Você está na página 1de 6

Cognition in multiple sclerosis

Dawn W. Langdon
Royal Holloway, University of London, Egham, Surrey,
UK
Correspondence to Dr Dawn W. Langdon, Reader in
Neuropsychology, Royal Holloway, University of
London, Egham, Surrey TW20 0EX, UK
Tel: +44 1784 443956; e-mail: d.langdon@rhul.ac.uk
Current Opinion in Neurology 2011, 24:244249

Purpose of review
A broad overview of cognition in multiple sclerosis (MS) is provided, taking account of
its impact on the lives of patients, how cognitive impairment relates to disease and
magnetic resonance variables, which cognitive domains are most vulnerable, the
influence of depression and fatigue and what treatment options are available.
Recent findings
The current focus is on cognitive reserve, which seems to offer some protection from the
cognitive impact of MS. There is also considerable momentum with new MRI techniques
and growing interest in PET studies.
Summary
Cognition in MS is a priority for patients. Although understanding of the natural history of
MS cognitive deficits is reasonably well understood, treatment options require further
work before precise recommendations can be made on an individual basis.
Keywords
cognition, information processing, memory, multiple sclerosis
Curr Opin Neurol 24:244249
2011 Wolters Kluwer Health | Lippincott Williams & Wilkins
1350-7540

Introduction
Cognitive impairment is an important predictor of healthrelated quality of life at all stages of multiple sclerosis
(MS) [1]. It reduces physical independence [2], competence in daily activities [3], coping [4], symptom management [5], medication adherence [6] and rehabilitation
potential [7]. It also reduces driving safety. Cognitive
function is the strongest predictor of poor lane positioning
during high cognitive load and also slower response times
to other car movements [8]. Information processing speed
is the best predictor of overall driving performance, with
spatial learning and recall the best predictors of driving
accidents [9]. Unemployed MS patients are more likely to
have cognitive impairments than MS patients in employment [10]. In addition, declining performance over time
on tests of attention and verbal memory has been shown
to predict reduced employment status [11].

Disease subtypes and stages, pattern of


deficits
Studies of large, unselected samples of MS patients have
reported cognitive impairment prevalence rates between
40 and 70% [12]. Cognitive impairments have been
demonstrated at all stages and in all subtypes of the
disease. This includes the clinically isolated syndrome
(CIS), relapsingremitting multiple sclerosis (RRMS),
secondary progressive multiple sclerosis (SPMS), primary
progressive multiple sclerosis (PPMS) and even benign
multiple sclerosis (BMS) [13,14]. Interest has now
extended to the boundaries of the disease, with studies
1350-7540 2011 Wolters Kluwer Health | Lippincott Williams & Wilkins

of cognition at both the CIS stage [15] and very late stage
of MS [16]. Although longitudinal studies over a few years
often fail to show significant cognitive decline, it seems
that if observations are made over a sufficiently long timescale, cognitive impairment will emerge and progress
[17]. The more severe levels of cognitive impairment
tend to occur in the progressive phase [13,18] and decline
is most pronounced in progressive patients [19]. Although
almost any configuration of cognitive deficits can be
observed in MS [20], the typical profile is of impairments
in information processing speed, memory and often
executive skills, with relative preservation of language
[12].

Measurement and self-report


The measurement of an MS patients cognitive abilities
requires expertise, because the cluster of physical and
cognitive symptoms they have may well confound performance on cognitive tests (e.g. visual or other sensory
impairments) [21]. Two cognitive batteries are particularly widely used in clinical and research settings, both
having good psychometric properties and having been
constructed to be relatively robust to the effects of other
MS symptoms. They are Brief Repeatable Battery of
Neuropsychological tests (BRB-N) [22] and Minimal
Assessment of Cognitive Function in MS (MACFIMS)
[21]. MS patients self-report of cognitive impairments,
although important clinically, is unlikely to be related to
objective cognitive test performance, but rather linked to
depression. Relatives reports of patients cognitive function are more likely to be reliable [23].
DOI:10.1097/WCO.0b013e328346a43b

Copyright Lippincott Williams & Wilkins. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.

Cognition in multiple sclerosis Langdon

Information processing speed

For several decades, it has been established that MS


patients are most likely to fail tests of information
processing speed [24]. Cognitive tests of information
processing speed often involve working memory, the
system which maintains and manipulates information for
brief periods. Over several years, MS patients performance on tests of information processing speed declines
more rapidly than on other cognitive tasks [25]. Detailed
methodological investigations of performance on a range of
cognitive tests point towards speed of processing as the
unitary underlying deficit [26]. Information processing
speed is reduced in MS patients, and the slowness is more
pronounced on tasks that are explicitly timed, compared
with normal participants [27]. When cognitive load is
varied, increasing the interval between stimuli presentations allows more processing time and improves the
accuracy of MS patients performance [28].
There are two widely used tests of processing efficiency
and speed in MS: the Paced Auditory Serial Addition
Task (PASAT) [29] and the Symbol Digit Modalities
Test (SDMT, oral form) [30]. The PASAT requires the
patient to listen to a string of auditory digits and continuously add up the last two heard. In the SDMT, the
patient is presented with nine graphical symbols, each
paired with a single digit, serving as a key. Below are rows
of the symbols, randomly ordered, and the patient must
say the numbers that go with each digit. The tests have
similar psychometric properties [24]. Unsurprisingly,
there is evidence that the two tasks utilize slightly
different cerebral networks, with the PASAT activating
more frontal areas [31].

245

Key points
 Cognitive dysfunction is common in multiple
sclerosis (MS) and the typical profile is slowed
information processing and weakened memory.
 Cognitive impairment is only loosely related to
disease variables, but is more closely related to
magnetic resonance variables, especially atrophy.
 Cognitive reserve modulates the adverse effects of
MS pathology on cognitive function.
 Treatment options remain largely at an individualized, clinical stage.
abnormalities of hippocampal functional connectivity
before spatial memory impairment is evident [35].
Executive functions, language and visual perception

Executive functions relate to planning and goal-directed


behaviour. Drew et al. [36] recorded a range of executive
dysfunction, including disinhibition, poor fluency and
failure to shift sets. In total, 17% of their MS sample
demonstrated deficits of this type. Executive abilities
have been implicated in the completion of informationprocessing speeded tasks with high cognitive loading
[37,38]. Language functions are typically intact in MS,
but some subtle comprehension deficits have been
demonstrated. Weakened sentence comprehension has
been linked to slowed information processing [39]. Deficits in semantic memory have also been identified [40].
Studies have also suggested some visual processing
impairments [41,42].

Cognitive reserve
Memory

Long-term memory refers to the learning and recalling of


new information. MS patients are very likely to experience memory problems, with prevalence rates of 4065%
reported [32]. The most usual tests of verbal memory are
list learning tasks. A widely used test of verbal memory in
clinical and research contexts is the California Verbal
Learning Test-II (CVLT-II) [33]. This comprises a 16item shopping list, with four items belonging to each of
four categories, arranged randomly. The list is read aloud
five times in the same order to the patient. Patients are
required to recall as many items as possible, in any order,
after each reading of the list. Delayed recall, recognition
and distracter tasks are also involved. The validity of the
CVLT-II in MS is well established [34]. Visuospatial
memory is also affected in MS. A frequently used test
of visuospatial memory in MS is the 10/36 spatial recall
task [22]. This comprises 10 counters and a 6  6 grid.
The patient is shown a pattern, which must then be
reproduced from memory by placing the counters on
the grid. Tests of this type have been shown to predict
driving accidents [9]. MS patients sustain substantial

There is large interpatient variability in the pattern and


severity of cognitive deficits in MS. Recently, in an effort
to explain this variability, investigators have addressed the
question of cognitive reserve. Could individual differences
at baseline differentially protect MS patients from cognitive decline? Both years of education and reading level
improved predictions of cognitive decline over 5 years
[43]. Strikingly, the SDMT showed no change in the MS
group with more than 14 years of education, but declined in
the MS group with 14 years or less education. Cognitive
reserve has also been shown to moderate the effects of
atrophy on cognitive function in MS [44]. It has been
suggested that cognitive reserve relies on a default
network, involving the anterior and posterior cingulated
cortices [45].

Relation to disease variables


Cognitive status is typically only loosely related to disease duration [16] and physical disability [46], although
larger studies have shown significant relations [47]. Cognition can predict future disease progression. Cognitive

Copyright Lippincott Williams & Wilkins. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.

246 Demyelinating diseases

status at CIS event stage predicts conversion to MS [48]


and cognitive status at MS diagnosis predicts accumulation of physical disability [49]. Cognitive dysfunction
also predicts disease evolution in benign MS [46]. A
modest relation has been reported between cognition
and some immune measures [50]. The genetics of cognitive dysfunction in MS are undetermined, but current
studies suggest a link between apolipoprotein E epsilon4
and cognitive impairment [51].

shown to increase across subtype cohorts from CIS to


SPMS [66]. PET studies of MS cognition are becoming
more numerous. It is suggested that PET measurements
of brain metabolism may serve as better indicators of
axonal loss or dysfunction in MS lesions, normal appearing white matter and grey matter [67]. Measurement of
microglial activation offers particular promise [68].

Relation to depression
Magnetic resonance (MR) variables are significantly
related to cognitive performance, with total cerebral
lesion area typically achieving modest correlations and
atrophy stronger correlations, especially third ventricle
width [52,53]. Cortical lesions and atrophy have been
demonstrated to be independent predictors of cognitive
dysfunction [54], raising the possibility that they may
represent separate processes. An increase in cortical
lesions over time has been related to increased cognitive
impairment [55]. Recent investigations of cortical thinning raise the possibility of identifying more widespread
pathology underlying cognitive deficits [56]. Changes in
gadolinium enhancement have been significantly linked
to changes in PASAT scores in physically stable patients,
suggesting that inflammation interferes with cognitive
processing efficiency [57]. MR is now considered to
contribute to cognitive assessment [58]. In addition
to MR investigations that utilize traditional clinical tests
of cognition, some experimental cognitive tasks are offering new insights into how MS pathology affects awareness of the world. Using a visual mask after stimuli was
presented to calibrate conscious and nonconscious perception, Reuter et al. [59] were able to demonstrate that
conscious access was directly related to white-matter
bundle integrity, especially in prefrontal cortex. Another
interesting recent development has been the validation
of MR variables as predictors of future cognitive outcomes [6062].

Depression adversely affects cognitive function in MS.


The lifetime prevalence of major depression in MS is
close to 50% [69], making it more common in MS than
the general population or other neurological disorders. An
early meta-analysis demonstrated that depressed MS
patients had more difficulty on the PASAT, reflecting
either reduced information processing speed and/or
weaker working memory [70]. When well matched
groups of depressed MS patients, nondepressed MS
patients and healthy controls completed five cognitive
tasks that made demands on cognitive processing
capacity, the depressed MS group performed less well
than the other two groups on three tasks (PASAT, SDMT
and Visual Elevator Task). There was no difference
among the three groups on two cognitive capacity
demanding tasks (CVLT and 7/24 Spatial Recall test),
or on a reference battery on tasks with low cognitive
capacity demands [71]. In order to distinguish whether
the MS depressed groups poor performance was due to
reduced cognitive capacity or psychomotor slowing, the
same three groupings of MS depressed, MS nondepressed and healthy controls were compared on a word
span task (no working memory involvement) and a reading span task, in which the last word of each sentence had
to be recalled when prompted (working memory capacity
task). The MS-depressed group were worse on the reading span task, but not the word span task [72]. These
elegant and well designed experiments have shown that
depression in MS affects cognitive processing capacity
and, in particular, working memory.

Functional MRI and PET

Relation to fatigue

Functional MRI (fMRI) techniques allow the investigation of activation in real time of the cerebral cortex.
These investigations have demonstrated that there is an
increased recruitment of cortical networks, which are
hypothesized to preserve cognitive performance via a
compensation mechanism. In fact, these increased activations have been demonstrated in MS patients with
normal cognitive performance and interpreted as evidence of pathology that is hardly, or not yet, clinically
eloquent [63,64]. According to this model, when
increased activation can no longer keep pace with the
failing integrity of the cerebral cortex, cognitive performance plummets [65]. Additional activations have been

The effect of fatigue on cognition in MS is not so well


delineated, in part because the construct of fatigue is less
clear cut [73]. Fatigue is one of the most commonly
reported symptoms in MS [74]. MS patients often report
that fatigue impairs their cognitive function [75]. However, the relation between self-reported fatigue and
objective cognitive performance is complex and inconsistent. Processing speed, measured by Digit Symbol
Coding, has been demonstrated to be slower in MS
patients reporting fatigue than those who did not report
fatigue. Processing speed was inversely related to selfreported fatigue, but only in the fatigued MS group [76].
Self-reported fatigue was an independent predictor of

Relation to magnetic resonance

Copyright Lippincott Williams & Wilkins. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.

Cognition in multiple sclerosis Langdon

scores on one of a battery of objective cognitive tests


(Test of Attentional Performance) [77]. Recognition reaction time and accuracy, in a condition which maximized
executive demands, have also been linked to selfreported fatigue [78]. In contrast, several investigations
have failed to find a significant relation between selfreport fatigue and objective cognitive test performance
[79,80,81]. Mental fatigue relates differently to selfreport of cognitive impairment, compared to physical
fatigue [82]. However, a study has reported decline in
sustained task performance over time in MS patients
compared with healthy controls [83] and another reported
decline in performance of MS patients on a variety of
cognitive tests, following a lengthy, cognitively demanding task, again compared with healthy controls [84]. It
appears that MS patients may not be as good at sustaining
cognitive performance over time as healthy people are,
which is most likely the result of fatigue, but they are not
very good at reporting the levels of fatigue they experience that affect their cognitive performance.

Treatment: presentation
Changing the presentation format of information can to
some extent reduce the handicaps that MS cognitive
impairments impose. For example, given that information processing speed is a major factor, it is unsurprising that performance accuracy improves when MS
patients are given more time to process the information
[28]. Simple repetition is often not helpful, but increased
processing (encoding) and more organization of the
information facilitates remembering. Paring down information to the essentials and avoiding unnecessary or
unrelated details are advantageous [85].

positive outcomes [88,89]. The use of symptomatic


agents for cognitive decline is not broadly supported
by convincing studies [90].

Conclusion
Cognition in MS is a priority for patients. Assessment
tools are psychometrically sound and can be used effectively by a specialist neuropsychologist. New and developing MRI techniques offer further insights into how MS
pathology translates to cognitive performance. Understanding of influential related factors, such as depression,
fatigue and cognitive reserve, increase the precision of
evaluation and management. Much further work is
required before a systematic treatment approach can
be recommended.

References and recommended reading


Papers of particular interest, published within the annual period of review, have
been highlighted as:

of special interest
 of outstanding interest
Additional references related to this topic can also be found in the Current
World Literature section in this issue (pp. 303304).
1

Mitchell AJ, Benito-Leon J, Gonzalez JM, Rivera-Navarro J. Quality of life and


its assessment in multiple sclerosis: integrating physical and psychological
components of wellbeing. Lancet Neurol 2005; 4:556566.

Rao SM, Leo GJ, Ellington L, et al. Cognitive dysfunction in multiple sclerosis. II:
Impact on employment and social functioning. Neurology 1991; 41:692696.

Goverover Y, Genova HM, Hillary FG, DeLuca J. The relationship between


neuropsychological measures and the timed instrumental activities of daily
living task in multiple sclerosis. Mult Scler 2007; 13:636644.

Ehrensperger MM, Grether A, Romer G, et al. Neuropsychological dysfunction, depression, physical disability, and coping processes in families with a
parent affected by multiple sclerosis. Mult Scler 2008; 14:11061112.

Vahter L, Zopp I, Kreegipuu M, et al. Clean intermittent self-catheterization in


persons with multiple sclerosis: the influence of cognitive dysfunction. Mult
Scler 2009; 15:379384.

Bruce JM, Hancock LM, Arnett P, Lynch S. Treatment adherence in multiple


sclerosis: association with emotional status, personality, and cognition.
J Behav Med 2010; 33:219227.

Langdon DW, Thompson AJ. Multiple sclerosis: a preliminary study of selected


variables affecting rehabilitation outcome. Mult Scler 1999; 5:94100.

Marcotte TD, Rosenthal TJ, Roberts E, et al. The contribution of cognition and
spasticity to driving performance in multiple sclerosis. Arch Phys Med Rehabil
2008; 8:17531758.

Schultheis MT, Weisser V, Ang J, et al. Examining the relationship between


cognition and driving performance in multiple sclerosis. Arch Phys Med
Rehabil 2010; 91:465473.

Treatment: cognitive rehabilitation


The evidence base for cognitive rehabilitation in MS is
unfortunately fairly sparse and inconsistent. The evidence for treatment of slowed information processing
is inconclusive. The studies of memory rehabilitation
suggest that computerized programmes that are individually tailored to each patients memory function are
most effective. For executive skills, direct training by a
therapist seems to be most beneficial [85]. A recent
promising study requires cautious interpretation, because
the computer training intervention was compared to wait
list control condition [86].

247

10 Honarmand K, Akbar N, Kou N, Feinstein A. Predicting employment status in


multiple sclerosis patients: the utility of the MS functional composite. J Neurol
2011; 258:244249.
11 Morrow SA, Weinstock-Guttman B, Munschauer F, et al. Subjective fatigue is
not associated with cognitive impairment in multiple sclerosis: cross-sectional
and longitudinal analysis. Mult Scler 2009; 15:9981005.
12 Chiaravalloti ND, DeLuca J. Cognitive impairment in multiple sclerosis. Lancet
 Neurol 2008; 7:11391151.
A very thorough and authoritative review that is essential reading.

Treatment: medication
Most of the studies of the effects of disease-modifying
drugs on cognition in MS have not been designed to
evaluate cognition as a primary outcome, but rather have
selected participants on the basis of physical or MRI
outcomes [87]. To date, two studies have reported

13 Potagas C, Giogkaraki E, Koutsis G, et al. Cognitive impairment in different


MS subtypes and clinically isolated syndromes. J Neurol Sci 2008; 267:100
106.
14 Portaccio E, Stromillo ML, Goretti B, et al. Neuropsychological and MRI

measures predict short-term evolution in benign multiple sclerosis. Neurology
2009; 73:498503.
An important study that illuminates BMS from the cognitive and MRI perspective,
showing that BMS patients can have cognitive symptoms.

Copyright Lippincott Williams & Wilkins. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.

248 Demyelinating diseases


15 Khalil M, Enzinger C, Langkammer C, et al. Cognitive impairment in relation to
MRI metrics in patients with clinically isolated syndrome. Mult Scler 2011;
17:173180.

39 Grossman M, Robinson KM, Onishi K, et al. Sentence comprehension in


multiple sclerosis. Acta Neurol Scand 1995; 92:324331.

16 Smestad C, Sandvik L, Landr NI, Celius EG. Cognitive impairment after three
decades of multiple sclerosis. Eur J Neurol 2010; 17:499505.

40 Laatu S, Hamalainen P, Revonsuo A, et al. Semantic memory deficit in multiple


sclerosis; impaired understanding of conceptual meanings. J Neurol Sci
1999; 162:152161.

17 Amato MP, Ponziani G, Siracusa G, Sorbi S. Cognitive dysfunction in earlyonset multiple sclerosis: a reappraisal after 10 years. Arch Neurol 2001;
58:16021606.

41 Vleugels L, Lafosse C, van Nunen A, et al. Visuoperceptual impairment in


multiple sclerosis patients diagnosed with neuropsychological tasks. Mult
Scler 2000; 6:241254.

18 Denney DR, Sworowski LA, Lynch SG. Cognitive impairment in three subtypes of multiple sclerosis. Arch Clin Neuropsychol 2005; 20:967981.

42 Laatu S, Revonsuo A, Hamalainen P, et al. Visual object recognition in multiple


sclerosis. J Neurol Sci 2001; 185:7788.

19 Bergendal G, Fredrikson S, Almkvist O. Selective decline in information


processing in subgroups of multiple sclerosis: an 8-year longitudinal study.
Eur Neurol 2007; 57:193202.

43 Benedict RH, Morrow SA, Weinstock Guttman B, et al. Cognitive reserve



moderates decline in information processing speed in multiple sclerosis
patients. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2010; 16:829835.
A very convincing careful study of cognitive reserve and its effects over 5 years.

20 Prakash RS, Snook EM, Lewis JM, et al. Cognitive impairments in relapsingremitting multiple sclerosis: a meta-analysis. Mult Scler 2008; 14:12501261.
21 Benedict RH, Fischer JS, Archibald CJ, et al. Minimal neuropsychological
assessment of MS patients: a consensus approach. Clin Neuropsychol 2002;
16:381397.
22 Rao SM, the Cognitive Function Study Group of the National Multiple
Sclerosis Society. A manual for the brief repeatable battery of neuropsychological tests in multiple sclerosis. Milwaukee: Medical College of Wisconsin;
1990.
23 Kinsinger SW, Lattie E, Mohr DC. Relationship between depression, fatigue,

subjective cognitive impairment, and objective neuropsychological functioning in patients with multiple sclerosis. Neuropsychology 2010; 24:573
580.
A study which clearly illustrates how depression and fatigue influence self-report of
cognitive function, but are not related to objective cognitive performance.
24 Strober L, Englert J, Munschauer F, et al. Sensitivity of conventional memory

tests in multiple sclerosis: comparing the Rao Brief Repeatable Neuropsychological Battery and the Minimal Assessment of Cognitive Function in MS.
Mult Scler 2009; 15:10771084.
A powerful psychometric exploration of the two main MS cognitive batteries.
25 Denney DR, Lynch SG, Parmenter BA. A 3-year longitudinal study of cognitive
impairment in patients with primary progressive multiple sclerosis: speed
matters. J Neurol Sci 2008; 267:129136.
26 Denney DR, Lynch SG, Parmenter BA, Horne N. Cognitive impairment in
relapsing and primary progressive multiple sclerosis: mostly a matter of
speed. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2004; 10:948956.
27 Denney DR, Gallagher KS, Lynch SG. Deficits in processing speed in patients
with multiple sclerosis: evidence from explicit and covert measures. Arch Clin
Neuropsychol 2011; 26:110119.
28 Leavitt VM, Lengenfelder J, Moore NB, et al. The relative contributions of
processing speed and cognitive load to working memory accuracy in multiple
sclerosis. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 2011 [Epub ahead of print].
29 Gronwall DMA. Paced Auditory Serial Addition Task: a measure of recovery
from concussion. Percept Mot Skills 1977; 44:367373.
30 Smith A. Symbol Digit Modalities Test (SDMT). Manual (revised). Los
Angeles: Western Psychological Services; 1982.
31 Forn C, Belenguer A, Belloch V, et al. Anatomical and functional differences
between the Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test and the Symbol Digit
Modalities Test. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 2011; 33:4250.
32 Rao SM, Grafman J, DiGuilio D, et al. Memory dysfunction in multiple
sclerosis: its relation to working memory, semantic encoding and implicit
learning. Neuropsychology 1993; 7:364374.
33 Delis DC, Kramer JH, Kaplan E, Ober BA. California Verbal Learning Test
(second edition) (CVLT-II). San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation;
2000.
34 Stegen S, Stepanov I, Cookfair D, et al. Validity of the California Verbal
Learning Test-II in multiple sclerosis. Clin Neuropsychol 2010; 24:189202.
35 Roosendaal SD, Hulst HE, Vrenken H, et al. Structural and functional
hippocampal changes in multiple sclerosis patients with intact memory
function. Radiology 2010; 255:595604.
36 Drew M, Tippett LJ, Starkey NJ, Isler RB. Executive dysfunction and
cognitive impairment in a large community-based sample with Multiple
Sclerosis from New Zealand: a descriptive study. Arch Clin Neuropsychol
2008; 23:119.
37 Drew MA, Starkey NJ, Isler RB. Examining the link between information
processing speed and executive functioning in multiple sclerosis. Arch Clin
Neuropsychol 2009; 24:4758.
38 Macniven JA, Davis C, Ho MY, et al. Stroop performance in multiple sclerosis:
information processing, selective attention, or executive functioning? J Int
Neuropsychol Soc 2008; 14:805814.

44 Sumowski JF, Chiaravalloti N, Wylie G, Deluca J. Cognitive reserve moderates


the negative effect of brain atrophy on cognitive efficiency in multiple sclerosis. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2009; 15:606612.
45 Sumowski JF, Wylie GR, Deluca J, Chiaravalloti N. Intellectual enrichment is

linked to cerebral efficiency in multiple sclerosis: functional magnetic resonance imaging evidence for cognitive reserve. Brain 2010; 133:362374.
An important study that explains functional anatomy underlying cognitive reserve.
46 Amato MP, Portaccio E, Goretti B, et al. Relevance of cognitive deterioration
in early relapsing-remitting MS: a 3-year follow-up study. Mult Scler 2010;
16:14741482.
47 Lynch SG, Parmenter BA, Denney DR. The association between cognitive
impairment and physical disability in multiple sclerosis. Mult Scler 2005;
11:469476.
48 Zipoli V, Goretti B, Hakiki B, et al. Cognitive impairment predicts conversion to
multiple sclerosis in clinically isolated syndromes. Mult Scler 2010; 16:62
67.
49 Deloire M, Ruet A, Hamel D, et al. Early cognitive impairment in multiple
sclerosis predicts disability outcome several years later. Mult Scler 2010;
16:581587.
50 Heesen C, Schulz KH, Fiehler J, et al. Correlates of cognitive dysfunction in
multiple sclerosis. Brain Behav Immun 2010; 24:11481155.
51 Ghaffar O, Feinstein A. APOE epsilon4 and cognitive dysfunction in multiple

sclerosis: a review. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 2010; 22:155165.
Recent review of relation between cognition and apolipoprotein E in MS.
52 Tiemann L, Penner IK, Haupts M, et al. Cognitive decline in multiple sclerosis:
impact of topographic lesion distribution on differential cognitive deficit
patterns. Mult Scler 2009; 15:11641174.
53 Grassiot B, Desgranges B, Eustache F, Defer G. Quantification and clinical

relevance of brain atrophy in multiple sclerosis: a review. J Neurol 2009;
256:13971412.
A thorough review of MRI and cerebral atrophy in MS, with a section on cognition
and atrophy.
54 Calabrese M, Agosta F, Rinaldi F, et al. Cortical lesions and atrophy associated with cognitive impairment in relapsing-remitting multiple sclerosis. Arch
Neurol 2009; 66:11441150.
55 Roosendaal SD, Moraal B, Pouwels PJ, et al. Accumulation of cortical lesions
in MS: relation with cognitive impairment. Mult Scler 2009; 15:708714.
56 Calabrese M, Rinaldi F, Mattisi I, et al. Widespread cortical thinning char
acterizes patients with MS with mild cognitive impairment. Neurology 2010;
74:321328.
A classic study investigating relation of cortical thinning to other MR parameters
and cognition.
57 Bellmann-Strobl J, Wuerfel J, Aktas O, et al. Poor PASAT performance

correlates with MRI contrast enhancement in multiple sclerosis. Neurology
2009; 73:16241627.
An important innovative study that links changes in gadolinium enhancement to
changes in cognition.
58 Filippi M, Rocca MA, Benedict RH, et al. The contribution of MRI in assessing
 cognitive impairment in multiple sclerosis. Neurology 2010; 75:2121
2128.
An important review that emphasizes importance of MRI and cognition in MS.
59 Reuter F, Del Cul A, Malikova I, et al. White matter damage impairs access to
consciousness in multiple sclerosis. Neuroimage 2009; 44:590599.
60 Summers M, Swanton J, Fernando K, et al. Cognitive impairment in multiple
sclerosis can be predicted by imaging early in the disease. J Neurol Neurosurg
Psychiatry 2008; 79:955958.
61 De Groot V, Beckerman H, Uitdehaag BM, et al. Physical and cognitive
functioning after 3 years can be predicted using information from the diagnostic process in recently diagnosed multiple sclerosis. Arch Phys Med
Rehabil 2009; 90:14781488.

Copyright Lippincott Williams & Wilkins. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.

Cognition in multiple sclerosis Langdon


62 Penny S, Khaleeli Z, Cipolotti L, et al. Early imaging predicts later cognitive
impairment in primary progressive multiple sclerosis. Neurology 2010;
74:545552.
63 Amann M, Dossegger LS, Penner IK, et al. Altered functional adaptation to
attention and working memory tasks with increasing complexity in relapsingremitting multiple sclerosis patients. Hum Brain Mapp 2010 [Epub ahead of
print].
64 Lesage E, Apps MA, Hayter AL, et al. Cerebellar information processing in
relapsing-remitting multiple sclerosis (RRMS). Behav Neurol 2010; 23:39
49.
65 Penner IK, Rausch M, Kappos L, et al. Analysis of impairment related
functional architecture in MS patients during performance of different attention tasks. J Neurol 2003; 250:461472.
66 Loitfelder M, Fazekas F, Petrovic K, et al. Reorganization in cognitive networks

with progression of multiple sclerosis: insights from fMRI. Neurology 2011;
76:526533.
A study of activation patterns in different MS subtypes.
67 Srensen PS, Jnsson A, Mathiesen HK, et al. The relationship between MRI
and PET changes and cognitive disturbances in MS. J Neurol Sci 2006;
245:99102.
68 Herholz K. Cognitive dysfunction and emotionalbehavioural changes in
MS: the potential of positron emission tomography. J Neurol Sci 2006;
245:913.
69 Feinstein A. Mood disorders in multiple sclerosis and the effects on cognition.

J Neurol Sci 2006; 245:6366.
A good review of how depression affects cognition in MS.
70 Thornton AE, Raz N. Memory impairment in multiple sclerosis: a quantitative
review. Neuropsychology 1997; 11:357366.
71 Arnett PA, Higginson CI, Voss WD, et al. Depressed mood in multiple
sclerosis: relationship to capacity-demanding memory and attentional functioning. Neuropsychology 1999; 13:434446.
72 Arnett PA, Higginson CI, Voss WD, et al. Depression in multiple sclerosis:
relationship to working memory capacity. Neuropsychology 1999; 13:546
556.
73 Bol Y, Duits AA, Hupperts RM, et al. The psychology of fatigue in patients with
multiple sclerosis: a review. J Psychosom Res 2009; 66:311.
74 Zajicek JP, Ingram WM, Vickery J, et al. Patient-orientated longitudinal study of
multiple sclerosis in south west England (The South West Impact of Multiple
Sclerosis Project, SWIMS). 1: Protocol and baseline characteristics of
cohort. BMC Neurol 2010; 10:88.
75 Krupp LB, Alvarez LA, LaRocca NG, Scheinberg LC. Fatigue in multiple
sclerosis. Arch Neurol 1988; 45:435437.

249

76 Andreasen AK, Spliid PE, Andersen H, Jakobsen J. Fatigue and processing


speed are related in multiple sclerosis. Eur J Neurol 2010; 17:212218.
77 Weinges-Evers N, Brandt AU, Bock M, et al. Correlation of self-assessed
fatigue and alertness in multiple sclerosis. Mult Scler 2010; 16:11341140.
78 Holtzer R, Foley F. The relationship between subjective reports of fatigue and
executive control in multiple sclerosis. J Neurol Sci 2009; 281:4650.
79 Parmenter BA, Denney DR, Lynch SG. The cognitive performance of patients
with multiple sclerosis during periods of high and low fatigue. Mult Scler
2003; 9:111118.
80 Bailey A, Channon S, Beaumont JG. The relationship between subjective
fatigue and cognitive fatigue in advanced multiple sclerosis. Mult Scler 2007;
13:7380.
81 Morrow SA, Drake A, Zivadinov R, et al. Predicting loss of employment over

three years in multiple sclerosis: clinically meaningful cognitive decline. Clin
Neuropsychol 2010; 24:11311145.
Cognition predicts employment status after 3 years.
82 Bol Y, Duits AA, Hupperts RM, et al. The impact of fatigue on cognitive
functioning in patients with multiple sclerosis. Clin Rehabil 2010; 24:854
862.
83 Schwid SR, Tyler CM, Scheid EA, et al. Cognitive fatigue during a test
requiring sustained attention: a pilot study. Mult Scler 2003; 9:503508.
84 Krupp LB, Elkins LE. Fatigue and declines in cognitive functioning in multiple
sclerosis. Neurology 2000; 55:934939.
85 Langdon DW. Cognitive rehabilitation in multiple sclerosis. In: Kesselring J,
Comi G, Thompson A, editors. Multiple sclerosis: recovery of function and
neurorehabilitation. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press; 2010. pp.
176182.
86 Flavia M, Stampatori C, Zanotti D, et al. Efficacy and specificity of intensive
cognitive rehabilitation of attention and executive functions in multiple sclerosis. J Neurol Sci 2010; 288:101105.
87 Langdon D. Therapeutic approaches disease modifying treatments. In: Amato
MP, editor. Cognitive impairment in multiple sclerosis. Elsevier (in press).
88 Fischer JS, Priore RL, Jacobs LD, et al., Multiple Sclerosis Collaborative
Research Group. Neuropsychological effects of interferon beta-1a in relapsing multiple sclerosis. Ann Neurol 2000; 48:885892.
89 Kappos L, Freedman MS, Polman CH, et al. Long-term effect of early
treatment with interferon beta-1b after a first clinical event suggestive of
multiple sclerosis: 5-year active treatment extension of the phase 3 BENEFIT
trial. Lancet Neurol 2009; 8:987997.
90 Christodoulou C, MacAllister WS, McLinskey NA, Krupp LB. Treatment of
cognitive impairment in multiple sclerosis: is the use of acetylcholinesterase
inhibitors a viable option? CNS Drugs 2008; 22:8797.

Copyright Lippincott Williams & Wilkins. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.

Você também pode gostar