Você está na página 1de 6

COBEHA 203 XX

Available online at www.sciencedirect.com

ScienceDirect
12

Brain oscillations in perception, timing and action

3
Q1

4
5
6
7
8
9
10
11
12
13
14
15
16
17
18
19
20
21
22
23
24
25
26
27
28
29

Daya S Gupta1 and Lihan Chen2,3


Catching a thrown ball requires a tight coupling between
perception and motor control. In this review, we examine
multidimensional information processing across various
perceptual and motor tasks. We summarize how perception,
timing and action can be understood in terms of the coupling of
gamma band oscillations, which represent the local activities of
brain circuits, to a specific phase of long-range low-frequency
oscillations. We propose a temporal window of integration that
emerges from cross-frequency coupling that serves to produce
optimized action.
Addresses
1
Department of Biology, Camden County College, Blackwood, NJ
08012-0200, USA
2
Department of Psychology and Beijing Key Laboratory of Behavior and
Mental Health, Peking University, Beijing 100871, China
3
Key Laboratory of Machine Perception (Ministry of Education), Peking
University, Beijing 100871, China
Corresponding authors: Gupta, Daya S (dayagup@gmail.com) and
Chen, Lihan (CLH@pku.edu.cn)

Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences 2016, 8:xxyy


This review comes from a themed issue on Timing behavior
Edited by Richard B Ivry and Warren H Meck

30
31
32

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cobeha.2016.02.021
2352-1546/# 2016 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

33
35
34
36
37
38
39
40
41
42
43
44
45
46
47
48
49
50
51
52
53

Introduction
To hit a pitched baseball requires that the batter generates
accurate estimates of the position, speed, and timing of the
moving ball and use this information to produce a coordinated movement. Humans are adept in compensating for
their own sensorimotor uncertainty in the execution of
skilled actions [1,2,3]; however, the neural mechanisms
underlying the temporal coordination between perception
and action have not been fully established.
Different lines of evidence have facilitated our understanding of the computations and physiological underpinnings that are involved in the coupling of perception
and action. For example, a recent computational modeling study revealed that the internal representations of
observers spatial visuomotor errors (in visuomotor decision tasks) are best described by a mixture of distributions
that differ in location and scale [2]. This indicates that
the internal probability density functions of their own
www.sciencedirect.com

visuomotor error distributions are discrete [2]. These


results suggest that discrete neurophysiological processes
may be responsible for various sensorimotor tasks. In this
review, we propose that the processing of multidimensional information by neuronal populations occurs in
separate epochs and that these epochs represent different
facets of the interaction between perception and action
(Figure 1).

Multidimensional processing of sensorimotor


information
Over the last decade, there has been considerable interest
in the roles of cross-frequency phase-amplitude coupling
in various cognitive functions [4]. Cross-frequency
phase-amplitude coupling is a phenomenon in which
the phase of a lower frequency brain oscillation modulates
the amplitudes, i.e., the differences in the maxima and
minima of oscillation waves, of a higher frequency oscillation to create packets of those higher frequency waves
[4]. Coupling of the phases of low-frequency theta (4
8 Hz) and alpha (812 Hz) oscillations to the amplitudes
of high-frequency gamma (30150 Hz) oscillations
[5,6,7,8,9,10,11,12,13,14,15] has been observed in various sensory and motor tasks. In one study, visual processing is found to reflect local activity, whereas working
memory retention and mental imagery, which likely involve multiple, distributed cortical areas, are related to
theta (48 Hz) and alpha (812 Hz) frequency oscillations
[11]. Furthermore, gamma band neuronal oscillations that
relate to the spiking activities of neurons [9,15,16] in
response to stimulus characteristics, including intensity
and duration, have been observed in many cortical and
subcortical structures [17,18,19,20,21]. Additionally, a recent study demonstrated zero and non-zero lags between
the phases of gamma oscillations in spatially separate sites
in the primary visual cortex [22]. These non-zero lags are
modulated by the stimulus, suggesting that gamma oscillations can also dynamically influence the exchange of
information among local populations of neurons [22].
In contrast to gamma oscillations, low-frequency oscillations generally synchronize larger areas of the cortex and
play a role in long-range interactions [4,15]. Thus, as a
result of phase-amplitude coupling, gamma-band oscillations reflect local activities, whereas low-frequency oscillations help to engage their long-range interactions
[4,15]. In the framework of multidimensional processing, the amplitudes of high-frequency oscillations that
synchronize the processing of information in multiple
local circuits are modulated by the phases of synchronous
long-range low-frequency brain oscillations. This phaseamplitude coupling causes information to be coherently
Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences 2016, 8:xx

Please cite this article in press as: Gupta DS, Chen L: Brain oscillations in perception, timing and action, Curr Opin Behav Sci (2016), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cobeha.2016.02.021

54
55
56
57
58
59
60
61
62
63
64
65
66
67
68
69
70
71
72
73
74
75
76
77
78
79
80
81
82
83
84
85
86
87
88
89
90
91
92
93
94
95
96
97
98
99
100
101
102
103
104

COBEHA 203 XX

2 Timing behavior

Figure 1

Local circuits
(Fast Oscillations)
Buzsaki G, Watson BO: Dialogues Clin Neurosci (2012)

Working Memory
Multimodal
Motor

Sensory

Pre
tal

fron

CORTEX

(a)

Multidimensional Processing of Information


(Low-frequency Oscillations)
Local spiking activity
is coupled to a-phase
of the fast oscillation
ISI = 30 ms
Belitski A, et al: J Neurosci (2008) 28(22):5696-5709

t1

t2

t3

Fast Oscillations

TWI

Local Circuits

Less Information

~50200 ms

ISI = 60 ms

t1

t2

t3

Low-frequency Oscillations
(~520 Hz) Multidimensional

More Information

Processing
(c)

(b)

Shannon Information
Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences

Schematic depiction of the multidimensional processing of information in the brain.


(a) Activities in local circuits are modulated by the phase of long-range low-frequency oscillations (indicated by curved lines spanning a large area
of the cortex). Sensory, motor, multimodal and association areas in the prefrontal cortex (oval boundaries) are shown as examples of local circuits.
(b) Depicts cross-frequency phase-amplitude coupling. Note that multiunit spike bursts (shown inside a circle) have been observed in experimental
studies to exhibit a dependency on the phase of fast (gamma band high-frequency) oscillations. t1, t2 and t3 are time points, present at intervals
that equal the periodicity of low-frequency oscillation, when fast oscillations synchronize activities (spike bursts) of local circuits. Multidimensional
information processing occurs as a result of the modulation of fast oscillations (synchronizing local circuits) by long-range low-frequency
oscillations. The phase of low-frequency oscillation when spike bursts in local circuits are synchronized by fast oscillations will provide the
temporal window of integration (TWI) of stimuli. Spike bursts contain information (see panel c) that is processed in the multidimensional domain
during a sensorimotor task.
(c) Interstimulus intervals (ISIs) between two frames of a Ternus display, which is a special case of apparent motion [52], are processed differently
depending on the length of the ISI to produce different content levels of Shannon information. Notice that there is less uncertainty regarding the
presence of spikes in time-bins of the same length that contain shorter ISIs, i.e., the information content is relatively reduced. The shorter ISIs,
with less information content, are associated with the percept of element motion, which contrasts with the group motion observed with the longer
ISIs.

Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences 2016, 8:xx

www.sciencedirect.com

Please cite this article in press as: Gupta DS, Chen L: Brain oscillations in perception, timing and action, Curr Opin Behav Sci (2016), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cobeha.2016.02.021

COBEHA 203 XX

Oscillatory structures in the brain Gupta and Chen 3

105
106
107
108
109
110
111
112
113
114
115
116
117
118
119
120
121
122
123
124
125
126
127
128
129
130
131
132
133
134
135
136
137
138
139
140
141
142
143
144
145
146
147
148
149
150
151
152
153
154
155
156
157
158
159
160
161

processed in distributed circuits, supporting the tight


temporal coupling between various aspects of a sensorimotor task. The degree of this coupling depends on the
demands of the task. For example, catching a fastermoving object requires a tighter coupling between various sensorimotor functions that occurs over shorter subsecond durations.
The information resulting from the activities of local
circuits can be described by Shannon entropy, i.e., the
amount of uncertainty about whether a neuron that represents an information source, such as the primary visual
cortex, will or will not fire within a particular temporal
window [23,24]. Information or entropy in the proposed
multidimensional domain is a consequence of the stochastic nature of the neuronal responses to a stimulus
[25,26,27]. Other sources of information for multidimensional processing may include higher functional areas, such
as the somatosensory areas, multimodal areas, motor areas
and the anterior association area in the prefrontal cortex.
Note that the information resulting from the coherent
neuronal activities that are responsible for sensorimotor
tasks can be quantified by different analyses, including
wavelet information analysis and the study of spike structures in terms of complexity and randomness [26,27,28].
In one study in which primates were presented with naturalistic scenes, a positive correlation was observed between
high-gamma local field potential (LFP) (60100 Hz) and
spikes, along with a strong positive correlation within highgamma LFPs in the primary visual cortex [16]. These
findings suggest that high-gamma LFPs and spikes are
generated within the same network [16]. Another study
revealed significant phase correlations in approximately
60% of the multiunit activities of neurons and LFP recordings from visual cortices during a specific visual stimulation
in addition to phase correlations between oscillatory events
at different frequencies [15]. As such, the information in
spike structure [27] during multidimensional processing is
indicative of sensorimotor responses.
It was observed that stimulus-driven excitability variations
in local cortical ensembles are related to the phases of brain
oscillations within the setting of a hierarchical cross-frequency coupling amplitude of a higher frequency
oscillation is coupled with the phase of a lower frequency
oscillation [29,30]. Accordingly, we propose that synchronized long-range low-frequency oscillations provide a temporal window of integration (TWI) in the framework
multidimensional processing. The TWI may manifest as
a peak or trough of a low-frequency brain oscillation
because coupling has been observed between the high
gamma power and the peak phases of alpha (812 Hz)
oscillations in a working memory task [31] and the troughs
of theta (48 Hz) oscillations in motor, sensory and cognitive tasks [5]. Moreover, the TWI is likely to be related to
the periodicity of the low-frequency oscillations. Typical
www.sciencedirect.com

integrations of auditory and visual events occur within a


time window of 100 ms, for example, those observed
during the sound-induced double-flash illusion [32]. The
sound-induced double-flash illusion may be explained by
the coupling of the activities of the auditory and visual
circuits to the same phase of a slow wave oscillation in the
framework of multidimensional processing. An important
supporting piece of evidence comes from spatial ventriloquism in which a visual object can attract the perceived
location of a spatially discordant but temporally synchronous sound [33,34]. This attraction can be understood as a
phase-amplitude coupling of the circuits processing the
sound to the synchronized low-frequency oscillations that
are devoted to processing different features of the visual
object.
Although spatial ventriloquism is an example of the
integration of spatially separate events, phase-amplitude
coupling is observed in the integration of information
associated with inputs arising from a single source. For
example, during simple visual tasks, alpha/high gamma
coupling preferentially increases in the visual cortical
regions [8]. A recent study provides the causal evidence
that the temporal window of integration associated with
sound-induced double-flash illusion is constrained by the
frequency of occipital oscillations in the alpha band [34].
Faster alpha frequencies also predict vision with a finer
temporal resolution, which is consistent with the hypothesis that faster alpha oscillations provide more cycles for
the separate rather than combined integration, which
results in better resolution [35].
In another study of the integration of two stimuli (i.e., audio
and tactile), the alpha power recorded over the superior and
inferior parietal lobules was relatively increased in a pretactile stimulus time-window of approximately 330 ms
prior to the subsequent perception of an integrated rather
than segregated stimulus [36]. Due to the lack of a poststimulus effect in this study, the integrated/segregated
perception was likely an effect of a network comprising
multisensory integration areas with unisensory areas rather than a specific multisensory area [36]. This interpretation is consistent with the multidimensional processing of
auditory and tactile stimuli within a network.
The increase in alpha power during the integration of two
stimuli can be understood in terms of multidimensional
processing. For multisensory integration to occur, the alpha
oscillations modulating both unisensory circuits, i.e., tactile
and auditory, must synchronize, producing an increase in
alpha power. Therefore, one may argue that the processing
of multiple sensory stimuli (as depicted in Figure 1) within
successive but discrete TWIs results in the integration of
sensory information from multiple sources in a dynamic
network of circuits over a time period. This integration
forms the basis of successful sensorimotor interactions with
the external physical environment.
Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences 2016, 8:xx

Please cite this article in press as: Gupta DS, Chen L: Brain oscillations in perception, timing and action, Curr Opin Behav Sci (2016), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cobeha.2016.02.021

162
163
164
165
166
167
168
169
170
171
172
173
174
175
176
177
178
179
180
181
182
183
184
185
186
187
188
189
190
191
192
193
194
195
196
197
198
199
200
201
202
203
204
205
206
207
208
209
210
211
212
213
214
215
216
217
218

COBEHA 203 XX

4 Timing behavior

219
220
221
222
223
224
225
226
227
228
229
230
231
232
233
234
235
236
237
238
239
240
241
242
243
244
245
246
247
248
249
250
251
252
253
254
255
256
257
258
259
260
261
262
263
264
265
266
267
268
269
270
271
272
273
274

Multidimensional processing in apparent


visual motion
In this section, we argue that pre-stimulus alpha power
and spontaneous brain oscillations influence apparent
motion perception [37]. Apparent motion is a visual
illusion in which motion is perceived when two spatially
distinct static objects alternate in sequence. Apparent
motion illusions are robust at presentation frequencies
of approximately 3 Hz. Apparent motion has been
deemed to be dependent on the ability to use predictive
feedback signals in the processing of fragmented sensory information [38,39]. The perception of good apparent
motion follows an optimal range of presentation frequencies. Lower alpha power predicts percepts between apparent motion and flickering at high presentation
frequencies. In a study, higher alpha power predicts
apparent motion percepts of visual objects with low
presentation frequencies [37]. Specifically, apparent motion perception depends on both local neural synchronization (i.e., the power within the frontal and occipital
regions of interests) and long-distance neural synchronization (i.e., frontal-occipital connectivity) in the pre-stimulus alpha oscillations [37].

Multidimensional processing in timing and


action
Phase-amplitude coupling provides a plausible mechanism
for the calibration of modular clocks. This calibration
involves endogenous oscillators within various networks,
modules that calibrate the oscillators, and downstream
circuits that process task-specific time intervals. These
components are connected via flexible connections as
proposed by Gupta (2014) [40]. The modular clock mechanism is calibrated by circuits, such as those in the cerebellum and posterior parietal cortex, which are important
for feedback control during various sensorimotor tasks
[40]. During complex sensorimotor tasks, such as catching
a flying ball or lifting a cup, irregular spike bursts are
produced during feedback processes and reflect unequal
temporal epochs that separate the activities of individual
muscles during these movements. Furthermore, the irregular changes in the neuronal activities during a sensorimotor task mirror the changes in the physical time-related
parameters, such as speed and duration. The input from
feedback circuits to endogenous oscillatory mechanisms
aid in the transfer of the physical time information. Note
that phase-amplitude coupling preserves the temporal
relationship between the parallel inputs of irregular activities that arrive at the oscillator/local clock mechanism from
a calibration module. Additionally, motor and sensory taskmodulated changes in the activities of neurons are observed in parts of the brain, such as the inferior temporal
lobe and prefrontal cortex, which are not directly responsible for the feedback control of external sensorimotor tasks
[41,42]. These changes may influence the effects of feedback processes and could also be responsible for the
calibration of endogenous oscillators.
Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences 2016, 8:xx

Several recent studies have also suggested the role of beta


band oscillations in the representation of various time
durations in the brain [43,44,45]. Interestingly, in a
recent study by Bartolo and Merchant (2015), putaminal
LFPs were recorded in monkeys that were performing a
synchronization-continuation task. The LFPs exhibited
an initial burst of beta band oscillation that was followed
by another increase during the continuation phase of the
task [45]. This latter increase depends on internally
generated cues [45]. The dependence on internally
generated cues suggests the involvement of other calibration modules, such as those arising from the basal
ganglia circuit as previously proposed by Gupta (2014)
[40], which may be responsible for the observed increase
in the beta power via an increase in the number of longrange beta-oscillation interactions during the continuation phase of the task.
Consistent with the role of low-frequency oscillations in
the modulation of the amplitudes of high-frequency
oscillations by long-range oscillations, previous studies
utilizing motor tasks have demonstrated phase-amplitude
coupling between movement-selective high gamma and
alpha oscillations in humans [14] and between gamma
and theta oscillations in the motor cortical areas of rats
during distinct movement states [12]. Another study
suggests a saccade-related phase-amplitude coupling between theta and low gamma activities [13]. Thus, the
amplitudes of gamma oscillations in motor areas, which
represent motor information including the time of the
onset, performance and execution [46,47], nested within
the low-frequency oscillations are likely to play an important role in the processing of information for the
execution of sensorimotor tasks.
A recent study simultaneously recorded neuronal activities
in multiple cortical regions in monkeys that were trained to
report the color or motion of the stimuli. This study
revealed complex dynamics of information flow [48]. When
information reaches one part of the brain from another, the
phase of the synchronized low-frequency oscillations connecting the two regions is advanced [49]. This delay in
phase affects the processing of the arriving information by
altering the available size of the TWI. The effect of longrange oscillations on information flow has been indicated
by an analysis of intracranial EEG data that revealed that
power changes in different frequency bands significantly
contribute to the late components of the event-related
potentials [50]. The micro-stimulation of V1 (i.e., the
primary visual area) generates gamma oscillations in the
feedforward direction, whereas such stimulation of V4 (i.e.,
a downstream visual area) generates alpha oscillations in
the feedback direction [49]. These findings suggest that
during the multidimensional processing of visual information, the coupling of the amplitude of gamma oscillations
(produced by feedforward activity) to the phase of lowfrequency oscillations (produced by feedback activity)
www.sciencedirect.com

Please cite this article in press as: Gupta DS, Chen L: Brain oscillations in perception, timing and action, Curr Opin Behav Sci (2016), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cobeha.2016.02.021

275
276
277
278
279
280
281
282
283
284
285
286
287
288
289
290
291
292
293
294
295
296
297
298
299
300
301
302
303
304
305
306
307
308
309
310
311
312
313
314
315
316
317
318
319
320
321
322
323
324
325
326
327
328
329
330
331
332

COBEHA 203 XX

Oscillatory structures in the brain Gupta and Chen 5

333
334
335
336
337
338
339
340
341
342
343
344
345
346
347
348
349
350
351
352
353
354
355
356
357
358
359

could also modulate the flow of information between lower


and higher visual areas. Thus, phase-amplitude coupling
could form one of the important bases of sensorimotor
choices in flexible visuomotor tasks; such choices are
believed to result from the integration of opposite flows
of sensory and task information [48].

Summary
VanRullen and Koch (2003) proposed that cross-frequency interactions between gamma and alpha oscillations
constrain perception [51]. We extend this proposal to
include motor functions to understand the role of the
multidimensional processing of information during sensorimotor tasks. The multidimensional processing provides an important basis for understanding how different
circuits of the brain can be temporally coupled during
various sensorimotor tasks.

Conflict of interest statement


Nothing declared.

Acknowledgements
Q2 This study was supported by grants from the Natural Science Foundation of

China (31200760), the National High Technology Research and


Development Program of China (863 Program, 2012AA011602) and the
Fund for Fostering Talents in Basic Science (J1103602) to LC. DSG would
like to acknowledge his gratitude to the students, faculty and staff of
Camden County College, New Jersey, USA and the ResearchGateTM
community.

References
360
361
362
363
364
365
366
367
368
369
370
371
372
373
374
375
376
377
379
378
380
381
382
383
384
385
386
387
388
389
390
391
392

1.

Trommershauser J, Maloney LT, Landy MS: Decision making,


movement planning and statistical decision theory. Trends
Cogn Sci 2008, 12:291-297.

2.


Zhang H, Daw ND, Maloney LT: Human representation of visuomotor uncertainty as mixtures of orthogonal basis
distributions. Nat Neurosci 2015, 18:1152-1158.
This study demonstrated that the brain maintains an internal representation of visuomotor error that is best described as a mixture of distributions. The authors used a forced-choice method to reveal the subjects
internal representations of their spatial visuo-motor errors in a speeded
reaching movement.

3.

Faisal AA, Wolpert DM: Near optimal combination of sensory


and motor uncertainty in time during a naturalistic perceptionaction task. J Neurophysiol 2009, 101:1901-1912.

4.


Buzsaki G, Watson BO: Brain rhythms and neural syntax:


Implications for efficient coding of cognitive content and
neuropsychiatric disease. Dialogues Clin Neurosci 2012, 14:345367.
This review discusses various brain oscillations and their respective roles.
It also features a useful glossary of terms related to neural oscillations.

5.

6.

Canolty RT, Edwards E, Dalal SS, Soltani M, Nagarajan SS,


Kirsch HE, Berger MS, Barbaro NM, Knight RT: High gamma
power is phase-locked to theta oscillations in human
neocortex. Science 2006, 313:1626-1628.
Osipova D, Hermes D, Jensen O: Gamma power is phase-locked
to posterior alpha activity. PLoS One 2008, 3:e3990.

7.


Voytek B, Canolty RT, Shestyuk A, Crone NE, Parvizi J, Knight RT:


Shifts in gamma phase-amplitude coupling frequency from
theta to alpha over posterior cortex during visual tasks. Front
Hum Neurosci 2010, 4:191.
This study demonstrated that high gamma amplitude is coupled to
theta and alpha troughs and demonstrated that during visual tasks,

www.sciencedirect.com

alpha/high gamma coupling preferentially increases in the visual cortical regions.


8.

Demiralp T, Bayraktaroglu Z, Lenz D, Junge S, Busch NA,


Maess B, Ergen M, Herrmann CS: Gamma amplitudes are
coupled to theta phase in human eeg during visual perception.
Int J Psychophysiol 2007, 64:24-30.

9.

Burns SP, Xing D, Shapley RM: Comparisons of the dynamics of


local field potential and multiunit activity signals in macaque
visual cortex. J Neurosci 2010, 30:13739-13749.

10. Kayser C, Montemurro MA, Logothetis NK, Panzeri S: Spike phase coding boosts and stabilizes information carried by
spatial and temporal spike patterns. Neuron 2009, 61:597-608.
This study quantifies information related to sound in the auditory cortices
of alert animals. An important finding of this study is that the nested code,
combining spike patterns with the phase of firing, contains the most
information about sound in the auditory cortex.
11. von Stein A, Sarnthein J: Different frequencies for different
scales of cortical integration: from local gamma to long range
alpha/theta synchronization. Int J Psychophysiol 2000, 38:301313.
12. Igarashi J, Isomura Y, Arai K, Harukuni R, Fukai T: A theta-gamma

oscillation code for neuronal coordination during motor
behavior. J Neurosci 2013, 33:18515-18530.
This study demonstrated high and low gamma oscillations entrain neuronal firing and are hierarchically related to theta oscillations in the motor
cortex.
13. Ito J, Maldonado P, Grun S: Cross-frequency interaction of the
eye-movement related lfp signals in v1 of freely viewing
monkeys. Front Syst Neurosci 2013, 7:1.
14. Yanagisawa T, Yamashita O, Hirata M, Kishima H, Saitoh Y,
Goto T, Yoshimine T, Kamitani Y: Regulation of motor
representation by phase-amplitude coupling in the
sensorimotor cortex. J Neurosci 2012, 32:15467-15475.
15. Schanze T, Eckhorn R: Phase correlation among rhythms
present at different frequencies: spectral methods,
application to microelectrode recordings from visual
cortex and functional implications. Int J Psychophysiol 1997,
26:171-189.
16. Belitski A, Gretton A, Magri C, Murayama Y, Montemurro MA,
Logothetis NK, Panzeri S: Low-frequency local field potentials
and spikes in primary visual cortex convey independent visual
information. J Neurosci 2008, 28:5696-5709.
17. Nikolic D, Fries P, Singer W: Gamma oscillations: precise

temporal coordination without a metronome. Trends Cogn Sci
2013, 17:54-55.
This paper argues that gamma oscillations is to concentrate spiking
activities within a particular phase of oscillation.
18. Bosman CA, Lansink CS, Pennartz CM: Functions of gammaband synchronization in cognition: From single circuits to
functional diversity across cortical and subcortical systems.
Eur J Neurosci 2014, 39:1982-1999.
19. Buzsaki G, Wang XJ: Mechanisms of gamma oscillations. Annu
Rev Neurosci 2012, 35:203-225.
20. Joliot M, Ribary U, Llinas R: Human oscillatory brain activity
near 40 Hz coexists with cognitive temporal binding. Proc Natl
Acad Sci U S A 1994, 91:11748-11751.
21. Senkowski D, Talsma D, Grigutsch M, Herrmann CS, Woldorff MG:
Good times for multisensory integration: effects of the
precision of temporal synchrony as revealed by gamma-band
oscillations. Neuropsychologia 2007, 45:561-571.
22. Besserve M, Lowe SC, Logothetis NK, Scholkopf B, Panzeri S:
Shifts of gamma phase across primary visual cortical sites
reflect dynamic stimulus-modulated information transfer.
PLoS Biol 2015, 13:e1002257.
23. Gray RM: Entropy and information theory. New York: SpringerVerlag; 1990.
24. Jung T, Vogiatzian F, Har-Shemesh O, Fitzsimons C, Quax R:
Applying information theory to neuronal networks: from
theory to experiments. Entropy 2014, 16:5721-5737.
Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences 2016, 8:xx

Please cite this article in press as: Gupta DS, Chen L: Brain oscillations in perception, timing and action, Curr Opin Behav Sci (2016), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cobeha.2016.02.021

392
393
394
395
396
397
398
399
400
401
402
403
404
405
406
407
408
409
410
411
412
413
414
415
416
417
418
419
420
421
422
423
424
425
426
427
428
429
430
431
432
433
434
435
436
438
437
439
440
441
442
443
444
445
446
447
448
449
450
451
452
453
454
455
456
457
458
459
460

COBEHA 203 XX

6 Timing behavior

462
461
463
464
465
466
467
468
469
470
471
472
473
474
475
476
477
478
479
480
481
482
483
484
485
486
487
488
489
490
491
492
493
494
495
496
497
498
499
500
501
502
503
504
505
506
507
508
509
510
511
512
513
514
515
516
517
518
519
520
521
522
523
524
525
526
527
528
529
530
531

25. Kollmorgen S, Hahnloser RH: Dynamic alignment models for


neural coding. PLoS Comput Biol 2014, 10:e1003508.
26. Arnold MM, Szczepanski J, Montejo N, Amigo JM, Wajnryb E,
Sanchez-Vives MV: Information content in cortical spike trains
during brain state transitions. J Sleep Res 2013, 22:13-21.
27. Haslinger R, Klinkner KL, Shalizi CR: The computational

structure of spike trains. Neural Comput 2010, 22:121-157.
These authors demonstrate that the algorithmic information content (i.e.,
the information needed to exactly describe the spike train) can be split
into three parts: (1) the time-invariant structure (complexity) of the minimal
spike-generating process, which statistically describes the spike train; (2)
the randomness (due to minimal spike generating process); and (3) a
residual noise term (NOT due to minimal spike generating process).
Complexity quantifies the structure of a spike pattern and is different
from its randomness.
28. Lopes-dos-Santos V, Panzeri S, Kayser C, Diamond ME, Quian
Quiroga R: Extracting information in spike time patterns with
wavelets and information theory. J Neurophysiol 2015,
113:1015-1033.

sources. The authors suggest the role of an ongoing synchronization of


frontal and occipital areas by alpha oscillations in an inferential process
that gives rise to the perception of apparent motion.
38. Alink A, Schwiedrzik CM, Kohler A, Singer W, Muckli L: Stimulus
predictability reduces responses in primary visual cortex. J
Neurosci 2010, 30:2960-2966.
39. Schwiedrzik CM, Alink A, Kohler A, Singer W, Muckli L: A spatiotemporal interaction on the apparent motion trace. Vision Res
2007, 47:3424-3433.
40. Gupta DS: Processing of sub- and supra-second intervals in

the primate brain results from the calibration of neuronal
oscillators via sensory, motor, and feedback processes. Front
Psychol 2014, 5:816.
The author proposes a modular clock mechanism for sub- to suprasecond intervals. The clock mechanisms include endogenous oscillators
within networks of circuits that perform various interval timing tasks. The
oscillators are calibrated by regions (modules) that play roles in the
feedback control of sensorimotor tasks involving interactions with the
external surrounding.

29. Lakatos P, Shah AS, Knuth KH, Ulbert I, Karmos G, Schroeder CE:
 An oscillatory hierarchy controlling neuronal excitability and
stimulus processing in the auditory cortex. J Neurophysiol
2005, 94:1904-1911.
This study analyzed current source density and multiunit activity in the
primary auditory cortices of awake macaque monkeys to study stimulusinduced and spontaneous neuronal activities. The results reveal that EEG
signals are hierarchically organized: the delta (14 Hz) phase modulates
theta (410 Hz) amplitude, and the theta phase modulates the gamma
(3050 Hz) amplitude. This oscillatory hierarchy is shown to control the
stimulus-induced responses in a neuronal ensemble.

41. McKee JL, Riesenhuber M, Miller EK, Freedman DJ: Task


dependence of visual and category representations in
prefrontal and inferior temporal cortices. J Neurosci 2014,
34:16065-16075.

30. Schroeder CE, Lakatos P: Low-frequency neuronal oscillations


as instruments of sensory selection. Trends Neurosci 2009,
32:9-18.

44. Cirelli LK, Bosnyak D, Manning FC, Spinelli C, Marie C, Fujioka T,


Ghahremani A, Trainor LJ: Beat-induced fluctuations in auditory
cortical beta-band activity: using eeg to measure age-related
changes. Front Psychol 2014, 5:742.

31. Tort ABL, Bonnefond M, Jensen O: Gamma activity coupled to


alpha phase as a mechanism for top-down controlled gating.
PLoS One 2015, 10:e0128667.
32. Shams L, Kamitani Y, Shimojo S:, Illusions: What you see is what
you hear. Nature 2000, 408:788.
33. Chen L, Vroomen J: Intersensory binding across space and

time: a tutorial review. Atten Percept Psychophys 2013, 75:790811.
This paper compiles various studies of intersensory binding in the ventriloquism effect. The authors discuss various studies of spatial and
temporal attractions between perceptions in the visual, auditory and
tactile modalities and the direct effects and after-effects. The modulation
factors, including attention, synesthetic correspondence and other cognitive factors, are discussed.
34. Cecere R, Rees G, Romei V: Individual differences in alpha

frequency drive crossmodal illusory perception. Curr Biol 2015,
25:231-235.
This study reveals a positive correlation between individual alpha oscillation peaks and the sizes of the temporal windows of sound-induced,
double-flash illusion. The authors suggest that the duration of the alpha
cycles provides a window that cyclically gates perception and promotes
audiovisual integration.
35. Samaha J, Postle BR: The speed of alpha-band oscillations
predicts the temporal resolution of visual perception. Curr Biol
2015.
36. Leonardelli E, Braun C, Weisz N, Lithari C, Occelli V, Zampini M:
Prestimulus oscillatory alpha power and connectivity patterns
predispose perceptual integration of an audio and a tactile
stimulus. Hum Brain Mapp 2015, 36:3486-3498.
37. Sanders LL, Auksztulewicz R, Hohlefeld FU, Busch NA, Sterzer P:

The influence of spontaneous brain oscillations on apparent
motion perception. Neuroimage 2014, 102(Pt 2):241-248.
This study shows that prestimulus alpha power is stronger when observers report apparent motion perception in subsequent trials with low
presentation frequencies. Subsequent dynamic causal modeling of
steady-state responses revealed that the most likely direction of this
fronto-occipital connectivity in alpha band was from frontal to occipital

Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences 2016, 8:xx

42. Mattia M, Ferraina S, Del Giudice P: Dissociated multi-unit


activity and local field potentials: a theory inspired analysis of
a motor decision task. Neuroimage 2010, 52:812-823.
43. Kononowicz TW, van Rijn H: Single trial beta oscillations index
time estimation. Neuropsychologia 2015, 75:381-389.

45. Bartolo R, Merchant H: Beta oscillations are linked to the



initiation of sensory-cued movement sequences and the
internal guidance of regular tapping in the monkey. J Neurosci
2015, 35:4635-4640.
This study in monkeys performing synchronization-continuation tasks
revealed an increase in beta power in recordings from the putamen during
the internally driven continuation phases of the tasks.
46. Crone NE, Miglioretti DL, Gordon B, Lesser RP: Functional
mapping of human sensorimotor cortex with
electrocorticographic spectral analysis. Ii. Event-related
synchronization in the gamma band. Brain 1998, 121(Pt
12):2301-2315.
47. Szurhaj W, Derambure P: Intracerebral study of gamma
oscillations in the human sensorimotor cortex. Prog Brain Res
2006, 159:297-310.
48. Siegel M, Buschman TJ, Miller EK: Brain processing. Cortical
information flow during flexible sensorimotor decisions.
Science 2015, 348:1352-1355.
49. van Kerkoerle T, Self MW, Dagnino B, Gariel-Mathis MA, Poort J,
van der Togt C, Roelfsema PR: Alpha and gamma oscillations
characterize feedback and feedforward processing in monkey
visual cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2014, 111:14332-14341.
50. Mormann F, Fell J, Axmacher N, Weber B, Lehnertz K, Elger CE,
Fernandez G: Phase/amplitude reset and theta-gamma
interaction in the human medial temporal lobe during a
continuous word recognition memory task. Hippocampus
2005, 15:890-900.
51. VanRullen R, Koch C: Is perception discrete or continuous?
Trends Cogn Sci 2003, 7:207-213.
52. Wallace JM, Scott-Samuel NE: Spatial versus temporal
grouping in a modified ternus display. Vision Res 2007, 47:2353
2366.
The results of this study show that while spatial properties influence the
percept of the Ternus display, a form of apparent motion, the temporal
properties produce a strong influence.

www.sciencedirect.com

Please cite this article in press as: Gupta DS, Chen L: Brain oscillations in perception, timing and action, Curr Opin Behav Sci (2016), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cobeha.2016.02.021

531
532
533
534
535
536
537
538
539
540
541
542
543
544
545
546
547
548
549
550
551
552
553
554
555
556
557
558
559
560
561
562
563
564
565
566
567
568
569
570
571
572
573
574
575
576
577
578
579
580
581
582
583
584
585
586
587
588
589
590
591
592
593
594
595
596
597
598
598

Você também pode gostar