Você está na página 1de 2

THE DANA REVIEW

http://www.g2conline.info/852
Author: Thomas S. May

Title: Terms of Empathy

What enables us to feel empathyto experience or share another persons pain, fear, joy, or any other emotion?
Empathy, research indicates, is made possible by a special group of nerve cells called mirror neurons, at various
locations inside the brain. These special cells enable us to mirror emotions.
However, the activity of these neurons can be modified by various factors, including the relationship of the people
involved. A new study suggests that, at least in men, whether we empathize with another persons pain depends
on how that person had behaved in the past, and, perhaps more important, whether we like or dislike them.
Mirror, Mirror in the Brain
Mirror neurons were first discovered in the early 1990s by Italian scientists who, while looking at the activity of
individual nerve cells inside the brains of macaque monkeys, noticed that neurons in the same area of the brain
were activated whether the animals were performing a partic-ular movement (reaching for a peanut, for instance)
or simply observing another monkeyor a researcher perform the same action. It appeared as though the cells
in the observers brain mirrored the activity in the performers brain.
A similar phenomenon takes place when we watch someone experience an emotion and feel the same emotion in
response, says Marco Iacoboni, a neuroscientist at the University of California, Los Angeles. The same neural
systems get activated in a part of the cortex called the insula, which is part of the mirror neuron system, and in the
emotional brain areas associated with the observed emotion.
However, the amount of activation is slightly smaller for the mirrored experience than when the same emotion is
experienced directly, Iacoboni adds.
A recent study by Iacoboni and colleagues highlights the impor-tance of mirror neurons and their role in the
development of autism spectrum disorder (ASD). ASD is a pervasive developmental disorder characterized by
impaired social interactions.
Iacobonis team used functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) to investigate neural activi-ty of 10 highfunctioning children with ASD and 10 normally developing children as they observed and imitated facial emotional
expressions. Although both groups performed the tasks equally well, children with autism showed reduced mirror
neuron activity, particularly in the area of the inferior frontal gyrus. Moreover, the degree of reduction in mirror
neu-ron activity in the children with autism correlated with the severity of their symptoms.
Iacoboni says, these results indicate that a healthy mirror neuron system is crucial for normal social development.
If you have broken mirrors, or deficits in mirror neurons, you likely end up having social problems, as patients
with autism do, he says.
There are many kinds of mirror neurons, Iacoboni says. They include neurons for hand actions such as grasping,
holding, and tearing, and mouth actions including biting, drinking, and a wide variety of facial gestures. We also
think there are super mirror neurons, although we do not yet have direct evidence for their existence, he says.
These super mirror neurons could control and inhibit the activity of lower-level mirror neurons, Iacoboni suggests.
He also hypothesizes that by modulating, or modifying, the activity of other neurons, super mirror neurons could
enable one to experience either pain or joy, depending on the circumstances, when watching another person
suffer.
From Pain to Joy
In a study published in the Jan. 26, 2006, issue of Nature, Tania Singer, a research fellow at the Wellcome
Department of Imaging Neuroscience at University College London, and colleagues found what could be,
according to an accompanying editorial, the first neuroscientific evidence for the existence of schadenfreude, the
joy derived from anothers troubles.
The DANA Foundation. All rights reserved.

THE DANA REVIEW


http://www.g2conline.info/852
Author: Thomas S. May

Title: Terms of Empathy

In order to investigate the conditions under which individuals empathize with another persons pain, the
researchers first had their subjects take part in a game in which the other participants were associates, or confederates, of the experimenters. The confederates played either fairly (displaying trust and cooperation) or
unfairly (exhibiting a lack of trust and cooperation), in an attempt to make the subjects like or dislike them.
The research subjects always moved first and could show they either trusted the other players, by sending them
money, or mistrusted them, by withholding the money. The confederates were always second movers and could
choose between a fair and an unfair response: returning high or low amounts of money.
We used this game because we know that it is a very good way to induce strong emotions, Singer says. If you
engage in cooperation and trust and the other person doesnt trust you, you really start disliking this person. But if
people respond with cooperation, you begin to like them.
An analysis of the results showed that subjects did indeed perceive the confederates as being fair or unfair
according to their game-playing strategy. Furthermore, behavioral ratings confirmed that both male and female
subjects rated the fair players as being significantly more agreeable, more likeable, and more attractive than the
unfair players.
In the second part of the experiment, the investigators used functional magnetic resonance imaging to look at
brain activity patterns while the subjects observed the confederates, who they thought were being given painful
electric shocks.
As expected, subjects showed strong activation of pain-related brain regions when witnessing fair players
receiving electric shocks. However, these empathy-related responses were significantly reduced in males when
observing an unfair person being shocked. Moreover, this effect was accompanied by increased activation in
reward-related areas, which correlated with an expressed desire for revenge, the investigators reported.
Singer says the fact that some people appear to be pleased when others who had been unfair are being punished
can be explained in terms of evolutionary psychology. It is important for people to cooperate with one another, if
society is to remain stable, she says.
Therefore, it might be good if people punish those who are unfair and do not cooperate, so that overall, society
gets more and more cooperators.
The scientists are not sure why men appeared to be less empathetic and more revengeful than women toward
unfair players. Singer speculates that this finding is attributable to the study having involved the application of
physical pain (electric shocks applied to the back of the hand).
If we had a psychological revenge, such as ruining the reputation of the other player, perhaps women would
behave the same way, she says.

The DANA Foundation. All rights reserved.

Você também pode gostar