Cirp
Volume 1 (dition spciale), pp. 119-129.
2010 Cirp (Cercle interdisciplinaire de recherches phnomnologiques)
ISBN 978-0-9866654-1-7

Barbro M. Giorgi

127

was relevant, the phenomenological approach used in this study offered a more comprehensive
picture than would, to some extent, otherwise have been possible.
References
Anderson, T. A. & Strupp, H. H. (1996). The ecology of psychotherapy research. Journal of Counseling
and Clinical Psychology, 64, 776-782.
Barlow, D. H. (1996). Health care policy, psychotherapy research, and the future of psychotherapy.
American Psychologist, 51 1050-1058.
Bergin, A. E.& Strupp, H. H. (1972). Changing frontiers in the science of psychotherapy. Chicago:
Aldine-Atherton.
Budge, S. (1983). A critical look at the psychotherapeutic outcome research paradigm. Psychotherapy:
Theory, Research and Practice, 20, 294-306.
Elliott, R (1983). That in your hands: A comprehensive process analysis of a significant event in
psychotherapy. Psychiatry, 46, 113-129.
Elliott, R. (1995). Therapy process research and clinical practice: Practical strategies. In M. Aneline & D.
A. Shapiro, (Eds.). Research foundations for psychotherapy practice. Sussex, England: Wiley, pp.
49-72.
Emmelkamp, P. M. G. (1981). The current and future status of clinical research. Behavioral Assessment,
3, 249-253.
Fishman, S. T. (1981). Narrowing the generalization gap in clinical research. Behavioral Assessment, 3,
243-248.
Frank, J. (1979). The prestatus of outcome studies. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 47,
310-317.
Gelso, C. J. & Fassinger, E. (1990). Counseling psychology: Theory and research on intervention. Annual
review of Psychology, 41, 355-386.
Giorgi, A. & Giorgi, B. (2003a). Phenomenology. In J. A. Smith (Ed.) Qualitative psychology: A practical
guide to research methods (pp. 25-50). London: Sage Publications Inc.
Giorgi, A. & Giorgi, B. (2003b). The descriptive phenomenological psychological method. In P. M.
Camic, J. E. Rhodes & L. Yardley (Eds.). Qualitative research in psychology: Expanding
perspectives in methodology and design (pp. 243-273). Washington, DC: American Psychological
Association.
Goldfried, M. R. & Wolfe, B. E. (1996). Psychotherapy practice and research: Repairing a strained
alliance. American Psychologist, vol. 51, p. 1007-1016.
Gottman, J. M. & Markman, H. J. (1978). Experimental design in psychotherapy research. In S. L.
Garfield & A. E. Bergin, (Eds.). Handbook of psychotherapy and behavior change: An empirical
analysis. New York: Wiley & Sons.
Greenberg, L. S. (1986). Research strategies. In L. S. Greenberg & W. M. Pinsof (Eds.). The
psychotherapy process: A research handbook. New York: The Garfield Press, pp. 707-734.
Hayes, S. C., & Nelson, R. O. (1981). On the nature of the clinical-research gap: The problems with some
solutions. Behavioral Assessment, 3, 235-242.
Heller, K. (1990). Social and community intervention. Annual Review of Psychology, 41, 141-168. Hollon,
S. D. (1996). The efficacy and effectiveness of psychotherapy relative to medications. American
Psychologist, 51, 1025-1030.
Goldfried, M. R. & Wolfe, B. E. (1996). Psychotherapy practice and research: Repairing a strained
alliance. American Psychologist, vol. 51, p. 1007-1016.

Les Collectifs du Cirp


Volume 1 (dition spciale), pp. 119-129.
2010 Cirp (Cercle interdisciplinaire de recherches phnomnologiques)
ISBN 978-0-9866654-1-7

128

Application of Descriptive Phenomenological Research Method to the Field of Clinical Practice

Gottman, J. M. & Markman, H. J. (1978). Experimental design in psychotherapy research. In S. L.


Garfield & A. E. Bergin, (Eds.). Handbook of psychotherapy and behavior change: An empirical
analysis. New York: Wiley & Sons.
Greenberg, L. S. (1986). Change process research. Journal of Counseling and Clinical Psychology, 54, 49.
Hayes, S. C. & Nelson, R. O. (1981). Clinically relevant research: Requirements, problems, and solutions.
Behavioral Assessment, 3, 209-215.
Heller, K. (1990). Social and community intervention. Annual Review of Psychology, 41, 141-168.
Hollon, S. D. (1996). The efficacy and effectiveness of psychotherapy relative to medications. American
Psychologist, 51, 1025-1030.
Horowitz, M. (1982). Strategic dilemmas the socialization of psychotherapy researchers. British Journal
of Clinical Psychology, 21, 119-127.
Hoshmond, L. T. (1989). Alternative research paradigms: A reviewed teaching proposal. Counseling
Psychologist, 17, (1), 3-79.
Howard, K. I., Orlinsky, D. E., Lueger, R. J. (1995). The design of clinically relevant outcome research:
Some considerations and an example. In M. Aneline & D. A. Shapiro, (Eds.). Research foundations
for psychotherapy practice. Sussex, England: Wiley, pp. 3-47.
Howard, K., Moras, K., Brill, P., Martinovich, Z., & Lutz, W. (1996). Evaluation of psychotherapy:
Efficacy, effectiveness, and patient progress. American Psychologist, 51, 1025-1030.
Hugdahl, H. & st, L. G. (1981). On the difference between statistical and clinical significance.
Behavioral Assessment, 3, 289-295.
Levy, R. (1981). On the nature of the clinical-research gap: The problems with some solutions. Behavioral
Assessment, 3, 235-242.
Magnusson, D. & Trestad, B. (1993). A holistic view of personality: A model revisited. Annual Review
of Psychology, 44, 427-452.
Mahrer, A. R. (1997). What are the breakthrough problems in the field of psychotherapy?
Psychotherapy, 34, 81-89.
Mahrer, A. & Nadler, W. P. (1986). Good moments in psychotherapy: A preliminary review, a list, some
promising research avenues. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 54, 10-15.
Maletzky, B. M. (1981). Clinical relevance and clinical research. Behavioral Assessment, 3, 283-288.
McCullough, J. P. (1984). Single-case investigative research and its relevance for the non-operant
clinician. Psychotherapy, 21, 382-390.
Meehl, P. (1978). Theoretical risks and tabular asterisks: Sir Karl, Sir Tonald and the slow progress of soft
psychology. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 46, 806-834.
Mintz, J., Drake, R. E., & Crits-Christoph, P. (1996). Efficacy and effectiveness of psychotherapy: Two
paradigms, one science. American Psychologist, 51, 1084-1085.
Norcross, J. C. (1997). Emerging breakthroughs in psychotherapy integration. Three predictions and one
fantasy. Psychotherapy, 34, 86-95.
Orlinsky, D. E. & Howard, K. I. (1978). The relation of process to outcome in psychotherapy, In S. L.
Garfield & A. E. Bergin, (Eds.). Handbook of psychotherapy and behavior change: An empirical
analysis. New York: Wiley & Sons, pp. 283-329.
Parsons, J. B. (1991). Psychotherapy outcome studies do not accurately represent current models of
psychotherapy. American Psychologist, 46, 99-106.
Rice, L. & Greenberg, L. S. (1984). Patterns of change: Intensive analysis of psychotherapy process. N.
Y.: Garfield Press.
Rorer, L. & Widiger, T. (1983). Personality structure and assessment. In M. Rosenzweig & Porter, L.
(Eds.). Annual review of psychology, 34, 431-464.

Les Collectifs du Cirp


Volume 1 (dition spciale), pp. 119-129.
2010 Cirp (Cercle interdisciplinaire de recherches phnomnologiques)
ISBN 978-0-9866654-1-7

Barbro M. Giorgi

129

Sechrest, L. McKnight, P., & McKnight, K. (1996). Calibration of measures for psychotherapy outcome
studies. American Psychologist, 51, 1065-1071.
Seligman, M. E. P. (1995). The effectiveness of psychotherapy: The Consumer Reports study. American
Psychologist, 50, 965-974.
Seligman, M. E. P. (1996). Science as an ally of practice. American Psychologist, 51, 1072-1079.
Shapiro, D. A. & Shapiro, D. (1983). Comparative therapy outcome research: Methodological
implications of meta-analysis. Journal of Counseling and Clinical Psychology, 51, 42-53,
Sher, K. J. & Trull, T. J. (1996). Methodological issues in psychopathology research. Annual Review of
Psychology, 47, 371-400.
Singer, J. (1980). The scientific of therapeutic practice: Question of value and ethics. Psychotherapy:
Theory, Research and Practice, 17, 369-375.
Strupp, H. H. (1996). The tripartite model and The Consumer Reports Study. American Psychologist, 51,
1017-1024.
Strupp, H. H. (1997). Research, practice and managed care. Psychotherapy, 34, 91-94.
Tedeschi, J., Heister, D., & Gahagan, J. (1969). Trust and the prisoners dilemma game. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 43-50.
Von Knorring-Giorgi, B. (1998). A phenomenological analysis of the experience of pivotal moments in
therapy as defined by clients. Unpublished Ph.D. Dissertation, Universit du Qubec Montral.
Wiggins, J. S. & Pincus, A. L. (1992). Personality: Structure and assessment. Annual Review of
Psychology, 43, 473-504.

Les Collectifs du Cirp


Volume 1 (dition spciale), pp. 119-129.
2010 Cirp (Cercle interdisciplinaire de recherches phnomnologiques)
ISBN 978-0-9866654-1-7

Você também pode gostar