Você está na página 1de 15

Behavioural Brain Research 146 (2003) 317

Review

Do rats have a prefrontal cortex?


Harry B.M. Uylings a,b, , Henk J. Groenewegen b , Bryan Kolb c
a

Netherlands Institute for Brain Research, KNAW, Graduate School Neurosciences, Meibergdreef 33, 1105 AZ Amsterdam, The Netherlands
b Department of Anatomy, Graduate School Neurosciences, VU University Medical Center, Amsterdam, The Netherlands
c Canadian Centre for Behavioural Neuroscience, University of Lethbridge, Lethbridge, AB, Canada T1K 3M4

Abstract
The lack of a single anatomical or functional definition of prefrontal cortex has led to different and, in some respects, controversial
views on the existence of a prefrontal cortex in non-primate mammals, in particular in rats. Until the classic paper by Rose and Woolsey
[Res. Publ. Assoc. Nerv. Ment. Dis. 27 (1948) 210], the general idea was that a prefrontal cortex is unique to primate species. Rose and
Woolseys prefrontal cortex definition was based upon a single anatomical criterion, i.e. the cortical projection area of the mediodorsal
thalamic nucleus. Single criteria, however, do not appear to be sufficient for defining the prefrontal cortex. Therefore, other anatomical
and functional characteristics are currently used to identify the prefrontal cortex in different species. Yet, recently the debate about the
nature of the prefrontal cortex in non-primate species has been resumed. In the present paper we will compare the structural and functional
characteristics of the prefrontal cortex of nonhuman primates and rats. We will argue that rats have a functionally divided prefrontal cortex
that includes not only features of the medial and orbital areas in primates, but also some features of the primate dorsolateral prefrontal cortex.
2003 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.
Keywords: Prefrontal cortex; Rat; Primates; Parallel circuits; Basal ganglia; Mesocortical dopaminergic system; Monoamines; Acetylcholine

1. Introduction
Abbreviations: ac, anterior commissure; ACd, dorsal anterior cingulate
area; ACv, ventral anterior cingulate area; AId, dorsal agranular insular
area; AIp, posterior agranular insular area; AIv, ventral agranular insular area; AM, anterior medial thalamic nucleus; AO, anterior olfactory
nucleus; BAC, basal amygdaloid complex; cc, corpus callosum; CPm,
caudateputamen complex, medial part; DStr, dorsal striatum; Ent, entorhinal area; FL, forelimb area, according to Zilles [157]; Fr1/3, frontal
cortical areas 1 and 3, according to Zilles [157]; Fr2, frontal cortical area
2, rostral to about 1 mm from bregma; GPe, globus pallidus, external
segment; GPi, globus pallidus, internal segment; Hip, hippocampus; HL,
hindlimb area, according to Zilles [157]; IL, infralimbic cortical area;
IMD, intermediodorsal thalamic nucleus; LO, lateral orbital cortical area;
MC, motor cortex; MDl, mediodorsal thalamic nucleus, lateral segment,
includes here MDpl; MDm, mediodorsal thalamic nucleus, medial segment; MDm(a), anterior part of MDm; MDm(p), posterior part of MDm;
MDpl, mediodorsal thalamic nucleus, paralamellar segment; MO, medial
orbital cortical area; OB, olfactory bulb; Oc1, primary occipital (visual)
cortex [157]; Oc2L, lateral part of occipital cortex, area 2 [157]; Oc2M,
medial part of occipital cortex area 2 [157]; Par1(dysgr), dysgranular part
of parietal cortex area 1 [157]; Par2, parietal cortex area 2 (supplementary somatosensory cortex) [157]; PC, posterior cingulate area; PC/CL,
paracentral and central lateral thalamic nuclei; PF, parafascicular thalamic nucleus; Pir, (pre)piriform cortex; PL, prelimbic cortical area; PRh,
perirhinal cortical area; PV, paraventricular thalamic nucleus; rs, rhinal
sulcus; RSA, agranular retrosplenial cortex; RSG, granular retrosplenial
cortex; SMA, supplementary motor area; SNc, substantia nigra pars compacta; SNr, substantia nigra pars reticulata; SNrdm, dorsomedial part of
SNr; STh, subthalamic nucleus; Te2, area 2 of temporal cortex [157]; TT,
0166-4328/$ see front matter 2003 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2003.09.028

The volume of the cerebral cortex of a rat is about a


hundred times smaller than that of the cerebral cortex of
macaques, and about a thousand times smaller than that
of humans [138]. This increase in cortical volume in primates is paralleled by an evolutionary differentiation of
cortical areas and by the development of more complex,
cognitive cerebral functions [114]. In this light it is not
surprising that discussions are ongoing about whether or
not particular cortical areas in the rat brain are comparable
with specific cortical areas in primates. Recently this issue
has been raised about the prefrontal cortex, in particular
whether or not rats possess a prefrontal region that is comparable with the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in primates
[111,114]. Such a question is complicated, since the rat
cortical fields are generally less evoluted, less differentiated and less segregated than those in the primate cerebral
cortex. The primate prefrontal cortex consists of various
taenia tecta; VA, ventral anterior thalamic nucleus; VL, ventral lateral
thalamic nucleus; VLO, ventrolateral orbital cortical area; VM, ventral
medial thalamic nucleus; VMm, medial part of VM; VO, ventral orbital
cortical area; VP, ventral pallidum; VStr, ventral striatum
Corresponding author. Tel.: +31-20-5665500; fax: +31-20-6961006.
E-mail address: h.uylings@nih.knaw.nl (H.B.M. Uylings).

H.B.M. Uylings et al. / Behavioural Brain Research 146 (2003) 317

anatomically different subfields [10,23,138], roughly divided in a dorsolateral, a medial and an orbital region [44].
These different subdivisions of the primate prefrontal cortex are thought to be involved in different cognitive and
emotion functions [8,12,31,44,99]. It is generally accepted
that the prefrontal cortex is involved in different aspects
of executive control and that the neuronal basis for these
functions is formed by the extensive neuronal networks in
which the prefrontal cortex is intricately involved. Although
Preuss [111] did not question the existence of a rat prefrontal cortex in general, this was the way it was perceived
by many primate researchers. However, Preuss [111] and
Preuss and Kaas [114] questioned explicitly the existence of
an equivalent of the primate dorsolateral prefrontal cortex
in non-primate species. To answer the question whether rats
have a dorsolateral-like prefrontal cortex we must consider
various anatomical and functional criteria that define the
different prefrontal regions. On the basis of the structural
and functional data reviewed below, we conclude that rats
have a prefrontal cortex, part of which (in particular the dorsomedial shoulder region) displays features that resemble
characteristics of the primate dorsolateral prefrontal cortex.

2. Criteria for the definition of a prefrontal cortex


For a long time after Brodmanns studies [17] the prefrontal cortex was considered unique to the primate species
and called the frontal granular cortex [14]. The definition
of prefrontal cortex at that time was based upon the cytoarchitectonic criterion of having a granular layer IV and
a location rostral to the agranular (pre)motor areas. However, comparing different cortical areas in more distantly related species solely on the basis of cytoarchitectonic criteria
appeared to be untenable. For example, the primary motor
cortex in rats is considered to be homologous to the one in
monkeys [105], but this cortical area is agranular in mature
primates and granular in rats. Likewise, Barbas and Pandya
[10] consider limbic cortices in primate brains, which are
agranular and dysgranular (i.e. layer IV is not and is not easily discernible, respectively) as part of the prefrontal cortex.
These are just two examples to emphasize why cytoarchitectonic criteria have been replaced by other criteria in seeking
homologies between different brain areas in more distantly
related species. It is now generally accepted that the following criteria have to be taken into account when discussing
homologies between cortical areas in different species: (1)
the pattern of specific connections and the relative density
of these connections; (2) the functional (i.e. electrophysiological and behavioral) properties; (3) the presence and
specific distribution of different neuroactive substances and
neurotransmitter receptors; (4) the embryological development; and (5) only for closely related species, the cytoarchitectonic characteristics. The greater the similarities between
the characteristics, the more likely it is that brain regions
are homologous. In the following account we will employ

the first three criteria for comparing prefrontal cortical areas


in primates and rats. These three criteria also were applied
by Preuss [111]. From this perspective we will consider the
connections of the rat and primate prefrontal cortices with
thalamic, basal ganglia, cortical, limbic and monoaminergic
structures, with respect to both pattern and density. Subsequently, we will review comparatively the functional properties of prefrontal areas in rats and primates.

3. Neuronal networks involving the prefrontal cortex


3.1. Connections between prefrontal cortex and thalamus
Thalamocortical connections are important for cortical
differentiation and specialization (e.g. [90]). The reciprocal
connections of the major thalamic nuclei are therefore used
to define cerebral cortical areas. At the time of Rose and
Woolsey [126], the mediodorsal nucleus of the thalamus
was assumed to be the only nucleus with thalamocortical projections to the prefrontal cortex, and was therefore
viewed as the defining nucleus. However, with the advent of more refined anterograde and retrograde tracing
techniques, it became apparent that the (prefrontal) cortical areas that receive mediodorsal thalamic input also
are connected with other thalamic nuclei. Thalamic nuclei
other than the mediodorsal nucleus that reach the prefrontal
cortex include the intralaminar and midline nuclei, the
anterior medial nucleus and the rostral parts of the ventral complex [11,12,15,35,50,55,56,67,72,133]. In addition,
thalamic mediodorsal nucleus projections appear to reach
some cortical areas outside the prefrontal cortex, such as
the premotor, motor, temporal and parietal cortices, as has
been demonstrated in, for example, macaque monkeys, cats,
sheep, and dogs [1,37,50,56,72,100,136]. Among others,
Nauta [103] and Leonard [88] regarded the reciprocity of
the cortical projections of the thalamic mediodorsal nucleus as an important criterion for defining the prefrontal
cortex. However, this definition also does not lead to an unambiguous delineation of the prefrontal cortex. Therefore,
Uylings and Van Eden [138] suggested inclusion of only
those cortical areas in the prefrontal cortex for which the
reciprocal connections with the mediodorsal nucleus (MD)
are stronger (i.e. in terms of a higher number of projecting
neurons and a higher density of terminals) than the reciprocal connections with other thalamic nuclei. This feature,
together with the pattern of cortico-cortical connections, has
led us to include the primate and rat anterior cingulate cortex in the prefrontal cortex [138]. This approach in defining
the prefrontal cortex is strengthened by the recent analysis
of Ktter, who demonstrated the special, predominant position of the mediodorsal nucleus for the macaque prefrontal
cortex on basis of multidimensional scaling analysis of
the thalamoprefrontal cortical projections (Ktter, personal
communication, 2003). In addition, Barbas et al. [11], Dermon and Barbas [35] and Ray and Price [122] showed in

H.B.M. Uylings et al. / Behavioural Brain Research 146 (2003) 317

Fig. 1. The extent of rats prefrontal cortex. (A, B). The increase in what is considered to be prefrontal cortex due to improvements in anatomical
techniques. Fine dots indicate the area described by Leonard [88], large dots the extension described by Krettek and Price [87], oblique lines the extension
proposed by Groenewegen [54] and vertical lines the extension proposed by Verwer in [138]. The nomenclature of Krettek and Price has been followed
with the exception of the neutral term frontal area 2 (Fr2), see Section 6 in text. MO and VO are the medial and ventral orbital areas and AI the rat
anterior insular area. (C) The view illustrated by Preuss and Kaas [114], modified from Preuss [111], in which ACd is now incorporated in the anterior
cingulate cortex. In the view of Preuss and Kaas shown in (C) the anterior cingulate (AC), the prelimbic (PL) and the infralimbic (IL) areas of (A) form
the prefrontal cortex, while the orbital and lateral prefrontal cortex are conspicuously lacking. For abbreviations see the Abbreviations section.

rhesus monkey that the majority of the thalamic neurons


projecting to the prefrontal cortex, as it has been defined before, are located in the mediodorsal nucleus. This also goes
for the rat prefrontal cortex (see Fig. 1) [54,87,123,140].
Dermon and Barbas [35] reported that area 25 in
macaques (which is considered to be a prefrontal cortical area, e.g. [10,106]) receives more thalamic afferents

from the thalamic ventral anterior nucleus than from


the mediodorsal nucleus. When this is also true for the
efferent corticothalamic projections from area 25, then
macaque area 25 should probably not be included in the
prefrontal cortex [12]. A similar situation holds likely
for the ventrolateral orbital cortical area (VLO) in the rat
[54,123,155].

H.B.M. Uylings et al. / Behavioural Brain Research 146 (2003) 317

For a further comparison of monkey and rat thalamic data


it is important to know that in non-human primates thalamic
ventral nuclei have a differential cortical projection pattern
for the prefrontal, premotor and motor cortex, respectively
[66]. The thalamic ventral anterior (VA) and posterior ventromedial (VMp) nuclei project quite diffusely and spread
out over an extensive region of neocortex (i.e. premotor, supplementary motor area (SMA), supplementary eye field, anterior cingulate (AC) and posterior parietal cortex), but these
projections have a relatively higher concentration in the prefrontal cortex. In contrast, the anterior ventrolateral (VLa)
cortical projections are more concise and concentrated in
premotor and SMA areas, with additional lesser projections
also to the motor area. In addition, the posterior ventrolateral nucleus (VLp) is the principal source of projections to
primary motor area 4, but projects also to premotor, SMA,
and some to posterior parietal cortex [66]. In rats the topography and the pattern of connectivity of the mediodorsal nucleus is rather comparable with the one in monkeys [54,56].
The VA in rats, however, is usually included in the VL due
to its difficult cytoarchitectonic delineation [56,65,138].
In the last decade the inclusion of the rat frontal area
2 (Fr2) and the dorsal anterior cingulate area (ACd) (see
Fig. 1) in the prefrontal cortex has been disputed by Preuss
[111] and Cond et al. [28,29]. The results of Cond et al.s
retrograde tracer studies on thalamic afferents [28] and
cortico-cortical afferents [29] to rat medial frontal cortex
were decisive for their and Preuss thesis, namely that the
rat Fr2 (also called the precentral medial area (PrCm) or
agranular medial cortex (Agm), see below) and the rat ACd
are premotor areas and do not belong to the prefrontal
cortex. As a consequence they denied the presence of
dorsolateral-like prefrontal cortical features in rats. Cond
et al. [28] described in their retrograde tracer study that
only a few neurons from the mediodorsal thalamic nucleus
project to Fr2 and ACd and that a higher number of thalamic
projecting neurons are positioned in the intralaminar, the
ventrolateral (VL) and ventromedial (VM) nuclei. In this
respect, their Fig. 12 [28] is conspicuous for illustrating a
case with a relatively high number of MD neurons projecting to ACd. Furthermore, several groups [54,87,121] have
observed in extensive anterograde and retrograde tracing
studies that the rat Fr2 and ACd (Fig. 1) have a very dense
reciprocal connection with the paralamellar or ventrolateral
segment of the thalamic mediodorsal nucleus. This can also
be concluded from the anterograde and retrograde tracer
studies of Reep and Corwin [124], which were directed
especially to the Fr2 (their AGm). Reep and Corwin distinguished caudal AGm from mid and rostral AGm, and
showed that rostral and mid AGm receives thalamic afferents from a higher number of neurons in the mediodorsal
nucleus. It appeared that only the caudal AGm has afferents
from cells mainly located in VL and only a few neurons in
the mediodorsal nucleus. However, this part of AGm or Fr2
[156] is not included in the rat prefrontal cortex as defined
by Krettek and Price [87], Groenewegen [54], Uylings and

Van Eden [138], and Van Eden et al. [142], because it is


caudal to about 1 mm from bregma (see also note below).
In addition, Vertes [143] showed that both ACd and Fr2
have strong projections particularly to the MD. Therefore,
we conclude that ACd and Fr2 have a more prefrontal than
premotor type of thalamic connections. On basis of both
thalamic and subcortical and cortical connections we suppose also that the caudal Fr2 and (supragenual) parts of ACd
incorporates a zone homologous to the macaque frontal eye
field (FEF) [138,142]. This is corroborated by electrophysiological studies [59,104] and by unilateral lesion studies
causing multimodal neglect [73,95].
We realize the pitfalls of the quantitative definition of our
anatomical prefrontal cortex definition. Even with the use
of modern neuroanatomical tracing techniques, it is rather
difficult to determine unequivocally for those cortical regions showing considerable overlap of connections from
various thalamic nuclei, which of these thalamic nuclei provides the strongest connections. For example, comparison
of numbers [139] of different types of neurons projecting to
a particular cortical area with retrograde tracing implies the
assumption that the extent of these axonal terminal fields is
similar in the different regions that have been injected with
a particular tracer. This is not always the case, however. It
is therefore important to consider in addition the quality of
connections (e.g. driver and modulator type of thalamic
connections [56,132]), the cortico-cortical and subcortical
neuronal networks and functional properties in which different prefrontal cortical areas have a particular, different
position.
3.2. Prefrontal cortexbasal ganglia relationships
The frontal lobe as a whole has a special relationship
with the basal ganglia in that it is the part of the cerebral
cortex that receives the main input from the basal ganglia,
through a relay in the thalamus [97]. This basal ganglia
influence has long been known for the motor and premotor
cortices, but the frontal areas anterior to the (pre)motor
cortices (i.e. mostly the prefrontal cortical areas) are not
excepted from this particular subcortical influence. Like the
frontal cortex, the parietal, occipital and temporal cortices
all project to the basal ganglia, in particular the striatum, but
these posteriorly located cortices do not receive information
back from the basal ganglia, with the exception of area TE
in the inferotemporal cortex [96]. A particular subset of
thalamic nuclei (i.e. the mediodorsal, ventromedial, ventral
anterior and, to a lesser degree, the ventral lateral nuclei)
form the essential link between the pallidum and substantia
nigra pars reticulata, as the output structures of the basal
ganglia [2,58,94,98] and the (pre)frontal cortex. It is also
relevant to emphasize the high degree of topographical organization in the frontal corticalbasal ganglia connections
and in the basal gangliathalamic projections. This topographical organization forms the basis for the existence of
a number of parallel, largely functionally segregated basal

H.B.M. Uylings et al. / Behavioural Brain Research 146 (2003) 317

Fig. 2. Schematic diagrams to illustrate, for the rat brain, the involvement of different parts of the (pre)frontal cortex in various basal gangliathalamocortical
circuits and amygdaloid and midline/intralaminar thalamic circuits. Four basal gangliathalamocortical circuits are depicted with similar organizational
characteristics. Compare also the organization of similar circuits in primates [2]. All four circuits involve different parts of the (pre)frontal cortex, the dorsal
or ventral striatum, the pallidal and nigral complex and the ventral and medial thalamic nuclei. The present diagram emphasizes the closed character
of these circuits, but they may in various specific ways be interconnected, and the (pre)frontal cortical and basal ganglia way stations have extensive
projections outside the depicted circuits. (A) Motor circuit: note the projections from the parafascicular thalamic nucleus to the motor cortex and the
dorsal striatum, together with the absence of the amygdala connections. (B) Dorsal shoulder prefrontal circuit (Fr2 and ACd): the cortical and striatal way
stations of this circuit receive specific projections from the intralaminar thalamic complex (PC/CL) and the anterior part of the basal amygdaloid complex.
(C) Medial prefrontal/anterior cingulate circuit (PL/IL/MO/ACv): the paraventricular thalamic nucleus and the posterior part of the basal amygdaloid
complex are strongly involved in this particular circuit. (D) Lateral prefrontal/agranular insular circuit (AIv/AId): the intermediodorsal thalamic nucleus
and the anterior part of the basal amygdaloid complex project to the cortical and striatal way stations of this circuit. For a comparison with the circuits
described in primates, it is important to note that the topography of subcortical afferents and thalamocortical efferents of the thalamic mediodorsal nucleus
in rats and primates is largely similar [56]. The medial (and central) segments of the rat mediodorsal nucleus resemble the magnocellular part of the
primate MD. The lateral (and paralamellar) segments of the rat MD are comparable with the multiform and densocellular parts of the primate MD.

gangliathalamocortical circuits that have been identified


both in primates [2,3,58,98]. Alexander et al. [2,3] identified five circuits in primates: a motor, an oculomotor,
an anterior cingulate/medial orbitofrontal, a lateral
orbitofrontal, and a dorsolateral prefrontal circuit, which
are further subdivided by Middleton and Strick [98]. In rats,
similar circuits have been described, as illustrated in Fig. 2.
Further important aspects of the circuitry in which the
prefrontal cortex and the basal ganglia are involved concern
the specific relationships of the projections from the midline/intralaminar thalamic complex as well as the amygdala
and, to a lesser extent the hippocampus, to the cortical and
striatal relay stations in the basal gangliathalamocortical
circuits (Fig. 2) [9,55,57,58,93,144]. In both rats and primates, the circuits that involve the prefrontal cortical areas
are characterized by amygdaloid inputs. However, in rats

the dorsal anterior cingulate (ACd) and Fr2 areas receive the
least amygdaloid fibers of the prefrontal areas [57], while
also in nonhuman primates the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex
has the weakest amygdaloid input in the prefrontal cortex [5]. All these connectional aspects point to similarities
between prefrontal cortexbasal gangliathalamocortical
circuits of primates and rats. In particular, they show a number of comparable features for both the primate dorsolateral
prefrontal cortex and the rat ACd and Fr2 areas.
3.3. Cortico-cortical networks
Both the thalamocortical and the cortico-cortical connections with the rat and primate prefrontal cortex are
predominantly ipsilateral [1,54,108,142]. The majority of
terminal axons in the prefrontal cortex are cortical afferents

H.B.M. Uylings et al. / Behavioural Brain Research 146 (2003) 317

[108,142]. In both rats and primates the prefrontal cortex is


extensively connected with different cortical areas such as
premotor, somatosensory, auditory, visual, olfactory, gustatory and limbic cortical areas [7,24,26,142]. Pandya and
Yeterian [108] showed that in macaques the connections
between the prefrontal and other cortical areas are reciprocal and that such connections exist preferentially between
cortical areas with a similar level of cytoarchitectonic differentiation. However, connections between distinct prefrontal
cortical areas exist both between areas with similar levels
of cytoarchitectonic differentiation as well as between areas
with different levels of differentiation. In addition, Barbas
et al. [12] showed that the cytoarchitectonically less differentiated prefrontal regions have fewer specific cortico-cortical
connections. They receive projections from two or more
cortical areas representing different sensory modalities together with a substantial input from limbic cortices. The
cytoarchitectonically more highly differentiated (thus eulaminate or strongly granular) prefrontal areas receive more
specific projections representing only one or two different
modalities, and relatively few projections from limbic cortices. In primates, in particular the macaque, the dorsolateral
prefrontal cortex has extensive cortico-cortical connections.
The rat data currently available do not contradict this
hierarchical theory of Pandya and Barbas, but more information is required before definitive conclusions can be
drawn. As mentioned above the retrograde tracer study on
cortico-cortical afferents of Cond et al. [29] has strengthened the view of Cond et al. and Preuss [111] that the rat
Fr2 and ACd is largely a premotor cortex and do not belong
to the prefrontal cortex. By excluding this dorsal shoulder
region from the rat prefrontal cortex, they arrived to their
opinion, that the prefrontal cortex in rats lack features of the
primate dorsolateral prefrontal cortex [29,111]. Cond et al.
[29] concluded that Fr2 and ACd have the main features
of a premotor cortex and none of the features of any area
of the macaque prefrontal cortex. A typical feature of the
macaque prefrontal cortical areas is, as noted above, however, the property of a multimodal association area in a hierarchical organized cortex [108] and the feature of a nodal
station in several distributed parallel networks [26,52,113].
Like the macaque prefrontal cortex, the rat prefrontal cortex
receives multimodal cortico-cortical projections from motor,
somatosensory, visual, auditory, gustatory, and limbic cortices in such a way that the rat prefrontal cortex appears to be
a nodal station embedded in several parallel networks [142].
Moreover, Van Eden et al. [142] have shown (with anterograde labeling) that Fr2 and ACd receive more projections
from somatosensory and associational visual cortices than
from the primary motor cortex. The macaque prefrontal cortex has the strongest connections with posterior parietal and
temporal association areas [26,27]. Rat equivalents are not
well described for these parietal and temporal association
areas [142], which makes a good comparison not yet possible. The data available indicate extensive, reciprocal connections with premotor, motor, somatosensory, visual, auditory

and paralimbic cortical regions (see Fig. 3). The reciprocal


cortico-cortical connections reveal at least three subfields in
the rat medial prefrontal cortex, i.e. a dorsal shoulder region (Fr2 and ACd); a rostral part of the medial prefrontal
cortex; and the prelimbic and infralimbic cortices.
The architectonically less differentiated areas in the rat,
the infralimbic and prelimbic cortices and the lateral prefrontal cortex (i.e. agranular insular cortices), have reciprocal connections with the perirhinal and entorhinal cortex,
and with the CA1 field and subiculum of the hippocampal
formation [57,62,145]. The rostral part of the rat medial prefrontal cortex has reciprocal connections with motor, mixed
somatosensory-motor, and somatosensory association cortices. Thus, premotor characteristics appear to coincide here
with prefrontal characteristics [138]. The dorsal shoulder
region has reciprocal connections mainly with visual cortices and the retrosplenial cortex. As mentioned above the
caudal part of the dorsal shoulder region appears to incorporate features of the frontal eye field.
It is of interest to note that Fuster [44,45] views the interactions of prefrontal with other cortices in the context
of perceptionaction cycle. This is a hierarchical concept
too. The prefrontal cortex and other cortices are functionally
connected for as long as the behavior contains novelty, uncertainty or ambiguity, and has to span time intervals with
short-term or working memory. These functional connections disappear or weaken when the action becomes automatic. The action is then integrated in lower brain structures
[45].
3.4. The prefrontal gating position in cholinergic and
monoaminergic systems
Data on the cholinergic and monoaminergic transmitter
systems together with prefrontal cortico-cortical connections
in primates and rats also show the unique gating position of
the prefrontal cortex.
In both rats and primates the entire neocortex receives
cholinergic innervation from the basal forebrain nuclei. The
prefrontal cortex, in addition, gets cholinergic fibers from
the laterodorsal tegmental nucleus. Likewise in both species,
the prefrontal cortex (in primates mainly the orbital and
medial PFC and in rats mainly the medial PFC) is the only
cortical region that has direct projections back to these basal
forebrain and brainstem nuclei [48,49,57,127,138,156].
The noradrenergic fibers from the locus coeruleus and the
serotonergic fibers from the dorsal and median raphe nuclei
are widely distributed over almost the entire neocortex. Also
with respect to these transmitter systems in both rats and
primates certain prefrontal areas are the only cortical areas
that project back to the locus coeruleus and to the dorsal and
median raphe nuclei. In primates the cortical projections to
the noradrenergic locus coeruleus and the serotonergic raphe
nuclei derive from the dorsolateral PFC [6]. In rats the cortical projections to the locus coeruleus derive from the medial
PFC and the agranular insular PFC areas, while the cortical

H.B.M. Uylings et al. / Behavioural Brain Research 146 (2003) 317

Fig. 3. A diagram of cortico-cortical connections of the rat prefrontal cortex (modified from [138]). For abbreviations see the Abbreviations section.

projections to the dorsal and median raphe nuclei are from


the medial PFC, especially its ventral part [60,63,64,138].
In rats, the cortical terminal fields of dopaminergic fibers
are mainly restricted to the prefrontal areas and the entorhinal cortex, whereas only the prefrontal cortex (both medial
and agranular insular PFC) projects back to the dopaminergic neurons in ventral tegmental area (VTA or A10) [25]
and the dopaminergic pars compacta of the substantia nigra (A9). The rat prefrontal cortex receives distinct parallel dopaminergic inputs. The supragenual anterior cingulate
cortex receives dopaminergic fibers in the superficial layers
from the pars compacta of the substantia nigra and in the
deeper layers from the lateral part of the ventral tegmental area [89,91]. The ventral tegmental area is the origin
of the major dopaminergic input in the rat prefrontal cortex and different parts of the ventral tegmental area appear
to project to different prefrontal areas [69]. Particularly in
primates, cortical dopaminergic fibers appear not to be restricted to the prefrontal cortex. As suggested [47,138] this
appears to be caused mainly by extension of dopaminergic
midbrain cellular groups such as the retrorubral areas A8
and A9 [152]. Direct recurrent projections from the primate
dorsolateral prefrontal cortex to the medial substantia nigra
pars compacta (A9) have been reported [89], but they can
also be expected to project to the ventral tegmental area on
the basis of the above.
In rats the histaminergic neurons in the tuberomammillary
hypothalamic region have reciprocal connections with ventromedial prefrontal cortex [154]. These connections have
not yet been described for primates.

4. Functional characteristics of prefrontal areas


The function of any brain region is to produce behavior.
Thus, a key issue in identifying similarities in cortical areas

across species must be function. One of the major obstacles in comparing the behavior of different species of mammals is that each species has a unique behavioral repertoire
that permits the animal to survive in its particular environmental niche. There is therefore the danger that neocortical
organization is uniquely patterned in different species in a
way that reflects the unique behavioral adaptation of different species. One way to address this problem is to recognize
that although the details of behavior may differ somewhat,
mammals share many behavioral traits and capacities (e.g.
[81]). For example, all mammals must detect and interpret
sensory stimuli, relate this information to past experience,
and act appropriately. Similarly, all mammals appear to be
capable of learning complex tasks under various schedules
of reinforcement (e.g. [146]). The details and complexity of
these behaviors clearly vary, but the general capacities are
common to all mammals. Warren and Kolb [146] proposed
that behaviors and behavioral capacities demonstrable in
all mammals could be designated as class-common behaviors. In contrast, behaviors that are unique to a species and
that have presumably been selected to promote survival in a
particular niche are designated as species-typical behaviors.
This distinction is important because it has implications for
the organization of the cerebral cortex.
Kaas [68] has argued, for example, that all mammalian
species have similar regions devoted to the analysis of basic sensory information (e.g. primary visual (V1), primary
auditory (A1), and primary somatosensory (S1) areas), the
control of movement (primary motor, M1), and a frontal region involved in the integration of sensory and motor information. We can extend Kaass idea by suggesting that these
regions have class-common functions. To be sure, there are
large species differences in the details of the class-common
behaviors. Monkeys (and humans) have chromatic vision
compared to the largely achromatic vision of cats or rats.
Nevertheless, in all mammalian species studied, removal of

10

H.B.M. Uylings et al. / Behavioural Brain Research 146 (2003) 317

visual cortex severely disrupts object recognition. Indeed,


although the visual cortex of the rat has often been portrayed
as primitive in organization, the visual acuity of rats is surprisingly good and the tuning characteristics of visual neurons is strikingly similar to that of larger-brained mammals.
Similarly, rats and cats have a large somatosensory representation of the whiskers, whereas monkeys and humans have
no such representation but in all species the somatosensory
cortex functions to represent skin-related receptors for tactile sensations. Thus, both the visual and tactile recognition
of objects are class-common functions, even though the details of this recognition may vary in a species-typical manner. A similar argument can be made for motor functions
[147]. Intracortical stimulation studies have shown that all
mammals have a motor map (e.g. [153]) in which the relative
motor facility of different body regions is reflected by the
size of the motor representation. Curiously, although there
are clear interspecies differences in the capacity to use the
forelimbs for object manipulation [61], it has become apparent from the work of Whishaw et al. [148,149] that the capacity for independent digit manipulation, and the cerebral
organization of this control, is strikingly similar between
rodents and primates. Indeed, recent taxonomic studies of
mammalian evolution have noted that rodents and primates
are more closely related than had been realized previously.
For example, they are more related than cats and primates
are [112,138]. This similarity is especially germane to comparisons of frontal lobe function between primates and rodents, given that one primary function of the frontal lobe is
to control the initiation and organization of movement.
As we look for general class-common functions of the
frontal cortex we might anticipate that if the frontal cortex
of mammals developed because all mammals face common
functional problems related to the organization of behavior, then we should be able to identify these functions. One
place to begin searching for class-common frontal functions
is to consider what animals use sensory inputs for. The most
obvious function is to guide behavior on line, such as in
the visuomotor control of movements in space or the identification of food items using visual, tactile, and olfactory
information. But the sensory world has far more information available than the brain can handle at one time so there
must be some system to select information as well as to
focus and maintain attention. Similarly, although behavior
can be directed to sensory stimuli on-line, it can also be
related to information that is stored or expected. Stored information may be in a type of scratch-pad memory system,
which is often referred to as working memory and implies
a short-term erasable storage of information, or by a type of
long-term memory system in which information is stored for
an extended time. In both instances the stored information
is used to select and generate behavior that is appropriate
for the particular context. The behaviors that are generated
may be novel, and directly related to the sensory events, or
they may be preprogrammed in chains that are innate but
still must be selected with respect either to ongoing sensory

information or to internal states. Thus, there must be some


type of master (sometimes referred to as executive) control
system that selects behavior. It is our contention that the
class-common function of the prefrontal cortex is to select
and generate behavior patterns. In addition, it is proposed
that this system has a working memory subsystem but that
it uses a long-term memory store that is largely a function of the medial temporal regions. Although this general
view of prefrontal functioning is hardly novel (see reviews
[44,52,78,109]), it is the idea that a prefrontal system with
such functions will be found in all mammals that is the key
concept in the current discussion.
One prediction from our general hypothesis is that all
mammals with damage to the prefrontal homologue should
show functional disruptions of the same general sort. The
prefrontal cortex is extensive in most mammals, however, so
we might also anticipate that there will be some subdivision
of functions across the prefrontal regions in different species
[138]. The organization of the subdivisions is likely to have
some species-typical characteristics, however. Consider, for
example, the relative importance of vision to the control of
forelimb movement in primates versus a parallel olfactory
control of forelimb movement in the rat (e.g. [148]). Such
differences would be expected to be reflected in differences
in the subtle organization of the prefrontal subregions. We
shall review first the evidence for general symptoms of
prefrontal injury in the major subdivisions of primates and
rodents, and then consider the issue of how to compare
functions of the prefrontal subregions across the two orders.
4.1. Effects of frontal lesions in primates
As noted earlier, it is possible to distinguish functionally three distinctly different frontal lobe regions in humans.
Thus, in addition to the motor and oculomotor areas, there
are the dorsolateral frontal region, the lateral orbitofrontal
region, and an anterior cingulate/medial orbitofrontal region.
Damage to the dorsolateral frontal region is characterized
especially by deficits in working memory, particularly as it
relates to certain executive processes such as the monitoring and planning of behaviors (e.g. [44,129]). Damage to
the orbitofrontal frontal region is characterized by altered
socio-emotional behaviors, hyperkinesis, deficits in the processing of olfactory and gustatory information, and in spontaneity (e.g. [22,83,84]). Damage to the anterior cingulate
region is not as well characterized, but can include reduced
response to pain, akinetic mutism, and impaired motor initiation (e.g. [36]). In addition, all three areas probably produce some form of attentional deficit but distinctions in the
attentional domain remain unclear and are likely related to
the type of sensory information being processed. Thus, attentional processes related to olfactory and gustatory inputs
are likely related to orbitofrontal regions, those related to
visual inputs are likely related to dorsolateral regions, and
those related to internal states (such as pain) are related to
anterior cingulate regions.

H.B.M. Uylings et al. / Behavioural Brain Research 146 (2003) 317

In monkeys the symptoms of lesions to the dorsolateral


prefrontal, orbitofrontal, and anterior cingulate regions respectively produce analogous behavioral deficits to those
observed in humans. In particular, dorsolateral lesions especially produce deficits in working memory, whereas lesions
that include the frontal eye fields (area 8) produce attentional
deficits (e.g. [44]). Orbitofrontal lesions produce a disinhibition of motor behaviors, abnormalities in social-affective
behaviors, deficits in certain types of olfactory and gustatory processing, and in the association between reward and
external cues (e.g. [125]). Cingulate lesions lead to deficits
in the encoding of movements (including eye movements),
and reduced response to pain. Although an early study
suggested that cingulate lesions produce deficits in delayed
alternation, this is only seen in acquisition and not in retention, which is rather different than the effect of dorsolateral
lesions (e.g. [115]).
4.2. Effects of frontal lesions in rats
It was demonstrated in the early 1970s that lesions to the
medial and lateral orbital regions in rats produced very different behavioral syndromes, and that these changes were
strikingly similar to those observed in primates with lesions
to the dorsolateral and orbitofrontal regions, respectively
(Table 1; for reviews see [78,79]). For example, damage to
the medial prefrontal (mPFC) area produces severe deficits
in acquisition and retention of working memory tasks such
as delayed response [86], delayed alternation [38,86,151],
different types of delayed nonmatching-to-sample tasks
(e.g. [19,40,85,107]) and related tasks (e.g. [71,118]). More
Table 1
Summary of the effects of mPFC and OFC lesions in rats
Behavioral impairment

References

mPFC
Visual working memory
Strategy formation
Spatial reversal
Habituation
Skilled reaching
Motor sequencing
Attention
Attention set shift
Food hoarding
Fear extinction
Conditioned emotional responses
Operant reversal learning
Conflict behavior
Operant working memory

[86]
[85]
[38,86]
[74,76]
[149]
[80]
[18,102]
[16]
[75]
[116]
[42,43]
[33,41]
[20]
[19]

OFC
Hyperactivity
Social behavior
Incentive association
Odor working memory
Configural odor learning
Odor reversal learning
Feeding

[74]
[32,77]
[46]
[107]
[150]
[128,131]
[75]

11

recently, deficits have been shown in various types of attentional tasks [102] and in a task requiring a reversal or a shift
of attention from one set of cues to another [16,33,41,82].
Medial frontal lesions also produce disruptions to the production of various motor and species-typical behaviors that
require the ordering of motor sequences, such as in nest
building, food hoarding, or latch opening [74,75,80,134].
Although these types of experiments were viewed by
many as convincing evidence of parallel (and perhaps homologous) functions in rodents and primates, Preuss [111]
remained unconvinced. Indeed, he has argued that in view
of what he believed to be significant anatomical differences,
and the failure to find prolonged or long-lasting deficits after
mPFC lesions in rodents that are equivalent to those observed
in primates with dorsolateral lesions, that the research on the
mPFC of the rat has little to offer to those interested in understanding frontal lobe functioning in primates. Preuss was
wrong on his conclusion that rats with medial frontal do not
have significant memory deficits (e.g. [85,86]), but the fact
that most studies of medial frontal function had made lesions
including all of the medial prefrontal subregions did provide grist for his skepticism. Accordingly, in the past decade
there has been considerable interest in dissociating the different medial prefrontal subregions in the rat. It has now
become clear that the dorsal anterior cingulate region, and
prelimbic/infralimbic region can be functionally dissociated
[51,60a]. In general, it appears that the PL region is involved
in attentional and response selection functions as well as visual working memory (e.g. [53]) whereas the more dorsal
regions (ACd) are involved with generating rules associated
with temporal ordering and motor sequencing of behavior
(see reviews [51,70]. Indeed, on the basis of such studies,
Kesner [70] has gone so far as to suggest that the anterior
cingulate region is homologous to Brodmanns areas 6/46
whereas the PL/IL regions are homologous with Brodmanns
areas 45 and 47. Additionally, although less is known about
its precise role in behavior, it appears that the IL region
plays a special role in autonomic control, and especially in
the modulation of fear-related behaviors (e.g. [101,116]).
Kesners hypothesis will be a difficult one to unequivocally prove or disprove, but it is not necessary for the current
argument, which is simply that the medial frontal regions
have class-common functions that are similar to those of the
dorsolateral and possibly cingulate regions in the monkey
frontal lobe. We suggest that these class-common functions
include functions that are often referred to as executive functions in primates. These functions would include working
memory, the selection of information (often referred to as
attention), and the shifting of attention from one stimulus
attribute to another (e.g. [21]).
There is much more parsimony in reviews comparing the
effects of lateral orbitofrontal lesions in rodents and primates [130]. The lateral orbital region receives significant
olfactory and taste input and although lateral orbital lesions
do not produce deficits in olfactory or taste discriminations,
they do produce deficits in tasks requiring working memory

12

H.B.M. Uylings et al. / Behavioural Brain Research 146 (2003) 317

for odor or taste information (e.g. [4,34,107,117,120]). Furthermore, lesions to the lateral orbital region disrupt the
learning of cross-modal associations that involve odor or
taste cues (e.g. [150]). More recently, studies by Schoenbaum and his colleagues (e.g. [46,131]) have emphasized
a role of the orbital cortex in the encoding of the acquired
incentive value of cues. For example, both rats and primates
can show intact performance on discriminations that require
responding to neutral cues (such as a light) that predicts reward, while at the same time showing marked deficits when
the incentive value of the stimulus is reduced. Such deficits
can be seen during extinction when the incentive value of
a stimulus is reduced to zero, yet animals continue to respond to the cue as though reward is expected (e.g. [13,46]).
The role of the orbitofrontal cortex in stimulus-reward associations is further seen in studies measuring the tuning
characteristics of neurons in the orbital region of both rats
and monkeys [130]. Finally, damage to the orbitofrontal
cortex produces deficits in social and play behavior in rats
(e.g. [32,77]).
The overall pattern of deficits related to orbitofrontal
(OFC) lesions leads to a general conclusion that there is a
class-common function related to making higher order use
of olfactory and taste information. This can be seen easily
in tests that require the association of such information with
events in the world, whether they are learned associations
such as neutral cues and reward or natural stimuli (such as
conspecific odors) or rewards that may be more abstract
(such as social bonding). Although odors obviously play a
reduced role in the control of social behaviors in humans,
the neural networks underlying many social functions remains related to the orbitofrontal cortex.
One region of the rat frontal cortex that has not been
subject to many lesion studies is the more medial and ventral
orbital area (but see [30]). We noted earlier that this region
has ambiguous anatomical characteristics so at present we
must see this region as a somewhat of an enigma.
Taken together, the lesion studies of the prefrontal regions in primates and rodents lead us to conclude that there
is a strong convergence of class-common symptoms across
mammals. Furthermore, it seems likely that most mammals have a gross subdivision of the frontal areas into an
orbital-like region involved especially with the control
of socioaffective behaviors, a dorsolateral-like area involved especially in working memory, and an anterior
cingulate-like area concerned primarily with visceromotor
behaviors and some forms of motor sequencing. To be sure,
there are likely to be significant cross species differences
in details of the anatomical and functional organization of
these areas. The critical point for this paper is that both primates and rodents, and probably all mammals, have a region
of frontal cortex that can be defined both anatomically and
functionally as prefrontal cortex. Further, it is likely that the
prefrontal cortex is subdivided into at least an orbital-like
and another region that may include both dorsolateral- and
anterior cingulate-like features. Finally, given the anatomy

reviewed above, it is likely that these latter areas are also


subdivided. If this is so, then it is likely that the primitive mammals that gave rise to extant mammals had a more
or less subdivided prefrontal cortex that evolved to solve
class-common behavioral problems.

5. Conclusions
The present anatomical and functional data indicate that
rats have a prefrontal cortex, in which Fr2 and ACd are incorporated. Preuss and Kaas [114] have stated that evidence
available to them at that time is consistent with the possibility that the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex is a primate
specialization. Certainly, the rat prefrontal cortex is not as
differentiated as it is in primates and evolutionary later specializations are likely, but dorsolateral-like features, including both anatomical and functional ones, are shown in rats.
They are present in areas Fr2 and ACd, and functional data
indicate that PL is implicated in some dorsolateral-like features. These areas have also features of other type of cortices,
e.g. premotor and anterior cingulate cortex [138]. However,
it holds also for primates that the closer prefrontal cortex
areas are to the premotor cortex the more premotor features
they have.
We focus in this review on rats, but Kosmals group from
Warsaw has indicated, that anatomical dorsolateral-like prefrontal characteristics are present in dogs as well [92,119,
135,136]. This too would be consistent with the conclusion
that the dorsolateral prefrontal regions of primates are not a
unique specialization.

6. Note on prefrontal nomenclature


A stable and unequivocal nomenclature is desired [110],
although neuroanatomical nomenclature should remain
flexible in order to incorporate new insights [137]. Unfortunately, a generally accepted nomenclature for the prefrontal
cortical areas in rats is still lacking. When a literature search
based on key words is executed, this has to be taken into
account. For the rat prefrontal cortex we prefer the most
commonly used nomenclature of Krettek and Price [87]. In
a developmental study, Van Eden and Uylings [141] adopted
their nomenclature with the exception of the term medial
precentral area (PrCm) [138]. We use the neutral term Fr2
introduced by Zilles [157] instead. We prefer to use the term
Fr2 rather than PrCm [87], since rats do not have a central
sulcus. Likewise, the term medial agranular area (AGm;
[39]) is not satisfactory, because more cortical areas in the
medial frontal cortex are agranular and the area lateral to
AGm, called lateral agranular area (AGl) is not agranular.
Unfortunately, the nomenclature of Krettek and Price [87]
for the rat prefrontal areas is not applied by Paxinos and Watson [110]. In this atlas no distinction has been made between
the infralimbic (IL) and the medial orbital (MO) areas: both

H.B.M. Uylings et al. / Behavioural Brain Research 146 (2003) 317

areas are included in MO of Paxinos and Watson [110]. In


addition, they have called the dorsal and ventral anterior cingulate areas (ACd and ACv) the cingulate cortices 1 (CG1),
and 2 (CG2), respectively. We prefer the AC areas, because
these areas have anterior cingulate features, which are different from other cingulate areas, see our review above. The
Fr2 area is indicated by Paxinos and Watson [110] as the
secondary motor area (M2). Interestingly, with the sole exception of the Fr2 area, Swanson [137] in his atlas adopted
the nomenclature of Krettek and Price [87]. In this atlas
Fr2 is indicated instead with the term secondary motor area
(MOs). From our review above, it is evident why we suggest to avoid the term secondary motor area (MOs and M2,
respectively).
Please note that besides differences in nomenclature there
are also discrepancies in the delineations of the pertinent
areas: identical names not necessarily imply a similar delineation by different authors. Our cytoarchitectonic definition
of borders of prefrontal areas [138,141] was described in
such a way that they appear reproducible for other PFC researchers. This delineation of the prefrontal cortex is more
or less comparable with the one of Price and coworkers
[87,121]. More specifically, our definition of Fr2 [138,141]
is comparable with the delineation of PrCm by Price and
colleagues. However, the caudal border of our Fr2 or
PrCm differs strongly from the Fr2 defined in Zilles [157],
M2 in Paxinos and Watson [110] and MOs in Swanson
[137]. The caudal border of this area defined by Krettek and
Price [87] and Van Eden and Uylings [141] is much more
rostral (i.e. at about 1 mm from bregma) than that of the
comparable areas in the atlases of Zilles [157], Swanson
[137] and Paxinos and Watson [110] at about 3.7 mm
from bregma). Our caudal border is not only based on cytoarchitectonics, but also on the patterns of thalamic and
other connections mentioned above. There are further differences in the delineation of the prefrontal cortical areas in
the atlases of Swanson [137] and Paxinos and Watson [110],
but it is outside the scope of this paper to discuss these in
detail.

[3]

[4]

[5]
[6]
[7]
[8]

[9]

[10]

[11]

[12]

[13]

[14]

[15]

[16]
[17]

[18]

[19]

Acknowledgements
We thank Ms. W.T.P. Verweij for her secretarial assistance
and Mr. H. Stoffels for drawing Figs. 1 and 3.

References

[20]

[21]
[22]

[1] Akert K, Hartmann-Von Monakov K. Relationships of precentral,


premotor, and prefrontal cortex to the mediodorsal and intralaminar nuclei of the monkey thalamus. Acta Neurol Exp 1980;40:7
25.
[2] Alexander GE, Crutcher MD, DeLong MR. Basal ganglia
thalamocortical circuits: parallel substrates for motor, oculomotor,
prefrontal and limbic functions. In: Uylings HBM, Van Eden
CG, De Bruin JPC, Feenstra MPG, editors. The prefrontal cortex:

[23]

[24]

13

its structure, function and pathology. Progress in brain research,


vol. 85. Amsterdam: Elsevier; 1990. p. 11946.
Alexander GE, DeLong MR, Strick P. Parallel organization of functionally segregated circuits linking basal ganglia and cortex. Ann
Rev Neurosci 1986;9:35781.
Alvarez P, Eichenbaum H. Representations of odors in the rat orbitofrontal cortex change during and after learning. Behav Neurosci
2002;116:42133.
Amaral DG, Price JL. Amygdalo-cortical projections in the monkey
(Macaca fascicularis). J Comp Neurol 1984;230:46596.
Arnsten AFT. Catecholamine regulation of the prefrontal cortex. J
Psychopharmacol 1997;11:15162.
Barbas H. Architecture and cortical connections of the prefrontal
cortex in the rhesus monkey. Adv Neurol 1992;57:91115.
Barbas H. Anatomic basis of cognitiveemotional interactions in the
primate prefrontal cortex. Neurosci Biobehavior Rev 1995;19:499
510.
Barbas H, Blatt GJ. Topographically specific hippocampal projections target functionally distinct prefrontal areas in the rhesus monkey. Hippocampus 1995;5:51133.
Barbas H, Pandya DN. Architecture and intrinsic connections
of the prefrontal cortex in the rhesus monkey. J Comp Neurol
1989;286:35375.
Barbas H, Henion TH, Dermon CR. Diverse thalamic projections
to the prefrontal cortex in the rhesus monkey. J Comp Neurol
1991;313:6594.
Barbas H, Ghasghaei HT, Rempel-Clower NL, Xiao D. Anatomic
basis of functional specialization in prefrontal cortices in primates.
In: Handbook of neuropsychology, vol. 7. Amsterdam: Elsevier;
2002. p. 127.
Baxter MG, Parker A, Lindner CCC, Izquierdo AD, Murray EA.
Control of response selection by reinforcer value requires interaction
of amygdala and orbitofrontal cortex. J Neurosci 2000;20:43119.
Benton AL. The prefrontal region: its early history. In: Levin HS,
Eisenberg HM, Benton AL, editors. Frontal lobe function and dysfunction. New York: Oxford University Press; 1991. p. 332.
Berendse HW, Groenewegen HJ. Restricted cortical terminal fields
of the midline and intralaminar thalamic nuclei in the rat. Neuroscience 1991;42:73102.
Birrel JM, Brown VJ. Medial frontal cortex mediates perceputal
attentional set shifting in the rat. J Neurosci 2000;20:43204.
Brodmann K. Vergleichende Lokalisationslehre der Grosshirnhinde.
Barth: Leipzig. Lokalisationslehre der Grosshirnrinde. Leipzig:
Barth-Verlag; 1909. p. 324.
Broersen LM, Uylings HBM. Visual attention task performance
in Wistar and Lister hooded rats: response inhibition deficits after
medial prefrontal cortex lesions. Neuroscience 1999;94:4757.
Broersen LM, Heinsbroek RPW, de Bruin JPC, Uylings HBM,
Olivier B. The role of the medial prefrontal cortex of rats in
short-term memory functioning: further support for involvement
of cholinergic, rather than dopaminergic mechanisms. Brain Res
1995a;674:2219.
Broersen LM, Heinsbroek RPW, de Bruin JPC, Laan JB, Joosten
RNJMA, Olivier B. Local pharmacological manipulations of prefrontal dopamine affect conflict behaviour in rats. Behav Pharmacol
1995b;6:395404.
Brown VJ, Bowman EM. Rodent models of prefrontal cortical
function. Trends Neurosci 2002;25:3403.
Butter CM, Snyder DR. Alterations in aversive and aggressive
behaviors following orbital frontal lesions in rhesus monkeys. Acta
Neurobiol Exp 1972;32:11556.
Carmichael ST, Price JL. Architectonic subdivision of the orbital
and medial prefrontal cortex in the macaque monkey. J Comp
Neurol 1994;346:366402.
Carmichael ST, Price JL. Limbic connections of the orbital and
medial prefrontal cortex in macaque monkeys. J Comp Neurol
1995;363:61541.

14

H.B.M. Uylings et al. / Behavioural Brain Research 146 (2003) 317

[25] Carr DB, Sesack SR. Projections from the rat prefrontal cortex to
the ventral tegmental area: target specificity in the synaptic associations with mesoaccumbens and mesocortical neurons. J Neurosci
2000;20:386473.
[26] Cavada C, Goldman-Rakic PS. Posterior parietal cortex in rhesus
monkey. II. Evidence for segregated corticocortical networks linking
sensory and limbic areas with the frontal lobe. J Comp Neurol
1989;287:42245.
[27] Cavada C, Company T, Tejedor J, Cruz-Rizzolo RJ, Reinoso-Surez
F. The anatomical connections of the macaque monkey orbitofrontal
cortex. A review. Cereb Cortex 2000;10:22042.
[28] Cond F, Audinat E, Maire-Lepoivre E, Crpel F. Afferent connections of the medial frontal cortex of the rat. A study using retrograde transport of fluorescent dyes. I. Thalamic Afferents. Brain
Res Bull 1990;24:34154.
[29] Cond F, Maire-Lepoivre E, Audinat E, Crpel F. Afferent connections of the medial frontal cortex of the rat. II. Cortical and
subcortical afferent. J Comp Neurol 1995;352:56793.
[30] Corwin JV, Fussinger M, Meyer RC, King VR, Reep RL. Bilateral destruction of the ventrolateral orbital cortex produces allocentric but not egocentric spatial deficits in rats. Behav Brain Res
1994;61:7986.
[31] Damasio AR. Descartes error: emotion, reason, and the human
brain. New York: Grosset/Putnam; 1994. p. 312.
[32] De Bruin JPC. Social behavior and the prefrontal cortex. In: Uylings
HBM, Van Eden CG, De Bruin JPC, Feenstra MPG, editors. The
prefrontal cortex: its structure, function and pathology. Progress
in brain research, vol. 85. Amsterdam: Elsevier; 1990. p. 485
96.
[33] De Bruin JPC, Feenstra MPG, Broersen LM, Van Leeuwen M,
Arens C, De Vries S, et al. Role of the prefrontal cortex of the rat in
learning and decision making: effects of transient inactivation. In:
Uylings HBM, Van Eden CG, De Bruin JPC, Feenstra MGP, Pennartz CMA, editors. Cognition, emotion and autonomic responses:
the integrative role of the prefrontal cortex and limbic structures.
Progress in brain research, vol. 126. Amsterdam: Elsevier; 2000.
p. 10313.
[34] DeCoteau WE, Kesner RP, Williams JM. Short-term memory for
food reward magnitude: the role of the prefrontal cortex. Behav
Brain Res 1997;88:23949.
[35] Dermon CR, Barbas H. Contralateral thalamic projections predominantly reach transitional cortices in the rhesus monkey. J Comp
Neurol 1994;344:50831.
[36] Devinsky O, Morrell MJ, Vogt BA. Contributions of anterior cingulate cortex to behavior. Brain 1995;118:279306.
[37] Dinopoulos A, Karamanlidis AN, Papadopoulos G, Antonopoulos
J, Michaloudi H. Thalamic projections to motor, prefrontal and somatosensory cortex in the sheep studied by means of the horseradish
peroxidase transport method. J Comp Neurol 1985;241:6381.
[38] Divac I. Frontal lobe system and spatial reversal in the rat. Neuropsychologia 1971;9:17183.
[39] Donoghue JP, Wise SP. The motor cortex of the rat: cytoarchitecture and microstimulation mapping. J Comp Neurol 1982;212:76
88.
[40] Dunnett SB. Role of the prefrontal cortex and striatal output
systems in short-term memory deficits associated with ageing,
basal forebrain lesions, and cholinergic-rich grafts. Can J Psychol
1990;44:21032.
[41] Feenstra MGP, De Bruin JPC. Strategy switching and the rat prefrontal cortex. In: Otani S, editor. Prefrontal cortex: from synaptic
plasticity to cognition. Dordrecht: Wolters-Kluwer; 2003, in press.
[42] Frysztak RJ, Neafsey EJ. The effect of medial frontal cortex lesions
on cardiovascular conditioned emotional responses in the rat. Brain
Res 1994;643:18193.
[43] Frysztak RJ, Neafsey EJ. The effect of medial frontal cortex lesions
on respiration, freezing, and ultrasonic vocalizations during conditioned emotional responses in rat. Cereb Cortex 1991;1:41825.

[44] Fuster JM. The prefrontal cortex: anatomy, physiology, and neuropsychology of the frontal lobe, 3rd ed. New York: Raven Press;
1997. p. 333.
[45] Fuster JM. Linkage at the top. Neuron 1998;21:12234.
[46] Gallagher M, McMahan RW, Schoenbaum G. Orbitofrontal cortex
and representations of incentive value in associative learning. J
Neurosci 1999;19:66104.
[47] Gaspar P, Stepniewska I, Kaas JH. Topography and collateralization
of the dopaminergic projections to motor and lateral prefrontal
cortex in owl monkeys. J Comp Neurol 1992;325:121.
[48] Gaykema RPA, Van Weeghel R, Hersh LB, Luiten PGM. Prefrontal
cortical projections to the cholinergic neurons in the basal forebrain.
J Comp Neurol 1991;303:56383.
[49] Ghashghaei HT, Barbas H. Neural interaction between the basal
forebrain and functionally distinct prefrontal cortices in the rhesus
monkey. Neuroscience 2001;103:593614.
[50] Giguere M, Goldman-Rakic PS. Mediodorsal nucleus areal, laminar, and tangential distribution of afferents and efferents in the
frontal lobe of rhesus monkeys. J Comp Neurol 1988;277:195
213.
[51] Gisquet-Verrier P, Winocur G, Delatour B. Functional dissociation
between dorsal and ventral regions of the medial prefrontal cortex
in rats. Psychobiology 2000;28:24860.
[52] Goldman-Rakic PS. Circuitry of the primate prefrontal cortex and
regulation of behavior by representational memory. In: Plum F, editor. Handbook of physiology: the nervous system. Vol. 5. Higher
functions of the brain, Part 1. Bethesda, MD: American Physiological Society; 1987. p. 373417.
[53] Granon S, Poucet B. Involvement of the rat prefrontal cortex in
cognitive functions: a central role for the prelimbic area. Psychobiology 2000;28:22937.
[54] Groenewegen HJ. Organization of the afferent connections of the
mediodorsal thalamic nucleus in the rat, related to mediodorsal
prefrontal topography. Neuroscience 1988;24:379431.
[55] Groenewegen HJ, Berendse HW. The specificity of the non-specific
midline and intralaminar thalamic nuclei. Trends Neurosci
1994;17:527.
[56] Groenewegen HJ, Witter MP. Thalamus. In: Paxinos G, editor. The
rat nervous system, 3rd ed. San Diego, CA: Academic Press; 2004.
p. 407453.
[57] Groenewegen HJ, Wright CI, Uylings HBM. The anatomical relationships of the prefrontal cortex with limbic structures and the
basal ganglia. J Psychopharmacol 1997;11:99106.
[58] Groenewegen HJ, Berendse HW, Wolters JG, Lohman AHM. The
anatomical relationship of the prefrontal cortex with the striatopallidal system, the thalamus and the amygdala: evidence for a parallel organization. In: Uylings HBM, Van Eden CG, De Bruin JPC,
Feenstra MPG, editors. The prefrontal cortex: its structure, function and pathology. Progress in brain research, vol. 85. Amsterdam:
Elsevier; 1990. p. 95118.
[59] Guandalini P. The efferent connections to the thalamus and brainstem of the physiologically defined eye field in the rat medial frontal
cortex. Brain Res Bull 2001;54:17586.
[60] Hajos M, Richards CD, Szekely AD, Sharp T. An electrophysiological and neuroanatomical study of the medial prefrontal cortical
projection to the midbrain raphe nuclei in the rat. Neuroscience
1998;87:95108.
[60a] Heidbreder CA, Groenewegen HJ. The medical prefrontal cortex
in the rat: evidence for a dorso-ventral distinction based upon
functional and anatomical characteristics. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev.
2003, in press.
[61] Iwaniuk A, Whishaw IQ. On the origin of skilled forelimb movements. Trends Neurosci 2000;23:3726.
[62] Jay T, Witter MP. Distribution of hippocampal CA1 and subicular
efferents in the prefrontal cortex of the rat studied by means of
anterograde transport of Phaseolus vulgaris leucoagglutinin. J Comp
Neurol 1991;313:57486.

H.B.M. Uylings et al. / Behavioural Brain Research 146 (2003) 317


[63] Jodo E, Aston-Jones G. Activation of locus coeruleus by prefrontal
cortex is mediated by excitatory amino acid inputs. Brain Res
1997;768:32732.
[64] Jodo E, Chiang C, Aston-Jones G. Potent excitatory influence of
prefrontal cortex on noradrenergic locus coeruleus neurons. Neuroscience 1998;83:6379.
[65] Jones EG. Thalamus. New York: Plenum Press; 1985. p. 935.
[66] Jones EG. A description of the human thalamus. In: Steriade M,
Jones EG, McCormick DA, editors. Thalamus, vol. 2. Amsterdam:
Elsevier; 1997. p. 42599.
[67] Jones EG, Leavitt RY. Retrograde axonal transport and the demonstration of non-specific projections to the cerebral cortex and striatum from thalamic intralaminar nuclei in the rat. J Comp Neurol
1974;154:34978.
[68] Kaas JH. The organization of neocortex in mammals: implications
for theories of brain function. Ann Rev Psychol 1987;38:12951.
[69] Kalsbeek A, Buijs RM, Hofman MA, Matthijssen MAH, Pool CW,
Uylings HBM. Effects of neonatal thermal lesioning of mesocortical
dopaminergic projection on the development of the rat prefrontal
cortex. Dev Brain Res 1987;32:12332.
[70] Kesner RP. Subregional analysis of mnemonic functions of the
prefrontal cortex in the rat. Psychobiology 2000;28:21928.
[71] Kesner RP, Holbrook T. Dissociation of item and order spatial
memory in rats following medial prefrontal cortex lesions. Neuropsychologia 1987;25:65364.
[72] Kievit J, Kuypers HGJM. Organization of the thalamo-cortical connexions to the frontal lobe in the rhesus monkey. Exp Brain Res
1977;29:299322.
[73] King V, Corwin JV. Neglect following unilateral ablation of the
caudal but not the rostral portion of medial agranular cortex of the
rat and the therapeutic effect of apomorphine. Behav Brain Res
1990;37:16984.
[74] Kolb B. Dissociation of the effects of lesions of the orbital or
medial aspect of the prefrontal cortex of the rat with respect to
activity. Behav Biol 1974a;10:32943.
[75] Kolb B. Prefrontal lesions alter eating and hoarding behavior in
rats. Physiol Behav 1974b;12:50711.
[76] Kolb B. Some tests of response habituation in rats with prefrontal
lesions. Can J Psychol 1974c;28:2607.
[77] Kolb B. The social behavior of rats with chronic prefrontal lesions.
J Comp Physiol Psychol 1974d;87:46674.
[78] Kolb B. Functions of the frontal cortex of the rat: a comparative
review. Brain Res Rev 1984;8:6598.
[79] Kolb B. Animal models for human PFC-related disorders. In:
Uylings HBM, Van Eden CG, De Bruin JPC, Corner MA, Feenstra MPG, editors. The prefrontal cortex: its structure, function and
pathology. Progress in brain research, vol. 85. Amsterdam: Elsevier;
1990. p. 50119.
[80] Kolb B, Whishaw IQ. Dissociation of the contributions of the
prefrontal, motor and parietal cortex to the control of movement in
the rat. Can J Psychol 1983a;37:21132.
[81] Kolb B, Whishaw IQ. Generalizing in neuropsychology: problems
and principles underlying cross-species comparisons. In: Robinson
TE, editor. Behavioral contributions to brain research. New York:
Oxford University Press; 1983b. p. 23764.
[82] Kolb B, Whishaw IQ. Neonatal frontal lesions in the rat: sparing
of learned but not species-typical behavior in the presence of reduced brain weight and cortical thickness. J Comp Physiol Psychol
1981;95:86379.
[83] Kolb B, Whishaw IQ. Brain plasticity and behavior. Ann Rev
Psychol 1998;49:4364.
[84] Kolb B, Whishaw IQ. Fundamentals of human neuropsychology,
5th ed. New York: Worth; 2003.
[85] Kolb B, Buhrmann K, MacDonald R, Sutherland RJ. Dissociation
of the medial prefrontal, posterior parietal, and posterior temporal
cortex for spatial navigation and recognition memory in the rat.
Cereb Cortex 1994;4:1534.

15

[86] Kolb B, Nonneman AJ, Singh R. Double dissociation of spatial


impairment and perseveration following selective prefrontal lesions
in the rat. J Comp Physiol Psychol 1974;87:77280.
[87] Krettek JE, Price JL. The cortical projections of the mediodorsal
nucleus and adjacent thalamic nuclei in the rat. J Comp Neurol
1977;171:15791.
[88] Leonard CM. The prefrontal cortex of the rat. I. Cortical projection
of the mediodorsal nucleus. II. Efferent connections. Brain Res
1969;12:32143.
[89] Lewis DA, Sesack SR. Dopamine systems in the primate brain. In:
Bjrklund A, Hkfelt T, Bloom FE, editors. Handbook of chemical neuroanatomy, Vol. 13. The primate nervous system, Part I.
Amsterdam: Elsevier; 1997. p. 36375.
[90] Lopez-Bendito G, Molnr Z. Thalamocortical development: how
are we going to get there? Nat Rev Neurosci 2003;4:27689.
[91] Loughlin SE, Fallon JH. Substantia nigra and ventral tegmental area
projection to cortex: topography and collateralization. Neuroscience
1984;11:42535.
[92] Markow-Rajkowska G, Kosmal A. Organization of cortical afferents to the frontal association cortex in dogs. Acta Neurobiol Exp
1987;47:13761.
[93] McDonald AJ. Organization of amygdaloid projections to the prefrontal cortex and associated striatum in the rat. Neuroscience
1991;44:114.
[94] McFarland NR, Haber SN. Thalamic relay nuclei of the basal
ganglia form both reciprocal and nonreciprocal cortical connections,
linking multiple frontal cortical areas. J Neurosci 2002;22:811732.
[95] Mesulam M-M. Spatial attention and neglect: parietal, frontal and
cingulate contributions to the mental representation and attentional
targeting of salient extrapersonal events. Philos Trans R Soc Lond
B: Biol Sci 1999;354:132546.
[96] Middleton FA, Strick PL. The temporal lobe is a target of output
from the basal ganglia. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 1996;93:86837.
[97] Middleton FA, Strick PL. Basal-ganglia projections to the prefrontal cortex of the primate. Cereb Cortex 2000;12:92635.
[98] Middleton FA, Strick PL. A revised neuroanatomy of frontal
subcortical circuits. In: Lichter DG, Cummings JL, editors. Frontal
subcortical circuits in psychiatric and neurological disorders. New
York: Guildford Press; 2001. p. 4458.
[99] Miller EK, Wallis JD. The prefrontal cortex and executive brain
functions. In: Squire LR, Bloom FE, Landis SC, Roberts JL, Zigmond MJ, editors. Fundamental of neuroscience. San Diego, CA:
Academic Press, Elsevier; 2003. p. 135376.
[100] Morn AM, Reinoso-Surez F. Topographical organization of the
thalamic afferent connections to the motor cortex in the cat. J Comp
Neurol 1988;270:6485.
[101] Morgan MA, Schulkin J, LeDoux JE. Ventral medial prefrontal
cortex and emotional perseveration: the memory for prior extinction
training. Behav Brain Res 2003, in press.
[102] Muir JL, Everitt BJ, Robbins TW. The cerebral cortex of the rat
and visual attentional function: dissociable effects of mediofrontal,
cingulate, anterior dorsolateral and parietal cortex lesions on a
five-choice serial reaction time task. Cereb Cortex 1996;6:47081.
[103] Nauta WJH. Neural associations of the amygdaloid complex in the
monkey. Brain 1962;85:50520.
[104] Neafsey EJ, Hurley-Gius KM, Arvanitis D. The topographical organization of neurons in the rat medial frontal, insular and olfactory
cortex to the solitary tract nucleus, olfactory bulb, periaqueductal
gray and superior colliculus. Brain Res 1986;377:26170.
[105] Northcutt RG, Kaas JH. The emergence and evolution of mammalian
neocortex. Trends Neurosci 1995;18:3739.
[106] ngr D, Price JL. The organization of networks within the orbital
and medial prefrontal cortex of rats. Cereb Cortex 2000;10:20619.
[107] Otto T, Eichenbaum H. Complementary roles of the orbital
prefrontal cortex and the perirhinalentorhinal cortices in an
odor-guided delayed-nonmatching-to-sample task. Behav Neurosci
1992;106:76275.

16

H.B.M. Uylings et al. / Behavioural Brain Research 146 (2003) 317

[108] Pandya DN, Yeterian EH. Prefrontal cortex in relation to other


cortical areas in rhesus monkey: architecture and connections. In:
Uylings HBM, Van Eden CG, De Bruin JPC, Corner MA, Feenstra
MPG, editors. The prefrontal cortex: its structure, function and
pathology. Progress in brain research, vol. 85. Amsterdam: Elsevier;
1990. p. 6394.
[109] Passingham R. The frontal lobes and voluntary action. Oxford
psychology series, vol. 21. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 1993.
p. 299.
[110] Paxinos G, Watson C. The rat brain in stereotaxic coordinates. San
Diego, CA: Academic Press; 1998.
[111] Preuss TM. Do rats have a prefrontal cortex? The RoseWoolsey
Akert program reconsidered. J Cogn Neurosci 1995;7:124.
[112] Preuss TM. Taking the measure of diversity: comparative alternatives to the modelanimal paradigm in cortical neuroscience. Brain
Behav Evol 2000;55:28799.
[113] Preuss TM, Goldman-Rakic PS. Connections of the ventral granular frontal cortex of macaques with perisylvian premotor and somatosensory areas: anatomical evidence for somatic representation
in primate frontal association cortex. J Comp Neurol 1989;282:293
316.
[114] Preuss TM, Kaas JH. Human brain evolution. In: Zigmond MJ,
Bloom FE, Landis SC, Roberts JL, Squire LR, editors. Fundamental of neuroscience. San Diego, CA: Academic Press; 1999.
p. 1283311.
[115] Pribram KH, Wilson WA, Connors J. Effects of lesions of the
medial forebrain on alternation behavior of rhesus monkeys. Exp
Neurol 1962;6:3647.
[116] Quirk GJ, Russo GK, Barron JL, Lebron K. The role of ventromedial
prefrontal cortex in the recovery of extinguished fear. J Neurosci
2000;20:622531.
[117] Ragozzino ME, Kesner RP. The role of the agranular insular cortex
in working memory for food reward value and allocentric space in
rats. Behav Brain Res 1999;98:10312.
[118] Ragozzino ME. The contribution of cholinergic and dopaminergic
afferents in the rat prefrontal cortex to learning, memory, and
attention. Psychobiology 2000;28:23847.
[119] Rajkowska G, Kosmal A. Intrinsic connections and cytoarchitectonic
data of the frontal association cortex in the dog. Acta Neurobiol
Exp 48:16992.
[120] Ramus SJ, Eichenbaum H. Neural correlates of olfactory recognition
memory in the rat orbitofrontal cortex. J Neurosci 2000;20:8199
208.
[121] Ray JP, Price JL. The organization of the thalamocortical connections of the mediodorsal thalamic nucleus in the rat, related to
the ventral forebrainprefrontal cortex topography. J Comp Neurol
1992;323:16797.
[122] Ray JP, Price JL. The organization of projections from the mediodorsal nucleus of the thalamus to orbital and medial prefrontal cortex
in macaque monkeys. J Comp Neurol 1993;337:131.
[123] Reep RL, Corwin JV, King VR. Neural connections of orbital cortex
in rats: topography of cortical and thalamic afferents. Exp Brain
Res 1996;111:21532.
[124] Reep RL, Corwin JV. Topographic organization of the striatal and
thalamic connections of rat medial agranular cortex. Brain Res
1999;841:4352.
[125] Rolls ET. The functions of the orbitofrontal cortex. In: Stuss DT,
Knight RT, editors. Principles of frontal lobe function. New York:
Oxford University Press; 2002. p. 35475.
[126] Rose JE, Woolsey CN. The orbitofrontal cortex and its connections
with the mediodorsal nucleus in rabbit, sheep and cat. Res Publ
Assoc Nerv Ment Dis 1948;27:21032.
[127] Russchen FT, Amaral DG, Price JL. The afferent connections of the
substantia innominata in the monkey. J Comp Neurol 1985;242:1
27.
[128] Saddoris MP, Setlow B, Nugent S, Schoenbaum G. A reexamination
of the role of orbitofrontal cortex and basolateral amygdala in

[129]

[130]

[131]

[132]
[133]

[134]

[135]

[136]

[137]
[138]

[139]
[140]

[141]
[142]

[143]

[144]

[145]

[146]

[147]

[148]

[149]

acquisition and reversal of odor-guided go, no go discrimination


task. Soc Neurosci Abstr 2000;23.
Sawaguchi T, Goldman-Rakic PS. The role of D1-dopamine receptor
in working memory: local injections of dopamine antagonists into
the prefrontal cortex of rhesus monkeys performing an oculomotor
delayed-response task. J Neurophysiol 1994;71:51528.
Schoenbaum G, Setlow B. Integrating orbitofrontal cortex into prefrontal theory: common processing themes across species and subdivisions. Learn Mem 2002;8:13447.
Schoenbaum G, Chiba AA, Gallagher M. Changes in functional
connectivity in orbitofrontal cortex and basolateral amygdala during
learning and reversal training. J Neurosci 2000;20:517989.
Sherman SM, Guillery RW. Exploring the thalamus. San Diego,
CA: Academic Press; 2001. p. 312.
Shibata H. Topographic organization of subcortical projections to the
anterior thalamic nuclei in the rat. J Comp Neurol 1992;323:117
27.
Shipley JE, Kolb B. Neural correlates of species typical behavior in
the Syrian Golden hamster. J Comp Physiol Psychol 1977;91:1056
73.
Stepniewska I, Kosmal A. Distribution of mediodorsal thalamic
nucleus afferents originating in the prefrontal association cortex of
the dog. Acta Neurobiol Exp 1986;46:31122.
Stepniewska I, Kosmal A. Differential projection from the motor
and limbic cortical regions to the mediodorsal thalmic nucleus in
the dog. Acta Neurobiol Exp 1989;49:2337.
Swanson LW. Brain maps: structure of the rat brain. Amsterdam:
Elsevier; 1992. p. 240.
Uylings HBM, Van Eden CG. Qualitative and quantitative comparison of the prefrontal cortex in rat and in primates, including
humans. In: Uylings HBM, Van Eden CG, De Bruin JPC, Corner
MA, Feenstra MPG, editors. The prefrontal cortex: its structure,
function and pathology. Progress in brain research, vol. 85. Amsterdam: Elsevier; 1990. p. 3162.
Uylings HBM. About assumptions in estimation of density of neurons and glial cells. Biol Psychiat 2002;51:8402.
Van Eden CG. Development of connections between the mediodorsal nucleus of the thalamus and the prefrontal cortex in the rat. J
Comp Neurol 1986;244:34959.
Van Eden CG, Uylings HBM. Ctyoarchitectonic development of
the prefronal cortex of the rat. J Comp Neurol 1885;241:25367.
Van Eden CG, Lamme VAF, Uylings HBM. Heterotopic cortical
afferents to the medial prefrontal cortex in the rat. A combined
retrograde and anterograde tracer study. Eur J Neurosci 1992;4:77
97.
Vertes RP. Analysis of projections from the medial prefrontal cortex
to the thalamus in the rat, with emphasis on nucleus reuniens. J
Comp Neurol 2002;442:16387.
Verwer RW, Van Vulpen EH, Van Uum JF. Postnatal development of amygdaloid projections to the prefrontal cortex in the rat
studied with retrograde and anterograde tracers. J Comp Neurol
1996;376:7596.
Verwer RWH, Meijer RJ, Van Uum HFM, Witter MP. Collateral
projections from the rat hippocampal formation to the lateral and
medial prefrontal cortex. Hippocampus 1997;7:397402.
Warren JM, Kolb B. Generalizations in neuropsychology. In: Finger
S, editor. Brain damage, behavior and the concept of recovery of
function. New York: Plenum Press; 1978.
Whishaw IQ. Change in sensory control related to behavioural and
anatomical evolution of primate skilled reaching. Behav Brain Res,
in press.
Whishaw IQ, Pellis SM, Gorny BP. Skilled reaching in rats and
humans: evidence of parallel development or homology. Behav
Brain Res 1992;47:5970.
Whishaw IQ, Pellis SM, Gorny BP. Medial frontal cortex lesions
impair the aiming component of rat reaching. Behav Brain Res
1992;50:93104.

H.B.M. Uylings et al. / Behavioural Brain Research 146 (2003) 317


[150] Whishaw IQ, Tomie J, Kolb B. Ventrolateral frontal cortex lesions
in rats impair the acquisition and retention of a tactileolfactory
configural task. Behav Neurosci 1992;106:597603.
[151] Wikmark RGE, Divac I, Weiss R. Retention of spatial delayed
alternation in rats with lesions of the frontal lobes: implications
for a comparative neuropsychology of the prefrontal system. Brain
Behav Evol 1973;8:32939.
[152] Williams SM, Goldman-Rakic PS. Widespread origin of the primate
mesofrontal dopamine system. Cereb Cortex 1998;8:32145.
[153] Woolsey CN. Organization of somatic sensory and motor areas of
the cerebral cortex. In: Harlow HF, Woolsey CN, editors. Biological
and biochemical bases of behavior. Madison, WI: University of
Wisconsin Press; 1957.

17

[154] Wouterlood FG, Steinbusch HW, Luiten PG, Bol JG. Projection from the prefrontal cortex to histaminergic cell groups in
the posterior hypothalamic region of the rat. Anterograde tracing with Phaseolus vulgaris leucoagglutinin ombined with immunocytochemistry of histidine decarboxylase. Brain Res 1987;406:
3306.
[155] Yoshida A, Dostrovsky JO, Chiang CY. The afferent and efferent
connections of the nucleus submedius in the rat. J Comp Neurol
1992;324:11533.
[156] Zaborsky L, Gaykema RPA, Swanson DJ, Culliman WE. Cortical
input to the basal forebrain. Neuroscience 1997;79:105178.
[157] Zilles K. The cortex of the rat. A stereotaxic atlas. Berlin:
Springer-Verlag; 1985. p. 121.

Você também pode gostar