Você está na página 1de 10

Akinesia in Parkinson's disease. I.

Shortening of simple reaction time with


focal, single-pulse transcranial magnetic stimulation
A. Pascual-Leone, J. Valls-Sol, J. P. Brasil-Neto, L. G. Cohen and M. Hallett
Neurology 1994;44;884

This information is current as of July 20, 2008

The online version of this article, along with updated information and services, is
located on the World Wide Web at:
http://www.neurology.org

Neurology is the official journal of the American Academy of Neurology. Published


continuously since 1951, it is now a weekly with 48 issues per year. Copyright 1994 by AAN
Enterprises, Inc. All rights reserved. Print ISSN: 0028-3878. Online ISSN: 1526-632X.

Downloaded from www.neurology.org by NIZAR YAMANIE on July 20, 2008

Akinesia in Parkinsons disease.


I. Shortening of simple reaction time with
focal, single-pulse transcranial magnetic stimulation
A. Pascual-Leone, MD, PhD; J. Valls-Sol& MD, PhD; J.P. Brasil-Neto, MD;
L.G. Cohen, MD; and M. Hallett, MD

Article a b s t r a c G W e studied the effects of transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) of the motor cortex on simple reaction time (RT) in 10 patients with Parkinsons disease compared with 10 age-matched normal controls. The subjects
flexed their right elbow rapidly in response to a visual go-signal. In random trials, TMS was applied to the left motor
cortex at varying delays after the go-signal. In trials without TMS, RT was longer in the patients. However, in the trials with subthreshold TMS, RT in the patients became as fast as RT in trials without TMS in the controls. This shortening was associated with normalization of the voluntary triphasic EMG pattern and the pre-movement cortical excitability increase.
NEUROLOGY 1994;44:884-891

Delayed motor initiation (akinesia) and slowness of


ongoing movements (bradykinesia) are probably
the most debilitating manifestations of Parkinsons
disease (PD).1,2Akinesia is marked by a prolonged
reaction time (RT),while bradykinesia leads to prolonged movement time. The pathophysiologic
mechanisms responsible for these disturbances remain unclear. Content, selection, and assembly of
motor programs are essentially n ~ r m a l , l ,while
~-~
there is marked impairment in their execution.1,2,9-13
This may be related to the fact that in PD it takes
abnormally long to activate the motor cortex suffciently to initiate the execution of the motor program.2 If so, externally increasing the excitability
of the motor cortex may help improve RT.
In normal volunteers, transcranial magnetic
stimulation (TMS) delivered over the motor cortex
at intensities below motor threshold speeds up simple RT to acoustic, visual, and somatosensory go~igna1s.l~
The effects of TMS on RT are thought to be
due to modulation of intracortical connection^.^^-^^ In
the present study, we investigated the effects of
TMS on simple RT in patients with PD.
Methods. Subjects. We studied six men and four women
with PD, aged 48 to 73 years (mean, 62 years), whose
characteristics are summarized in the table. All patients
were evaluated by the Cognitive Neuroscience Section at
NINDS with a battery of neuropsychologic tests, and
none of the patients entered in this study showed any evidence of cognitive impairment. All patients were taking
carbidopailevodopa and deprenyl; patients 2, 4,5, and 7
were also on anticholinergic medications. The medication

regimen had been stable in all patients for a t least 4


months prior to the study. The experiments were conducted at the time of peak levodopa effect in all patients.
The peak levodopa effect was defined based on the patients subjective reports of their motor capabilities in relation to the time of the last levodopa dose and on serial
neurologic examinations between doses. In addition, patients l, 3, and 6 were also studied after withholding
their medications for 3 days in order to evaluate the effects of levodopa on the results.
We also studied 10 normal volunteers, six men and
four women, aged 52 to 71 years (mean, 60 years). All
these control subjects were naive to the RT task used in
the study, but all had participated in previous studies in
our laboratory and had experienced TMS. All subjects
gave written informed consent for participation in the
study, which had been approved by the institutional review board.
Experimental design. The subject was seated comfortably on a chair with the right arm slightly abducted a t
t h e shoulder and flexed 90 at the elbow so t h a t the
pronated arm rested on a horizontal platform. An auditory warning signal was followed at a random interval (1
to 5 seconds) by a visual go-signal. In response to the gosignal, the subject flexed the right elbow as rapidly as
possible to touch the right shoulder with the hand. The
visual go-signal was a flash generated by a Grass PS22
photic stimulator and delivered at 100% of the stimulators output intensity by a lamp positioned at eye level 30
cm in front of the subject.
The subjects were allowed some time to practice before
the experiments began in order to familiarize them with
t h e task. During 30 practice trials, the subjects were
given feedback about their RTs and encouraged to move
as fast as possible in order to minimize RT variability.

From the Human Cortical Physiology Unit, Human Motor Control Section, Medical Neurology Branch, National Institute of Neurological Disorders and
Stroke, National Institutes of Health, Bethesda, MD.
Received December 27, 1991. Accepted for publication in final form October 26, 1993.
Address correspondence and reprint requests to Dr. Mark Hallett, Building 10, Room 5N226, NINDS, NIH, Bethesda, MD 20892.
884 NEUROLOGY 44 May 1994

Downloaded from www.neurology.org by NIZAR YAMANIE on July 20, 2008

Table. Clinical characteristics of the patients with Parkinsons disease

I
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
10

Age*

Sex

H & Yt

S & ES

Freezing

56
62
59
67
73
70
65
48
54
69

M
M

I1
11

F
M
F
F
M
M
F
M

111

80%
70%
80%
70%
60%
60%
70%
70%
70%
80%

0
1
0
1
3
2
2
1
3
2

I1
IV
111
111
111

I11
I11

URSPO
Tremor Rigidity
0
0
1
1
1

2
1

0
0
2

1
2
2
2
3
3
3
1
2
2

Bradykinesia#

Duration*

2
3
3
2
4
3
4
4
3
3

9
12
9
7
14
6
9
2
3
4

* Expressed in years.
t Hoehn and Yahr classification.
$ Schwab and England Activities of Daily Living Scale.
0 Unified Rating Scale for Parkinsonism.
Mean score for the following URSP categories: tremor, tremor a t rest, and action or postural tremor.
# Mean score for the following URSP categories: finger taps, hand movements, rapid alternating movements of hands, leg agility, and body
bradykinesia.

Experiment 1: reaction time. The experiment was performed in sets of 10 trials presented in random order. In
each set, five of the trials were control trials (go-signal
only), three were test trials (go-signal plus TMS), and
two were catch trials (TMS only) (figure 1A). In the test
trials, TMS was delivered before (negative) or after (positive) the go-signal; delay ranged from -50 msec to +60
msec and was randomly varied in the different trials. We
also studied the effects of varying intensities of TMS delivered concurrently with the go-signal (delay = 0). The
catch trials served t o ensure that the subjects were responding to the go-signal and not to the magnetic stimulus.l4JSTwenty sets of trials (200 trials) were completed
by each subject. If the subject responded to any of the
catch trials in a given set, the entire set of 10 trials was
discarded from further analysis. We found no difference
in the frequency of responses t o catch trials between PD
patients and controls. The main reason for omitting an
entire set of trials when the subject responded to a single
catch trial was t o allow comparison of the results with
those obtained in our previous studies on normal volunt e e r ~ . In
~ ~addition,
J~
errors in catch trials indicate a
lapse of attention, and we wanted to make sure that the
attention level and the performance of each subject
across sets of trials was as constant as possible.
Experiment 2: pre-movement excitability buildup. We
recorded 100 t o 200 trials per subject. In one-half of the
trials we used the visual go-signal (control trials). In the
other half, the go-signal was the same visual stimulus
coupled with a subthreshold transcranial magnetic stimulus delivered t o the ideal position for inducing motor
evoked potentials in the contralateral biceps (test trials)
(figure 1B). In both control and test trials, a subthreshold transcranial magnetic stimulus (probing stimulus)
was delivered at varying times after the go-signal to assess the probability of inducing motor evoked potentials
in the biceps as a function of the proximity of voluntary
EMG onset.14J9In test trials, the probing stimulus was
identical in intensity and localization to the magnetic
stimulus coupled with the visual stimulus as part of the
go-signal. The amplitude of the motor evoked potential
was expressed a s a percentage of the maximal M-response following peripheral electrical nerve stimulation.
Electromyographic recording. Pairs of surface elec-

trodes (DISA 13K60) were placed 4 cm apart on the skin


overlying the belly of the right biceps and triceps brachii,
and a Grass accelerometer was taped to the subjects
forearm. EMG and accelerometer signals were amplified
and filtered (100 to 2,000 Hz) by Grass amplifiers, digitized with a sampling rate of 5,000 Hz per channel, rectified, and collected using an AST personal computer. Alternatively, EMG activity was recorded by a Dantec
Counterpoint electromyograph with a bandpass of 30 Hz
to 2 kHz and sensitivity ranging from 50 to 1,000 pV per
division.
Transcranial magnetic stimulation. We used a Cadwell MES 10 magnetic stimulator (experiment 1) or a
Cadwell Rapid-Rate magnetic stimulator (experiment 2)
and a n 8-shaped coil in which each wing measured 4.5
cm in diameter. The Cadwell Rapid-Rate magnetic stimulator is capable of delivering twin pulses at intervals as
short as 30 msec without changes in the amplitude of the
p u l ~ e .The
~ ~coil
, ~ was
~ held flat on the scalp with the intersection of the two wings centered over the position at
which TMS induced motor evoked potentials of maximal
amplitude in the contralateral biceps. This position was
determined, with the patient a t rest, by stimulating several times at the stimulators maximal output intensity
(approximately 2.5 T) over different scalp positions.21
The handle of the coil was held parallel t o the sagittal
axis of the subjects head, pointing occipitally. This technique allows relatively focal stimulation, mostly limited
to cortical layers of the brain.22,23Stimulation intensity
was expressed in relationship to the stimulators lowest
output intensity capable of inducing five motor evoked
potentials in the contralateral biceps of at least 50 pV in
a series of 10 stimuli (threshold intensity).
Data analysis. RT was measured from the go-signal to
the onset of biceps EMG activity. Movement onset was
measured to the first deflection of the accelerometers
trace. In experiment 1,mean f SD RT was calculated for
all control trials and for each delay tested in the test trials. Results across subjects were compared with one-way
analysis of variance (ANOVA). Comparison of RTs in
control and test trials and between normal volunteers
and patients was performed using one-way ANOVAs, collapsing across subjects. Significance level, tested with
Scheffgs test, was set at p < 0.05. To analyze the probaMay 1094 NEUROLOGY 44 885

Downloaded from www.neurology.org by NIZAR YAMANIE on July 20, 2008

CONTROL TRIALS

CONTROL TRIALS

Warning
signal

Reaction
(Arm flexion)

Go-Signal

Warning
Signal

Go-Signal Probing
(Flash) Stimulus

React ion
(Arm flexion)

TEST TRIALS
Magnetic
Stimulus

TEST TRIALS
Go-Signal
(Flash t
Magnetic
Stimulus)

+lv

Reaction t h e

CATCH TRIALS
'

Reaction time

Figure 1. Experimental designs for experiments 1 (A)


and 2 (BI. See text for details.
bility of the probing stimulus inducing a motor evoked
potential in experiment 2, we aligned the trials a t EMG
onset (response) and expressed the probability as a function of the interval between probing stimulus and EMG
onset. We compared the probability curves in the control
trials with those in the test trials to assess the effect of
TMS in the go-signal (test trials) on the buildup of motor
cortex excitability during the RT. We compared the probability curves in normal volunteers and patients to assess the effect of PD on the results.

Results. E x p e r i m e n t 1: reaction t i m e . In control


trials, RT in the patients with PD was slower than
in the normal volunteers (173.6 f 22.8 msec versus
151.6 * 17.9 msec, p < 0.001). In test trials, RT was
shortened in patients and normal subjects. The
amount of RT shortening by TMS depended on
stimulation intensity and delay between go-signal
and TMS (figure 2). At delay of 0 msec and 90%
motor threshold intensity, the mean shortening of
RT by TMS was significantly larger in the patients
than in the normal volunteers (41.9 msec versus 28
msec,p < 0.001).
In normal volunteers, the shortening of RT by
subthreshold TMS was significant ( p < 0.01 to

40

-xi-40.30-20

-10 o 10 20

40

so 60

Delay (ms)

so

70

110

no

tso

TMS intensity
(% of motor threshold)

Figure 2. Difference in RT i n trials with and without


TMS according to delay and TMS intensity in normal
volunteers (open circles) and patients with PD (filled
circles). Negative RT difference implies shortening of RT
by TMS;positive values imply prolongation of RT b.y
TMS.Asterrsks mark significant differences between
normal Subjects arid patients (p < 0.0011.

886 NEUKOI,O(:Y 44 May 1994

Downloaded from www.neurology.org by NIZAR YAMANIE on July 20, 2008

BICEPS

WITHOUT
MAGNETIC
STIMULATION

I
I
TRICEPS ,
I

BICEPS

Figure 3. RT (mean S D ) i n the parkinsonian patients


i n trials without T M S (control, open circle) and i n trials
with T M S at various intensities. The gray band marks
the range of R T s i n trials without T M S i n the normal
volunteers.

0.005) at delays of -10 msec to +20 msec. At 2 +50


msec delay, RT w a s significantly prolonged
( p < 0.05) by subthreshold TMS in normal volunteers. In the patients, RT was significantly shortened ( p < 0.01 to 0.005)by subthreshold TMS at delays of -10 msec to +50 msec. Prolongation of RT
by subthreshold TMS in the patients was never observed a t the delays tested. Comparison of the effects of subthreshold TMS on RT according t o the
delay in normal volunteers and patients revealed
significant differences (figure 2).
The effects of TMS on RT according to TMS intensity were also significantly different between
normal volunteers and patients (figure 2). In the
normal subjects, TMS significantly shortened RT ( p
< 0.01 to 0.005) at 70% to 90% motor threshold intensity, whereas it significantly prolonged RT
( p c 0.01 to 0.005) at 120% to 150% motor threshold intensity. In the patients, TMS significantly
shortened RT ( p < 0.01 to 0.005) at 70% to 130%
motor threshold intensity. Prolongation of RT by
TMS in patients was seen only in occasional trials
at 150% motor threshold intensity.
In the patients, RT in trials with TMS at 70% to
130%)motor threshold intensity and 0 msec delay
became as short as RT in the normal volunteers in
trials without TMS (figure 3). This normalization
of RT by TMS was associated with a change in the
agonist-antagonist EMG pattern (figure 4). In trials without TMS, patients frequently displayed an
EMG pattern characterized by multiple agonist
and antagonist bursts. In trials with TMS, the first
agonist burst was frequently of larger amplitude,
and was part of a triphasic (agonist-antagonist-agonist) pattern characteristic of ballistic movements
in normal subject^.^^^'" The duration of the agonist
and antagonist bursts did not change in trials with
or without TMS.
The possibility that the effects of TMS on RT
were a function of RT itself was evaluated with a
correlation analysis between t h e RT a n d t h e

TRICEPS

1.

WITH
MAGNETIC
STIMULATION

I
I

Go-Signal

200 p v

100 rns

Figure 4. Representative examples of rectified EMG


traces i n a patient with PD i n trials without and with
T M S . I n the trial with T M S , the shortened RT is
associated with higher amplitude bursts and the absence
of both a second antagonist and a third agonist burst.

amount of RT shortening by TMS. We found no correlation between RT and TMS effects on RT among
the normal volunteers, among the PD patients, or
among all subjects studied. Therefore, the difference in RT reduction by TMS between PD patients
and normal volunteers has to be considered a function of PD itself.
In the three subjects studied off medications, the
RT in the control trials was significantly longer
t h a n on medications (191.4 f 27.4 msec versus
166.2 k 18.6 msec, p < 0.01). However, the effects of
TMS on RT were essentially unchanged, with significant shortening of RT by subthreshold TMS at
delays of -10 msec to +50 msec ( p < 0.01 to 0.005)
a n d a t TMS intensities of 70% t o 130% motor
threshold intensity ( p < 0.01).
Patients 5 to 10 occasionally reported that the
movement in response to trials with high TMS intensity was triggered entirely by TMS rather than
being voluntary. The analysis of the EMG pattern
in such trials revealed that the motor evoked potentials in agonist and antagonist were followed by
a n antagonist burst and then a n agonist burst,
with absence of an initial agonist burst (figure 5).
We do not feel that in such trials a first agonist
b u r s t could have been obscured by t h e motor
evoked potential since the configuration of the
motor evoked potential, its latency, and its duration always matched those of trials with a clearly
present first agonist burst. In addition, if the motor
evoked potential in such trials had coincided with a
first agonist burst, it should have been greatly facilitated by the ongoing contraction of the target
May 1994 NEUROLOGY 44 887

Downloaded from www.neurology.org by NIZAR YAMANIE on July 20, 2008

ME?
GO-SIGNAL+

- 1.0

/I

- 0,s
h

*
-=
s

.
I

-0.6

-0.4

TRICEPS

d2

- 0.2
-nn
-150-140-130-120-110-100 -90 -80 -70 -60 -50 -40 -30 -20

Time to EMG onset (ms)

Figure 6. Probahility of inducing a motor evoked


potential with the probing stimulus according to the
interval between probing stimulus and EMG onset i n
normal volunteers (circles) and parkinsonian patients
(squares). Open symhols indicate trials without T M S i n
the go-signal; filled symbols indicate trials with T M S .

Figure 5. Representative examples of trials with highintensity T M S i n a patient with PD. I n quotations are the
patients perception of the movement. I n the top example,
the motor evoked potentials (MEPs) i n agonist and
antagonist are followed after a pause by a n initial
agonist burst. I n this case the patient felt he had executed
the movement voluntarily. I n the bottom example, the
MEPs are followed by a burst i n the triceps (antagonist)
i n the absence of a n initial agonist burst. The patient felt
that the movement had been normalbut that T M S ,
rather than he, had generated it entirely.

muscle, and this was never the case. Analysis of


the movement pattern clinically and on forearm accelerometer recordings in such trials did not reveal
any substantial differences. Such trials were not
included in the analysis of RT described above.
Experiment 2: pre-movement excitability buildup.
The pre-movement motor cortex excitability increase in control trials was slower in patients than
in normal subjects (figure 6). In relationship to
EMG onset, it began approximately 40 msec earlier
in the patients. Considering a mean difference of
approximately 35 msec in RT in trials without
TMS, the pre-movement facilitation began a t approximately the same time in relation to the go-signal in patients and normal volunteers. Eventually,
20 msec before voluntary EMG onset, the probability of evoking a motor potential with the probing
stimulus was 1.0 in both patients and normal volunteers. Concurrently with the probability of inducing motor evoked potentials with the probing
stimulus, the amplitude of the evoked potentials
increased with shorter times to EMG onset and
reached approximately 40% of the maximal M-response in both patients and normal subjects.
Comparing control a n d t e s t trials, t h e time
course of pre-movement facilitation did not change
in the normal volunteers. However, in the patients,

the excitability increased faster and started closer


to EMG onset in t h e test trials, resembling the
course of the pre-movement facilitation in normal
subjects (figure 6).
The duration of the pre-movement excitability
buildup might have been related to the RT. However, we found no correlation between RT and duration of the excitability buildup in either normal
subjects or PD patients. In addition, comparison of
results of the two PD patients with the fastest RTs
in control trials (155.7 13.9 msec and 159.8 14.9
msec) with those of the two normal subjects with
the slowest RTs in control trials (155.1 17.2 msec
and 162.6 16.1 msec) still showed a mean difference in duration of the excitability buildup of 22
msec. Therefore, the differences in the duration of
the excitability buildup must be interpreted as a
function of PD.

Discussion. Theoretical model of response preparation and execution. We divide the processes required for response preparation and execution into
a stimulus evaluation system, a task-specific circuitry, a n d a response ~ h a n n e 1 . lThe
~ stimulus
evaluation system has to detect, process, and interpret the go-signal. The task-specific circuitry prepares the motor program for the required response.
The response channel includes all the necessary
structures to execute the response as rapidly as
possible. In a warned, simple RT paradigm, such as
the one used in this study, activation of the taskspecific circuitry may begin even before the warning signal, since the subject is given all the necessary information to plan the appropriate response
in advance. During the foreperiod, the task-specific
circuitry and the stimulus evaluation system are
active in parallel since the prepared motor program
has to be held in memory and the stimulus evaluation system is preparing to detect the go-signal (attentional aspects of set). Identification of a stimulus as the go-signal is completed in the time for

888 NEUROLOGY 44 May 1994

Downloaded from www.neurology.org by NIZAR YAMANIE on July 20, 2008

recognition. The transfer of the motor program


encing the time for initiation without affecting the
from the task-specific circuitry t o the response
time for recognition or the time of development.
channel occurs during the time for initiation. FiTMS could induce an earlier transfer of the motor
nally, the response channel needs a period of time
program from the task-specific circuitry to the re(time of development) to execute the response.
sponse channel or it could speed up the transfer
Therefore, during the RT we can identify a time or
process itself, thus shortening the time for initiarecognition, a time for initiation, and a time of detion.14We think of this transfer process as a switch
ve10pment.l~The identification of these three times
of activity from set-related neurons to movement
before response initiation does not imply that such
onset-related n e u r o n ~ . ~ ~
Subthreshold
-~O
TMS to the
times have to occur in a fixed sequence and cannot
motor cortex may activate corticocortical connecoccur partly in parallel. In fact, there is some evitions, enhancing the information transfer between
dence to suggest that parallel processing does occur
set-related neurons in the premotor and suppleduring response preparati~n.~~+~
mentary motor cortices and movement onset-reSimple RT and PD. In agreement with multiple
lated neurons (corticospinal neurons) in the priprevious studies,s,11,13,2e-31
we found RT in patients
mary motor cortex.
with PD t o be longer than in normal subjects. A
Contrary to the findings in normal subjects,
warning stimulus before the go-signal reduces RT
shortening of RT by TMS in patients with PD was
by the same amount in patients with PD and in
associated with a change in the pre-movement cornormal subjects, and the variation of RT according
tical excitability increase. We hypothesize that
t o the go-signal modality (time for recognition) is
TMS shortens RT in patients with PD by shortenthe same in patients as in normal s ~ b j e c t s .In
~ ~ , ~ing
~ both the time for initiation and the time of deaddition, parkinsonian patients are able to use advelopment. The additional effect on the time of devance information normally to plan a movevelopment may account for the fact t h a t TMS
ment3,jJ1and can hold a motor program in store
shortened RT more in patients than in normal subnormally during a delay RT task.33Therefore, the
jects. As mentioned above, akinesia may be due to
differences in simple RT between parkinsonian padecreased responsiveness of corticospinal neurons
tients and normal subjects cannot be due to differto the inputs involved in movement initiation due
ences in the stimulus evaluation system or in the
to the increased inhibition of thalamocortical neutask-specific circuitry.
r o n ~ In
. ~macaque
~
monkeys with MPTP-induced
Differences in the pre-movement facilitation in
parkinsonism, movement-related neurons in the
patients and normal subjects (experiment 2) have
primary motor cortex demonstrate abnormally low
been preliminarily reported.34 They suggest that
peak discharge frequencies and prolonged latencies
the main abnormality in the slow simple RT in PD
from onset of discharge to motion onset51(time of
development). Subthreshold TMS may raise the
lies in the time of development. We think of time of
development as a n energizing phenomenon
level of corticospinal neurons activation either dimarked by an increase in firing rate of movement
rectly or through activation of excitatory intracortionset-related neurons in the primary motor corcal i n t e r n e u r o n ~ , ~thus
~ - ~ temporarily
j
reversing the
t e ~by~which
~ -the~ motor
~ system excitability is inabnormality underlying akinesia.
creased and response execution occurs when a parNormalization of ballistic EMG pattern by TMS.
ticular threshold level is reached.41 Therefore, the
In normal individuals, ballistic limb movements
a r e characterized electromyographically by a
time of development will depend on the baseline
triphasic (agonist-antagonist-agonist)pattern in
level of cortical activation. In parkinsonism, priwhich the amplitude of the initial agonist burst
mate42and, more recently, human
have
correlates with the movement v e l o ~ i t y .Patients
~~,~~
shown that the motor cortex excitability is lower
with PD display an inability to generate an adea n d inversely correlated with t h e degree of
bradykinesia. In PD, the inhibitory input of the
quate initial agonist burstgJOso that the exerted
force is insufficient to reach the desired target.57
substantia nigra pars compacta on the putamen is
r e d ~ c e d .Therefore,
~ ~ - ~ ~ there is an increased inhibiThe movement is completed by a series of small
tion of the external segment of the globus pallidus
amplitude movements characterized by repeated
and subsequently a disinhibition of the subthalamagonist and antagonist burstsgJOor by a prolonged
ic nucleus. The internal segment of the globus palcontinuous discharge.57D e L ~ n g
has
~ ~suggested
that in PD, due to the high-level tonic discharge of
lidus and the substantia nigra pars reticulata exert
neurons in the internal segment of the globus palexcessive inhibition on thalamocortical neurons belidus, phasic modulation of their activity during
cause of the increased excitatory input from the
subthalamic nucleus. This results in decreased cormovement execution may not be faithfully transmitted to the cortex. Therefore, parkinsonian patical f a ~ i l i t a t i o n ,thereby
~ ~ - ~ ~ prolonging the time
tients are able to scale the first agonist burst ac(time of development) required to build up premovement cortical excitability past the threshold
cording to movement velocity but do so over a rerequired for movement execution.
stricted range.12 In trials with TMS, the first agonist burst was consistently of higher amplitude
Effects of TMS on simple RT, Our present re(figure 4).We suggest that subthreshold TMS actisults on normal volunteers confirm our previous
vates corticospinal neurons, thus transiently norstudies.14Js TMS shortens RT primarily by influMay 1994 NEUROLOGY 44 889

Downloaded from www.neurology.org by NIZAR YAMANIE on July 20, 2008

malizing the scaling of the amplitude of the initial


agonist burst. In any case, the speeding up of RT
and the normalization of the EMG pattern do not
have to be considered to originate from influences
of TMS on different structures. EMG patterns depend on response duration,58and PD patients who
move as rapidly as normal subjects tend to have
normal EMG p a t t e r n ~ . ~ J ~ J ~
Effect of TMS on the perception o f voluntary
movements. Occasionally, some of our patients perceived the response movements in trials with high
TMS intensity as triggered entirely by TMS rather
than being voluntary. In such instances, the motor
evoked potentials were followed by an antagonist
burst with absence of an initial agonist burst separable from the motor evoked potential (figure 5 ) .
An abnormal modulation of the motor cortical output by the basal ganglia46may lead to misinterpretation of the motor evoked potential as the initial
agonist burst, a phenomenon never encountered in
normal subjects. Such an abnormality may also explain why, in parkinsonian patients, slight increases of TMS intensity lead to abnormally large
increases in the amplitude of the induced motor
evoked potential^^^,^^ while facilitation of motor
evoked potentials by isometric contraction of the
target muscle is d e ~ r e a s e d . ~ ~

Acknowledgment
The authors thank Nguyet Dang for technical assistance during
the experiments.

References
1. Marsden CD. Slowness of movement in Parkinsons disease.
Mov Disord 1984;4(suppl l):S26-S37.
2. Hallett M. Clinical neurophysiology of akinesia. Rev Neurol
(Paris) 1990;146:585-590.
3. Day BL, Dick JPR, Marsden CD. Patients with Parkinsons
disease can employ a predictive motor strategy. J Neurol
Neurosurg Psychiatry 1984;47:1299-1306.
4. Dick JPR, Rothwell JC, Berardelli A, et al. Associated postural adjustments in Parkinsons disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1986;49:1378-1385.
5. Benecke R, Rothwell JC, Dick JPR, et al. Performance of simultaneous movements in patients with Parkinsons disease. Brain 1986;109:739-757.
6. Stelmach GE, Worringham CJ, S t r a n d EA. Movement
preparation in Parkinsons disease. The use of advanced information. Brain 1986;109:1179-1194.
7. Benecke R, Rothwell JC, Dick JPR, et al. Simple and complex movements off and on treatment i n patients with
Parkinsons disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1987;
50:296-303.
8. Rafal RD, Friedman JH, Lannon MC. Preparation of manual movements in hemiparkinsonism. J Neurol Neurosurg
Psychiatry 1989;52:1267-1273.
9. Hallett M, Shahani BT, Young RR. Analysis of stereotyped
voluntary movements at the elbow in patients with Parkinsons disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1977;40:11291135.
10. Hallett M, Khoshbin S. A physiological mechanism of
bradykinesia. Brain 1980;103:301-314.
11. Bloxham CA, Mindel TA, Firth CD. Initiation and execution
of predictable and unpredictable movements in Parkinsons
disease. Brain 1984;107:371-384.

12. Berardelli A, Dick JPR, Rothwell JC, et al. Scaling of the


size of the first agonist EMG burst during rapid wrist movements in patients with Parkinsons disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1986;49:1273-1279.
13. Sheridan MR, Flowers KA, Hurrell J . Programming and execution of movement i n Parkinsons disease. B r a i n
1987;110:1247-1271.
14. Pascual-Leone A, Valls-Sole J , Brasil-Net0 J , et al. Effect of
focal transcranial magnetic stimulation on simple reaction
time to visual, acoustic and somatosensory stimuli. Brain
1992;115:1045-1059.
15. Day BL, Rothwell JC, Thompson PD, et al. Delay in the execution of voluntary movement by electrical or magnetic
brain stimulation in intact man. Brain 1989;112:649-663.
16. Rothwell JC, Day BL, Thompson PD, Marsden CD. Interruption of motor programs by electrical or magnetic brain
stimulation in man. Prog Brain Res 1989;80465-472.
17. Brasil-Net0 J , Pascual-Leone A, Valls-Sole J , et al. Focal
transcranial magnetic stimulation and response bias in a
forced-choice t a s k . J Neurol Neurosurg P s y c h i a t r y
1992;55:964-966.
18. Pascual-Leone A, Brasil-Net0 J, Valls-Sole J , e t al. Simple
reaction time to focal transcranial magnetic stimulation:
comparison with reaction time to acoustic, visual, and somatosensory stimuli. Brain 1992;115:109-122.
19. Starr A, Caramia M, Zarola F, Rossini PM. Enhancement of
motor cortical excitability in humans by non-invasive electrical stimulation appears prior to voluntary movement.
Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 1988;70:26-32.
20. Pascual-Leone A, Gates JR, Dhuna A. Induction of speech
arrest and counting errors with rapid-rate transcranial magnetic stimulation. Neurology 1991;41:697-702.
21. Wassermann EM, McShane LM, Hallett M, Cohen LG. Noninvasive mapping of muscle representations in human motor
cortex. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 1992;85:1-8.
22. Cohen LG, Roth BJ, Nilsson J , et al. Effects of coil design on
delivery of focal magnetic stimulation. Technical considerations. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 1990;75:350357.
23. Roth BJ, Saypol JM, Hallett M, Cohen LG. A theoretical calculation of the electric field induced in the cortex during
magnetic stimulation. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol
1991;81:47-56.
24. Hallett M, Shahani BT, Young RR. EMG analysis of stereotyped voluntary movements i n man. J Neurol Neurosurg
Psychiatry 1975;38:1154-1162.
25. Hallett M, Marsden CD. Ballistic flexion movements of the
human thumb. J Physiol (Land) 1979;294:33-50.
26. Hening W, Favilla M, Ghez C. Trajectory control in targeted
force impulses. Exp Brain Res 1988;71:116-128.
27. Miller J. Discrete and continuous models of human information processing: theoretical distinctions and empirical results. Acta Psycho1 (Amst) 1988;67:191-257.
28. Heilman KM, Bowers D, Watson RT, Greer M. Reaction
times in Parkinsons disease. Arch Neurol 1976;333:139-140.
29. Evarts EV, Teravainen H, Calne DB. Reaction time in
Parkinsons disease. Brain 1981;104:167-186.
30. Rafal RD, Posner MI, Walker JA, Friedrich FJ. Cognition
and the basal ganglia. Brain 1984;107:1083-1094.
31. Pullman SL, Watts RL, Juncos JL, Chase TN, Sanes JN.
Dopaminergic effects on simple and choice reaction time performance i n Parkinsons disease. Neurology 1988;38:249254.
32. Bloxham CA, Dick DJ, Moore M. Reaction time and attention in Parkinsons disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry
1987;50:1178-1183.
33. Labutta RJ, Miles RB, Hallett M, Sanes JN. Reaction time
during delayed response tasks in Parkinsons disease [abstract]. Ann Neurol 1990;28:236.
34. Hallett M, Cohen LG, Bierner SM. Studies of sensory and
motor cortex physiology. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 1991;suppl43:76-85.
35. Evarts EV. Pyramidal tract activity associated with a conditioned h a n d movement in t h e monkey. J Neurophysiol
1966;29:1011-1027.
36. Luschei ES, Johnson RN, Glickstein M. Response in neurons

890 NEUROLOGY 44 May 1994

Downloaded from www.neurology.org by NIZAR YAMANIE on July 20, 2008

in the motor cortex during performance of simple repetitive


arm movement. Nature 1968;217:190-191.
37. Fetz EE, Baker MA. Operantly conditioned patterns of precentral unit activity and correlated responses in adjacent cells
and contralateral muscles. J Neurophysiol 1973;36:179-204.
38. Evarts EV, Tanji J. Reflex and intended responses in motor
cortex pyramidal tract neurons of the monkey. J Neurophysiol 1976;39:1069-1080.
39. Fetz EE, Cheney PD. Postspike facilitation of forelimb muscle activity and correlated responses in adjacent cells and
contralateral muscles. J Neurophysiol 1980;44:751-771.
40. Requin J . Looking forward to moving soon: ante factum selective processes in motor control. In: Posner MI, Marin
OSM, eds. Attention and performance, vol X. Hillsdale, N J
Lawrence Erlbaum, 1985:147-167.
41. Gratton G, Coles MGH, Sirevaag E J , e t al. Pre-and poststimulus activation of response channels: a psychophysiological analysis. J Exp Psycho1 [Hum Percepl 1988;14:331-343.
42. Evarts EV, Fromm C, Kroller J , Jennings VA. Motor cortex
control of finely graded forces. J Neurophysiol 1983;49:11991215.
43. Davey N J , Dick JPR, Ellaway PH, Maskill DW. Raised
motor cortical threshold associated with bradykinesia as revealed by transcranial magnetic stimulation in normal man
a n d Parkinsons disease [ a b s t r a c t ] . J Physiol (Lond)
1991;438:35P.
44. Albin RL, Young AB, Penney JB. The functional anatomy of
basal ganglia disorders. Trends Neurosci 1989;12:366-375.
45. Alexander GE, Crutcher MD. Functional architecture of
basal ganglia circuits: neural substrates of parallel processing. Trends Neurosci 1990;13:266-271.
46. DeLong MR. Primate models of movement disorders of basal
ganglia origin. Trends Neurosci 1990;13:281-285.
47. Tanji J , Kurata K. Comparison of movement-related activity
in two cortical motor areas of primates. J Neurophysiol
1982;48:633-653.
48. Wise SP, Weinrich M, Mauritz K-H. Motor aspects of cue-related neuronal activity in premotor cortex of the rhesus
monkey. Brain Res 1983;260:301-305.

49. Evarts EV, Shinoda Y, Wise SP. Neurophysiological approaches to higher brain functions. New York Wiley, 1984.
50. Tanji J , Kurata K. Changing concepts of motor areas of the
cerebral cortex. Brain Dev 1989;11:374-377.
51. Mandir AS, Watts RL, Buchholz SR, Montgomery EBJ.
Changes in primary motor cortex neuronal activity associated with increased reaction time and movement time in
MPTP parkinsonism [abstract]. Mov Disord 1990;5(suppl
1):77.
52. Day BL, Thompson PD, Dick JP, et al. Different sites of action of electrical and magnetic stimulation of the human
brain. Neurosci Lett 1987;75:101-106.
53. Hess CW, Mills KR, Murray NMF. Responses in small hand
muscles from magnetic stimulation of the human brain. J
Physiol (Lond) 1987;388:397-419.
54. Amassian VE, Maccabee PJ, Cracco RQ, Cracco JB. Basic
mechanisms of magnetic coil excitation of the nervous system in humans and monkeys: application in focal stimulation of different cortical areas in humans. In: Chokroverty S,
ed. Magnetic stimulation i n clinical neurophysiology.
Boston: Butterworth, 1989:73-112.
55. Amassian VE, Quirk GJ, Stewart M. A comparison of corticospinal activation by magnetic coil and electrical stimulation of monkey motor cortex. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 1990;77:390-401.
56. Berardelli A, Inghilleri M, Cruccu G, Manfredi M. Descending volley after electrical and magnetic transcranial stimulation in man. Neurosci Lett 1990;112:54-58.
57. Wierzbicka MM, Wiegner AW, Logigian EL, Young RR. Abnormal most-rapid isometric contractions in patients with
Parkinsons disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry
1991;54:210-216.
58. Ghez C, Gordon J. Trajectory control in targeted force imp u l s e s . I. Role of opposing muscles. Exp B r a i n Res
1987;67:225-240.
59. Valls-Sole J , Pascual-Leone A, Brasil-Net0 JP, Cammarota
A, McShane L, Hallett M. Abnormal facilitation of the response to transcranial magnetic stimulation in patients with
Parkinsons disease. Neurology 1994;44:735-741.

May 1994 NEUROLOGY 44 891

Downloaded from www.neurology.org by NIZAR YAMANIE on July 20, 2008

Akinesia in Parkinson's disease. I. Shortening of simple reaction time with


focal, single-pulse transcranial magnetic stimulation
A. Pascual-Leone, J. Valls-Sol, J. P. Brasil-Neto, L. G. Cohen and M. Hallett
Neurology 1994;44;884
This information is current as of July 20, 2008
Updated Information
& Services

including high-resolution figures, can be found at:


http://www.neurology.org

Permissions & Licensing

Information about reproducing this article in parts (figures,


tables) or in its entirety can be found online at:
http://www.neurology.org/misc/Permissions.shtml

Reprints

Information about ordering reprints can be found online:


http://www.neurology.org/misc/reprints.shtml

Downloaded from www.neurology.org by NIZAR YAMANIE on July 20, 2008

Você também pode gostar