Você está na página 1de 33

NeuroRehabilitation

381
IOS
Press

23

(2008)

381394

Geriatric neuropsychology: Implications for front line clinicians


Christina Weyer Jamoraa,, Ronald M. Ruffb and
Bonnie B. Connor c
a San Francisco Clinical Neurosciences, San Francisco General
Hospital, San Francisco, CA, USA
b San Francisco Clinical Neurosciences, University of California San
Francisco, San Francisco, CA, USA
c University of San Francisco, San
Francisco, CA, USA

Abstract. Consistent with the aging population, neuropsychologists are being


asked with increased frequency to evaluate older adults. These assessments are
often complicated by medical and psychiatric co-morbidities, polypharmacy, and
complex psy- chosocial and legal issues that are frequently encountered in this
population. The aim of this review article is to address the chal- lenges
neuropsychologists and other frontline clinicians often confront when evaluating
older individuals. Specifically, we review psychiatric and medical co-morbidities,
testing accommodations, diagnostic versus descriptive testing approaches,
normative issues, polypharmacy, and reimbursement rates. Finally, future
implications are discussed for advancing the neuropsychologists role in
evaluating and treating older individuals.
Keywords:
Geriatric
neuropsychology,
assessment and treatment challenges

1. Introduction
In the United States, older
individuals are the fastest growing
segment of the population with 20% of
these older Americans experiencing
some type of mental dis- order [25].
One in seven adults 70 years of age
and older have some type of dementia
[60].
As a result, psychological
services to older adults are rapidly
grow- ing. To meet the increasing
needs of older individ- uals various

national and state agencies have


encour- aged psychologists to deepen
and extend their exist- ing knowledge
base and clinical expertise to include
this population. The American
Psychological Associa- tion (APA) has
developed guidelines for psychological
practice with older adults that address
the psycholo- gists attitudes, general
knowledge about adult devel- opment,
and special skills required for
assessment, in- tervention, and

consultation [7]. Many psychology internship programs now offer rotations


in geriatric psy- chiatry, and post
doctoral fellowships in geropsycholo-

Address for correspondence:


Christina Weyer Jamora, 909 Hyde
Street, Suite 620, San Francisco, CA
94109, USA. Tel.: +1 415 771
7833; Fax: +1 415 922 5849; E-mail:
cyweyer@aol.com.

ISSN 1053-8135/08/$17.00
reserved

gy have emerged. The importance of


geriatric specific education has also
been recognized by the California
Board of Psychology, which now
requires licensed psy- chologists to
complete training in aging and longterm care.
The aim of this review article is to
address many of the issues that
neuropsychologists and other frontline
clinicians face when evaluating older
individuals. The following questions
will be discussed: (a) Can psychi- atric
illnesses, in particular depression,
exacerbate or cause cognitive deficits?
(b) What testing accommo- dations
can yield the most benefit for older
individu- als?
(c) How should
neuropsychologists distinguish the
effects of brain damage in the context
of comor- bid medical illnesses? (d)
What effects do medications or
polypharmacy have on patients
mental status? (e) Given that pain is
prevalent in this population, what effects
do
pain
have
on
neuropsychological assessments? (f)
What role should norms play in our
test selection? (g) What are the
strengths and weaknesses of descriptive versus diagnostic norms? (h) For
most referrals, is a juxtaposition of a
descriptive and diagnostic ap- proach
called for? (i) What services are
reimbursable
for
clinical
neuropsychologists, and what ethical
issues

2008 IOS Press and the authors. All rights

126

C.W. Jamora et al. / Geriatric neuropsychology: Implications for front line clinicians

are
raised
by
reimbursement
limitations? and (j) How can we
advance our future services?
1.1. Can psychiatric illnesses, in
particular depression,
exacerbate or cause cognitive
deficits?
Almost one quarter of individuals
over the age of
55 meet the criteria for a psychiatric
disorder [39,
77]. Specifically, in older individuals
it has been esti- mated that 1035%
have significant depressive symptomatology, 1321% have significant
anxiety, and 3.6
4.5% experience psychosis secondary
to major depres- sion [34,47,52].
Moreover, depression following an
acute medical crises, such as stroke
and traumatic brain injury (TBI), are
relatively common.
For example,
some studies estimate that one out of
four stroke sur- vivors will experience
a major depressive episode with- in
one year [64], and 80% of older
individuals with a TBI meet criteria
for an Axis I disorder [70]. Giv- en
the prevalence of these psychiatric
disorders, most referral questions are
best addressed by adding a psychodiagnostic evaluation component
to the cognitive assessment.
A study by Feil and colleagues found
that older individuals with psychiatric diagnoses
have increased lev- els of apathy and
depression and demonstrate slowed
speed of cognitive processing and
diminished executive functioning [27].
High levels of depression were relat-

ed to cognitive inflexibility, decreased


word generation, and diminished
abstract reasoning [27]. These findings suggest that as the severity of the
psychiatric dis- turbance increases the
effect on cognition can become more
pronounced. Other research findings
suggest that the clusters of cognitive
deficits may differ depending upon the
nature of the psychiatric illness. For
exam- ple, after controlling for
medical burden, patients diag- nosed
with generalized anxiety disorder were
found to have deficits in attention, as
well as short and long term memory
[32]. In comparison, individuals with
major depression also demonstrated
deficits in attention and memory, with
compromised word finding ability,
speed of processing, and global
cognitive functioning also noted
[32,51].
In conclusion, neuropsychological
examinations should capture the
presence and severity of psychiatric
disorders, including anxiety and
depression.
Milder forms of
depression and adjustment disorders
can be caused by stressors including
retirement,
outliving loved ones,
limited social networks, and age
related physical and cognitive decline
[14]. Bereavement and

C.W. Jamora et al. / Geriatric neuropsychology: Implications for front line clinicians

the availability of psychosocial


support are two specif- ic areas that
should be fully explored when
assessing
depression
in
older
individuals [14].
1.2. What testing accommodations
yield the most benefit for older
individuals?
Clinical experience coupled with an
appropriate ap- preciation of emerging
literature provides an avenue for
identifying
ways
to
adapt
neuropsychological tests to meet the
needs of older adults. There are three
basic categories that may require
adaptation of testing pro- cedures: (a)
the patients physical status, (b) cognitive limitations, and (c) psychological
status. Severe motor (e.g. dysarthria,
hemiplegia) and sensory limita- tions
(e.g. blindness, deafness) often require
clinicians to administer tests that rely
on the patients preserved abilities.
Similarly, some older adults may
fatigue eas- ily and it is recommended
that rest breaks be provided during the
evaluation as needed. Also, it may be
bene- ficial to complete testing
sessions in the morning hours and/or
divide the testing up over several days.
Adapta- tions for cognitive limitations
may be most pronounced for
individuals with non-fluent aphasia
and in these in- stances adaptive
techniques such as picture books may
be useful. Accomodations may also
be beneficial to alleviate deficits in
comprehension, speed of cognitive
processing and behavioral responding,
and word find- ing difficulties. The
interested reader is referred to the

127

article by Caplan and Shechter in this


special issue of NeuroRehabilitation
for a thorough review of testing
accomodations for older adults.
In comparison to these physical and
cognitive adaptations, psychological accomodations
are frequently not considered during
the evaluation process. It is important to remember that many older
individuals may feel threatened by the
assessment process. Especially when
the intent of the referral is to address
their abil- ity to drive, handle their
finances, live independently, and/or
determine their need for supportive
interven- tions. These types of referral
questions may cause pa- tients to
refuse participation or prematurely
terminate
the
testing
session.
Informed consent is central to respecting a patients autonomy and
rights, and neuropsy- chologists are
encouraged to identify and address
any questions or patient concerns
upfront.
The therapeutic assessment model
from the psychodiagnostic assessment literature
provides an ef- fective approach to
assessing older individuals [28
30]. Therapeutic assessment is
complimentary
to
standard
assessment practices and proposes that
assessment

is more than just an information


gathering exercise. Rather it can be
viewed as an opportunity for patients
to come to a new understanding about
themselves
and
their
life
accomplishments. In particular, older
individ- uals frequently view their
lives on the basis of past accomplishments. This is understandable
since a forward looking perspective
may be marked with uncertainty, a
progressive decline in health, chronic
pain, and loss. The clinician must be
sensitive to the developmental state of
the older individual, which Erikson
describes as the stage of Integrity
versus Dispair [26]. Thus, the
neuropsychological evaluation should
allow for older individuals to review
their life accomplishments whilst
coming to terms with prominent
negative
events
they
have
experienced.
The
therapeutic
assessment
approach places strong emphasis on
integrating therapeutic skills such as
re- spect, trust, genuine enthusiastic
curiosity, and collab- oration into the
standard assesment. The major
compo- nents of the therapeutic
assessment model include (a) treating
the patient as a collaborator, (i.e. lets
find out these answers together); (b)
addressing the patients personal
concerns; (c) giving feedback that is
tied to the patients everyday
functioning and concerns; and (d)
following up with a letter outlining the
assessment findings using the patients
questions to guide the feed- back [28].
The following case example
illustrates the benefit of

the therapeutic assessment approach.


An older individ- ual was referred for a
neuropsychological evaluation of her
functional status. While the evaluation
suggested that the patient was unable
to live independently, she firmly
refused in-home nursing care. This
was partic- ularly worrisome since her
children lived out of state and they
were unable to provide support or
supervi- sion to their mother. During
the feedback session, the patients
concerns
about
losing
her
independence were identified. It also
became clear that while she wanted to
please her family, she resolutely felt
that in-home care was unnecessary
because she believed she possessed no
cognitive impairments. The patients
concerns were reframed by focusing
on her lifetime achievement of having
been a good mother and that her
children were worried for her welfare.
The in-home support subse- quently
was viewed as a gift of peace of mind
to her children which was congruent
with her long held val- ue that the role
of a mother is to sacrifice for the sake
of her children.
Emotional accommodations can be
utilized to align
ourselves with our patients by
acknowledging their dif- ficulties,
regardless of our ability to help, and
prais-

ing them for their own efforts to


enhance their sense of well-being and
control [53]. Indeed, research has
found that clinicians who utilize the
therapeutic assess- ment model had
fewer incomplete assessments due to
premature termination, and patients
were more likely to experience
decreased distress and increased hope,
even when the assessment findings
were negative [30,
57
].
1.3. How should neuropsychologists
distinguish the effect of brain
damage in the context of
comorbid medical illnesses?
As individuals grow older they tend
to have multiple co-morbid medical
diseases, many of which can have a
detrimental impact on cognitive
functioning. For ex- ample, medical
conditions common in older
individuals include congestive heart
failure, diabetes, pulmonary disorders,
cardiovascular disease, and renal
failure, with many of these conditions
being known to compro- mise
cognitive functioning [2,37,41,61].
Specifically, congestive heart failure
has been linked to deficits in global
cognitive functioning [2]; diabetes
with deficits in episodic memory
[61], processing speed, execu- tive
functioning and verbal memory [37];
respirato- ry diseases with deficits in
attention, processing speed and
memory [41]; cardiovascular disease
with deficits in executive
functioning, attention, and
processing speed [43]; and renal

failure with declines in executive


functioning, verbal memory, and
processing speed [48]. While older
individuals often experience multiple
chronic medical conditions, research
studies typically focus on single
disease states, excluding those individuals with co-morbidities.
Accordingly these find- ings may bear
little resemblance to the older
individu- als typically encountered in
clinical practice. Tightly controlled
studies may be the paragon of internal
valid- ity; however, the questionable
generalizability of these results limits
their clinical utility. This is
particularly concerning since research
has indicated that there is a positive
relationship between disease burden
and cog- nitive impairment [22]. In
particular, older individu- als who
have five or more comorbid medical
diseases were found to be prone to
deficits in selective, speed- ed, and
divided attention [22]. Moreover,
marginal- ized older individuals are
particularly at risk, due in part to
higher base rates of stress, unstable
housing, and chronic medical
conditions such as diabetes, and
hyperlipidemia
[75].
Understanding the interactions
between multiple medical comorbidities can be daunting for even
the

most experienced neuropsychologist.


To reduce diag- nostic errors
clinicians should strive to find ways to
measure the dynamic relationship
between cognition and multiple
disease states. Since extensive
neuropsy- chological testing is often
contraindicated with these patients, it
is important to have a clear
understanding of the signs associated
with reversible versus nonre- versible
medical diseases. Thus consulting
with med- ical staff as needed, and
recognizing the limitations of our
cognitve assessments may be ways to
increase our diagnostic accuracy.
While it is not within our scope of
practice to diagnose medical diseases
such as car- diovascular disease or
diabetes, we should aspire to understand how multi-system diseases
influence the cog- nitive and
emotional functioning of our patients.
Col- laboration and consultation with
medical professionals can enhance our
evaluations and promote accurate conclusions about patients who are
experiencing
multiple
medical
conditions.
When assessing older individuals
with multiple med- ical conditions the
following questions should be considered:
1. What medical diagnoses are
active at this point?
What are the patients chronic
medical diseases? What is the
severity and length of these
diseases?
Is
the
patient
metabolically stable based on
recent laboratory results, vital
signs, oxygenation, and hydration?

While is it not necessary to master


the nuances of medical laboratory
tests, it is impor- tant to ask
medical providers the extent to
which
our
patients
are
metabolically and hemodynamically stable, and the extent to
which these diseases may be
influencing their daily functioning.
2. If the patient is confused, what
is the duration and quality of his
or her mentation (i.e. acute versus
chronic, waxing and waning,
worse in the evening)? Is the
onset recent or prolonged?
Delirium is common in older
individuals. Prior to testing it may
be necessary to consult with the
referring physician to rule out
medical
instability
in
the
following areas:
metabolic,
oxygenation, vascular, endocrine,
seizures, tumor, trauma, ure- mia,
psychiatric, infection, medications
and de- generative diseases.
3. Has the patient recently fallen or
had a history
of falls in the past, and if so was
brain imaging ordered? Does the
patient have a steady gait? Is the
patient incontinent or have urinary
urgency? Falls are common in
older adults and are the most
frequent mechanism of traumatic
brain injury in this population
[10]. In particular, the combina-

tion of unsteady gait, high


anticholinergic burden, and urinary
urgency can precipitate falls [1]. It
is important to note that many falls
in nursing homes are not witnessed
by staff and age associated cerebral atrophy can allow blood and
cerebrospinal fluid to collect to a
significant degree before mass
effect causes neurological changes
[39]. Simi- larly, normal pressure
hydrocephalus secondary to a
subdural
hematoma
mimics
dementia [39]. When patients
experience persistent confusion,
headaches,
and
loss
of
consciousness following a fall,
brain imaging may be needed to
rule out the possibility of a
subdural hematoma.
Admittedly, obtaining answers to all
of the above questions is often
unrealistic in settings where staff time
is significantly limited and records are
sparse. Alter- natively, individuals
familiar with the patient such as
family members, friends, caregivers,
and staff members can often be useful
sources of collateral information. In
addition, the patients list of
medications and respec- tive dosages
can be invaluable in terms of orienting
the clinician to the patients major
medical illnesses.
Often, neuropsychologists may focus
primarily on
cognitive deficits, secondarily on
emotion, and to a less- er extent on
physical functioning, with limited
integra- tion of these three areas.
Overly compartmentalized di- agnostic
formulations can lead to diagnostic
errors, in- appropriate treatment
recommendations, and ultimate- ly

harm the patient. For example, it


would be short- sighted of a
neuropsychologist, on the basis of
severly impaired cognitive deficits, to
diagnose an older indi- vidual with
dementia, when it is subsequently
discov- ered that the patient was
dehydrated and their confusion
subsided after their water intake
increased.
This ex- ample is
particularly common in nursing
homes, where there is little available
collateral information, few med- ical
professionals with whom to consult,
and high staff to patient ratios, as well
as, frequent staff turnover. The
patients medical acuity, limited
medical records, and time constraints
also can limit the accuracy, breadth,
and depth of our assessments.
Admittedly, it is diffi- cult to achieve
a balanced approach that gives voice
to the patients physical, cognitive,
and emotional func- tioning relative to
their social and cultural functioning.
However, keeping these issues in mind
can reduce our tendency to fall victim
to diagnostic pitfalls.
1.4. What effects do medications or
polypharmacy have on the
patients mental status?
As people age they tend to be
prescribed more med- ications [45]
have greater difficulty managing
multiple

medications [19], and experience


more adverse drug reactions and
hospitalizations
secondary
to
polyphar- macy [31]. Depending on
the setting, older individu- als on
average may take anywhere from four
to 10 or more medications daily
[31,72]. Also, it is notewor- thy that
medication nonadherence rates in
older adults ranges from 26%59%
[78], which can lead to medi- cal
decompensation,
unnecessary
hospitalizations, and death [31].
Pharmacodynamics refers to the
effects
of
a
drug,
while
pharmacokinetics refers to the
movement of a drug through the
body [42, p. 630]. As an individual
ages, factors such as decreased
gastrointestinal blood flow, increased
porosity of the blood brain barrier, reduced blood circulation, increased
body fat composi- tion, altered cell
binding, reduced hepatic and renal efficiency,
and
endogenous
neurotransmitter decline can lead to
alterations in the metabolization,
distribution, absorption, and excretion
of drugs [6,42]. As outlined in Table
1,
benzodiazepines,
diuretics,
narcotics,
nonsteroidal
antiinflammatory, and anticholinergic
medi- cations may be particularly
detrimental
to
the
cognitive
functioning of older adults.
Unnecessary
polypharmacy
commonly occurs in the older adult
population. One study found that 44%
of older individuals were prescribed at
least one medi- cation unnecessarily
[36]. While co-morbid disease states
often require multiple medications, the

use of unnecessary medications is


particularly problematic in older
individuals.
According
to
the
American Medical Association Council
on Scientific Affairs, Polyphar- macy
is problematic for older persons
because it is the greatest risk factor for
ADRs (adverse drug reactions), drug
interactions, reduced compliance, and
increased emergency room visits,
hospitalizations, and nursing home
admissions (para. 5) [6]. Also, up to
70% of older individuals do not inform
their physicians when using dietary
supplements which can negatively
inter- act with medications. The
interested reader is referred to
Gonzalez and colleagues for a detailed
discussion
of
the
implications
associated with dietary suppliment use
among the elderly [35]. Polypharmacy
can poten- tially lead to cognitive
impairment, delirium, falls, and
decreased functional status in older
individuals [6].
When assessing an older individual
with multiple
medications the following questions
should be consid- ered:
1.

What
are
their
current
medications/supplements,
the
dosages, and indications for use?
Are there any recent changes to
their medication regime? Are they
taking
their
medications
regularly?

Abrupt medication changes and


medication non- adherence can
lead to delirium in older individuals. For patients with multiple
and psychiatric providers, the
neuropsychological report can be
a central document highlighting
who is prescrib- ing what. It may
be helpful to consult with pertinent medical providers to see if
the number of medications can be
streamlined.
2. What medications did the patient
take prior to the assessment?
Ideally, clinicians should be
aware of what medications the
patient took prior to the
assessment and work with a
pharmacist or physi- cian to
ascertain the degree to which
medica- tion effects could
influence testing performance.
Pharmacokinetic indicators such
as medication half-life and peak
absorption time can be helpful in
this regard.
1.5. What effect does pain
have on
neuropsychological
performance?
Approximately 80% of nursing home
patients and
50% of home dwelling older
individuals have signifi- cant pain
issues [69]. Common sources of pain
in old- er individuals include arthritis
and orthopedic issues, improper
positioning, postherpetic neuralgia,
vitamin deficiencies, constipation,
pressure ulcers, post-stroke syndrome,

contractures, and cancer [69]. Pain can


have psychosocial implications such
as increased isolation, depression,
anxiety, fatigue, learned helplessness,
re- duced independence, and substance
abuse and depen- dence. While pain
is common in older individuals, it
often goes under-treated and underdiagnosed, espe- cially in those
patients who are cognitively impaired.
For example, Cohen-Mansfield and
Lipson
found
that
physicians
consistently rated pain levels as lower
for patients with severe cognitive
impairments, regardless of their
medical diagnosis [18]. Alternatively,
many patients believe that pain is part
of growing older and subsequently
may under-report their symptoms to
prac- titioners.
On
neuropsychological
tests
uncontrolled pain has been linked with
cognitive inflexibility, distractibility,
and poor memory scores [38].
Eccleston found that attention and
concentration
were
negatively
influenced by severe pain [24]. Heyer
and colleagues found older individuals
with active pain after spinal surgery
showed diminished scores on the Rey
Complex Figure Test and Trail
Making Test-Part A [38]. Contrarily,
others have opined that mild to
moderate levels of pain in older in-

Tabl
e1
Medications that may have cognitive
effects in older individuals
Drug class Medications Indication
manifestations

Side effects

Adverse behavioral

Anticholinerics
Ditropan, Detrol
Incontinence
Anticholinergic effects
Confusion
Antihistamines
Benadryl, Vistaril
Sleep
Sedation, anticholinergic
Confusion, agitation, effects,
urine retention delirium
Antipsychotics
Haldol, Zyprexa,
Agitation, Psychosis
Akathisia hypotension,
Increased fall risk
due hypotension, Risperdal, Seroquel
orthostatic
anticholinergic
confusion, restlessness, FDA has a
side, effects heart arrhythmias black
box warning on the use of
antipsychotics with
older individuals due
to increased risk of
stroke and cardiac
arrhythmias
Benzodiazepines
Ativan, Serax, Anxiety
Motor slowing,
Increased falls, delirium, may have
Valium, Xanax
diminished motor withdraw symptoms
if discontinued coordination, sedation
abruptly, long acting
are particularly
inappropriate
Diuretics Lasix, Aldactone,
Hypertension, fluid load in
Electrolyte imbalance,
Confusion and falls secondary to
Hydrochlorothiazide, Congestive Heart Failure dehydration
dehydration, fluid volume loss Thiazide
Narcotics Morphine, Vicodin,
Pain
Sedation Confusion,
falls
Loritab, Demerol
Steroids
Prednisone Autoimmune diseases,
Confusion
Confusion, falls, psychosis, mania
Myeloma,

Inflammatory
diseases
Selective Serotonin Paxil
Depression
High anticholinergic
profile
Confusion
Reuptake Inhibitors
Tricyclic Elavil, Tofranil,
Depression Pain High anticholinergic
profile
Increased falls related to orthostatic
Antidepressants
Nortriptyline
hypotension, Confusion
Adapted with permission from (Hunter & Cyr, 2006).
dividuals has negligible effects on
neuropsychological test performance
[11,49].
It can be difficult to assess pain in
patients with
neuropsychological impairment. For
example, patients with cognitive
impairments such as dementia or
apha- sia may be unable to articulate
the severity of their pain. They also
may have reduced pain detection due
to slowed cortical processing [62].
Neuropsychologists may need to
educate family, caregivers, and staff
mem- bers who work with older adults
who are experiencing cognitive
dysfunction or expressive language
difficul- ties
about non-verbal
indicators of pain.
A listing of
common pain behaviors in older adults
with cognitive impairments is
provided in Table 2.
In terms of clinical issues it is
important to assess
the location, frequency, duration,
quality, intensity, as well as, relieving
and exacerbating factors associated
with the patients pain experience
[40]. It is also help- ful to explore
whether patients feel their pain is adequately controlled, what the impact of
pain is on their functional abilities,

and what their knowledge, expectations, and beliefs are regarding pain
management and control [40].
Assessments of emotional function
such as depression and anxiety should
be incorporated as

well. Lastly, there are often tradeoffs


when testing old- er individuals who
are in severe pain. For example, a
preoccupation
with
pain
may
compromise attention and stamina. In
these instances, it may be prudent to
postpone testing until their pain is
better controlled. Of course, if a
patients pain is being inadequately
treated
or
overlooked,
the
neuropsychologist should collabo- rate
with medical providers to ensure that
the patients pain is appropriately
assessed and treated.
1.6. What role should norms
play in our test selection?
The utility of neuropsychological
assessment largely depends on
normative data. It is essential that
clinicians
are
appropriately
acquainted with the quality and
quantity of available norms.
As
diagnosticians a dis- tinction should be
made between referral questions that
are diagnostic in nature and those that
are descriptive. Descriptive referral
questions focus on how an indi- vidual
is functioning cognitively relative to
the appro- priate reference group.
Thus, norms for this purpose usually
rely on the general population as the
reference standard. That is, the
individuals test performance is

Tabl
e2
Common pain behaviors in older
adults with cognitive impairment
Facial
Vocalizations/verbalizations
expression
Sighing, moaning, groaning
s
Grunting, chanting, calling out
Slight frown; sad,
frightened face
Grimacing, wrinkled
forehead, closed or
tightened eyes
Any distorted
expression
Rapid
blinking
Body
movement
s
Rigid, tense body
posture, guarding

Fidgeti
ng
Increased pacing,
rocking
Restricted movement
Gait or mobility
changes
Mental status changes

Noisy breathing
Asking for help
Verbally abusive
Changes in interpersonal interactions

Aggressive, combative, resisting care


Decreased social interactions
Socially inappropriate, disruptive
Withdrawn

Changes in activity patterns/routines

Refusing food, appetite change


Increased periods of resting
Increased sleep
Sudden cessation of common routines
Increased wandering

Crying
or tears
Increased confusion

Irritabil
ity
Adapted with permission from American Geriatric Association
(2007)
referenced to norms that allow for
characterizations as Below Average,

Average, Above Average and so


on. Yet, an intelligence quotient or

memory score of
85 neither rules in, nor rules out, the
diagnosis of brain impairment.
In contrast to the descriptive
approach, the diagnos- tic referral
raises the question of whether or not
an in- dividual is impaired. Indeed,
diagnostic assessments follow the
pathognomonic sign approach [20].
Thus, premorbid functioning becomes
the reference standard, with the
neuropsychologist
charged
to
determine if the patients testing
performance deviates from expected
premorbid levels of functioning. In
this context, test performance is
characterized as Intact versus Mildly Impaired, Moderately Impaired,
or Severely Im- paired. However,
these scores or labels by themselves
do not identify abnormal performance.
A score in the
10th or even 5th percentile does not
necessarily indicate an abnormal
performance, unless the clinician
infers that this score deviates
significantly from the patients
premorbid level of functioning.
Therefore, population norms serve
only as points of reference and
premorbid estimates must be taken
into consideration.
Various
neuropsychological
screening batteries used in the
assessment of older individuals are
patterned af- ter the pathognomonic
model, and thus can be characterized as diagnostic or deficitoriented rather than descriptive or
population-based. Indeed, near errorless performances becomes the
comparison standard against which
deviations from this expectation are

clas- sified according to degrees of


abnormality. Examples

of such measures include the


Alzheimers Disease As- sessment
Scale [65] with an estimated
administration time of 45 minutes, the
Blessed Dementia Scale [12] (<10
min), the Consortium for the
Establishment of a Registry for
Alzheimers Disease Battery [55]
(CER- AD; 2040 min), Cognistat
[46] (520 min), Demen- tia Rating
Scale-2 [44] (3045 min), Folstein
Mini- Mental State Exam [33] (510
min),
Montreal
Cognitive
Assessment [56] (10 min), the
Repeatable
Battery
for
the
Assessment of Neuropsychological
Status [63] (2030 min), St. Louis
University Mental Status [74] (7 min),
and the 7 Minute Screen [71].
In addition to diagnostic screening
batteries that were developed for
older individuals, neuropsychologists commonly rely on descriptive
tests (e.g. Wiscon- sin Card Sorting
Test, Trail Making Test) that were not
specifically designed for older
individuals. The geri- atric clinician
must be mindful of the population
char- acteristics from which the
normative data is based. For example,
small cell sizes in the upper age
ranges may limit the utility of a
particular test with older adults.
Moreover, psychometrically sound
tests may rely on norms that are
grounded in the principles of a normal
curve and central tendency and small
cell sizes may increase the likelihood
of diminished reliability and va- lidity
[54] with older adults. Therefore, it

is impor- tant that clinicians who rely


on descriptive norms un- derstand the
inherent strengths and weakness of the
psychometric measures they employ.

1.7. What are the strengths and


weaknesses of descriptive
norms?
The
practice
of
clinical
neuropsychology is strongly rooted in
psychometric assessment. Given that
we re- ly heavily on norms, it is
disappointing that normative samples
for older individuals have historically
been in- sufficient. Similarly, Bush
and colleagues have noted that many
normative samples have been stitched
to- gether from control groups that
were recruited to match specific
research project parameters [15].
This rais- es questions of whether
older individuals with mild cognitive
impairment were included versus
excluded from normative samples.
Indeed, norms based sole- ly on the
most cognitively intact individuals
may not comprise a representative age
matched normal distri- bution since
some degree of cognitive decline is
part of normal aging. When age
and geographic strati- fications vary
across tests, the comparability of
these reference groups is called into
question.
Indeed, for older
individuals, this heterogeneity among
norms is even more pronounced. Thus,
the application of norms for older
adults requires a familiarity with the
specific inclusion criteria of the
normative sample.
Questions that
should be considered when reviewing
descriptive
norms
include
the
following:
1. What is the age range of the
normative sample? A majority of
the neuropsychological tests are

not normed for individuals above


the age of 70 or 80.
2. Since most tests provide multiple
scores, which of
these test scores are normally
distributed? Note that many error
scores or ratio scores are not normally distributed.
3. Is the test based on a censusmatched sample?
If not, is the sample derived from
one or multi- ple geographic
regions? What demographic features are underrepresented and
what effect might this have? For
example, is the patients ethnicity
adequately represented?
4. What demographic stratifications
were used for education, gender,
and age?
5. Are the older age stratifications of
equal cell size? (Some tests have
but a few individuals that are over
70 years old, thus the older age
sample
may
be
underrepresented.)
6. What age break downs were used
and are these sufficiently sensitive
for this particular patient? (Age
break down in decades for adults
between
3040, 4050, 5160 are typically
more stable than those for adults
between 6170, and espe- cially
for the 7180, 8190, 91100 age
groups.)

7.

What inclusion or exclusion


criteria were used, that may have
resulted in a skewed representation
for the older samples?

Why has advancing the quality and


quantity of norms for older individuals
not been a priority?
Bush and
colleagues opined that financial
interests are one of the main reasons
for this paradox [15]. Specifically, the
development of norms is not
considered scientif- ic enough to
secure research funding. In academic
settings, the publication of tests is not
viewed as be- ing commensurate with
peer-reviewed research publi- cations.
Consequently,
test
development
activities typ- ically fall to publishing
companies, whose existence is
dependent on a broad market base and
instruments in the public domain
typically are not attractive to test
publishers.
One of the authors (Ruff), normed a
flexible test bat- tery that was
originally designed for a National
Insti- tutes of Mental Health
multicenter study of patients with
moderate to severe brain injury. While
selecting this test battery, our team
(Arthur Benton, Neil Brooks, Harvey
Levin, Igor Grant, Jeff Barth, and Ron
Ruff) selected tests based on their
merit without consider- ing what
company owned the publishing rights.
This project produced a flexible battery
normed on 360 indi- viduals stratified
according to gender, age, and education. When we approached test
publishers to consider publishing the
normative data, they congratulated us
on the quality of the normative study,

but none were will- ing to publish the


test battery, since many of the tests
were owned by competing test
publishers. This exam- ple highlights
one reason why test batteries
comprised of tests that were developed
by
different
publishers
face
tremendous obstacles.
1.8. What are the strengths and
weaknesses of diagnostic
norms?
Many screening batteries developed
specifically for older adults were
designed to diagnose dementia with
many of these measures utilizing cutoff scores. While screening batteries
can be quite useful, it is important to
remember that these tests are best
applied to those cases where the
referral question is soliciting a diagnosis, rather than a psychometric
description of the indi- viduals
cognitive abilities.
It is important for neuropsychologists
to be aware of
the reliability and validity of the
various indices and subtest scores they
use to guide clinical decisions be- fore
interpreting the test findings. For
example, while

the RBANS has acceptable test-retest


reliability for the global score, this
reliability is questionable for various
subtests or index scores [63].
Similarly, research has shown that the
Cognistat can lead to high misclassification rates if one relies on the
subtests rather than the total score [6].
Factor analytic research with older
adults yielded a unitary factor
structure, and this single composite
score was shown to be more capable
of de- tecting cognitive deficits in
older individuals in reha- bilitation
groups [6]. Thus, clinicians should be
cau- tious when rendering conclusions
on various subtests of these batteries.
When interpreting diagnostic test
norms, the follow- ing questions
should be considered:
1. On which clinical populations
was the diagnostic test validated?
(Was the test designed to diagnose
Alzheimer disease or differentiate
among differ- ent dementias such
as Alzheimers, Huntingtons, or
Parkinsons disease?)
2. What stand-alone subtests within
the battery have
acceptable test-retest reliability?
(Most often, these subtests have
too few items to yield accept- able
test-retest reliability; however,
the global scores may be more
useful in terms of diagnostic
validity.)
3. If the diagnosis is known, based
on neurologi- cal or medical
evidence (e.g. severe TBI, CVA,
MS), are the test norms sufficient

to allow for re- liable assessment


gradations to capture cognitive
changes over time?
1.9. For most referrals is a
juxtaposition of a descriptive and
diagnostic approach called for?
Generally, while referral sources are
interested in di- agnostic issues [76]
their concerns usually extend well
beyond that [23,50]. According to
research, three pri- mary referral
questions emerge [59]: evaluation of
ca- pacity for independent living
(95%), the etiology of the observed
cognitive deficits (e.g. is it depression
or dementia) (31%), and driving
capacity (15%). Inter- estingly, 66%
of all referral questions include all
three of these questions [59].
Moreover,
the
geriatric
neuropsychologist often assumes the role
of a behavioral consultant. Most often
what referral sources want is for
clinicians to create and implement
interventions
that
enhance
the
capabilities of the patient while
reducing the patients problematic
behaviors [50].

The diagnostic and descriptive


approaches
are
not
mutually
exclusive, but rather can be blended
for cer- tain referral questions. If the
referral
source
wants
the
neuropsychologist to determine if the
patients cogni- tive decline meets the
criteria of mild cognitive impair- ment
versus dementia, then a diagnostic test
should be selected. However, for
most other etiologies, such as cerebral
vascular accident, traumatic brain
injury,
multiple
sclerosis,
Parkinsons disease, meningitis or
other brain infections, and intracranial
tumors a descriptive approach should
be used. In other words, if the diagnosis of the neurological illness is not
in question, but the referral is aimed at
understanding the degree of impairment, then a descriptive test
measure is preferable to capture the
gradations of decline. The latter is
especially
important
when
establishing a baseline against which
change can be monitored over time as
well as ascertaining cognitive
strengths and weaknesses that play a
role in care planning.
Selecting the most fitting
neuropsychological test
battery is driven by the referral
question and the pa- tients
symptomatology with neurologists
being our most prominent referral
source [73]. In order for neurologists to continue to value our
unique contribu- tion we must
understand and take into account their
needs. The American Academy of
Neurology has rec- ommended seven
reasons for neuropsychological consultations [3].
In Table 3, we

categorized these seven referral


questions into either descriptive,
diagnostic or a combination of both.
As depicted in Table 3 pure
diagnostic referrals to
neuropsychologists are relatively rare,
however, our de- scriptive norms for
older individuals remain inadequate
compromising our ability to answer
referral questions in a solid manner.
Physicians do not usually depend on
neuropsychologists to diagnose a
cerebrovascular accident or traumatic
brain injury; instead they utilize
clinical history, labs, and neuroimaging
to
buttress
their
diagnostic
impressions. Neuropsychologists can
be the most helpful in determining an
individuals cognitive status, whether
age-accelerated decline meets the criteria of a mild cognitive impairment or
dementia, and when cognition
negatively influences a patients decisional capacity.
However, our
descriptive tests lack solid older adult
norms to firmly answer many of these
questions.
Also, while referral
questions commonly require attention
to a particular cognitive domain, it is
important to select assessment
measures with a com- prehensive
approach in mind.
Therefore,
combining diagnostic and descriptive
testing approaches are rec- ommended
to offset the shortcomings of each.

Tabl
e3
Diagnostic and descriptive
neuropsychological referrals
Referral questions for neuropsychological consultations according to the
Amer- Diagnostic Descriptive Diagnostic and descriptive ican Academy
of Neurology (1996)
1. When mental status examination reveals mild or questionable cognitive
deficits
X
such as mild cognitive impairment.
2. When following the course of a disorder is either in recovery or decline
such
X
as dementia and stroke.
3. When cognitive strengths and weakness are considered for living
independently.
X
4. In the context of providing specific rehabilitation or other therapeutic
services
X
such as stroke rehabilitation.
5. When a comprehensive profile buttressed with
clinical, laboratory and imaging
X data may assist in diagnosis such as
differentiating subcortical dementia from
frontotemporal dementia.
6. When considering epilepsy surgery.
X
7. When litigation issues arise questioning the patients cognitive status such
as
X
capacity for decision-making.
1.10. What levels of reimbursements
are available?
Beginning July 1, 1990 nonphysician providers of mental health
services (psychologists and licensed
clin- ical social workers) were granted
independent billing authority under
Medicare [66]. Obtaining these billing
privileges
for
geriatric
neuropsychologists has been both a
blessing and a curse. The blessing is
the oppor- tunity to provide services to
a population that is not on- ly growing,

but in great need of psychological


services. The curse continues to be the
restrictions Medicare and other
managed care systems place on which
services can be reimbursed, and the
rate at which reimbursement can be
made.
Psychologists in private
practice have been among the hardest
hit.
A 1993 Pennsylvania study [13] of
174 psychologists in private practice
found respondents reported prob- lems
with
insurance
reimbursement,

decreased fees, in- creased paperwork,


and difficulty obtaining authoriza- tion
for longer treatment. Despite the fact
that psychol- ogists had been granted
independent billing authority under
Medicare three years earlier, twothirds of the re- spondents reported
being excluded from Medicare eligibility to participate as clinical
psychologists. Per- haps because
psychologists in private practice infrequently initiate research, the issue of
how Medicare and managed care
reimbursements impact the ethical
provision
of
neuropsychological
services is often not addressed in the
literature. For example, two of the
most recently published books on the
topic of geriatric neuropsychology,
Geriatric Neuropsychology Assessment and Intervention [9] and
Geriatric Neuropsychol- ogy: Practice
Essentials [16] do not discuss how to
ad- equately address referral questions
and provide com- petent and ethical
services from a neuropsychological
perspective. This is especially
problematic since Medi-

care reimbursement schedules are


substantially lower than mean
neuropsychological billing rates.
The need for neuropsychological
services in the old- er adult population
is increasing, as are the costs of doing
business. Against this backdrop,
managed care reimbursements rates
are, at best, holding steady and in
some cases declining. As reported by
the APA Practice Organization the
Centers for Medicare and Medicaid
Services (CMS) proposed a rate
increase for Medicare physician
evaluation
and
management
reimbursement rates leading to an
additional $4.5 billion in Medicare
costs [8]. To offset these costs, CMS
cut the reim- bursement rates for
services provided predominantly by
psychologists and social workers.
Specifically, men- tal health and
testing services were cut 9% in
addition to a 5.1% reduction in the
rate at which fees increase over time
[8]. APA, in response, has encouraged
psy- chologists to be persistent with
trying to negotiate with managed care
companies, and offers a Managed
Care Reimbursement Toolkit to assist
in their negotiations. Interestingly,
APA suggests that if psychologists are
unable to negotiate an acceptable rate
then they are encouraged to look to
other sources of income.
Not surprisingly, despite the
increased reimburse- ment rates by
CMS for physicians, psychologists
and physicians who work with
Medicare patients are facing the same
issues. Based on a report prepared by
the American Medical Association [5],

from 2006 to 2011, physician


Medicare payments are expected to
drop by
26%. However, while Medicare
payments will be de- creasing the
costs associated with practicing
medicine are expected to increase by
15%. The question for any health care
provider inevitably becomes: How
can I ethically provide appropriate
services to Medicare pa- tients when I
cannot sustain my practice on the low
Medicare reimbursement rates?

In the presence of an expanding role


for neuropsy- chology in geriatric
psychiatry [58] reimbursement for
services
is
shrinking.
Neuropsychology, as a profes- sional
practice, places a strong emphasis on
psychometric
evaluation
of
neurocognitive functions, as well as
interventions based on information
provided
by
psychometric
evaluations
[58].
In
contrast,
neuropsychol- ogists in private
practice working in skilled nursing
facilities anecdotally report resorting
to clinical inter- views of the patient,
family and staff, as an alterna- tive to
standardized
neuropsychological
testing due to reimbursement related
time constraints. While such methods
may provide sufficient information to
make treatment recommendations,
these clinical interviews are not
standardized,
normed,
or
psychometrically cor- related with
standardized neuropsychological tests
to establish their validity or reliability.
In essence, clini- cal interviews alone
are neither evidence based, nor the
standard of care for diagnosing
cognitive impairment in older adults.
What then is the impact, on the patient and on the profession of
Neuropsychology when we abandon
standardized assessment measures in
favor of more brief, but less thorough
services? If clinical interviews are
replacing standardized assessments,
re- search needs to be carried out to
establish the content and concordant
validity, as well as reliability, of this
approach to addressing referral

questions in the older adult population.


2. Future considerations
In our everyday clinical practice we
are often faced with patients who have
multiple medical illnesses. While our
training and experiences allow us to be
experts
in
brain-behavior
relationships, frequently we fall short
of
understanding
brain-body
relationships. In- deed, most studies
continue to focus on the neuropsychological effects of one medical
disease state at a time rather than comorbid disorders which are more common in older individuals. To improve
our diagnostic accuracy we must have
a better grasp of the influence that
multi-system disease has on brain
behavior rela- tionships. Similarly,
neuropsychologists must become more
medically savvy and be more aware of
how medi- cal decompensation
detrimentally influences cognition.
While we are not physicians we should
continue to ex- pand our understanding
of physical illness, polyphar- macy,
and pain, as well as actively
collaborate
with
our
physician
counterparts.

Cognitive examinations remain as


challenging as di- agnosing physical
illnesses. In our role as the examiner, we must be integrate our
knowledge about cog- nition, test
instruments, psychopathology, and the
con- sequence of physical illness on
cognition. Yet, our referral stream is
also dependant on a high degree of
interpersonal skill [53]. In the future,
neuropsycholo- gists should consider,
when appropriate, implementing
aspects of the therapeutic assessment
model into their practice.
By
collaborating with our patients and
ac- knowledging their difficulties,
regardless of our ability to help, we
can facilitate a process by which the
patient leaves feeling validated,
helped, and understood.
Clinical evaluations depend on the
quality of the nor- mative data;
however, our older adult norms
remain quite poor.
Instead,
neuropsychological
research
remains focused on localizing cognitive
components to specific regions of the
brain. While localization is academically interesting, it has limited
clinical application in the older adult
population. Indeed, we receive few if
any referrals for localization per se
[68]. However, we do receive
referrals to capture gradual declines,
mild cognitive deficits that are not
captured
in
neurological
examinations, and delineating both
strengths and weak- nesses for
purposes of rehabilitation. To more
effec- tively answer these referral
questions, we must contin- ue to
improve our normative data for older

individuals.
Ideally,
our
neuropsychological tests should have
more representative sample sized for
older individuals that are stratified by
age and education, as well as include
those
individuals
who
have
diminished cognition re- lated to
normal aging. That said, test
companies have begun to recognize
the lack of appropriate norms for
older adults and have made greater
efforts to design tests with older adults
in mind and include older adults in
their normative samples (e.g. WAISIV, WMS-IV, Independent Living
Scales, and expanded norms for the
RBANS). Our neuropsychological
tests should al- so be based on our
theoretical understanding of cognitive functions. Finally, more effort
is called for to establish evidencebased links between tests and daily
functioning (i.e. ecological validity).
As we look to the future, we must
begin to devel- op our role in
promoting cognitive health, increasing
the applicability of our research, and
appropriate reim- bursement for
services. Despite the expanding role
of neuropsychology in older adult
populations, it is con- cerning that
reimbursement for services are
shrinking. Rather than solely focusing
on neuropsychopatholo- gy, we should
work toward developing evidencebased treatment approaches that
incorporate case manage-

ment, caregiver support and education,


and as appropri- ate environmental
engineering. Accordingly, our profession should continue to advocate
for the allocation of funds to research
innovative, individualized treat- ment
approaches and provide services for
patients who are cognitively impaired
and
their
caregivers.
Private
practitioners valuable knowledge and
expertise with older individuals is
regrettably being lost since their time
is often filled with the demands of
clinical work. However, to increase
the applicability of our geriatric
research, private practitioners should
strive to increase their involvement in
research.
Referen
ces
[1]

D. Aizenberg, M. Sigler, A.
Weizman and Y. Barak, Anticholinergic burden and the risk of
falls among elderly psy- chiatric
inpatients: A 4-year case-control
study,
International
Psychogeriatrics 14 (2002), 307
310.
[2] A. Akomolafe, A. Quarshie, P.
Jackson, J. Thomas, O. Def- fer, A.
Oduwole et al., The prevalence of
cognitive impair- ment among
African American patients with
congestive heart failure, J Natl
Med Assoc 97 (2005), 689694.
[3]
American Academy of
Neurology,
Assessment:
Neuropsy- chological testing of
adults,
Considerations
for
neurologists, Neurology 47 (1996),

592599.
[4] American Geriatric Society,
The management of persis- tent
pain in older persons, Retrieved
October 31, 2007.
http//www.americangeriatrics.org/
products/positionpapers/ perisitent
pain guide.shtml.
[5] American Medical Association,
Medicare participation options for physicians, Medicare
Physician Payment Action Kit,
2006, Retrieved November 4,
2007, from www.ama.org.
[6] American Medical Association
Council on Scientific Affairs,
Featured CSA Report: Improving
the
Quality
of
Geriatric
Pharmacotherap, 2004, Retrieved
October
31,
2007
from
http://www.amaassn.org/ama/pub/
category/13592.html.
[7]
American Psychological
Association, Guidelines for the
eval- uation of dementia and agerelated cognitive decline, Amer
Psychologist 53 (1998), 1298
1303.
[8]
American Psychological
Association Practice Organization,
CMS payment rule gravely
impacts Medicare mental health
services, 2006, Washington, DC:
Government Relations Of- fice,
Retrieved November 4, 2007 from
http://geropsych.org/
medicare.html.
[9] D. Attix and K. Welsh-Bohmer,
Geriatric
Neuropsychology
Assessment and Intervention, New
York: Guilford Press,
2006.
[10]
J. Bazarian, J. McClung, M.

Shah, Y. Cheng, W. Flesher and


J. Kraus. Mild traumatic brain
injury in the United States,
19982000, Brain Injury 19,
(2005), 8591.
[11] I. Bergh, G. Steen, M. Waern,
B. Johansson, A. Oden, B.
Sjo stro m and B. Steen, Pain and
its relation to cognitive func- tion
and depressive symptoms: A
Swedish population study of
70-year-old men and women, J
Pain Sympt Manage 26 (2003),
903912.
[12] G. Blessed, B. Tomlinson and
M. Roth, The association between quantitative measures of
dementia and of senile change in
the cerebral grey matter of elderly
subjects, Brit J Psych
114 (1968), 797811.

[13]
T. Bowers and S. Knapp,
Reimbursement
issues
for
psycholo- gists in independent
practice, Psychothera Private
Pract 12(3), (1993), 7387.
[14]
H. Brodaty, A. Withall, A.
Altendorf and P. Sachdev, Rates of
depression at 3 and 15 months
poststroke and their relationship
with cognitive decline: The
Sydney stroke study, Amer J Geri
Psychtry 15 (2007), 477486.
[15] R. Bush, G. Chelune and Y.
Suchy, Using norms in neuropsychological assessment of the
elderly, in:
Geriatric Neuropsychology: Assessment and
Intervention, D. Attix and K.
Welsh-Bohmer, eds, New York:
Guilford Publications, 2006, pp.
133157.
[16] S. Bush and T. Martin, Geriatric
Neuropsychology: Practice
Essentials, New York:
Taylor & Francis, 2005.
[17] B. Caplan and J. Shechter, Test
accommodation
in
geriatric
neuropsychology, in:
Geriatric
Neuropsychology:
Practice
Essentials, S. Bush, and T. Martin,
eds, New York: Taylor & Francis,
2005, pp. 97114.
[18]
J. Cohen-Mansfield and S.
Lipson, Pain in cognitively impaired nursing home residents:
how well are physicians diagnosing it? J Amer Geri Soc 50
(2002), 10391044.
[19]
N. Col, J. Fanale and P.
Kronholm, The role of medication
noncompliance and adverse drug
reactions in hospitalizations of the
elderly, Arch Intern Med 150
(1990), 841845.

[20]
L. Davison, Current status of
clinical neuropsychology, Clinical Neuropsychology: Current
Status and Applications, Ox- ford,
England: V. H. Winston & Sons,
1974.
[21]
Department of Health and
Human Services, Mental Health: A
report of the Surgeon GeneralExecutive Summary, Rockville,
MD: US Department of Health and
Human Services, Sub- stance
Abuse and Mental Health Services
Administration, Center for Mental
Health Services, National Institutes
of Health, National Institutes of
Mental Health, 1999.
[22] K. Duff, J. Mold, M. Roberts
and S. McKay, Medical burden and
cognition in older patients in
primary care: Selective deficits in
attention, Arch Clin Neuropsych
22 (2007), 569
57
5.
[23]
M. Duffy, Psychological
services in long term care resource
guide, American Psychological
Association Public Interest Office
on Aging, 2006, Retrieved
November
4,
2007,
from
http://www.apa.org/pi/aging/long
term care resource guide
2006.
pdf.
[24] C. Eccleston, Chronic pain and
distraction:
An
experimental
investigation into the role of
sustained and shifting attention in
the processing of chronic persistent
pain, Behav Res Thera py 33
(1995), 391405.
[25] D. Elmore, Reaching the
underserved, Monit on Psych 34(8)

(2003)
, 32.
[26]
E. Erikson, The Life Cycle
Completed: A Review, New York:
Norton & Company, 1982.
[27] D. Feil, J. Razani, K. Boone and
I. Lesser, Apathy and cog- nitive
performance in older adults with
depression, Internat J Geri
Psychtry 18 (2003), 479485.
[28] S. Finn, Assessment feedback
integrating MMPI-2 and
Rorschach findings, J Personality
Assess 67 (1996), 543557. [29] S.
Finn, Therapeutic assessment of a
man with ADD, J
Personality Assess 80
(2003), 115129.
[30]
S. Finn and M.Tonsager,
Therapeutic effects of providing
MMPI-2 feedback to college
students awaiting therapy, Psych
Assess 4 (1992), 278287.
[31]
J. Flaherty, H. Perry, G.
Lynchard
and
L.
Morley,
Polyphar- macy and hospitalization
among older home care patients, J
Geront: Med Sci 55A (2000),
M554M559.

[32] A. Flint, Anxiety disorders in


later life: From epidemiology to
treatment, Amer J Geri Psychtry 15
(2007), 635638.
[33] M. Folstein, S. Folstein and P.
McHugh, Mini-mental state, A
practical method for grading the
cognitive state of patients for the
clinician, J Psychtr Res 12 (1975),
189198.
[34] B. Ford, K. McKeever Bullard,
R. Taylor, A. Toler, H. Neigh- bors
and J. Jackson, Lifetime and 12month prevalence of Di- agnostic
and Statistical Manual of Mental
Disorders, Fourth Edition disorders
among older African Americans:
Findings from the National survey
of American life, Amer J Geri Psychtry 15 (2007), 652659.
[35] E. Gonzalez, M. Mustelier and
J. Rey, Geriatric psychopharmacology,
in:
Geriatric
Neuropsychology:
Practice
Essen- tials, S. Bush and T.
Martin, eds, New York: Taylor &
Francis,
2005, pp.
401428.
[36] E. Hajjar, J. Hanlon, R. Sloane,
C. Lindblad, C. Pieper, C.
Ruby et al., Unnecessary drug use
in frail older people at hospital
discharge, J Amer Geri Soc 53
(2005), 15181523.
[37] L. Hassing, M. Grant, S. Hofer,
N. Pedersen, S. Nilsson, S.
Berg et al., Type 2 diabetes
mellitus contributes to cognitive
decline in old age: A longitudinal
population-based study, J Internat
Neuropsych Soc 10 (2004), 599

607.
[38] E. Heyer, R. Sharma, C.
Winfree, J. Mocco, D. McMahon, P.
McCormick et al., Severe pain
confounds neuropsychological test
performance, J Clini Experi
Neuropsych 22 (2000), 633
63
9.
[39] M. Hibbard, S. Breed, T.
Ashman and J. Williams, Cooccurring
psychiatric
and
neurological impairments in older
adults,
in:
Geriatric
Neuropsychology:
Practice
Essentials, S. Bush and T. Martin,
eds, New York:
Taylor &
Francis,
2005, pp.
327362.
[40] F. Hill-Briggs, J. Kirk and S.
Wegener, Geriatric pain and
neuropsychological assessment, in:
Geriatric Neuropsychol- ogy:
Practice Essentials, New York:
Taylor & Francis, 2005, pp. 385
400.
[41]
R. Hopkins, S. Gale and L.
Weaver, Brain atrophy and cognitive impairment in survivors of
acute
respiratory
distress
syndrome, Brain Injury 20 (2006),
263271.
[42] K. Hunter and D. Cyr,
Pharmacotherapeutics in older adults,
J Wound Ostomy Continence
Nurs 33 (2006), 630638.
[43] A. Jefferson, A. Poppas, R. Paul
and R. Cohen, Systemic hypoperfusion is associated with
executive dysfunction in geri- atric
cardiac patients, Neurobio Aging
28 (2007), 477483.

[44] P. Jurica, C. Leitten and S.


Mattis, Dementia Rating Scale2: Professional Manual, Lutz:
Psychological Assessment
Resource
s, 2001.
[45] D. Kaufman, J. Kelly, L.
Ronsenberg, T. Anderson and A.
Mitchell, Recent pattern of
medication use in the ambularty
adult population of the United
States, J Amer Med Asso 185
(2002), 914919.
[46]
R. Kiernan, J. Mueller, J.
Langston and C. Van Dyke, The
Neurobehavioral Cognitive Status
Examination:
A brief but
quantitative approach to cognitive
assessment,
Annual
Intern
Medicine 107 (1987), 481500.
[47] S. Kivela, K. Pahkala and P.
Laippala, Prevalance of depression in the elderly Finnish
population, Acta Psychiatrica
Scan- danavica 78 (1988), 401
413.
[48] M. Kurella, G. Chertow, J. Luan
and
K.
Yaffe,
Cognitive
impairment in chronic kidney
disease, J Amer Geri Soc 52
(2004), 18631869.
[49] A. Lake, B. Branca, T. Lutz and
J. Saper, Headache level during
neuropsychological testing and test
performance in pa- tients with
chronic posttraumatic headache, J
Head Trauma Rehab 14 (1999),
7080.

[50] P. Lichtenberg, A Guide to


Psychological Practice in Geriatric
Long-term Care, New York:
Haworth Press, Inc, 1994.
[51] R. Mantella, M. Butters and M.
Dew, Cognitive impairment in late
life Generalized Anxiety Disorder,
Amer J Geri Psychtry
15 (2007),
673679.
[52] B. Meyers, Geriatric delusional
depression, Clinical Geriatric
Medicine
8
(1992),
299
308.
[53]
D. Michel, Psychological
assessment as a therapeutic intervention in patients hospitalized
with eating disorders, Profess
Psych: Res Pract 33 (2002), 470
477.
[54]
M. Mitrushina, K. Boone, J.
Razani and L. DElia, Handbook
of
Normative
Data
for
Neuropsychological Assessment,
2nd ed., New York: Oxford
University Press, 2005.
[55] J. Morris, A. Heyman, R. Mohs
and J. Hughes, The consortium to
establish a registry for Alzheimers
disease (CERAD): I. Clinical and
neuropsychological assessment of
Alzheimers disease, Neurology 39
(1989), 11591165.
[56] Z. Nasreddine, N. Phillips, V.
Bedirian, S. Charbonneau, V.
Whitehead, I. Collin, J. Cummings
and H. Chertkow, The Montreal
Cognitive Assessment, MoCA: A
brief screening tool for mild
cognitive impairment, J Amer Geri
Soc 53 (2005),
695

699.
[57] M. Newman and P. Greenway,
Therapeutic effects of providing
MMPI-2 test feedback to clients at
a university counseling service: A
collaborative approach, Psych
Asses 9 (1997), 122
13
1.
[58] B. Palmer, The expanding role
of neuropsychology in geriatric
psychiatry, Amer J Geri Psychtry
12 (2004), 338341.
[59] L. Patrick, C. Leclerc and M.
Perugini, Is rehabilitation neuropsychology an evidence-based
practice? Topics Geri Rehab
10 (2003),
160168.
[60] B. Plassman, K. Langa, G.
Fisher, S. Heeringa, D. Weir, M.
Ofstedal, J. Burke, M. Hurd, G.
Potter, W. Rodgers, D. Stef- fens,
R. Willis and R. Wallace,
Prevalence of dementia in the
United
States:
The
aging,
demographics and memory study,
Neuroepidemiology 29 (2007),
125132.
[61] W. Qiu, L. Price, P. Hibberd, J.
Buell et al. Executive dysfunction in homebound older
people with diabetes mellitus, J
Ameri Geri Soc 54 (2006), 496
501.
[62] I. Rainero, S. Vighetti, B.
Bergamasco, L. Pinessi and F.
Benedetti, Autonomic responses
and pain perception in Alzheimers
disease, Euro J Pain 4 (2000),
267274.
[63] C. Randolf, Repeatable Battery
for the Assessment of Neuro-

logical Status, San Antonio, TX:


The Psychological Corpora- tion,
1998.
[64]
R. Robinson, Poststroke
depression: Prevalence, diagnosis,
treatment and disease progression,
Biol Psychtry 54 (2003),
376
387.
[65] W. Rosen, R. Mohs and K.
Davis, A new rating scale for
Alzheimers disease, Amer J
Psychtry 141 (1984), 13561364. [66]
M. Rosenbach. Trends in Medicare
Part B mental health uti- lization and
expenditures: Health Care Financing
Review, Washington, DC: U.S.
Department of Health and Human Services, 1996,
19871992.
[67] R. Ruchinskas, N. Repetz and H.
Singer, The use of the neurobehavioral
cognitive
status
examination with geriatric rehabilitation patients, Rehab Psychol
46 (2001), 219228.
[68] R. Ruff, A friendly critique of
neuropsychology:
facing the
challenges of our future, Arch
Clinical Neuropsych 18 (2003),
847
864.
[69]
J. Shega, L. Emanuel, L.
Vargish, S. Levine et al., Pain in
persons with dementia: Complex,
common and challenging, The
Journal of Pain: J Amer Pain Soc
8 (2007), 373800.

[70]
J. Silver, R. Kramer, S.
Greenwood and M. Weissman, The
association between head injuries
and psychiatric disorders: Findings
from the New Haven NIMH
epidemiologic catch- ment area
study, Brain Injury 15 (2001),
935945.
[71]
P. Solomon, M. Brush, V.
Calvo, F. Adams, R. DeVeaux,
W. Pendlebury and D. Sullivan,
Identifying dementia in the
primary care practice, Interna
Psychogeriatrics 12 (2000),
483493.
[72] R. Stewart, M. Moore, F. May,
R. Marks and W. Hale, Chang- ing
patterns of therapeutic agents in
the elderly: A ten-year overview,
Age and Ageing 20 (1991), 182
188.
[73] J. Sweet, N. Nelson and P.
Moberg, The TCN/AACN 2005
salary survey: Professional
practices, beliefs, and incomes of
U.S. neuropsychologists. Clini
Neuropsychologist 20 (2006),
325364.
[74]
S. Tariq, N. Tumosa, J.
Chibnall, M. Perry and J. Morley,
Comparison of the Saint Louis
University mental status examination and the mini-mental
state examination for detect- ing
dementia and mild neurocognitive
disorder a pilot study, Amer J
Geri Psychtry 14 (2006), 900910.

[75]
A. Trivedi, A. Zaslavsky, E.
Schneider and J. Ayanian, Trends
in the quality of care and racial
disparities in Medicare man- aged
care, New England J Med 353
(2005), 692700.
[76]
H. Tuokko and T.
Hadjistavropoulos, An Assessment
Guide
to
Geriatric
Neuropsychology, Mahwah, NJ,
US: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates
Publishers, 1998.
[77] U.S. Department of Health and
Human Services, Older adults and
mental health, in Mental Health: A
Report of the Surgeon GeneralExecutive Summary, Rockville,
MD: US Department of Health and
Human Services, Substance Abuse
and Men- tal Health Services
Administration, Center for Mental
Health Services, National Institutes
of Health, National Institute of
Mental Health, 1999.
[78] M. Van Eijken, S. Tsang, M.
Wensing, P. de Smet and R.
Grol, Interventions to improve
medication compliance in older
patients living in the community,
Drugs Ageing 20 (2003),
229240.

Você também pode gostar