Você está na página 1de 12

Children's Perceptions of Corporal Punishment, Caretaker Acceptance, and Psychological

Adjustment in a Poor, Biracial Southern Community


Author(s): Ronald P. Rohner, Shana L. Bourque, Carlos A. Elordi
Source: Journal of Marriage and Family, Vol. 58, No. 4 (Nov., 1996), pp. 842-852
Published by: National Council on Family Relations
Stable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/353974
Accessed: 27/01/2010 22:26

Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of JSTOR's Terms and Conditions of Use, available at
http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp. JSTOR's Terms and Conditions of Use provides, in part, that unless
you have obtained prior permission, you may not download an entire issue of a journal or multiple copies of articles, and you
may use content in the JSTOR archive only for your personal, non-commercial use.

Please contact the publisher regarding any further use of this work. Publisher contact information may be obtained at
http://www.jstor.org/action/showPublisher?publisherCode=ncfr.

Each copy of any part of a JSTOR transmission must contain the same copyright notice that appears on the screen or printed
page of such transmission.

JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of
content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms
of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact support@jstor.org.

National Council on Family Relations is collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to
Journal of Marriage and Family.

http://www.jstor.org
RONALDP. ROHNER,SHANAL. BOURQUE,AND CARLOSA. ELORDI
University of Connecticut

Children's Perceptions of CorporalPunishment,


CaretakerAcceptance, and Psychological Adjustment
in a Poor, Biracial SouthernCommunity

This study explores two related questions about A large national, and even international, contro-
relationships between perceived justness and per- versy exists over whether corporal punishment
ceived harshness of corporal punishment, per- has negative effects on children's behavioral and
ceived caretaker acceptance-rejection, and chil- psychological adjustment (Simons, Johnson, &
dren's psychological adjustment: Are children's Conger, 1994). Even though the popular press
perceptions of caretaker harshness and unjust- and many child advocates criticize the use of cor-
ness of physical punishment associated with chil- poral punishment, Straus (1977, 1994) and others
dren's psychological maladjustment? Or does the (Wauchope & Straus, 1990) estimated that
relationship between punishment and maladjust- 90%-97% of the children in the United States
ment disappear after controlling for perceived have been physically punished at some time in
caretaker acceptance-rejection? The research is their lives. Critics of corporal punishment often
based on a proportional, stratified, random sam- cite research linking its use with negative out-
ple of 281 Black and Whiteyouths in grades 3-12 comes such as increased aggression, delinquency,
within the public school system of a poor, biracial and psychosocial maladjustment in children
county of southeastern Georgia. Results of struc- (Greven, 1991; Kandel, 1991; Larzelere, 1986;
tural equation modeling suggest that physical McCord, 1988; Straus, 1991; Turner & Finkelhor,
punishment is associated with children's psycho- 1996). Harsh physical punishment, in particular,
logical maladjustment only if punishment is seen has been correlated with increased displays of ag-
by youths as a form of caretaker rejection. The gressive behavior by children (Howes & El-
findings contribute information to an ongoing de- deredge, 1985). Additionally, Bryan and Freed
bate about the relationship between physical pun- (1982) found that college students who reported
ishment and children's psychological adjustment. receiving a high level of physical punishment
when they were younger were significantly more
likely to report probleits with aggression. In addi-
Centerfor the Study of ParentalAcceptanceand Rejection, tion to these reports of increased behavioral prob-
School of Family Studies, Universityof Connecticut,U-58, lems, Stemberg et al. (1993) found that the use of
Storrs,CT 06269-2058(rohner@uconnvm.uconn.edu). physical punishment is associated with poor self-
esteem and emotional problems of youths.
Key Words: children's psychological adjustment, corporal
punishment, parental acceptance-rejection.

842 Journal of Marriage and the Family 58 (November 1996): 842-852


Perceptions of Corporal Punishment 843

Researchers on the other side of the debate cite ishment did have a slight correlation with psycho-
conflicting evidence and believe that many of the logical maladjustment, it was negatively associat-
studies linking physical punishment with negative ed with children's psychological adjustment most
consequences have methodological problems that prominently when it was perceived by them to be
forbid making such conclusions (Becker, 1964; a form of caretakerrejection.
Erlanger, 1979; Kandel, 1991; Simons et al., Because social scientists are seriously divided
1994). Erlanger, for example, found that the cor- over the apparent effects of corporal punishment
relation between childhood punishment and adult and because there is such widespread use of this
violence was, in fact, rather low. Baumrind form of discipline, it is importantto understandits
(1966, 1973,1994) argued that when physical true impact. It is clear that relationships among
punishment is used within a loving family envi- corporal punishment, psychological adjustment,
ronment, it is effective in reducing unwanted be- and other developmental sequellae need to be de-
havior without increasing aggression. Agnew fined more accurately. This study seeks to expand
(1983) found that corporal punishment is associ- on the work of Rohner et al. (1991) by analyzing
ated with higher rates of delinquency only when interrelationships among physical punishment,
the demands placed on children are inconsistent. perceived caretaker acceptance-rejection, and
If parental demands on the child are consistent, youths' psychological adjustment. Specifically,
however, corporal punishment was reported to be this article seeks to answer the following ques-
negatively correlated with delinquency. Conclu- tions: Are children's perceptions of caretaker
sions such as these point to a complex relation- harshness and the unjustness of physical punish-
ship between corporal punishment and its corre- ment associated with children's psychological
lates. Moreover, Lefkowitz, Eron, Walder, and maladjustment?Or, does the relationship between
Huesman (1977) concluded that the relationship punishment and maladjustment disappear after
between physical punishment and aggression is controlling for perceived caretaker acceptance-
curvilinear, where high levels of physical punish- rejection?
ment are associated with increased aggression,
but moderate levels are not. THE RESEARCHSETTING
Critics of past research also contend that many
of these studies are methodologically flawed be- Research for this study was conducted in a poor,
cause they fail to differentiate between the impact biracial (Black and White) county of southeastern
of corporal punishment and the influence of other Georgia. As is true for much of the American
dimensions of parenting, such as parental nonin- South (Flynn, 1994), corporal punishment is
volvement and parental rejection (Becker, 1964; widely accepted within McIntosh County, the re-
Rohner, Kean, & Cournoyer, 1991; Simons et al., search site. Two thirds of the parents there agree
1994). Simons et al., for example, found that the that children should be physically punished when
lack of parental support and involvement was they misbehave. In addition, 91% of the county
more strongly associated with psychological mal- children have been physically punished at some
adjustment than was the use of corporal punish- point during their lives. However, 25% of these
ment. After controlling for parental involvement, children have been punished physically only once
physical punishment was left unrelated to adoles- or twice.
cent aggressiveness, delinquency, or psychologi-
cal adjustment. Similarly, Larzelere, Klein, METHOD
Shumm, and Alibrando (1989) found that the
negative correlations between spanking and Sample
youths' self-esteem and perceptions of fairness of
punishment disappeared after controlling for the The sample included 281 Black and White school
influence of positive parent-child communication. children ranging in age from 8 through 18 years
Finally, Rohner, Kean, and Couroyer studied the (M = 12.5, SD = 3.1) in grades 3 through 12. It
influence of corporal punishment on youths' psy- was a proportional (35%) random sample, strati-
chological adjustment as mediated by perceived fied for sex, race, and grade in school. The sam-
caretaker acceptance-rejection. The authors ar- ple represented all socioeconomic levels from
gued that many of the reported effects of physical middle class (22%) to the chronically poor (13%).
punishment mimic the effects of parental rejec- However, the majority of children (65%) came
tion. They found that even though corporal pun- from skilled, semi-skilled, or unskilled working-
844 Journal of Marriage and the Family

class families (on a 6-point scale, M = 3.6, SD = hostility-aggression); "my mother ignores me as
1.3). Children's social class was determined by long as I do nothing to bother her" (perceived
whichever parent held the higher status occupa- indifference-neglect); "my mother does not really
tion. Forty-six percent of the children lived at or love me" (perceived undifferentiated rejection).
below the poverty level. The sample contained Youths responded to items such as these on a 4-
about equal numbers of females (54%) and males point Likert-like scale ranging from almost al-
(46%). Blacks (54%) and Whites (46%) were also ways true to almost never true.
about evenly distributed. The sample included The items are keyed in such a way that the
only one child per household, and it excluded all higher the score, the more rejection youths per-
known foreign national children, as well as chil- ceive. The sum of items on the PARQ (without
dren with serious mental handicaps. the warmth-affection and control scales) results in
an overall acceptance-rejection score that can
Instruments and Procedures range from a low of 40-indicating maximum ac-
ceptance-to a high of 160-indicating maxi-
The research reported here is part of a larger mum rejection. Scores of 100 or more on this re-
ethnographic and quantitative study within the duced version of the PARQ/Control indicate that
county. This study is based on four standardized the child experienced more caretaker rejection
self-report questionnaires. The questionnaires than acceptance. Evidence regarding the validity
were administered to the 281 youths during and reliability of the PARQ (Rohner, 1991, 1995;
school hours, and they were administered in a Rohner & Cournoyer, 1994) demonstrates that
counterbalanced order to minimize potential re- this measure is psychometrically sound. This
sponse bias. Each of the instruments is described body of evidence is supported in McIntosh Coun-
below. ty itself where, for example, coefficient alphas on
the PARQ scales range from .78 to .94.
Parental Acceptance-Rejection/Control Question-
naire (PARQ/Control). Youths reported their per- Physical Punishment Questionnaire (PPQ). On
ceptions of their major caretaker's (usually moth- the first part of the questionnaire (Rohner, 1995)
ers) acceptance-rejection on the child version of youths indicated the level of physical punishment
the PARQ/Control (Rohner, 1991). The child they experienced at the hands of their major care-
PARQ/Control consists of 73 items that ask chil- taker. The second part of the questionnaire asked
dren to reflect on their experiences of acceptance- youths to reflect on their experiences of physical
rejection and control (from permissiveness to punishment received from their major disciplinar-
strictness) within their families. It is made up of ian. The caretaker and disciplinarian, however,
five separate scales. Four of the scales-warmth- could be the same person. Respondents were
affection, hostility-aggression, indifference- asked to make an overall, global judgment about
neglect, and undifferentiated rejection-relate di- nine issues related theoretically to the experience
rectly to perceived acceptance or rejection by the of corporal punishment. These issues included
caretaker. Undifferentiated rejection refers to a overall frequency of punishment, overall severity
child's feelings of being unloved or rejected with- of punishment, consistency of punishment, pre-
out necessarily having any objective indicators dictability of punishment, average incidence of
present of caretaker coldness, aggression, or ne- physical punishment received in any week, per-
glect. The fifth scale relates to perceived control ceived fairness of punishment, perceived de-
(permissiveness-strictness). The warmth-affection servedness of punishment, timing of punishment
scale (20 items) and the control scale (13 items) (whether it followed immediately a wrongdoing
were not used in the portion of the research re- or whether it was delayed), and caretaker's or dis-
ported here because the control scale was not rel- ciplinarian's use of an explanation of what was
evant to the major concerns of this article, and the wrong about the punished behavior. Each of these
warmth-affection scale did not fit well the mea- nine aspects of physical punishment is treated as a
surement model discussed later. discrete single-item scale.
Youths responded to the PARQ/Control by The PPQ also includes 13 forms of punish-
making judgments about how well each statement ment that a child may or may not have experi-
described their caretaker's treatment of them. Ex- enced. These include spanking, slapping, shoving,
amples of test items include: "My mother goes yanking, kicking, beating severely with an object
out of her way to hurt my feelings" (perceived (leaving a mark on the body), hitting firmly but
Perceptions of Corporal Punishment 845

not severely (no mark is left on the body), pulling dren's perceptions and said that the punishment
hair, twisting an ear, making the child kneel on was "very fair" when the children reported it to
hard objects, making the child stand for a long be "very unfair." Finally, 41% of the caretakers
time, pinching, and shaking. A total score is cre- agreed with their children within 1 point (on a 4-
ated from the sum of all forms of corporal punish- point scale) about the relative deservedness of
ment experienced at least once. This total score punishment. Here, the majority (77%) of caretak-
comprises the 13 items listed plus the possibility ers reported their punishing behaviors to be "al-
of up to four additional write-in forms of physical most always" deserved. The majority (71%) of
punishment. Thus the scale for forms of punish- children, on the other hand, said the punishment
ment experienced can range from a low of 0 was "sometimes" (57%) or "often" (14%) de-
(never punished physically in any way) to a high served, but only 21% said it was "almost always"
of 17. As shown in Table 1, significant intercorre- deserved. Overall, however, caretakers and chil-
lations allow two additional composite scales to dren agreed in more than 90% of the cases that
be created. The incidence of punishment and the the punishment was deserved more often than not.
severity of punishment items may be summed to Additional evidence about the validity and relia-
create an overall measure of the perceived harsh- bility of the Physical Punishment Questionnaire is
ness of punishment, and the fairness of punish- provided in Rohner (in press).
ment and deservedness of punishment items may
be summed to create an overall measure of the Family Information Sheet (FIS). The youth form
perceived justness of punishment. Both of these of the FIS (Rohner, 1991) elicits information
independent, composite variables are used in this about the child's age, sex, race, grade in school,
research. Perceived harshness of punishment in and social class, as well as who the major caretak-
this sample ranged from a low of 0 (never pun- er is and the whereabouts of the child's mother
ished) to a high of 16 (punished 12 or more times and father. Moreover, the FIS elicits information
a week, and when punished, it was very hard). about the child's place of residence, household
Perceived justness of punishment ranges on the size and composition, and other sociodemograph-
PPQ from 2 (very unfair and almost never de- ic information.
served) to 8 (very fair and almost always de-
served). Personality Assessment Questionnaire (PAQ).
Validity of the children's reports about care- Youths reported on their own psychological ad-
takers' punishment is provided by a random, justment by responding to the child version of the
stratified subsample of caretakers who responded PAQ (Rohner, 1991). Here, youths responded to
to the parent version of the PPQ. The Parent PPQ 42 items addressing seven personality disposi-
and Youth PPQ are nearly identical except that tions that are expected theoretically and empiri-
the youth version says, "My caretaker does . . . " cally to be related to parental acceptance-
whereas the parent version says, "I do .... The rejection. Research reported here incorporates six
level of agreement between caretakers and chil- of the seven scales. The seventh scale, depen-
dren on each of the four specific punishment dence, is dropped here for reasons discussed in
items used here was high. More particularly, 88% Rohner (1986). Thus the abbreviated version of
of the caretakers agreed with their children to the PAQ used here comprises 36 items divided
within 1 point difference (on a 13-point scale) into six scales, including hostility-aggression,
about the average weekly incidence of corporal self-esteem, self-adequacy, emotional responsive-
punishment. That is, the child might have report- ness, emotional stability, and worldview. Exam-
ed being punished once a week, on the average. ples of scale items include: "I think about fighting
Eighty-eight percent of the caretakers either or being mean" (hostility-aggression); "I like my-
agreed with this number or said zero times per self' (positive self-esteem); "I can compete suc-
week or twice a week. Additionally, 73% of the cessfully for the things I want" (positive self-
caretakers agreed with their children's reports adequacy); "it is easy for me to show my friends
within 1 point (on a 4-point scale) about the that I really like them" (emotional responsive-
severity of the punishment. Moreover, 65% of the ness); "I am cheerful and happy one minute and
caretakers agreed with their children's report gloomy or unhappy the next" (emotional instabil-
within 1 point (on a 4-point scale) about the over- ity); "I think the world is a good, happy place"
all fairness of the punishment. Only 9% of the (positive worldview). Youths responded to such
caretakers disagreed substantially with their chil- items on a 4-point Likert-like scale ranging from
846 Journal of Marriage and the Family

almost always true of me to almost never true of (M = 11.72, SD = 4.03). The difference between
me. these two groups in mean level of hostility-
The PAQ is keyed in such a way that the high- aggression is significant, t(168) = 4.43, p < .001),
er the score, the higher the level of psychological thus adding confirmatory evidence about the con-
maladjustment. Items are summed to reveal an current validity of children's self-reports on the
overall PAQ score, which can range on the abbre- PAQ. Extensive additional evidence of validity and
viated PAQ from 36 (indicating positive psycho- reliability reported by Rohner (1991, 1995) con-
logical adjustment) to 144 (indicating negative firms the psychometric soundness of this measure.
psychological adjustment). A score of 90 or more This body of evidence is supported in McIntosh
indicates more self-reported psychological malad- County where coefficient alpha for the PAQ as an
justment than adjustment. overall measure of psychological adjustmentis .88.
Because psychological adjustment is the major Alphas for individual scales are: hostility-
outcome variable in this research and because all aggression, .74; self-adequacy, .55; self-esteem,
the major research instruments used here are self- .63; emotional responsiveness, .50; emotional
reports, it is especially importantto be able to doc- stability, .58, and; worldview, .74.
ument the validity of children's reporting about
their own psychological adjustment using the RESULTS
PAQ. Here, two external sources of evidence from
foster parents and from a school social worker and Table 1 shows that the mean acceptance-rejection
several guidance counselors are particularly ger- score for children in the county is 66.01 (SD =
mane. In the first source, Coumoyer (1989) found 18.92). This score demonstrates that most of the
in a sample of 50 foster families that the children's children in the county feel loved and accepted by
PAQ scores correlatedstrongly (r = .64, p = .0001) their major caretakers. Six percent of the children,
with foster mothers' reports about the children's however, scored above the midpoint of 100, re-
behavior and psychological adjustment on the vealing that they experience significantly more
Child Behavior Checklist (Achenbach & Edel- rejection than acceptance. The majority of chil-
brock, 1983). Further evidence of the validity of dren in the county are relatively well adjusted
children's self-reported psychological adjustment psychologically, with a mean psychological ad-
on the PAQ comes directly from third- through justment score of 71.44 (SD = 14.61). Scores
eighth-grade students in McIntosh County itself. above the midpoint of 90 were reported for 16%
There, the school social worker and several guid- of the children, indicating that these children have
ance counselors evaluated each sample child's psy- significant self-reported psychological adjustment
chological adjustment on a 4-point scale, from no problems. As expected from prior studies that ex-
discernible psychological problem (0), to mild (1), amined parental acceptance-rejection theory
moderate (2), and severe (3) problems. One or two (Rohner, 1986; Rohner & Rohner, 1980), a corre-
counselors included in their evaluation such pe- lation coefficient of .50 (p < .001) significantly
ripherallyrelevant considerations as "dysfunctional links psychological adjustment with perceived
family" and "health problems." Despite these parental acceptance-rejection.
marginally relevant criteria, the evaluations of the Children's perception of caretaker acceptance-
social worker and counselors correlated signifi- rejection, perceived harshness of punishment, and
cantly with the children's overall PAQ scores (r = perceived justness of punishment, as well as chil-
.26, p < .001, n = 181). The most common single dren's psychological adjustment, tended not to
form of psychological adjustment problem identi- vary significantly by age, sex, race, or social
fied by the social worker and counselors was class. The only exceptions to this conclusion were
anger-aggression. On the average, youths who found in the fact that White youths in the county
manifested these problems in school (codes 1-3 (M = 63.15, SD = 15.84) tended to report slightly
above) reported themselves to be above the mid- more overall caretaker acceptance (t = 2.35, p =
point on the PAQ's hostility-aggression scale (M = .023) than did Black youths (M = 68.31, SD =
6.19, SD = 3.82). Scores above the midpoint indi- 20.94). Similarly, White youths (M = 5.80, SD =
cate significant self-reportedproblems with hostili- 1.71) tended to perceive caretaker punishment to
ty and aggression. On the other hand, youths who be slightly more just (t = -4.03, p. = .001) than
were reportedby the social worker and counselors did Black youths (M = 4.87, SD = .43). Addition-
to have no discernible hostility-aggression problem ally, the older the children were, the less frequent-
(code 0 above), self-reported well below this level ly and less severely (i.e., the less harshly) they
Perceptions of Corporal Punishment 847

TABLE 1. DESCRIPTIVE STATISTICS AND INTERCORRELATIONSAMONG MEASURES OF CORPORAL PUNISHMENT,


PERCEIVED CARETAKER ACCEPTANCE-REJECTION, AND YOUTHS' PSYCHOLOGICALADJUSTMENT

Variable M SD 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Punishment
1. Harshness 4.02 2.13
2. Incidence 1.66 1.65 .905**
(171)
3. Severity 2,27 .98 .664*** .282***
(171) (171)
4. Justness 5.30 1.63 -.075 -.073 -.021
(170) (171) (189)
5. Fairness 2.81 1.00 -.132 -.114 -.090 .847***
(170) (171) (186) (186)
6. Deserved 2.49 .95 .013 -.003 .051 .830*** .406***
(170) (171) (185) (186) (186)
7. Acceptance-
rejection 66.01 18.92 .322*** .265*** .268*** -.401*** -.439*** -.246***
(171) (172) (187) (186) (187) (186)
8. Psychological
adjustment 71.44 14.61 .176* .226** .036 -.273*** -.275*** -.183* .501***
(171) (172) (187) (186) (187) (186) (281)
Note: Numbersin parenthesesbeneathcorrelationsare in's.
*p <.05. **p <.01. ***p <.001

tended to be punished (r = -.30, p < .001). Final- week. The average harshness of punishment re-
ly, there was a slight but significant tendency for ported by children in McIntosh county is well
children from higher social classes to perceive below the midpoint of 9, at 4.02 (SD = 2.13), in-
more caretaker acceptance than children from dicating that these children reported being pun-
lower classes (r = .14, p < .05). ished not very often, and when they were pun-
The magnitude of reported racial and social ished, they experienced the punishment as being
class differences in perceived caretaker not very hard. On the average (M = 5.30, SD =
acceptance-rejection and justness of punish- 1.63), county children felt that physical punish-
ment-though statistically significant-is trivial ment was not wholly just, but that overall it was
from a theoretical and practical point of view. equitable. Forty-two percent of the children in
The reported age differences in perceptions of this sample viewed punishment as clearly more
harshness of punishment, however, may not be just than unjust.
trivial. A hierarchical regression analysis was run Table 1 also provides the intercorrelations
to assess the possible influence of children's age, among measures of corporal punishment, per-
sex, race, and social class on relationships among ceived caretaker acceptance-rejection, and
perceived harshness and justness of punishment, youths' psychological adjustment. The table
perceived caretaker acceptance-rejection, and shows that perceived harshness of physical pun-
children's psychological adjustment. None of ishment is only weakly correlated (r = .18, p <
these control variables added significantly to the .05) with psychological adjustment. It is possi-
prediction of psychological adjustment above and ble, though, that the Pearson's correlation hides
beyond caretaker acceptance-rejection and per- any curvilinear relationship that may be present.
ceived harshness and justness of punishment. For example, low levels of perceived harshness
Thus, in analyzing further results of this research, may not impact negatively on children's psycho-
we pooled the sample across age, gender, race, logical adjustment, but when punishment be-
and social class differences. comes increasingly harsh, psychological adjust-
Children in McIntosh County reported being ment may become increasingly impaired. In order
physically punished an average of less than once to test this possibility, we ran two multiple regres-
a week (M = .76 times per week, SD = 1.49). One sion analyses. First, psychological adjustment
percent of the children, however, reported receiv- was regressed onto perceived harshness. Second,
ing physical punishment more than seven times a to incorporate curvilinearity into the model, a
848 Journal of Marriage and the Family

squared perceived harshness variable was added. TABLE 2. INTERCORRELATIONSAMONG LATENT VARIABLES
IN FIGURES 1 AND 2
The addition of the transformed perceived harsh-
ness variable did not significantly improve the LatentVariable 1 2 3
predictability of the model (p = .55). Thus it ap- 1. Harshness
pears that the relationship between harshness of 2. Unjustness .16
punishment and children's psychological adjust- 3. Acceptance-rejection .49 .50
ment is not appreciably curvilinear. 4. Psychological adjustment .26 .26 .53
Because perceived harshness of punishment Note: n = 186.
appeared to have only a weak linear and no curvi-
linear relationship with children's psychological ables is mediated by perceived caretaker
adjustment, another test was carried out to ex- acceptance-rejection. To explore this question, we
plore a possible threshold effect, where harshness analyzed two structural equation models with la-
of punishment might have no measurable effect tent variables (Bollen, 1989; Hayduk, 1987;
on children's psychological adjustment until it
Kenny, 1979; Loehlin, 1992), displayed in
reaches a specified level. In order to explore this
Figures 1 and 2. LISREL VII with the maximum
possibility, we performed a t test contrasting the likelihood fitting function was used (Joreskog &
mean psychological adjustment scores of children S6rbom, 1989). All path coefficients are standard-
who experienced harsh versus nonharsh punish- ized. Latent variables or constructs in Figures 1
ment. Results showed that the mean psychologi- and 2 are displayed within the circles, observed
cal adjustment score (M = 72.47, SD = 16.06) of variables are displayed within rectangles, and
children who were punished on the average of measurement errors (6s) are left unenclosed.
less than once a week and who felt the punish- Intercorrelations among latent variables are pre-
ment was not hard when it did occur (i.e., non- sented in Table 2.
harsh punishment) contrasted significantly, t(44) The first model (Figure 1) explored the direct
= 2.50, p = .02, with mean psychological adjust-
impact of perceived harshness and perceived un-
ment scores (M = 82.57, SD = 10.72) of children
justness of punishment on children's psychologi-
who were punished on the average of three or cal adjustment. The model also explored the indi-
more times a week and who felt the punishment rect influence of these variables, mediated by
varied from a little hard to very hard (i.e., harsh children's perceptions of caretakers' acceptance-
punishment). The mean score for the latter group rejection. We made no assumption about a causal
was close to the midpoint of 90 on the modified
relationship between perceived harshness and
version of the PAQ, indicating the presence of
perceived unjustness of punishment. Therefore,
significant psychological maladjustment. we merely computed the correlation between
Table 1 also reveals that perceived justness of these variables. All loadings in the measurement
punishment and psychological adjustment are sig- model were significant (p < .01). The structural
nificantly but negatively correlated (r = -.27, p < equation model fit the data well, X2(49, n = 186)
.0001). As perceived justness of punishment in- = 51.75, p = .367. Both of the direct paths from
creases, psychological adjustment also increases. perceived harshness and from perceived unjust-
Moreover, perceived justness of physical punish- ness to psychological adjustment were nonsignifi-
ment is significantly but negatively correlatedwith cant. The indirect paths from perceived harshness
perceived caretakerrejection (r = -.40, p < .0001), and perceived unjustness mediated by perceived
revealing that the more just youths perceive pun- acceptance-rejection, however, were significant
ishment to be, the more accepted they feel. (p < .001). From these results, we concluded that,
Both perceived harshness of punishment (r = after controlling for the influence of perceived
.32, p < .001) and perceived justness of punish- acceptance-rejection, perceived harshness and un-
ment (r = -.40, p < .001) are more strongly corre-
justness of punishment by themselves do not
lated with children's perceptions of caretaker make significant contributions to variations in
acceptance-rejection than with children's psycho- children's psychological adjustment.
logical adjustment (r = .18, p < .05; and r = -.27, Because of these findings, the nonsignificant
p < .001, respectively). These relationships raise direct paths from harshness and justness were re-
the question of whether perceived harshness and moved in Model 2. Deleting the nonsignificant
perceived justness by themselves are directly paths from the model did not substantially change
related to children's psychological adjustment or the fit of the model. A third model then was creat-
whether the influence of one or both of these vari- ed (Figure 2) with the paths from perceived
Perceptions of Corporal Punishment 849

FIGURE 1. WHOLE MODEL OF RELATIONSHIPS AMONG CORPORAL PUNISHMENT, PERCEIVED ACCEPTANCE-REJECTION,


AND CHILDREN'S PSYCHOLOGICALADJUSTMENT

8 6

.07
.
V

6 .

r
Unfair Undeserved
t t
5 5 2(49, n = 186) = 51.75, p = .37
GFI = .96

Note: n = 186.
**p <.01. ***p <.001.

harshness and perceived unjustness of punish- Finally, it is possible that the relationship be-
ment constrained to be equal. The X2 difference tween harsh or unjust punishment and children's
test between Model 1 and Model 3 was nonsignif- psychological adjustment might be contingent on
icant (X2= 1.62, df = 2, ns). Similarly, the differ- the child's gender, race, age, or social class. For
ence between Model 2 and Model 3 was non- example, boys might differ from girls, or younger
significant (X2= .03, df= 1, ns). This left the final children might differ from older children in their
trimmed model shown in Figure 2, X2(52, n = responses to perceptions of harsh or unjust pun-
186) = 53.67, p = .41. Both perceived harshness ishment. In order to examine these possibilities, a
and perceived unjustness contribute to variations series of multiple regression analyses were run. In
in children's psychological adjustment only when these tests we regressed children's psychological
they are mediated by the child's perceptions of adjustment onto their perceptions of acceptance-
caretaker acceptance-rejection. rejection, punishment (harshness and unjustness),
850 Journalof Marriageand the Family

FIGURE 2. RELATIONSHIIPSAMONG CARETAKERS' BEHAVIOR AND CHILDREN'S PSYCHOLOGICALADJUSTMENT WITH PATHS


FROM HARSHNESS AND UNJUSTNESS SET TO BE EQUAL

6 5

.42***
V

8
6

.43***

6 6 X2(52, n = 186) = 53.67, p = .41


GFI = .96

Note: n = 186.
**p <.01. ***tp<.001.

and the product variable representing the interac- DISCUSSION


tion term (e.g., gender by harsh punishment).
None of the interactions is significant. That is, the Preliminary analyses revealed that both perceived
harshness and perceived justness of punishment
psychological adjustment of boys does not differ
from that of girls under varying conditions of are more strongly correlated with children's per-
harsh or unjust punishment and perceived care- ceptions of caretaker acceptance-rejection than
taker acceptance-rejection. Similarly, relation- they are with children's psychological adjust-
ment. Additionally, analyses of two structural
ships among perceived punishment, acceptance-
rejection, and children's psychological adjust- equation models showed that the direct paths
ment are not contingent on children's age, race, or from perceived harshness and perceived justness
social class. to psychological adjustment were nonsignificant,
indicating that these two measures of physical
Perceptions of Corporal Punishment 851

punishment by themselves are not significantly race, or social class. That is, the psychological ad-
related to variations in children's psychological justment of younger children does not appear to
adjustment. However, the indirect paths from be significantly different from that of older chil-
these two variables, mediated by perceived care- dren under varying levels of harsh or unjust pun-
taker acceptance-rejection, were significant, re- ishment and caretaker acceptance-rejection. Simi-
vealing that these measures of physical punish- larly, the psychological adjustment of boys (vs.
ment are associated with poor psychological ad- girls), Blacks (vs. Whites), or working-class (vs.
justment when they are seen by children to be a middle-class) children does not appear to be sig-
form of caretaker rejection. These results support nificantly different under these conditions.
conclusions by Agnew (1983), Baumrind (1994), A word of caution should be introduced at this
Erlanger (1979), and Simons et al. (1994), who point. Specifically, this research is based princi-
contend that corporal punishment does not neces- pally on children's self-reports. Even though ex-
sarily negatively affect children's behavior and teral evidence was available to support most of
psychological adjustment. These results also sup- these reports, it is possible that we might have
port the work of Rohner et al. (1991), who con- reached somewhat different conclusions if other
cluded that the love and acceptance children feel sources of data had been used. Moreover, even
from their major caregivers is much more strong- though the measures of corporal punishment used
ly related to children's psychological well-being here are not directly related to the children's psy-
than is physical punishment per se. chological adjustment measured by the PAQ,
Results from this research do not support con- other unmeasured indicators of children's adjust-
clusions by Lefkowitz et al. (1977) and others ment (such as depression, suicidal ideation, with-
who report a curvilinear relationship between drawal, or conduct problems) might be. Unex-
physical punishment and adjustment issues. Re- plained variability in the measures of psychologi-
gression analyses here revealed that a transformed cal adjustment used here suggests that important
perceived harshness variable, thought to capture a but unmeasured factors influence children's psy-
curvilinear relationship, did not significantly im- chological adjustment. Further research is war-
prove the predictability of the model. Evidence ranted to identify these factors (such as the influ-
provided here does suggest, however, the exis- ence of perceived predictability or unpredictabili-
tence of a threshold effect where corporal punish- ty of punishment or the consistency or
ment appears to have no measurably negative in- inconsistency of punishment). Despite these limi-
fluence on children's psychological adjustment tations, however, at least one major conclusion
until it reaches a specific magnitude of perceived stands out. The influence of perceived parental
harshness or perceived unjustness. More specifi- acceptance-rejection should be considered in any
cally, children who reported being punished less future inquiry into the relationship between chil-
than once a week and who perceived this punish- dren's experiences with corporal punishment and
ment to be not hard also reported significantly their psychological adjustment.
less psychological maladjustment than children
who reported being punished three or more times NOTE
a week and who perceived the punishment to be a
little hard or very hard. Children who exceeded This article is based on field research in McIntosh
this latter threshold had an average psychological County, Georgia, from August, 1993, through January,
1994. The work was funded by a grant to the first au-
adjustment score that indicated the presence of thor by the University of Connecticut Research Founda-
significant psychological maladjustment. Very tion. The authors gratefully acknowledge the assistance
high levels of perceived harshness of physical of Nancy D. Rohner, who participated in all aspects of
data collection and data reduction and in the prepara-
punishment, therefore, may be directly related to tion of this article. We also acknowledge the research
negative psychological adjustment. Further re- assistance of Ms. Johnnie M. Sharpe, who helped so ca-
search is needed to explore this question and the pably with data collection in her native county. We
concept of a threshold effect itself, in association thank, too, the McIntosh County Board of Education
with perceived caretaker acceptance-rejection. and Dr. Hannah Tostensen, superintendent of schools,
for allowing us to work with children in the public
Finally, results of this research suggest that re- schools. And we express our appreciation to all the
lationships among perceived harsh and unjust principals, teachers, other school personnel, parents,
punishment, perceived caretaker acceptance- and children who gave so graciously of their time and
rejection, and children's psychological adjust- expertise to help bring this project to completion.
ment are not contingent on children's age, gender,
852 Journal of Marriage and the Family

REFERENCES Lefkowitz, M., Eron, L., Walder, L., & Huesmann, L.


(1977). Growing up to be violent: A longitudinal
Achenbach, T. M., & Edelbrock, C. (1983). Manualfor study of the development of aggression. New York:
the child behavior checklist. Burlington: University Pergamon.
of Vermont, Department of Psychiatry. Loehlin, J. C. (1992). Latent variable models: An intro-
Agnew, R. (1983). Physical punishment and delinquen- duction to factor, path, and structural analyses (2nd
cy: A research note. Youthand Society, 15, 225-236. ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Baumrind, D. (1966). Effects of authoritative parental McCord, J. (1988). Parental behavior in the cycle of ag-
control on child behavior. Child Development, 37, gression. Psychiatry, 51, 14-23.
887-907. Rohner, R. P. (1986). The warmth dimension: Founda-
Baumrind, D. (1973). The development of instrumental tions of parental acceptance-rejection theory. New-
competence through socialization. In A. Pick (Ed.), bury Park, CA: Sage.
Minnesota symposium on child psychology (Vol. 7, Rohner, R. P. (1991). Handbook for the study of
pp. 3-46). Minneapolis: University of Minnesota parental acceptance and rejection. Center for the
Press. Study of Parental Acceptance and Rejection, Univer-
Baumrind, D. (1994). The social context of child mal- sity of Connecticut at Storrs.
treatment.Family Relations, 43, 360-368. Rohner, R. P. (1995). Parental acceptance and rejection
Becker, W. (1964). Consequences of different kinds of bibliography. Center for the Study of Parental Accep-
parental discipline. In M. L. Hoffman & L. W. Hoff- tance and Rejection, University of Connecticut at
man (Eds.), Review of child development research Storrs.
(Vol. 1, pp. 169-208). New York: Russell Sage Foun- Rohner, R. P. (in press). Physical punishment question-
dation. naire: Test manual. Center for the Study of Parental
Bollen, K. A. (1989). Structural equations with latent Acceptance and Rejection, University of Connecticut
variables. New York: Wiley. at Storrs.
Bryan, J., & Freed, F. (1982). Corporal punishment: Rohner, R. P., & Cournoyer, D. E. (1994) Universals
Normative data and sociological and psychological and cultural specifics in children's perceptions of
correlates in a community college population. Jour- parental acceptance and rejection: Evidence from fac-
nal of Youthand Adolescence, 11, 77-87. tor analyses within eight societies worldwide. Cross
Cournoyer, D. E. (1989). Response bias as an explana- Cultural Research, 28, 371-383.
tion for observed correlations between self-reported Rohner, R. P., Kean, K. J., & Cournoyer, D. E. (1991).
parental acceptance and self-reported psychological Effects of corporal punishment, perceived caretaker
adjustment. Unpublished manuscript, Center for the warmth, and cultural beliefs on the psychological ad-
Study of Parental Acceptance and Rejection, Univer- justment of children in St. Kitts, West Indies. Journal
sity of Connecticut at Storrs. of Marriage and the Family, 53, 681-693.
Erlanger, H. S. (1979). Childhood punishment experi- Rohner, R. P., & Rohner, E. C. (Eds.). (1980). World-
ence and adult violence. Children and YouthServices wide tests of parental acceptance-rejection theory. Be-
Review, 1, 75-86. havior Science Research, 15, 1-88.
Flynn, C. P. (1994). Regional differences in attitudes to- Simons, R. L., Johnson, C., & Conger, R. D. (1994).
ward corporal punishment. Journal of Marriage and Harsh corporal punishment versus quality of parental
the Family, 56, 314-324. involvement as an explanation of adolescent malad-
Greven, P. (1991). Spare the child: The religious roots justment. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 56,
of physical punishment and the psychological impact 591-607.
of physical abuse. New York: Knopf. Sternberg, K. J., Lamb, M. E., Greenbaum, C., Cicchet-
Hayduk, L. A. (1987). Structural equation modeling ti, D., Dawud, S., Cortes, R. M., Krispin, O., &
with LISREL: Essentials and advances. Baltimore: Lorey, F. (1993). Effects of domestic violence on
Johns Hopkins University Press. children's behavior problems and depression. Devel-
Howes, C., & Eldredge, R. (1985). Responses of abused opmental Psychology, 29, 44-52.
and neglected and nonmaltreated children to the be- Straus, M. A. (1977). A sociological perspective on pre-
haviors of peers. Journal of Applied Developmental vention and treatment of wife-beating. In M. Roy
Psychology, 6, 221-270. (Ed.), Battered women. New York: Van Nostrand-
Joreskog, K. G. & S6rbom, D. (1989). LISREL 7: A Reinhold.
guide to the program and applications (2nd ed.). Straus, M. A. (1991). Discipline and deviance: Physical
Chicago: SPSS. punishment of children and violence and other crimes
Kandel, E. (1991). Physical punishment and the devel- in adulthood. Social Problems, 38, 133-154.
opment of aggression and violent behavior: A review. Straus, M. A. (1994). Beating the devil out of them:
Unpublished manuscript, Family Research Laborato- Corporal punishment in American families. New
ry, University of New Hampshire at Durham. York: Lexington Books.
Kenny, D. A. (1979). Correlation and causality. New Turner, H. A., & Finkelhor, D. (1996). Corporal punish-
York: Wiley. ment as a stressor among youth. Journal of Marriage
Larzelere, R. E. (1986). Moderate spanking: Model or and the Family, 58, 155-166.
deterrentof children's aggression in the family? Jour- Wauchope, B. A., & Straus, M. A. (1990). Physical pun-
nal of Family Violence, 1, 27-36. ishment and physical abuse of American children: In-
Larzelere, R. E., Klein, M., Schumm, W. A., and Ali- cidence rates by age, gender, and occupational class.
brando, S., Jr. (1989). Relations of spanking and In M. A. Strauss & R. J. Gelles (Eds.), Physical vio-
other parenting characteristics to self-esteem and per- lence in American families: Risk factors and adapta-
ceived fairness of parental discipline. Psychological tions to violence in 8,145 families. New Brunswick,
Reports, 64, 1140-1142. NJ: Transaction Publishers.

Você também pode gostar