Você está na página 1de 11

Creativity and Music Education: The Impact

of Social Variables
Raymond A.R. MacDonald and Dorothy Miell
Department of Psychology, Glasgow Caledonian University, Scotland and
Department of Psychology, The Open University, England

publications within the psychology of music and music education have


begun to highlight the importance that social factors have for a childs
developing musicality, but there is still much to understand about this influence. This paper highlights two projects that investigated aspects of the intrinsically social nature of musical participation. One study investigated the
communication (both verbal and musical) taking place during compositions
created by friends and non-friends. The second study investigated the musical
and communication gains made by individuals with special needs who
attended Gamelan workshops. The educational implications of these studies
Recent

are

considered,

as are

directions for future research.

The social context of creativity

Whilst there is a strong commitment to developing creative musical skills


within the current UK National Curriculum for music, there is evidence that in
practice there are challenges and difficulties with this aspect of music teaching.
Creative tasks are under-used in classroom situations, and this might be at least
partly the result of some teachers concerns about the most effective ways in
which to teach something as difficult to define as creativity (Byrne, 1996).
Although research has begun to focus on both the process and outcomes of
childrens creative endeavours in classroom situations (Barrett, 1998; Folkestad,
1998; Morgan, 1999; Webster, 1994), investigating factors which affect creative
music making remains one of the least studied aspects of music education. The
focus of this paper is on identifying some key variables likely to affect creativity
in music making. In particular, our own research has suggested that social and
communicative factors are central issues to consider in determining the nature
and quality of group compositions and performances, and evidence from two
studies is presented here which illustrate this importance. We argue that since
so much of the work on creative music which children and indeed adults
engage in takes place in a social context, more research attention should be
paid to the nature and effect of this context on the creative process and
outcomes.

Recent advances in the psychology of music literature have begun to emphasise that music is an intensely social activity (Hargreaves & North, 1997). There
is an increasing amount of literature that highlights the key impact which peer
groups, the family, the relationships between teacher and pupil and between
pupils themselves, have upon a childs interest in and knowledge about music
and indeed on their developing personal identity as musical (ONeill, 1997;
Taebel, 1994). This has direct relevance for music education research, as it is
important to consider this wider social context in which children listen to, play
and learn about music when investigating how children respond to different
types of music and to different musical tasks ~(Folkestad, 1998). We are all
58

Downloaded from ijm.sagepub.com at UNIV ESTDL PAULISTA DE MESQIT on February 17, 2016

by music in our daily lives (Mertz, 1998) and this exposure as well
experiences with family, friends and in the school setting play an important
role in developing childrens specific musical preferences and abilities.
Consequently, music education research can be enriched by including some
analysis of these influences in developing studies of childrens musicality.
surrounded
as

Socio-cultural

approach to examining collaboration

In this paper, however, we want to move beyond the recognition and exploration
of the social context of music making and listening to examine the impact of
social factors at a more micro level. That is, to examine the ways in which
social variables not only provide an important backdrop to what is being
produced when children or adults create and perform music but also the way
in which they directly affect and shape the nature of the interaction and the
music itself. This is to say more than that we need to look at the effect of the
presence of others on a childs or an adults individual performance, but is

adopt a socio-cultural approach to the study of their collaborative


Such
an approach, based on the work of Vygotsky, emphasises the
working.
co-construction of knowledge through joint action and discourse, mediated
through the use of cultural tools and artefacts (Vygotsky, 1978).
Much of the rapidly expanding field of research on collaborative learning
(e.g. Joiner & Littleton, in press) has employed this approach to studying how
children engage with each others ideas as they work together. From this
perspective, the children learn by expressing their own ideas and listening to
those of others, working to develop a shared conception of the problem and of
instead

to

possible routes towards a common goal (Teasley & Rochelle, 1995). By actively
engaging with each others ideas, modifying and extending them, the children
develop their understanding and their critical thinking skills. This view emphasises the mediating role of cultural tools such as language, computers and, in
the

case we are

interested in here, musical instruments and music itself, in

enabling childrens learning.


Talking with others is a vital way in which children acquire understanding
they engage with more knowledgeable others and internalise not only an
understanding of a field of knowledge but also of the ways in which others
think and talk about that field. As Bruner (1996) explains,
It is the give and take of talk that makes collaboration possible. For the agentive
mind is not only active in nature, but it seeks out dialogue and discourse with
other active minds. And it is through this dialogic, discursive process that we

come

to know the Other and his points of view, his stories (sic). We learn an
amount not only about the world but about ourselves by discourse

enormous

with Others.

(p.93).

In studying interactions between individuals making music, it is clear that


music can provide another mediation route between the children besides their
talk. This other channel can not only express thoughts, emotions and ideas just
as words can (indeed, learning how to communicate through the medium of
music is a key aim of National Curriculum music teaching) but also, learning
how to use and combine elements of music is as important for this field as is
learning how to talk scientifically in science lessons. Thus, if we are to study
the negotiation processes in collaborative compositions we will need to examine
both the verbal and the musical channels of communication. However, whilst
several theorists have suggested that talk and music might fulfil similar functions
in communication (Aldridge, 1996; Campbell, 1997), very few empirical studies
59

Downloaded from ijm.sagepub.com at UNIV ESTDL PAULISTA DE MESQIT on February 17, 2016

have attempted to study the ways in which music might be used to communicate
between individuals as they compose or perform.
Another feature of adopting a socio-cultural approach to understanding collaborative working is the importance placed on studying the nature and quality
of the interaction itself. That is, on studying the ease with which children can
establish a shared social reality (Rogoff, 1990), the confidence with which they
offer their own views and the sensitivity to and understanding of the views
and needs of their partner(s) which they show. Where partners or groups have
difficulty in establishing this level of intersubjectivity, research on childrens
problem solving in a number of curriculum areas has shown that they are likely
to be less successful in their work together. We suggest here that this will also
apply to childrens work in music making, since the confidence children feel
with each other might be important in enabling them to offer creative and
imaginative suggestions which would result in new combinations of ideas
which the individuals would not have come up with on their own - clearly
important for creativity. Specifically, we believe that some children who are
already used to working and/or playing together are likely to be particularly
adept at creating and working with intersubjectivity, and thus more successful
in collaborating than children who do not know each other. Indeed there is
some evidence to support this already, suggesting that friends find it easier
than non-friends to establish productive joint working activities (Azmitia &
Montgomery, 1993; Tharp & Gallimore, 1988). However, the relatively little
research concerned with the effects of friendship on childrens collaborative
work has primarily involved studies of childrens mathematical and scientific
reasoning (as indeed is the case with most research on collaborative learning).
It is only recent work (Miell & MacDonald, in press) which has begun to
examine how childrens relationships with each other might affect their creative

partnerships.
A further reason why social relationships might be an important factor to
consider when investigating creative collaborations between young people is
that their friendships are in fact often based around their joint appreciation of
one form or another of music. There are many examples of the importance that
young people place on music. For example, Fitzgerald, Joseph, Hayes and
ORegan (1995) provided experimental evidence showing music listening as the
most preferred leisure activity of adolescents. The vast majority of money spent
by young people on music is another indication of the influence that it has on
their own lives (Frith, 1987; Tarrent, North & Hargreaves, 1999). Zillman and
Gan (1997) suggest that music is a key factor in determining and characterising
friendship between young people. Indeed, these authors suggest that music is
a central part of young peoples identity and as such will play a significant role
in influencing their ongoing social interactions. Of particular importance to the
theme of this special issue is that, in comparison to all other forms of music,
popular music is by far the most important genre of music for young people

(Geter

&

Steisand, 1995).

Friendship effects on the process and product of collaborative


compositions
Motivated by these considerations, we have recently carried out a study which
has investigated the impact which social variables have upon childrens musical
creativity (Miell & MacDonald, in press). In this study 10-11 year old children
were asked to compose a piece of music entirely of their own and in a style of
their choosing to reflect the theme of the rain forest. The children all began
60

Downloaded from ijm.sagepub.com at UNIV ESTDL PAULISTA DE MESQIT on February 17, 2016

their involvement with the


researchers during which

workshop with one of the


with different instruments,
rhythms, dynamics etc. and discussed ways in which compositions can be
developed and different effects achieved. The experimental sessions involved
the children working on their compositions in same sex pairs and they were
given 15 minutes to complete the task. Half the children worked on the task
with one of their best friends while the other half of the children worked on
the task with a child from a different class who they would have known by
sight but who was not a friend.
We were interested in both the nature and quality of the interactive process
as well as in the quality of the musical end product, and with this in mind we
videotaped all the composition sessions and also recorded onto an audiocassette
each pairs final performance of their composition. All the talk and music from
the videotapes were transcribed and the talk was then coded in accordance
with a system introduced by Berkowitz, Gibbs, and Broughton (1980) and
developed by Kruger (1992). This divides utterances into transactive and nontransactive types. Transactive communication is defined as communication
which builds upon and extends ideas that have already been voiced (either by
the self or another person) and the presence of transactive communication has
been shown to be a key factor in good quality collaboration. We adapted this
verbal coding system to allow us to also code the music played by the children
as either transactive and un-transactive and to track the occurrence and elaboration of each musical motif throughout the composition session (MacDonald,
Miell & Morgan, in press). The final compositions were rated for quality by a
teacher from another school who worked from the audiotape of each composition and was unaware of the hypothesis of the experiment, the experimental
conditions and all details of individual pairs. She rated the compositions using
a set of marking scales developed by Hargreaves, Galton and Robinson (1996).
These authors developed a rating scale that could be used by music teachers
to assess childrens musical compositions. The scale includes bipolar constructs
such as dull/lively, disjointed/flowing, and unoriginal/original.
The results of this study highlighted the impact that social factors such as
friendship have upon both the process and outcomes of childrens collaborative
compositional work. Looking first at the outcome measure, the teacher rated
the compositions produced by friends as of significantly higher quality than
the compositions of children who had been working in non-friendship pairs.
Having established this difference in the overall quality of the music produced,
we then turned to the measures of the processes involved in the talk and music
of the interaction to see if there were also differences there which related to
the outcome scores. We indeed found that the musical and verbal communication styles of the friendship pairs were qualitatively different from those of
the non-friends. The friends both spoke and played more music in total than
the non-friends, but also had a different pattern of interacting within these
overall differences. The friendship pairs used proportionally more transactive
communication in both the verbal and the musical domains than the nonfriends. This meant that the friends were building on, extending and elaborating
on each others ideas, expressed in both the talk and music, and developing
their compositions by this gradual process of offering and refining of suggestions.
This style of interaction was found to be significantly related to the teachers
higher score for these pairs, suggesting that the presence of more transactive
communication was what led to the higher quality compositions from the pairs
of friends. In contrast, non-friends were more likely to spend their time in the
session experimenting with the instruments for themselves and did not offer
project by attending
they experimented

61

Downloaded from ijm.sagepub.com at UNIV ESTDL PAULISTA DE MESQIT on February 17, 2016

or develop ideas together in the same way. The smaller amount of talk
which they produced was characterised by information giving and simple,
unelaborated agreements and disagreements with each other. Sometimes the
music seemed to be played to cover their embarrassment and the lack of talk
between them.
At the individual level, there was some variability amongst the pairs of
children. Specifically, not all the friendship dyads produced high levels of
transactive communication, and similarly two non-friendship pairs produced a
substantial amount of transactive talk. Therefore we suggest that working with
a friend does not necessarily always lead to high levels of transactive communication but, with respect to the pattern of results obtained, working with a friend
does make transactive communication more likely.
Thus it appears from this study that social factors such as friendship are key
variables that influence the nature of childrens interactions - in both the verbal
and musical domains. The musical coding scheme which we developed allowed
us to track interactive processes expressed musically as they occurred in the
composition sessions and holds great promise for future studies of other groups
and pairs collaborating to compose and improvise. Future research could also
focus on the impact that age may have on these results. Indeed, in a study
which extended the findings of the study reported above, we observed that
there was a similar pattern of verbal and musical communication found in
younger children working on a similar but more structured task (MacDonald,
Miell & Mitchell, 2000), but this effect was not observed with older children.
It would also be interesting to investigate the effects of giving children either
longer or more sessions to work on their composition.

up

Working in harmony: establishing a shared social reality in


Gamelan workshops
MacDonald, ODonnell and Davies (1999) undertook

series of studies which

highlighted the importance of social factors in a rather different music education


environment. These studies demonstrated how structured music workshops for
adults with special needs could facilitate developments in both musical and

communication skills for the participants. In these studies, groups of around


15 individuals with special needs took part in a series of music workshops
focused on playing a Javanese Gamelan. These workshops were organised by
Sounds of Progress (SOP) a music production company based in Glasgow,
Scotland, who specialise in working with individuals who have special needs.
The musical techniques and working environment of Sounds of Progress are of
particular importance to the theme of this paper. The company has explicit
objectives for participants from all backgrounds and all abilities, not only in
social skills, but also in terms of musical development, seeking to develop
actual musical skills and social opportunities for all the individuals who participate in SOP activities, rather than therapy. In terms of the musical objectives,
the company works to enhance a wide range of skills but developments in
rhythmic ability on percussion instruments, singing skills, compositional and
improvisational skills are some of the key areas focussed on in particular.
Enhanced self-esteem and improved general communication skills are two of
the social objectives of the work of SOP.
The studies reported by MacDonald, ODonnell and Davies (1999) investigated
the processes involved in structural music workshops held by SOP over a
62

Downloaded from ijm.sagepub.com at UNIV ESTDL PAULISTA DE MESQIT on February 17, 2016

period of 18 months. Pre- and post-testing was conducted with the individuals taking part in the workshops to investigate developments in both socialcommunicative abilities and musical skills (i.e. rhythmic awareness, instrumental ability and pitch perception). Analyses were conducted which compared
each individuals performances on these tests with those of a control group of
individuals (also with special needs) who had been involved in non-musical
group activities such as cooking workshops over the same period of time.
Participants in the music workshops not only developed their basic musical
skills over the course of the 18 months but they also improved their performance
on a range of communication skills. Qualitative reports from carers also highlighted the beneficial impact of the workshops for the participants in terms of
their daily living. The carers felt that the music workshop offered an environment where participants could express ideas that were valued and that participants were highly motivated to take part in the workshops as they were enjoying
them. Finding activities that individuals enjoy and are motivated to take part
in were seen as key social objectives for this client group (MacDonald, ODonnell
& Dougall, 1996). The participants in the non-musical group activities did not
show any of these general social and communication skills improvements, and
so it is clear that it was something about participating in the musical interaction,
rather than just being in group activities, that was responsible for these wider
improvements. Once again, adopting a socio-cultural approach to these data,
we would suggest that it is the development and maintenance of a shared social
reality and the co-ordination of activity through communication and musical
participation which accounts for these gains as a result of the music workshops.
The concept of transactive communication and the impact which the music
itself had upon the interactive process is crucial within the SOP context. All
Gamelan workshops begin with a warm up session which involves all participants standing in a circle and participating in rhythmic games. This period is
vital to the success of a workshop as during this period the workshop facilitators
are attempting to set up cohesive group dynamics. The essential element of this
warm up period is the development of a flow of communication between all
participants and the gradual encouragement of more transactive elements in
this communication. It is important for the success of the music that the
facilitators communicate their ideas effectively, since in Gamelan music there
is no conductor present and all musical directions are given by the drummer
(usually the workshop facilitator) who provides musical signals to the other
musicians. The facilitator also has to bring all participants into this process as
the music develops and build on their comments and suggestions as well as
encouraging participants to build on each others views and on those of the
facilitator. This process does not only occur in talk. There are also non-verbal
elements and transactive musical communication evident in the way the music
is developed. Thus in order for the music to develop cohesively all participants
must be monitoring each others talk, actions and music and communicating
effectively with each other through all these channels. These are complex social
skills and, as the results of the post-testing and interviews with carers showed,
once acquired and developed in the context of the workshops these skills
generalised to other contexts and the individuals gained considerable benefit.
Video analysis of the assessment sessions demonstrated that the developments
in communication skills could be linked to developments in joint attention
(ODonnell, MacDonald & Davies, 1999). In this context, joint attention is
defined as a shared focus of attention to the same object by two individuals.
Joint attention is similar to the concept of shared social reality (Rogoff, 1990)
they both involve the need for a shared focus of activity or definition of the
-

63

Downloaded from ijm.sagepub.com at UNIV ESTDL PAULISTA DE MESQIT on February 17, 2016

to work towards, and these


group members developing transactive
communication.

task and

an

partners

or

agreed goal

Educational

are

negotiated through

(musical and verbal)

implications

the profoundly
evident in both
workshops, and in
particular are seen in the communication between creative partners. There
are a number of important educational implications which emerge from the
results of these studies. The key role played by transactive communication
in the childrens collaborative example has obvious utility in terms of enhancing the learning environment of children in classroom settings. We have
shown that transactive communication is a key determinant of how well
children can collaborate together in school music activities, thus teachers
should seek to create learning environments that facilitate the use of transactive
communication.
Many teachers would not place friends together in work settings as they
believe that friends would talk too much and that this talk might be characterised
as off task. However, although our collaborative composition study found that
friends do indeed talk more than non-friend pairs their talk was not at all offtask but was wholly focused on the task in hand, was very transactive (and
therefore productive) and did not stop the children also playing more music
than the non-friends. Furthermore these compositions were rated as being of
better quality than the children working in non-friend pairs.
It is perhaps interesting to speculate as to why the friends might work together
well on this task. Friends have experience of taking each others perspective
and engaging in joint planning (Hartup, 1996) and they also have a history of
shared experiences. They typically monitor each others talk and actions closely
and resolve any disagreements which emerge (Garvey & Shantz, 1992; Pellegrini,
Galda & Flor, 1997). Also, since the children are emotionally invested in each
other and in the progress of their relationship they are likely to have more
invested in the success of their interactions and in supporting each others
work and self-esteem. The study reported above (Miell & MacDonald, in press)
did find evidence that children with less experience of musical training were
more enabled to ask questions and make suggestions when working with a
friend, which suggests that issues of confidence and willingness to take risks
might be useful ones to explore further when investigating participation in
musical activities. In order to be able to collaborate effectively Hakkinen (in
press) suggests that individuals need to have a profound and well established
understanding of each others perspectives and shared interpretations of the
situations at hand. It seems that friends would be in a position to offer these
conditions to the collaborative situation and in turn develop their ideas through
the extended use of transactive communication. Working with a friend may
therefore help because of their established communication strategies, mutual
trust and confidence. It seems that working with others who share goals and
are supportive in this way can facilitate classroom activities that are both
rewarding and educational for the participants.
Researchers and educators should pay attention to a number of linked issues
which will impact on peoples approach to working together on music. For
example, the extent to which people identify themselves as musical, and how
they see their musicality relative to the skills and abilities of the other(s) they
As stated

earlier, the aim of this paper has been

to

highlight

social aspects of musical creativity. These social aspects


the classroom composing of children and the Gamelan

64

Downloaded from ijm.sagepub.com at UNIV ESTDL PAULISTA DE MESQIT on February 17, 2016

are

working with is likely to be an issue within an educational context. As we


showed in the study of childrens compositions, some children with little or
no formal musical training were unwilling to play an active role in a musical
task unless they were working with a friend, and this suggests that we need to
explore further the link between childrens ease of identifying themselves as
musical and their confidence to have a go at a task in the classroom. The new
music technologies that are currently being introduced into UK schools may be
a way forward for some of these children who may have more confidence
in using computers than playing musical instruments. It might be worth
researching whether or not there is such an issue about linking performance to
musical identity when the mediating tools are more familiar to them.
Popular culture, through television, radio and magazines plays a central part
in childrens lives (Frith, 1987). This informal learning environment is one in
which many children are developing identities that are intrinsically linked to
popular music. Some authors have indeed speculated on whether these informal
learning environments have more to do with a childs developing musicality
than the conventional classroom setting (Folkestad, 1998). It is important for
educationalists to consider the impact of these informal learning environments
and the ways in which they could be used to improve other, more structured,
learning experiences. One impact may be in terms of using a wider range of
music styles in the classroom, particularly popular music as this may have a
greater resonance with children.
are

Future research
this line of research we are currently interested in
but
related areas:
exploring
separate
The first involves investigating ways of improving collaboration in musical
tasks. It is possible to train children to collaborate effectively in other curriculum
areas, primarily by training them to communicate in a more transactive way.
We have a current research proposal which proposes investigating the effects
of training children to collaborate in this way in a creative music making task.
The research investigates effects upon the process of their interactions as well
as the end products of their musical collaborations (both the quality of their
composition and their levels of enjoyment and self-confidence).
The second strand of our future work investigates the effects of informal
learning environments on childrens creative work together and their developing
musical identity. We plan to use video cameras to record young people rehearsing and playing popular music together to study the ways in which these issues
are interlinked.
For both of these proposed projects, further development of the musical
coding scheme will be needed. We are also interested in extending the use of
this system to other musical contexts, e.g. Jazz musicians improvising together
and other studies of performance and compositional/workshop settings.
There are certainly a wide range of potential areas of interest and further
research questions for psychologists interested in developing our understanding
of the intrinsically social nature of music and music teaching.
In terms of

developing

two

References
Aldridge,

D.

(1996).

Music

Silence. London: Jessica

Therapy
Kingsley.

Research and Practice in Medicine: From out

of

the

65

Downloaded from ijm.sagepub.com at UNIV ESTDL PAULISTA DE MESQIT on February 17, 2016

Montgomery, R. (1993). Friendship, transactive dialogues, and the development


reasoning. Social Development, 2 (3), 202-221.
Barrett, M. (1998). Researching childrens compositional process and products: connection
to music education practice In B. Sundin, G.E. McPherson & G. Folkestad, (Eds.) Children
Composing, pp. 10-35. Lund University: Malmo, Sweden.
Berkowitz, M.W., Gibbs, J.C. & Broughton, J. (1980). The relation of moral judgement disparity
to developmental effects of peer dialogue. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 26, 341-357.
Byrne, C. (1996). The use of pattern and echo in developing the creative abilities of secondary
school pupils. British Journal of Music Education, 13, 143-154.
Bruner, J. (1996). The culture of education. Cambridge, Mass: Harvard University Press.
Campbell, P.S. (1997). Music, the universal language: fact or fallacy. International Journal
of Music Education, 29, 32-40.
Folkestad, G. (1998). Musical Learning as cultural practice as exemplified in computer-based
creative music making. In B. Sundin, G.E. McPherson & G. Folkestad (Eds.) Children
Composing (pp. 97-135), Lund University: Malmo, Sweden.
Fitgerald, M., Joseph, A.P., Hayes, M. & ORegan, M. (1995). Lesire activities of adolescent
school children. Journal of Adolescence, 18, 349-358.
Frith, S. (1987). The industrialization of Popular music. In: J. Lull (Ed.) Popular Music and
communication (pp. 53-77), Sage: CA, USA.
Garvey, C. & Shantz, C.U. (1992). Conflict Talk: approaches adversative discourse. pp. 93-121
in C.U. Shantz & W. Hartup (Eds.) Conflict in Childhood and Adolescent Development,
New York: Cambridge University Press.
Geter, T. & Streisand, B. (1995). Recording sound sales: the music industry rocks and rolls
to the newest financial rhythms, US News and World Report, 70, 67-68.
Hakkinen, T.B. (in press). Early peer-collaboration in the classroom: communication devices
and strategies children use. In R. Joiner & K. Littleton (Eds.) Rethinking Collaborative
Learning. London: Free Association Press.
Hargreaves, D.J., Galton, M.J. & Robinson, S. (1996). Teachers assessment of primary chilAzmitia, M.

&

of scientific

drens classwork in the creative arts. Educational Research, 38 (2), 199-211.


Hargreaves & North (1997). The Social Psychology of Music. London: Oxford University Press.
Hartup, W.W. (1996). The company they keep: friendships and their developmental signifiChild Development, 67, 1-13.
R. & Littleton, K. (in press). Rethinking Collaborative Learning. London: Free
Association Press.
Kruger, A.C. (1992). The effect of peer- and adult-child transactive discussions on moral
reasoning. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 38, 191-211.
MacDonald, R., Miell, D. & Mitchel, L. (2000). An empirical investigation of the social and
musical processes involved in childrens collaborative compositions. Paper presented at
The 6
th International Conference on Music Perception and Cognition, Keele, UK.
MacDonald, R.A.R., Miell, D. & Morgan, L. (in press). Social processes and creative collaboration in children. European Journal of the Psychology of Education.
MacDonald, R.A.R., ODonnell, P.J. & Davies, J.B. (1999). Structured music workshops for
individuals with learning difficulty: an empirical investigation. Journal of Applied Research
in Intellectual Disabilities, 12 (3), 225-241.
MacDonald, R.A.R., ODonnell, P.J. & Dougall, G. (1996). Research considerations: bridging
the gap between qualitative and quantitative research methods. Paper presented at The
8th World Congress of Music Therapy. Hamburg, Germany.
Mertz, M. (1998). Some thoughts on music education in a global culture. International Journal
of Music Education, 32, 72-78.
Miell, D. & MacDonald, R.A.R. (in press). Childrens creative collaborations: The importance
of friendship when working together on a musical composition. Social Development.
Morgan, L. (1999). Childrens Collaborative Music Compositon: Communication through
Music. Unpublished dissertation, University of Leicester, UK.
North, A.C. & Hargreaves, D.J. (1997). Experimental aesthetics and everyday music listening.
In D.J. Hargreaves and A. C. North (Eds.) (pp. 84-107) The Social Psychology of Music.
London: Oxford University Press.
ODonnell, P.J., MacDonald, R.A.R. & Davies, J.B. (1999). Video analysis of the effects of
structured music workshops for individuals with leading difficulties. In D. Erdonmez &
cance.

Joiner,

66

Downloaded from ijm.sagepub.com at UNIV ESTDL PAULISTA DE MESQIT on February 17, 2016

R.R. Pratt (Eds.) Music Therapy & Music Medicine: Expanding Horizons (pp. 219-228).
Saint Louis: MMB Music.
ONiell, S.A. (1997). Gender and music. In D.J. Hargreaves & A.C. North (Eds.) The Social
Psychology of Music. (pp. 46-60). London: Oxford University Press.
Pellegrini, A.D., Galda, L. & Flor, D. (1997). Relationships, individual differences, and
childrens use of literate language. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 67, 139-152.
Rogoff, B. (1990). Apprenticeship in thinking: Cognitive development in social context.
Oxford University Press: Oxford.
Taebel, D.K. (1994). Changes in teaching and teachers: Effects of time and place. In H. Lees
(Ed.) Musical Connections: Tradition and Change. Proceedings of the 1994 World
Conference of the International Society for Music Education. (pp. 255-262). Auckland:

Uniprint.
Tafuri, J. (1999). Learning together through research method. International Journal of Music
Education, 33, 88-94.
Tarrant, M., North, A.C. & Hargreaves, D.J. (1999). Youth identity and music. Paper presented

symposium. Musical Identity. Milton Keynes, England.


S.D. & Roschelle, J. (1993). The construction of Shared Knowledge in Collaborative
Problem Solving. In S. Lajoie & S. Derry (Eds.) Computers as cognitive tools. Hillsdale,
NJ: Erlbaum.
Vygotsky, L.S. (1978). Mind and Society. Cambridge, MA: Harvard Press.
Webster, P.R. (1994). Thinking in sound: studying childrens improvisation. In H. Lees (Ed.)
Musical Connections: Tradition and Change. Proceedings of the 1994 World Conference
of the International Society for Music Education. (pp. 146-153). Auckland: Uniprint.
Zillman, D. & Gan, S. (1997). Musical taste in adolescence. In D.J. Hargreaves & A.C. North
(Eds.) The Social Psychology of Music. (pp. 161-188). London: Oxford University Press.
at BPS

Teasley,

Creativite et enseignement musical:


socialisation

limpact des facteurs de

la psychologie de la musique et son enseignement commencent


des facteurs de socialisation dans le d6veloppement de la musicalite
chez 1enfant, toutefois il reste encore beaucoup a d6couvrir sur ce sujet. Larticle met en
lumi6re deux projets de recherche sur les aspects de sociabilite intrinseques a la participation
musicale. Lune de ses tudes traite de la communication (a la fois verbale et musicale) qui
a lieu pendant les compositions musicales r6alis6es entre groupes damis et non-amis. La
deuxi6me etude porte sur les gains en musique et en communication obtenus par des
personnes ayant des difficultes qui ont particip6 aux ateliers Gamelan. Les implications de
ces recherches sur le plan 6ducatif ainsi que les directions a suivre pour progresser ces
tudes sont actuellement en cours dexamen.
Des ouvrages r6cents

sur

souligner limportance

Kreativitat und

Musikerziehung. Der Einflul3 sozialer Faktoren

Ver6ffentlichungen in Musikpsychologie und Musikpddagogik haben die Bedeutung


sozialer Faktoren auf die sich im Kinde entwickelnde Musikalitat hervorgehoben. Dennoch
bleibt fiir unser Verstandnis dieses Einflusses noch vieles offen. Dieser Beitrag greift zwei
Projekte auf, die die genuin soziale Natur musikalischer Teilhabe untersuchten. Die erste
Studie behandelte die (sprachliche und musikalische) Kommunikation wdhrend der
Komposition unter befreundeten und nicht befreundeten Personen. Die zweite Studie untersuchte den musikalischen und kommunikativen Gewinn fur behinderte Personen, die einen
Gamelan Workshop besuchten. Es werden pddagogische Konsequenzen aus diesen Studien
gezogen und Richtungen kiinftiger Forschung angedeutet.
Neuere

Creatividad y educaci6n musical: El


sociales
Recientes

publicaciones

musical han

relacionadas
resaltar la

comenzano a

impacto

de las variables

con la psicologia de la musica y de la educacion


importancia que los factores sociales tienen para la

67

Downloaded from ijm.sagepub.com at UNIV ESTDL PAULISTA DE MESQIT on February 17, 2016

musicalidad en desarrollo del nifio, pero aun queda mucho por comprender acerca de esta
influencia. Este trabajo resalta dos proyectos que investigaron aspectos de la naturaleza social
intrinseca de la participaci6n musical. Un estudio investig6 la comunicaci6n (tano verbal
como musical) que tuvo lugar durante las composiciones creadas por amigos y no amigo. El
segundo estudio investig6 los logros musicales y de comunicaci6n de individuos con necesidades especiales que asistieron a un taller sobre Gamelan. Se consideran las implicaciones
educacionales de estos estudios asi como orientaciones para futuras investigaciones.

68

Downloaded from ijm.sagepub.com at UNIV ESTDL PAULISTA DE MESQIT on February 17, 2016

Você também pode gostar