Você está na página 1de 13

The Influence of Individualism, Collectivism, and Locus of

Control on Environmental Beliefs and Behavior


John A. McCarty and LJ. Shrum
This study investigates the influence of value orientations measured at the individual level
(individualism, collectivism, and locus of control) and of economic status on environmental
beliefs and behavior. Structural equation modeling reveals that the preferred model is one
in which the value orientations and economic status influence beliefs about recycling, which
in turn influence recycling behavior, but the influence of the value orientations and eco-
nomic status differs as a function of the type of environmental belief. Individualism and eco-
nomic status are related to beliefs about the inconvenience of recycling; collectivism and
locus of control are related to beliefs about the importance of recycling. Moreover, specific
beliefs about the importance of recycling have both a direct influence on recycling behavior
and an effect that is mediated by beliefs about the inconvenience of recycling. The authors
present the implications of the study for public policy and marketing communication efforts.

A s the environment has become an increasingly visi-


ble social and political concern in the last 20 years,
issues in the environmental domain have begun to
attract the attention of researchers in marketing and the
We propose that there are at least two interrelated reasons
it has been so difficult to understand and predict pro-
environmental behavior. First, the nature of the behavior
and the motivations underlying it are quite different from
social sciences (e.g., Alwitt and Pitts 1996; Berger 1997; most types of consumer purchase behavior. Most models of
Berger and Corbin 1992; Pieters et al. 1998; Shrum, purchase intention assume that the consumer has some level
McCarty, and Lowrey 1995). Examples of such issues of self-interest and that the purchase of a particular product
include understanding consumer motivations underlying the or service is driven by an assessment of the benefits that
purchase of environmentally friendly products and services would accrue directly to the individual or household, rela-
(e.g., Bagozzi and Dabholkar 1994), explicating the relation tive to the assessment of costs. Unlike most consumer
between general psychological constructs and environmen- behaviors, however, the direct, extrinsic benefits that accrue
tal behavior (e.g., values, attitudes, beliefs; Berger and from proenviron mental behaviors (e.g., a noticeably better
Corbin 1992; Biswas et al. 2000; Dietz, Stern, and Guag- environment) are at best long-term for the individual and
nano 1998), and pinpointing the antecedents of post- may in fact never be realized.' The costs, conversely, tend
pure hase/postcon sumption behavior {e.g., household waste to be realized immediately. Products thai are made from
disposal, product reuse; Alwitt and Pitts 1996; Shrum, recycled material may be more expensive or perceived as
McCarty, and Lowrey 1995). inferior, and recycling requires certain behaviors that may
For the most part, understanding and predicting proenvi- be perceived as inconvenient. Therefore, although there are
ronmental behavior has proved to be remarkably difficult. benefits and costs associated with proenviron mental behav-
One of the constant refrains in both the academic and popu- iors, the nature of the benefits and the assessment of them
lar press is that even though it seems as if nearly everyone relative to the costs is likely a different matter than in the
has positive attitudes and beliefs regarding the environment, case of most consumption activities (Thogersen 1996).
proenvironmental behavior (purchase, disposal) has not Second, because the nature of the benefits of proenviron-
been correspondingly ubiquitous. Public opinion polls con- mental behaviors and their assessment relative to their costs
sistently fmd that a vast majority of people say that their may be different from those of other consumer behaviors,
purchases are infiuenced by environmental concerns (Chase the antecedents of proenvironmental beliefs may differ from
and Smith 1992; Dagnoli 1991), indicate that they would the antecedents commonly investigated by consumer
pay more for environmentally friendly products (Dagnoli researchers. We propose that fundamental beliefs people
1991; Hume and Strnad 1989), and classify themselves as hold that pertain to their interaction with the world around
environmentalists (Gutfeld 1991). Yet few "green" products them and with other people influence the formation of their
have been particularly successful (Reitman 1992), and beliefs about environmental issues and their propensity to
increases in recycling over the years have not been impres-
sive (Porter, Leeming, and Dwyer 1995).
'Monetary incentives for engaging in recycling behaviors would be
direct and immediate benefits to individuals for engaging in the behavior.
Biswas and colleagues (2000) note, however, that [he use of economic
incentives, such as high redemption values and coupons, has decreased in
JOHN A. MCCARTY is an assistant professor. School of Manage- recent years because of the reduction in value of many recyclable materi-
ment, George Mason University. LJ. SHRUM is an associate pro- als. Thus, direcl and immediate monetary incentives to individuals for
fessor, Department of Marketing, Rutgers University. engaging in recycling behavior have declined as recycling programs have
developed.

Vol. 20(1)
Spring 2001, 93-104 Joumal of Public Policy & Marketing 93
94 Environmental Beliefs and Behavior

engage in proenvironmental behaviors. In particular, individualistic or coUectivistic in their orientation (Triandis


because the benefits that accrue from proenvironmental 1994). At the individual level, however, research suggests
behaviors are future oriented and unlikely to benefit directly that individualism and collectivism represent separate
the person performing the behavior, it is likely that funda- dimensions '(Triandis and Gelfand 1998). Both individual-
mental concepts that relate to people's beliefs about their ism and collectivism can exist within the same culture
ability to influence future outcomes and their desire to pro- (Sinha and Tripathi 1994; Triandis 1994), and a person may
vide benefits for others may influence proenvironmental possess both individualistic and coUectivistic tendencies
beliefs and behaviors. (Sinha and Tripathi 1994; Triandis 1989, 1994). Different
In this article, we develop and test a model of the rela- situations may cause a person to sample individualistic or
tionship between such fundamental beliefs (or value orien- coUectivistic aspects of the self (Traflmow, Triandis, and
tations) and proenvironmental beliefs and behaviors. We Goto 1991). Thus, a person may believe in personal initia-
propose that three basic values that relate to people's inter- tive and independence yet also value group harmony and
action with the world and with others will influence pro- sharing. Because the present study involves individual-level
environmental behavior. Two of these constructs, individu- activity, we treat individualism and collectivism as two sep-
alism and collectivism, relate to basic beliefs about humans' arate dimensions that pertain to the ways in which individu-
relationships and interactions with other humans. The third als relate to other individuals.
construct, locus of control, is a basic belief about humans'
relationships and interactions with their environment. We Locus of Control
test this model for a particular proenvironmental behavior Locus of control refers to the extent to which people believe
(recycling) and beliefs related to this behavior. Because that they have the ability to affect outcomes through their
other factors (e.g., socioeconomic variables) have been own actions (Rotter 1966). On the one hand, some people
shown to relate to recycling (Berger 1997), we test the fun- believe that they have substantial infiuence over their lives
damental beliefs of interest in conjunction with the variable and that their actions infiuence particular outcomes. These
of economic status. people are said to have an internal locus of control (inter-
nals). They typically perceive themselves to have control
Background over their future and believe that outcomes are related to the
work they put into them (Lefcourt 1991). On the other hand,
Fundamental Beliefs: Value Orientations people who believe that they are relatively powerless and
Individualism, collectivism, and locus of control are basic have little infiuence over outcomes are said to have an exter-
beliefs that people hold with respect to their interaction with nal locus of control (externals). Triandis (1984) has noted
others and the world around them. In that sense, these basic that this psychological construct of locus of control is equiv-
beliefs can be referred to as value orientations (Kluckhohn alent to the cultural-level value orientation dimension of
1951), which have been described as fundamental beliefs beliefs about human-nature interactions, which relates to
that cultural groups or individuals have that assist in the the extent to which a cultural group believes that it is supe-
adaptation to their physical and social environment. rior to nature, lives in harmony with nature, or is subjugated
Although these value orientations have often been investi- to nature.
gated by social scientists at the cultural level (e.g., differ-
ences in individualism across countries), the current investi- Beliefs Related to Recycling
gation focuses on these beliefs at the individual, Although we expect that these abstract, fundamental value
psychological level. orientations (individualism, collectivism, and locus of con-
trol) will relate to people's propensity to engage in recy-
Individualism and Collectivism cling, we do not expect that their effects on behavior will be
Individualism and collectivism have been identified as ori- direct. Instead, we expect that these fundamental values will
entations that can be taken with respect to a person's or infiuence more specific psychological constructs (beliefs
group's relationship to others. Individualism can be broadly about recycling), which in turn will infiuence behavior.
characterized as the tendency to value the individual over Research in a variety of areas has shown that specific atti-
the group and give priority to persona! goals over group tudes and beliefs often mediate the values-behavior relation
goals (Triandis 1989). Individualism stresses individual ini- (e.g., Alwitt and Pitts 1996; Homer and Kahle 1988;
tiative, a greater focus on the self, and emotional indepen- McCarty and Shrum 1994).
dence (Hofstede 1980). It also emphasizes self-reliance and Two particular belief constructs that have consistently
freedom of choice (Bellah et al. 1985), stresses individual been shown to relate to recycling are a general attitude or
rights over duties, and puts emphasis on cost-benefit analy- belief about the importance of recycling and a specific belief
ses in determining behavior (Triandis 1994). In contrast, about the inconvenience of recycling (Hines, Hungerford,
collectivism emphasizes the goals of the group over per- and Tomera 1987; McCarty and Shrum 1994; Vining and
sonal goals, stresses conformity and in-group harmony, and Ebreo 1990). Therefore, we consider these two belief con-
defines the self in relation to the group (Triandis 1995). Col- structs as ones that potentially mediate the relations between
lectivism emphasizes sharing, duties, and obligations (Hof- value orientations and recycling behaviors.
stede 1980). Importance and Inconvenience can be contrasted on three
When studied at tbe cultural level, individualism and col- important and related dimensions that pertain to how they
lectivism are considered to represent opposite ends of one potentially mediate relations between the abstract, funda-
continuum, and cultures are often described as being either mental beliefs and specific behaviors. First, the importance
Journal of Public Policy & Marketing 95

of recycling relates to the benefits of engaging in the behav- vidualistic people also place greater importance on the rela-
ior (e.g., a cleaner environment), whereas inconvenience tion between their behavior and their own needs and beliefs
focuses on the costs (e.g., the time required to prepare mate- (Leung and Bond 1984). On the other hand, collectivism
rials for collection). Second, the importance of recycling is tends to promote a consideration of the implications of peo-
a long-term consideration, but inconvenience is primarily ple's behavior for others. The behavior of people who are
short-term in nature. As we have noted, the benefits of the collectivistic tends to be driven by social norms, and these
environment will generally only be realized in the future, people show a willingness to share scarce resources (Sinha
and therefore people may not see these positive results in and Verma 1987). Thus, it is reasonable that a behavior such
any immediate way, whereas the costs to the individual in as recycling may be a function of individualism and collec-
terms of engaging in the behavior are relatively immediate. tivism. Individualism tends to focus a person's attention
Third, the two belief constructs differ in their level of toward immediate benefits relative to costs. In the short run,
abstraction. Because the importance of recycling relates to however, extrinsic benefits are few relative to the perceived
long-term rewards for the environment and society, these costs (e.g., inconvenience). Collectivist tendencies should
beliefs tend to be more abstract and general in nature than focus a person toward group benefits, even if these benefits
beliefs about the inconvenience of recycling, which focus on are not immediate.
immediate costs to the individual and tend to be more con- There is some limited empirical evidence for these con-
crete and specific. jectures. McCarty and Shrum (1994), using a student sam-
ple, found that collectivism is positively related to the level
Economic Status of reported recycling behavior, and Hopper and Nielsen
Although the individual-level values and beliefs we have (1991) provided evidence that participation in a community
described are the main focus of the study, other variables recycling program is influenced by social norms. Dunlap
have been shown to be related to recycling. Several studies and Van Liere (1984) found that traditional American (indi-
have investigated the relationship of demographic variables vidualistic) values are negatively related to beliefs about
and recycling behavior, showing mixed results with respect resource conservation.
to the impact of these variables on environmental behaviors Therefore, people who are more individualistic will tend
(for reviews, see Berger 1997; Shrum, Lowrey, and to focus on personal goals, and their behavior will tend to be
McCarly 1994). Berger (1997) argues that the role of guided by immediate benefits relative to costs. Such people
socioeconomic factors such as income and education may are likely to consider recycling costly and as having few
be a complex one, and otber factors may mediate the rela- immediate benefits, and they are unlikely to perceive recy-
tionship between these variables and behavior. For example, cling as contributing to tbeir personal goals. Given this, we
socioeconomic factors may facilitate conditions for recy- predict the following:
cling; Wealthier neighborhoods may have more access to
recycling facilities, and a household's recycling may there- H|:The degree of individualism will relate positively to the
beliefs about the inconvenience of recycling. People who are
fore be as much a function of the economic conditions of the
more individualistic will tend to believe that recycling is more
community as of the household's own income level. A study inconvenient than will those who are less individualistic.
by Jacobs, Bailey, and Crews (1984) supports this notion,
fmding that participation in recycling was related to the People who are more collectivistic are more concerned
housing prices of a neighborhood. Along these same lines, with the good of the group and focus on group goals, shar-
Berger also notes that perceptions of the inconvenience of ing, and duty. Because collectivism relates to the impact of
recycling may vary as a function of socioeconomic status: It actions on the collective (society) and on the future, people
may simply be more inconvenient for people living in who are more collectivistic should consider recycling more
smaller houses to store recyclable materials until the day important than will those who are less collectivistic. There-
they can be left for curbside pickup. fore, we predict the following:
H2: The degree of collectivism will relate positively to the
Proposed Model beliefs about the importance of recycling. People who are
The research just reviewed provides information that is use- more collectivistic will tend to believe that recycling is
ful in developing specific hypotheses about how the con- more important than will those who are less collectivistic.
structs of individualism, collectivism, and locus of con- Implicit in these two hypotheses is the assumption that
trolall considered at the psychological levelas well as individualism and collectivism are independent constructs
the construct of economic status might influence recycling rather than opposite ends of a single continuum, consistent
behavior through the recycling beliefs about importance and with previous research on these constructs at the psycholog-
inconvenience. ical level.
Individualism, Collectivism, and Recycling Locus of Control and Recycling
The nature of the two constructs, individualism and collec- Several studies have shown that locus of control (or variables
tivism, suggests that tbey may relate in different ways to the similar to it) is associated with environmental issues. Balder-
propensity for people to engage in proenvironmental behav- jahn (1988) found that greater perceived ideological control
iors. On the one hand, Sinha and Verma (1987, p. 124) note is positively related to attitudes toward ecologically con-
that individualism promotes "contractual relationships scious living and environmentally friendly purchase behav-
which are based on the principles of exchange. People cal- ior, and Schwepker and Cornwell (1991) reported evidence
culate profit and loss before engaging in a behavior." Indi- showing that an internal locus of control is related to the
96 Environmental Beliefs and Behavior

propensity to purchase ecologically packaged products. In a tend to report that they recycle less than will those who
meta-analysis of previous work on environmental behavior, believe recycling is less inconvenient.
Hines, Hungerford, and Tomera (1987) concluded that an Figure 1 shows the hypothesized relationships of the pro-
internal locus of control is positively related to environmen- posed model. We expect that the influence of the value ori-
tally responsible behavior. As we have noted, many polls entations and economic status (the four exogenous vari-
show that most people have a positive attitude toward the ables) on recycling behavior will be mediated by recycling
environment. An internal locus of control might provide peo- beliefs (importance and inconvenience) and in the specific
ple with the belief that they can do something to change the ways shown in the figure. We do not expect otber relation-
environment; thus, internals should be more likely to believe ships between the exogenous variables and the mediated
that their actions with respect to the environment are worth- variables to be present. Thus, individualism and economic
while. Therefore, we predict the following: status are not expected to affect the importance of recycling,
H3: An internal locus of control will relate positively to the beliefs and collectivism and locus of control are not expected to
about the importance of recycling. People who are more inter- relate directly to the inconvenience of recycling.
nal will tend to believe that recycling is more important than
will those who are less intemal (more extemal). Method
Economic Status and Recycling Sample and Procedure
Given previous research that suggests a relationship of the The data for the study were collected through a mail ques-
economic status of people and their communities with recy- tionnaire sent to a stratified random sample of adults in a
cling, we expect that the income of a family, as well as the heavily populated Midwestern state. Only people who lived
economic level of a community, will relate to perceptions of in communities in which curbside recycling was available
the inconvenience of recycling, thereby indirectly influenc- were included in the sampling frame. Therefore, cities and
ing the likelihood that a person will engage in recycling suburbs in the state were first selected on the basis of the
behavior. Specifically, we predict tbe following: availability of recycling. Individuals within these strata
were then randomly selected for inclusion in the study.
H4: Economic status will he negatively related to perceptions of Because of the need to restrict sampling to strata that pro-
the inconvenience of recycling. People with lower incomes vided recycling opportunities, neitber the most rural areas of
and/or living in areas of lower econorfiic status will believe the state nor the major city in the state was represented in the
recycling is more inconvenient than will those with higher sample. In general, the sample was obtained from ZIP codes
incomes and/or living in areas of higher economic status.
in cities of 10,000 to 250,000 people and from ZIP codes of
the suburbs of the major urban areas.
Beliefs About Recycling and Behaviors Each member of the sample was sent a questionnaire with
The differences in the nature of beliefs about the importance a cover letter explaining that the study was designed to
and inconvenience of recycling lead to expectations about investigate the beliefs and activities of the people living in
how these beliefs might relate to recycling behaviors. Given the state. The questionnaire included items on a variety of
the differences in the level of abstraction and specificity of topics, including those of interest for the curTent study.
importance and inconvenience, we expect that the more gen- Therefore, although several questions pertained to recy-
eral beliefs about importance will influence the more specific cling, the focus of the study was masked to a large degree by
beliefs about inconvenience. That is, as people develop more the inclusion of items unrelated to environmental issues.
positive beliefs about the long-term importance of recycling, Following the recommendations of Dillman (1978),
they should hold less negative beliefs about the inconve- reminder postcards were sent out approximately three weeks
nience of engaging in the behavior. Therefore, although we after the original mailing to people who had not returned the
expect that beliefs about the importance of recycling will questionnaire. Members of the sample who had not returned
relate directly to the likelihood that a person will engage in the questionnaire after another two weeks were sent a new
recycling behaviors, these general beliefs about importance cover letter and replacement questionnaire.
should also influence perceptions of the inconvenience of A total of 1891 surveys were mailed; 30 were returned as
engaging in the bebaviors, which in turn will influence die undeliverable, which left 1861 presumably delivered. Of the
behavior. Therefore, we predict the following: 1861 surveys delivered, 727 were returned by the respon-
dents, for a return rate of 39.1%. Of those responding,
H5: Beliefs about the importance of recycling will have a direct, 57.9% were male,2 the median income was $45,000, age
positive relationship with recycling behaviors. People who
believe that recycling is more important will tend to report ranged from 18-92 years with an average of 48 years, and
that they recycle more than will those who believe that recy- the average number of years of education was 14.6. A total
cling is less important. of 193 respondents were excluded from the analyses
Hg: Beliefs about the importance of recycling will be negatively because they had missing data on one or more measures
related to beliefs about the inconvenience of recycling. Peo- used in the present study. Thus, the analyses were based on
ple who believe that recycling is more important will tend to 534 respondents.
believe that recycling is less inconvenient than will those
who believe that recycling is less important. ^The overrepresentation of male respondents was a function of our selec-
H7: Beliefs about the inconvenience of recycling will have a tion criteria and not a result of response bias. Parts of tbe questionnaire
direct, negative relationship with recycling behaviors. Peo- were used for purposes unrclaied to this study, and Ihese purposes necessi-
ple who believe that recycling is more inconvenient will tated holding sex constant for a subset of the entire sampling frame.
Journal of Public Policy & Marketing 97

Figure 1. Proposed Model of the Relations Among Value Orientations, Economic Status, Recycling Beliefs, and Behavior

Measures study was sociopolitical control (control in large-group


We measured each latent construct using multiple indica- [societal] situations). The items composing this measure of
tors. The particular items used to measure each construct locus of control scale were evaluated on six-point scales
appear in Table 1. with anchors of "strongly disagree" (1) and "strongly agree"
(6). Higher scores on these measures indicate a more inter-
Exogenous Measures nal locus of control.
The four exogenous latent factors in the study were eco- Endogenous Measures
nomic status, individualism, collectivism, and locus of con-
trol. Three variables were used to measure economic status. The measures of beliefs ahout the inconvenience and impor-
One of these was the family income of the respondent as tance of recycling were also evaluated on six-point agree-
reported in the questionnaire. Respondents selected 1 of 11 ment scales and were similar to ones used in previous stud-
categories; the first was $0-$10,000, and the categories con- ies (McCarty and Shrum 1994; Williams 1991). Two items
tinued with $iO,000 ranges up to $100,000. The final cate- were related to the inconvenience of recycling and three
gory was "more than $100,000." The other two variables items were related to the importance of recycling.
considered for the economic status construct were from the The behavioral measures consisted of three items that
1990 U.S. Census. These were the median housing value measured the extent to which respondents recycled newspa-
and the per capita income of the ZIP code area in which each pers, cans, and glass/jars in their households. The frequency
respondent resided. of these recycling activities was evaluated on seven-point
scales with anchors of "never" (I) and "always" (7).
Five items were used to measure collectivism and three
items were used to measure individualism. These items were
similar to ones used in previous studies (Breckler. Green- Analysis
wald, and Wiggins 1986; Han 1991). Respondents were We used structural equation analysis by means of EQS
asked to evaluate these items on five-point scales with (Bentler 1995) to evaluate a measurement model and a
anchors of "not at all important" (1) and "extremely impor- causal model simultaneously, using the maximum likelihood
tant" (5). estimation method. Thus, the analysis evaluated the extent to
Ten items from a scale developed by Paulhus (1983) were which the observed variables were indicators of the latent
used to measure iocus of control. Paulhus partitioned locus constructs in the model and the strength of the relationships
of control into three distinct and independent behavioral among the latent variables as specified by the hypothesized
spheres; the sphere deemed most appropriate for the current paths. We evaluated the viability of the specific hypotheses
98 Environmental Beliefs and Behavior

Table 1. Measurement Model Results

Standardized
Factor
Construct Loading t-Value
Economic Status (a = .761)
Income .377 8.45
Median household value (of ZIP code area) .932 23.37
Per capita income (of ZIP code area) .912 11.21
Individualism (a = .440)
Being unique, different from others in many respects. .418 4.93
Being competitive with others. .276 3.74
Working independently from others. .698 5.43
Collectivism (a = . 746)
Working hard for the goals of a group even U doesn't result in personal recognition. .555 12.42
Being a cooperative participant in group activities. .570 11.24
Readily helping others in need of help. .647 13.58
Doing what is good for most of the people in the group, even if it means that the individual
will receive less. .607 13.27
Sharing with others. .661 13.56
Locus of Control (a = .816)
There is very little we, as consumers, can do to keep the cost of living from going higher. (R) .521 12.59
With enough effort we can wipe out political corruption. .528 12.35
The average citizen can have an influence on government decisions. .730 20.57
This world is run by the few people in power, and there is not much the little guy can do about it. (R) .545 12.38
By taking an active part in political and social affairs we, the people, can control world events. .743 18.36
When I look at it carefully, I realize it is impossible to have any really important influence over
what politicians do. (R) v679 17.22
I prefer to concentrate my energy on other things rather than on solving the world's problems. (R) .328 7.00
One of the major reasons we have wars is because people don't take enough interest in politics. .372 8.28
In the long run we, the voters, are responsible forbad government on a national as well as local level. .512 11.48
It is difficult for people to have much control over the things politicians do in office. (R) .600 13.94
1 Recycling Beliefs: Importance (a = .643)
Recycling will reduce pollution. .608 a
Recycling is important to save natural resources. J52 7.63
Recycling will save land that would be used as dump sites. .674 7.87
2 Recycling Beliefs: Inconvenience (ex = .685)
Recycling cans, bottles, newspapers, etc. is inconvenient. .747 a
I hate having to wash out bottles for recycling. .690 . 10.16
j Recycling Behaviors (a = .874)
I recycle newspapers used at home. .818 a
I recycle cans used at home. .803 15.53
I recycle glass jars/bottles used at home. .889 20.12

Notes: (R) indicates items that werereversed-scored, a = fixed during analysis, no t-values given. All loadings are significant atp < ,001.

by the significance of specific paths between latent con- The overall fit of the proposed model was evaluated in a
structs. We assessed the reliabilities of the measures by nested manner as recommended by Anderson and Gerbing
Cronbach's alphas. We tested individual parameters in the (1988). The proposed model was tested against a baseline
proposed model (factor loadings and hypothesized paths of model that included no directional paths among the latent
the model) by univariate t-tests and used robust standard
errors (Bentler 1995) to form the test statistics because of the degrees of freedom of the models being considered. The other two fit
their ability to protect against violations of the assumption of statistics were selected because they are sensitive to different aspects of
evaluating a proposed model. The SRMR is an analysis of the residuals
multivariate normality of the observed variables. We evalu- between the hypothetical covariance matrix and the fitted covariance matrix;
ated the overall ftt by the standardized root mean squared Hu and Bentler (1998) indicate that anwng indices they tested, the SRMR
residual (SRMR; Bentler 1995) and the root mean square was the most sensitive to misspecified factor covariances. The RMSEA is an
error of approxitnation (RMSEA; Steiger 1990).^ indication of the lack of fit of the model to the population covariance matrix;
Hu and Bentler (1998) found that it is particularly sensitive to models with
misspecified factor loadings. Hu and Bentler (1999) suggest a cutoff of .08
difference in x^ values for nested models (as these models are) is for the SRMR iwid .06 for the RMSEA before researchers conclude that they
itself distributed as a x^ with degrees of freedom equal to the difference in have an adequate fit of a hypothesized model.
Journal of Public Policy & Marketing 99

Table 2. Causal Model Results: Correlations Among Exogenous Constructs and Standardized Path Coemdents Among Latent
Constructs

Proposed Model Mediated/Direct Model


Coefficient t-Value CoefTicient t-Value
Correlations Among Exogenous Variables
^12'. Economic status <- individualism .034 .54 .029 .51
013: Economic status <-> collectivism -.066 -1.24 -.066 -1.25
(fi4: Economic status <-> locus of control .009 .19 .009 .19
^23- Individualism <-> collectivism -.023 -.33 -.041 -.63
(t24: Individualism *-* locus of control -.076 -1.09 -.090 -1.39
^24- Collectivism <-> locus of control .252 4.62*** .252 4.63***

Standardized Path Coefficients


Y21: Economic status * inconvenience -.197 -4.58*** -.169 -3.74**
Y22- Individualism -* inconvenience .138 L96* .139 2.01*
713: Collectivism -^ importance .223 3.20** .225 3.23**
7)4: Locus of control > importance .307 4.16*** .304 4.11***

731: Economic status * recycling behaviors .115 2.64**


732: Individualism > recycling behaviors .051 .95
733: Collectivism - recycling behaviors -.007 -.12
734: Locus of control > recycling behaviors .021 M
p2i- Importance > inconvenience -.387 ^99*** -.386 ^99***
P31: Importance > recycling behaviors .139 2.12*** .148 1.87
p32- Inconvenience recycling behaviors -.544 -7.36*** -.516 -6.65***

R2 R2
(Importance) .179 .177
(Inconvenience) .207 .198
(Recycling behaviors) .375 .378

*p < .05.
*V< 01.
**V<.ooi.

constructs and a model that included the hypothesized and therefore its relation with other variables should be
mediated paths as well as direct paths from the exogenous interpreted with caution.
constructs and recycling behavior (mediated/direct model). An additional consideration of the measurement model
The appropriateness of these models was evaluated hy the relates to our expectation that individualism and collectivism
change in x^ relative to the change in degrees of freedom. would emerge as distinct constructs at the psychological
The Satorra-Bentler scaled x~ (Bentler 1995) was used level As the top portion of Table 2 shows, the correlation
because it is robust to deviations from multivariate normal- between the individualism and collectivism factors was not
ity. The Lagrange-Multiplier test (Bentler 1995) was used to significant for the proposed model. The only significant cor-
evaluate whether the addition of individual paths not relation among the exogenous factors was a moderate, posi-
hypothesized in the proposed model would significantly tive relation between collectivism and locus of control.
improve the fit of the model.
Therefore, the different exogenous variables can be consid-
ered to represent relatively independent sources of variation.
Results
Proposed Model: Hypothesized Relationships
Measurement Model
Table I provides information about the quality of measure- Exogenous Predictors
ment of the latent constructs in the model. As the standard- We expected that the degree of individualism would relate
ized factor loadings in the table show, all the measured vari- positively to perceptions of the inconvenience of recycling;
ables loaded on the latent constructs as expected. Except for people who are more individualistic should perceive recy-
the individualism construct, the Cronbach's alphas for the cling as more inconvenient than will those who are less indi-
measured variables for each of the latent constructs were in vidualistic. As the middle portion of the proposed model
the acceptable range, which indicates that they are generally columns of Table 2 shows, this path was significant and in
reliable measures of the latent constructs. The low alpha for the predicted direction. Thus, H| was supported. We also
individualism suggests that it was not measured reliably. expected that degree of collectivism (H2) and intemal locus
100 Environmental Beliefs and Behavior

Table 3. Model Fit Summary for Competing Models

Degrees of ADegrees of
Model X^ Freedom Freedom SRMR RMSEA
Baseline 941.86 372 .092 .057
Proposed 707.37 364 234.49* 8 .052 .046
Mediated/direct 699.44 360 7.93 4 .051 .046

/<-01.

of control (H3) would relate positively to the importance of influence on behavior that is mediated by inconvenience, as
recycling. The results shown in Table 2 (proposed model hypothesized, and a direct effect that is over and above this
columns) indicate that these hypotheses were supported. mediated influence. All other direct relationships between
The standardized path coefficients for each of these paths the exogenous factors and recycling behavior were not sig-
were significant and in the predicted direction. Finally, we nificant, which indicates that the relation of collectivism and
expected that economic status would show a negative rela- locus of control with recycling behavior was completely
tionship with inconvenience such that people with a lower mediated by importance and that the influence of individu-
economic status would perceive recycling as more inconve- alism on recycling was completely mediated by beliefs
nient than would those with a higher economic status (H4). about the inconvenience of recycling.
As the results for the proposed model show, this hypothesis
was supported. Tests of Additional (Nonhypothesized) Paths
We used the Lagrange-Multiplier test to determine whether
Endogenous Predictors
additional paths from the exogenous variables to the media-
We expected that beliefs about the importance of recycling tors (e.g., economic status to importance) would provide a
would relate positively to the recycling behavior (H5) and significant improvement in fit. No additional paths were
negatively to beliefs about the inconvenience of recycling found to improve the fit of the model significantly. Thus, as
(H^). The results shown in the bottom portion of Table 2 for expected, degree of individualism and economic status were
the proposed model indicate that these two hypotheses were unrelated to beliefs about the importance of recycling; col-
supported. We also expected that beliefs about the inconve-
lectivism and locus of control did not show significant,
nience of recycling would relate negatively to recycling
direct relationships with beliefs about the inconvenience of
behavior (H7); this too was supported. Thus, beliefs (both
recycling.
importance and inconvenience) were related to recycling
behavior in the expected ways. As shown at the bottom of
Table 2, somewhat more than one-third of the variability m Hierarchical Tests of Models
recycling behaviors is explained by the proposed model Table 3 shows the measures of overall goodness of fit for a
null model (a model specifying no directional paths among
Mediated/Direct Model the latent constructs), the proposed model, and the medi-
The mediated/direct model of Table 2 provides a test of the ated/direct model. As the fit indices indicate, the proposed
extent to which the relationships of the exogenous variables model fit the data wetl in that the SRMR was below .08 and
(the fundamental values and economic status) to recycling the RMSEA was below .06. Moreover, as the change in x^
behavior are mediated by the recycling beliefs. Complete relative to the change in degrees of freedom indicates, the fit
mediation is evident if the direct paths between the exoge- of the proposed model was a significant improvement over
nous variables and recycling behavior are not significant.^ the fit of the null model.^ The change in x^ relative to the
When direct relationships between the exogenous vari- change in degrees of freedom for the mediated/direct model
ables and recycling behavior were considered in conjunction compared with the proposed model suggests that estimating
with mediated relationships, there was a significant, direct all of the direct relationship between the exogenous con-
relation between economic status and recycling behaviors, structs and recycling behavior did not significantly improve
as shown in Table 2. Economic status appears to have an the fit.6

^Thc comparison of the proposed model with this mediated/direct model 'The removal of the directional paths between the latent variables to test
represents an important consideration, because it is necessary for the medi- the baseline model led to the underidentification of the etidogenous vari-
ated paths to be significant in the presence of the direct paths from the able inconvenience because there were only two indicators of this latent
exogenous variables to behavior for mediation to be evident (Baron and constnict. The error variances of the two indicators of this construct were
Kenny 19S6); a completely mediated relationship is suggested if the direct constrained to be equal, which thus corrected the underidentification for
path that corresponds to a mediated relationship is not significantly differ- this baseline model.
ent from zero when niediation is tested. According to Baron and Kenny, ^A model, similar to the proposed model but with the addition of the
additional requirements to establish mediiUion are [hat (I) the exogenous direct path of economic status to recycling behaviors, significantly
variables are correlated with the outcome variables, (2) the exogenous vari- improved the fit compared with the proposed nfwdel. This would be
ables are correlated with the mediators in the expected manner, and (3) the expected, because this direct path was significant for the niediated/direct
mediator affects the outcome variable. Tests were conducted that deter- model. Al! the significant hypothesized paths of the proposed model were
mined that these three requirements were met. also significant for this revised model.
Journal of Public Policy & Marketing 101

Discussion of engaging in recycling behaviors. Thus, importance will


have an influence on recycling behavior through this indi-
The results of this study provide new infortnation regarding rect route.
the antecedents of recycling behavior. Previous research has
The extent of such efforts, however, will likely depend on
shown, for example, that beliefs about the importance of
the value orientations held by particular people. Our main
recycling and beliefs about the inconvenience of recycling
focus in this study was how these value orientations relate to
are stable predictors of the propensity of consumers to recy-
recycling beliefs and behavior, and our findings with respect
cle. These findings are intuitive. However, this study's
to these relations also have certain policy implications. The
results suggest that these relationships are not that simple.
implications are mainly related to segmentation and target-
For some people, such as those who are more individualis-
ing efforts and in particular to what types of messages are
tic or have a lower economic status, the importance of recy-
constructed to persuade people to recycle more. One clear
cling is not a motivating issue; only inconvenience predicts
implication is that because the path to behavior varies as a
recycling behavior. For others, such as those who are more
function of value orientation, the appeal of a persuasive
collectivistic or have a more internal locus of control,
communication should also vary according to the value ori-
beliefs about the importance of recycling are positively
entation of the target. In particular, the importance of recy-
related to the propensity to recycle. As are the constructs of
cling is a critical mediator of recycling behavior and thus
individualism and economic status, collectivism and locus
should be addressed in a persuasive communicationbut
of control are related to beliefs about the inconvenience of
only for people high in collectivism. For those high in indi-
recycling, but in a different way: Collectivism and locus of
vidualism, only inconvenience is a potential mediator, and
control relate to beliefs about inconvenience, but only
therefore attempts to change beliefs regarding the impor-
through the mediating influence of beliefs about the impor-
tance of recycling for those people may have little direct
tance of recycling.
effect on recycling behavior.
The results of this study also shed some light on the rela- These recommendations are consistent with several stud-
tion between economic status and recycling behavior. Some ies in cross-cultural consumer behavior. For example, the
research suggests that this relation is mediated by access to persuasiveness of advertising appeals has been shown to
recycling {Berger 1997). That is, people who have a higher vary across cultures; appeals that stress individualistic ben-
economic status tend to have greater access to recycling efits are more persuasive in individualistic cultures than col-
than do those who have a lower economic status, and this lectivistic cultures {United States versus Korea), and appeals
access differential influences recycling behavior. Implicit in that stress group {family) benefits are more persuasive in
this notion is that less access leads to more inconvenience. collectivistic cultures than in individualistic cultures {Han
The results of this study support this contention: Economic and Shavitt 1994). Therefore, communications aimed at
status was negatively related to beliefs about the inconve- changing recycling attitudes and behavior should attempt to
nience of recycling, which in turn negatively influenced match the focus of the benefits (individual/inconvenience
recycling behavior. Note, however, that economic status did versus group/importance) to the predominant value orienta-
not show any relation lo beliefs about the importance of tion of the target. Other research provides more support for
recycling, only to inconvenience. this notion: Aaker and Maheswaran {1997) found that the
extent to which people consider particular cues or informa-
Implications for Marketing and Public Policy tion (e.g., consensus information versus attribute informa-
The importance of these findings to marketing and public tion) pertinent to a particular judgment varies across cul-
policy practices centers on the complex interplay of the tures. Finally, research has also shown that the
antecedents of recycling behavior. This study focused on persuasiveness of types of emotional appeals {e.g.. ego-
two belief constructs that have consistently been shown to focused versus other-focused) used in persuasive communi-
predict recycling behavior: inconvenience and importance. cations varies as a function of the value orientation of the
The results of this study depart from previous ones by message recipient {Aaker and Williams 1998). The results of
demonstrating that the manner in which beliefs about the these studies, coupled with the results of our study, provide
importance and inconvenience of recycling are predictive of clear direction in the design of persuasive communications.
behavior varies across types of people. In terms of segmentation, the results of this study suggest
We have shown that importance has both a direct effect that the easiest people to infiuence may be those who are
on recycling behavior and an effect on perceptions of the collectivistic and/or have an internal locus of control. Com-
inconvenience of recycling, which in turn predict recycling munication efforts need to remind people high in collec-
behavior. To the extent that people's perceptions about the tivism of the importance of the behavior for the society.
importance of recycling can be changed, this should have a People with an intemal locus of control may only need to be
two-pronged influence on behavior. First, as people's reminded that their behavior in the area of the environment
beliefs in the importance of recycling increase, the likeli- will make a difference.
hood that they will engage in the behavior should also Others may require more effort to reach and convince.
increase. Second, increasing people's beliefs in the impor- People high in individualism but low in collectivism are
tance of recycling should, over time, change their percep- probably the most difficult to change. If people who are high
tions of the inconvenience of recycling. That is, as con- in individualism tend to think in terms of short-term benefits
sumers begin to understand the importance of recycling for relative to costs, marketing communications efforts may be
the environment and for their own and future generations, used to shift the time frame and context of the benefits that
this should influence their thoughts about the inconvenience recycling provides to coincide with the short-term nature of
102 Environmental Beliefs and Behavior

perceived costs. That is, communication efforts can focus on focused on a limited set of antecedents to the behaviors.
how the benefits of recycling are more immediate and can Therefore, several known antecedents to recycling (e.g.,
hit closer to home than people might think. There is some knowledge, social norms) were not included in the study.
evidence that such a strategy would pay off. White (1999) Furthermore, this study was limited to the proenvironmental
reported that the city of Boston used an advertising cam- behavior of recycling. Thus, although the study has
paign to influence nonrecyclers that focused on neighbor- advanced the understanding of how value orientations relate
hood-specific themes and consequently made the benefits to recycling, it does not specifically address recycling in the
more immediate and localized to the nonrecyclers' areas. broader context of the full range of antecedents to the
Another approach that may work with people who are behavior, nor does it specifically address the relationship of
high in individualism is to make other rewards and benefits these orientations to other environmental behaviors.
salient to them. De Young (1985-86) has shown that intrin- A second limitation pertains to population and sample
sic motivations (i.e., personal satisfaction) can play a role in issues. In our study, the population was limited to commu-
recycling behavior. To the extent that these immediate nities where curbside recycling was available. The results
intrinsic rewards can be made salient to individualists, such thus may not represent areas where recycling would be
benefits may aid in overcoming these people's resistance to either more difficult (i.e.. no recycling made available by the
recycling because of their typical perceptions of high costs municipality) or far more easy (i.e., the separation is done
relative to low short-term benefits. by the municipality). Clearly, people's perceptions of incon-
The most interesting communications challenge relates to venience would be influenced by the ease of recycling in the
area where they live. With respect to the sample, the return
this study's finding that individualism and collectivism can
rate was healthy for a mail survey to a general population
coexist within a person, which replicates previous work on
sample; however, the nature of the nonrespondents is not
the psychological nature of these constructs. As noted, a
known. Nonresponse bias in surveys is most critical when a
person can be high in both individualism and collectivism. researcher is interested in describing a population rather
The marketing challenge in these cases may be to induce than understanding the relationship among a set of variables.
people to think collectivistically rather than individualisti- In this respect, the focus of this study was not to report the
cally. As Trafimow, Triandis, and Goto (1991) argue, dif- incidence of recycling but rather to shed light on the com-
ferent situations and contexts ean influence whether people plex relationships between beliefs and behavior. It seems
sample either their collectivistic or individualistic selves. unlikely that the nature of these relationships would differ
This suggests that marketing communication efforts aimed dramatically between people who responded and people
at increasing recycling can benefit from an approach that who did not.
causes people high in individualism to sample their collec-
tive selves rather than their individualistic selves, thus shift- It is also worth noting that the effect sizes observed in this
ing their considerations from the costs of the behaviors to study were not large. At first glance, it might not seem fruit-
the rewards. ful or cost effective to implement the recommendations we
Although segmenting on the basis of such individual dif- made with regard to segmentation and targeting. Low effect
ferences has typically been viewed as too difficult, it may be sizes are often the norm when measuring input into social
easier than it sounds. Given the fragmented and nanowly behavior in uncontrolled settings. Our findings simply show
that the variables we measured (however imperfectly) have
targeted nature of today's media (Turow 1997), it may be
an infiuence on a particular behavior, and this influence has
possible and even relatively easy to reach audiences that
implications for what types of messages might prove most
hold similar basic beliefs. Thus, if the readership of a par-
persuasive. Given the large size of the target (those who
ticular magazine is known to have a more individualistic or have the ability to recycle) and the relatively low compli-
collectivistic orientation (e.g., readers of Forbes versus ance in many cases, even a small change would be welcome.
readers of The Progressive), different appeals can easily be
In this study, we have attempted to provide a better under-
constructed for targets with different value orientations.
standing for the motivations underlying proenvironmental
Although we have stressed the contribution of individual,
behavior. In doing so, we have drawn on theory and
psychological constructs (i.e., value orientations) on recy-
research in several domains, but in particular that which
cling behavior and their implications for policy, the rela- focuses on abstract but fundamental beliefs about tbe inter-
tionship of economic status (primarily the economic status action of humans with their environment and with other
of the community) and recycling bebavior demonstrates that humans. We have shown that these fundamental psycholog-
efforts to increase recycling compliance should be consid- ical constructs relate to beliefs and behavior in logical and
ered at various levels. Community-level action that will predictable ways. Understanding these relations, we believe,
make recycling more accessible, particularly in lower has important implications for understanding and promoting
income areas, will complement the segmentation and com- proenvironmental and other prosocial behaviors.
munication efforts we have described.

Limitations of the Study References


There are a few limitations to the present study tbat readers Aaker, Jennifer and Durairaj Maheswaran (1997), "The Effect of
should consider wben drawing conclusions from the results. Cultural Orientation on Persuasion," Journal of Consumer
The first limitation pertains to the narrow focus of the study. Research, 24 (December), 315-28.
Our primary Interest was in understanding how value orien- and Patti Williams (1998), "Empathy Versus Pride: The
tations, measured at the psychological level, relate to recy- Influence of Emotional Appeals Across Cultures," Journal of
cling beliefs and behavior. Because of this interest, we Consutner Research. 25 (December), 241-61.
Journal of Public Policy & Marketing 103

Alwitt, Linda F. and Robeit E. Pitts (1996). "Predicting Purchase Gutfeld, Rose (1991), "Eight of 10 Americans Are Environmen-
Intentions for an Environmentally Sensitive Product," Journal talists, at Least So They Say," The Wall Street Journal, (August
of Consumer Psychology, 5 ( 1 ) , 49-64. 2), 1.
Anderson, James C. and David W. Gerbing (1988), "Stnictura! Han, Sang-Pil (1991), "Individualism and Collectivism: Its Impli-
Equation Modeling in Practice: A Review and Recommended cations for Cross-Cultural Advertising," doctoral dissertation.
Two-Step Approach," Psychological Bulletin, 103 (May), College of Communications, University of Illinois.
411-23. and Sharon Shavitt (1994), "Persuasion and Culture:
Bagozzi, Richard P. and Pratibha A. Dabholkar (1994), "Consumer Advertising Appeals in Individualistic and Collectivistic Soci-
Recycling Goals and Their Effect on Decisions to Recycle: A eties," Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 30 (July),
Means-End Chain Analysis," Psychology and Marketing, 11 326-50.
(July/August), 3 1 3 ^ 0 . Hines, Jody M., Harold R. Hungerford, and Audrey N. Tomera
Balderjahn, Ingo (1988), "Personality Variables and Environmen- (1987), "Analysis and Synthesis of Research on Responsible
tal Attitudes as Predictors of Ecologically Responsible Con- Environmental Behavior: A Meta-Analysis," Journal of Envi-
sumption Patterns," Journal of Business Research, 17 (August), ronmental Education, 18 (Winter), 1-8.
51-56. Hofstede, Geert (1980), Culture's Consequences: International
Baron, Reuben M. and David A. Kenny (1986), 'The Modera- Differences in Work-Related Values. Beveriy Hills, CA: Sage
tor-Mediator Variable Distinction in Social Psychological Publications.
Research; Conceptual, Strategic, and Statistical Considera- Homer, Pamela and Lynn R. Kahle (1988), "A Structural Equation
tions," Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51
Test of the Values-Attitude-Behavior Hierarchy," Journal of
(December), 1173-82.
Personality and Social Psychology, 54 (April), 638-46.
Bellah, Robert N., Richard Madsen, William M. Sullivan, A. Hopper, Joseph R. and Joyce McCad Nielsen (1991), "Recycling
Swindler, and Steven M. Tipton (1985), Habits of the Heart: as Altruistic Behavior: Normative Strategies to Expand Partici-
Individualism and Commitment in American Life. Berkeley, CA: pation in a Community Recycling Program," Environment and
University of California Press. Behavior, 23 (March), 195-220.
Bentler, Peter M. (1995), EQS Structural Equation Program Man- Hu, Li-tze and Peter M. Bentler (1998), "Fit Indices in Covariance
ual. Encino, CA: Multivariate Software (nc. Structure Modeling: Sensitivity to Underparameterized Model
Berger, Ida E. (1997), "The Demographics of Recycling and the Misspecification, Psychological Methods, 3 (December),
Structure of Environmental Behavior," Environment and Behav- 424-53.
ior, 29 (Suiy), 5\ 5-31. and (1999), "Cutoff Criteria for Fit Indexes in
and Ruth M. Corbin (1992), "Perceived Consumer Effec- Govariance Structure Analysis: Conventional Criteria Versus
tiveness and Faith in Others as Moderators of Environmentally New Alternatives," Structural Equation Modeling, 6(1), 1-55.
Responsible Behaviors," Journal of Public Policy & Marketing, Hume, Scott and Patricia Stmad (1989), "Consumers Go 'Green,'"
11 (Fall), 79-89. Advertising Age, 60 (September 25), 3, 92.
Biswas, Abhijit, Jane W. Licata, Daryl McKee, Chris Pullig, and Jacobs, Harvey E., Jon S. Bailey, and James I. Crews (1984),
Christopher Daughtridge (2(X)0), 'The Recycling Cycle: An "Development and Analysis of a Community-Based Resource
Empirical Examination of Consumer Waste Recycling and Recovery Progrznx," Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 17
Recycling Shopping Behaviors," Journal of Public Policy & (Summer), 121-45.
Marketing, 19 (Spring), 93-105.
Kluckhohn, Clyde (1951), "Values and Value Orientations in the
Breckler, Steven J., Anthony G. Greenwald, and E.C. Wiggins Theory of Action: An Exploration in Definition and Classifica-
(1986). "Public, Private, and Collective Self-Evaluation: Mea- tion," in Toward a General Theory of Action, Talcott Parsons
surement of Individual Differences," paper presented at an and Edward Shils, eds. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University
invited international exchange (Poland-U.S.) on self and social Press, 388^33.
involvement, Princeton, NJ.
Lefcourt, Herbert M. (1991), "Locus of Control," in Measures of
Chase, Dennis and Therese Kauchak Smith (1992), "Consumers Personality and Social Psychological Attitudes, John P. Robin-
Keen on Green but Marketers Don't Deliver," Advertising Age, son, Phillip R. Shaver, and Lawrence S. Wrightsman, eds. San
63 (June 29), s2-s4. Diego: Academic Press, 413-99.
Dagnoli, Judann (1991), "Consciously Green," Advertising Age, 62 Leung, Kwok and Michael H. Bond (1984), 'The Impact of Cul-
(September 16), 14. tural Collectivism on Reward Allocation," Journal of Personal-
De Young, Raymond (1985-86), "Encouraging Environmentally ity and Social Psychology, 47 (October), 793-804.
Appropriate Behavior The Role of Intrinsic Motivation," Jour- McCarty, John A. and L.J. Shrum (1994), "The Recycling of Solid
nal of Environmental Systems, 15 (4), 281-92. Wastes: Personal and Cultural Values and Attitudes About
Dietz, Thomas, Paul G. Stem, and Gregory A. Guagnano (1998), Recycling as Antecedents of Recycling Behavior," Journal of
"Social Structural and Social Psychological Bases of Environ- Business Research, 30 (May), 53-62.
mental Concern," Environment and Behavior, 30 (July), Paulhus, Delroy (1983), "Sphere-Specific Measures of Perceived
450-71. Control," Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 44
Dillman, Donald A. (1978), Mail and Telephone Surveys: The (June), 1253-65.
Total Design Method. New York: John Wiley & Sons. Pieters, Rik, Tammo Bijmolt, Fred van Raaij, and Mark de Kiuijk
Dunlap, Riley E. and Kent D. Van Liere (1984), "Commitment to (1998), "Consumers' Attributions of Proenvironmental Behav-
the Dominant Social Paradigm and Concern for Environmental ior, Motivation, and Ability to Self and Others,"
Quality," Social Science Quarterly, 65 (December), 1013-1028, lic Policy & Marketing, 17 (Fall), 215-25.
104 Environmental Beliefs and Behavior

Porter, Bryan E.. Frank C. Leeming, and William O. Dwyer Thogersen, John (1996). "Recycling and Morality: A Critical
(1995), "Solid Waste Recovery: A Review of Behavioral Pro- Review of the Literature." Environment and Behavior, 28 (July).
grams to Increase Recycling," Environment and Behavior, 27 536-58.
(March). 122-52. Trafimow, David. Harry C. Triandis. and Sharon G. Goto (1991).
Reitman, Valarie (1992), "Green Product Sales Seem to Be Wilt- "Some Tests of the Distinction Between the Private Self and the
ing." The Wall Street Journal, (May 18), Bl. Collective Self," Journal of Personality ami Social Psychology,
60 (May). 649-55.
Rotter. Julian B. (1966). "Generalized Expectancies for Internal
Versus External Control of Reinforcement," Psychological Triandis, Harry C. (1984), "A Theoretical Framework for the More
Monographs, 80 (1, Whole No. 609). Efficient Construction of Cultural Assimilators," International
Journal of Intercultural Relations, 8 (3), 301-30.
Schwepker. Charles H. and T. Bettina Comwell (1991), "An
Examination of Ecologically Concerned Consumers and Their (1989), "The Self and Social Behavior in Differing Cul-
Intention to Purchase Ecologically Packaged Products," Journal tural Contexts," Psychological Review, 96 (July), 506-20.
of Public Policy & Marketing, 10 (Fall), 77-101. (1994), "Theoretical and Methodological Approaches to
Shrum, L.J.. Tina M. Lowrey. and John A. McCarty (1994), the Study of Collectivism and Individualism," in Individualism
"Recycling as a Marketing Problem: A Framework for Strategy and Collectivism: Theory, Method, and Applications, Uichol
Development," Psychology and Marketing, 11 (4), 393416. Kim et al.. eds. Thousand Oaks. CA: Sage Publications, 41-51.
, John A. McCarty, and Tina M. Lowrey (1995). "Under- (1995), Individttalism and Collectivism. Boulder, CO:
standing the Buyer Characteristics of the Green Consumer: West view Press.
Implications for Advertising Strategy," Journal of Advertising, and Michele J, Gelfand (1998), "Converging Measurement
24 (Summer), 71-82. of Horizontal and Vertical Individualism and Collectivism,"
Sinha, Durganand and Rama Charan Tripathi (1994), "Individual- Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74 (January),
ism in a CoUectivistic Culture: A Case of Coexistence of Oppo- 118-28.
sites," in Individualism and Collectivism: Theory, Method, and Turow, Joseph (1997), Breaking Up America: Advertisers and the
Applications, Uichol Kim et al., eds. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage New Media World. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Publications. 123-36.
Vining, Joanne and Angela Ebreo (1990), "What Makes a Recy-
Sinha, Jai B.P. and Jyoti Verma (1987), "Structure of Collec- cler? A Comparison of Recyclers and Nonrecyciers." Environ-
tivism," in Growth and Progress in Cross-Cultural Psychology, ment and Behavior, 22 (January), 55-73.
Cigdem Kagitcibasi, ed. Berwyn, PA: Swets North America,
123-29. White, Kathleen M. (1999), "Recycling's Rising Rates," Waste
Age, 30 (May), 194, 200-203.
Steiger, James H. (1990). "Structural Modei Evaluation and Mod-
ification: An Interval Estimation Approach," Multivariate Williams, Elizabeth (1991), "College Students and Recycling:
Behavioral Research, 25 (April), 173-80. Their Attitudes and Behaviors," Journal of College Student
Development, 32 (January). 86-88.

Você também pode gostar