Você está na página 1de 12

See

discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: http://www.researchgate.net/publication/232440754

The Effects of Readers' Misconceptions on


Comprehension of Scientific Text.

ARTICLE in JOURNAL OF EDUCATIONAL PSYCHOLOGY APRIL 2005


Impact Factor: 3.08 DOI: 10.1037/0022-0663.97.2.235

CITATIONS DOWNLOADS VIEWS

63 18 180

2 AUTHORS, INCLUDING:

Paul van den Broek


Leiden University
104 PUBLICATIONS 2,681 CITATIONS

SEE PROFILE

Available from: Paul van den Broek


Retrieved on: 17 June 2015
Journal of Educational Psychology Copyright 2005 by the American Psychological Association
2005, Vol. 97, No. 2, 235245 0022-0663/05/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0022-0663.97.2.235

The Effects of Readers Misconceptions on Comprehension


of Scientific Text
Panayiota Kendeou and Paul van den Broek
University of Minnesota

The aim of the present study was to investigate the effects of readers misconceptions on text compre-
hension. College students with misconceptions in science were asked to read and recall a text that
contradicted their misconceptions. Students with no misconceptions served as the control group. Both
online (think-aloud, reading times) and offline (recall) measures were obtained. The results suggest that
readers misconceptions often do not affect the online processes themselves but do influence the content
of those processes and, consequently, the offline memory representation for the text after reading is
completed.

Keywords: misconceptions, comprehension, scientific text

Much of the learning that takes place in and out of schools is and understanding of the text in its entirety, the final product of
based on successful comprehension of texts. Readers actively reading. On the other hand, online investigation provides informa-
construct a memory representation of the text that critically de- tion about the different types of processes in which readers engage
pends on their interpretation in light of prior knowledge. The during actual reading that lead or fail to leadto a coherent and
success of the comprehension process depends on the integration accurate memory representation, including inferring, elaborating,
of readers prior knowledge with textual information (Goldman & explaining, summarizing, paraphrasing, integrating information,
Bisanz, 2002; Kintsch, 1988, 1998; van den Broek, Virtue, Ever- and so on. Insofar as background knowledgeincluding miscon-
son, Tzeng, & Sung, 2002). The powerful effects of readers prior ceptionsinfluences readers comprehension, a primary way in
knowledge in text comprehension have been documented early on which it does so is by influencing the processes during reading.
(Bartlett, 1932), resulting in a large body of literature indicating Thus, simultaneous consideration of offline and online aspects of
that prior knowledge increases memory of texts for both young and comprehension is important because both aspects are causally
adult readers (e.g., Chiesi, Spilich, & Voss, 1979; Dochy, Segers, related: online processes that take place during reading lead to the
& Buehl, 1999; Means & Voss, 1985; Recht & Leslie, 1988). The offline product of reading (e.g., Goldman & Varma, 1995; Kintsch,
role of inaccurate prior knowledge, however, has received far less 1988; Trabasso & Suh, 1993; van den Broek, Young, Tzeng, &
attention despite the fact that readers with inaccurate knowledge Linderholm, 1999; Zwaan & Singer, 2003).
are the default case rather than the exception (Driver, Squires, To understand the online and offline effects of prior knowledge
Rushworth, & Wood-Robinson, 1994). Our aim in this article is to on text comprehension, one must consider both quantity and qual-
explore the effects of inaccurate prior knowledge on text compre- ity of a persons prior knowledge (Dole, 2000; Kendeou, Rapp, &
hension, focusing both on the final product of reading a text and on van den Broek, 2004; Lipson, 1982; Maria & MacGinitie, 1987;
the actual processes that take place during reading and lead to this Shapiro, 2004). Quantity refers to the amount of knowledge one
product. has, whereas quality refers to the accuracy of that knowledge.
To assess the effects of prior knowledge on text comprehension, Quantity of prior knowledge has been the main focus of research
one must consider both offline products and online processes (Just in which experts and novices in a domain are compared. Experts in
& Carpenter, 1980; Kintsch & van Dijk, 1978; Magliano & a domain engage in more inference generation and integration of
Graesser, 1991; Pressley, 2000). On the one hand, offline investi- those inferences with textual information than do novices (Leon &
gation provides information about the final mental representation Perez, 2001; Noordman & Vonk, 1992). Experts also recall more
of the text. Thus, it allows researchers to gauge readers memory and are more accurate in their recalls than are novices, provided
that the topic of the text is related to their expertise (Alexander,
Kulikowich, & Schulze, 1994; Royer, Carlo, Dufresne, & Mestre,
Panayiota Kendeou and Paul van den Broek, Department of Educational 1996; Schneider, Koerkel, & Weinert, 1989; Voss & Bisanz,
Psychology, University of Minnesota. 1985).
This research was supported by the Fulbright Scholarship Program and Quality of prior knowledge has been investigated specifically
the Center for Cognitive Sciences at the University of Minnesota through with respect to students inaccurate ideas in science. Inaccurate
National Institute of Child Health and Human Development Grant HD-
ideas, also called misconceptions, have been found to interfere in
07151. We thank Douglas Huffman for his assistance in the development
the process of acquiring new knowledge (Alvermann et al., 1985;
of the science questionnaire.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Panay- Diakidoy & Kendeou, 2001; Maria & MacGinitie, 1987). Individ-
iota Kendeou, Department of Educational Psychology, University of Min- uals construct different mental models by which they explain
nesota, 206 Burton Hall, 178 Pillsbury Drive SE, Minneapolis, MN 55455. concepts and processes in the physical world; indeed, they do so
E-mail: kend0040@umn.edu already as young children and before receiving formal instruction

235
236 KENDEOU AND VAN DEN BROEK

on those concepts (Carey, 1985). These mental models usually tions have a negative impact on what students remember from the
contain misconceptions that are far from the scientifically correct text (Alvermann et al., 1985; Lipson, 1982), little is known about
ideas (Diakidoy, Vosniadou, & Hawks, 1997; Nussbaum & No- exactly how and under what circumstances these misconceptions
vak, 1976; Vosniadou & Brewer, 1992, 1994). Such misconcep- affect the comprehension processes as they occur online. Further-
tions have been found to be very resistant to change and to hinder more, although we have strong evidence from research in reading
learning from texts (Alvermann et al., 1985; Lipson, 1982; Peeck, and science education about the effects of misconceptions on the
van den Bosch, & Kreupeling, 1982; Smith, Readence, & Alver- offline representation of the text (see Guzzetti et al., 1993, for a
mann, 1984). review), we know much less about the processes by which such
Although quantity and quality of background knowledge often representations are constructed during reading. In the present
go hand in handa person with more knowledge about an area study, we address this issue by investigating the online and offline
may also have more accurate knowledge than another personthis effects of misconceptions on text comprehension. Our investiga-
need not always be the case. Even experts in a domain may hold tion follows the three-pronged method suggested by Magliano and
inaccurate ideas about concepts (Borges, Horizonte, & Gilbert, Graesser (1991) that coordinates predictions based on theories of
1999; Stocklmayer & Treagust, 1996). Although inaccurate ideas discourse processing, evidence from verbal protocols, and evi-
by experts may be quite different from those by novices, the dence from behavioral measures. In Experiment 1, we use a
underlying problems posed by the misunderstandings are similar think-aloud methodology to investigate the cognitive processes
(Perkins & Simmons, 1988). Given the fact that inaccuracies in that take place when readers with and without misconceptions read
knowledge are pervasive especially among studentsit is im- scientifically correct information presented in a simple science
portant to investigate the effects of such inaccuracies on the text. Readers memory for the text is also assessed using free
process and product of reading. recall. In Experiment 2, we use a reading-time methodology to
The effects of readers lack of sufficient and accurate prior provide converging evidence for and expand on the results ob-
knowledge are moderated by the structure of the text, with some tained in Experiment 1. In both experiments we used a text on
formats resulting in better comprehension and learning than others. electrical current because students often have misconceptions
As a result, various methods for revising texts have been proposed, about the nature of electrical current and its properties (Borges et
ranging from simple single-sentence revisions to complete restruc- al., 1999; Gentner & Gentner, 1983; Heller & Finley, 1992;
turing of texts by content experts (Beck, McKeown, Sinatra, & Osborne & Freyberg, 1985; Stocklmayer & Treagust, 1996).
Loxterman, 1991; Britton, Van Dusen, Gulgoz, & Glynn, 1989).
Tests of these methods have shown that students comprehend
better when texts are more coherent and when implicit connections Experiment 1
between text elements are made more explicit (Britton & Gulgoz,
1991; Linderholm et al., 2000). Other methods have focused on The purpose of this experiment was to investigate the effects of
including refutation statements in the text that explicitly contrast misconceptions on the cognitive processes of science text compre-
misconceptions and correct notions and explain how potential hension and to determine whether misconceptions are represented
misconceptions are less plausible than the correct notions (Alver- in readers memory for the text. We used a think-aloud method-
mann & Hague, 1989; Hynd & Guzzetti, 1998). These refutation ology to investigate the different cognitive processes online be-
texts have been found to enhance students learning in cases in cause it allows us to consider a variety of readers responses
which students hold misconceptions related to the topic of the texts (Ericsson & Simon, 1993; Pressley & Afflerbach, 1995; Trabasso
(Alvermann & Hynd, 1989; Diakidoy, Kendeou, & Ioannides, & Suh, 1993). Furthermore, this methodology has received exten-
2003; Dole, 2000; Guzzetti, Snyder, Glass, & Gamas, 1993). sive validation as a tool to reveal comprehension processes in
The focus on altering texts to improve comprehension and reading (Afflerbach, 2002; Cote & Goldman, 1999; Magliano &
learning reflects an often implicit shared assumption that improved Graesser, 1991; Magliano & Millis, 2003; Zwaan & Brown, 1996).
comprehension depends on improving readers active, online pro- With respect to the online effects of misconceptions, there are
cessing of texts. Altered texts would make it more likely that these several possible scenarios for the anticipated results. One scenario
online processes succeed, and this would improve comprehension. is that readers processing is not affected by their having miscon-
Improved processing could pertain to a number of things including ceptions. This scenario would follow, for example, if readers do
generating explanations (Chi, Bassok, Lewis, Reimann, & Glaser, not actually use their background knowledge as they process the
1989), generating and answering questions (King, 1994; text. We argue that this scenario is an unlikely one, given the
Woloshyn, Paivo, & Pressley, 1994), monitoring comprehension extensive research findings cited earlier in this articlethat dif-
(Baker, 1979; Baker & Anderson, 1982), adopting appropriate ferences in the amount and accuracy of background knowledge do
standards of coherence (van den Broek et al., 2002), or generating affect the product of reading. Moreover, psychological research in
knowledge-based inferences (Cote, Goldman, & Saul, 1998; text comprehension has provided considerable evidence that the
Graesser & Bertus, 1998). Improved online processing presumably readers knowledge has profound effects on the readers online
would result in a more complete and more coherent mental repre- text processing itself (e.g., inference generation; see Leon & Perez,
sentation of the text that efficiently integrates textual information 2001; Noordman & Vonk, 1992). Thus, it is likely that readers
with prior knowledge (Kintsch, 1988). misconceptions also affect online processing. Such effects could
In the current studies, we directly investigate the processing of involve the actual processes or the contents of those processes (or
scientific texts as a function of the quality of readers prior knowl- both). Thus, a second possible scenario is that readers with mis-
edge related to the text. Focusing on this issue is important because conceptions engage in different types of processes than do readers
although readers with misconceptions are the default case rather without misconceptions. A third possible scenario is that readers
than the exception (Driver et al., 1994) and although misconcep- misconceptions do not affect the online comprehension processes
EFFECTS OF READERS MISCONCEPTIONS 237

per se but the content of these processes when they involve prior stored in a battery. Also, 38% of the participants provided a number of
knowledge. incorrect explanations about how the bulb in a circuit lights up. In addition,
With respect to the offline effects, there are also several possible 67% of the participants provided answers that indicated they believed that
scenarios. One scenario is that readers memory is not affected by electrical current is consumed either in the bulb or in the circuit. These
identified incorrect ideas were also consistent with research that identified
their having misconceptions. Again, we argue that this scenario is
common misconceptions in electricity (Borges et al., 1999; Osborne &
an unlikely one, given prior findings that differences in back-
Freyberg, 1985).
ground knowledge do affect the product of reading (e.g., recall; see The science questionnaire used in the pilot study was revised to include
Chiesi et al., 1979; Royer et al., 1996). Also, students with mis- 16 multiple-choice items that were related to the nature of electrical
conceptions do not perform as well as students without miscon- current, simple electrical circuits, and the fact that electrical current is not
ceptions on postreading tasks such as application problems, consumed in the bulb or the circuit (see Appendix A for sample questions).
multiple-choice questions, and so forth (e.g., Maria & MacGinitie, Each question included one correct answer and a number of incorrect
1987). On the basis of these findings it is likely that readers alternatives that corresponded to common student misconceptions (identi-
misconceptions affect readers offline text representation. Such fied by the pilot study). The reliability of the instrument fell within
effects could involve the quantity of memory of the text (i.e., how moderate range (Cronbachs .61).
The interview consisted of a hands-on activity and three accompanying
much readers remember), the content of this memory (i.e., how
questions. The hands-on activity required students to attempt lighting up a
accurate their memory is), or both. Thus, a second possible sce-
bulb by using a battery, a bulb, and a few wires. The three questions
nario is that readers with misconceptions remember less informa- targeted three common readers misconceptions about electrical current
tion from the text than do readers with no misconceptions. A third identified in the pilot study: the nature of electrical current, the processes
scenario is that misconceptions not only affect the quantity of by which electrical current lights up a bulb, and the fact that electrical
memory but also the content of memory. In this case, we would current is not consumed in the bulb.
anticipate readers with misconceptions to remember less informa- Reading comprehension test. The Passage Comprehension and Read-
tion from the text and to be less accurate in what they do recall ing Vocabulary subtests of the Woodcock Language Proficiency Battery
than readers with no misconceptions. Revised (Woodcock, 1991) were used to assess students reading compre-
Given that the online processes form the basis for the construc- hension ability. The average score on the two tests was the students
reading comprehension score.
tion of a memory representation (Goldman & Varma, 1995;
Texts. Three expository texts were used. Two texts, five sentences long
Kintsch, 1988; Langston & Trabasso, 1999; Myers & OBrien,
each and entitled Tornado and TV, respectively, were used for practice.
1998; van den Broek et al., 1999; Zwaan, 1999), an important Both texts were adapted from Millis, Morgan, and Graesser (1990). The
additional expectation is that the observed patterns of online pro- third text was 19 sentences long and entitled How Flashlights Work (see
cesses will be reflected in the patterns of results for the offline Appendix B). The text was adapted from Bloomfield (2001). The text
memory task. Put differently, conclusions about the processes that about flashlights contained three target sentences that stated the correct
take place during reading should be consistent with the observa- knowledge about electrical current and simple circuits. Each target sen-
tions concerning the end product of reading and vice versa. tence related to one of the three common misconceptions about electrical
current. For example, the target sentence Electrical current is not con-
sumed in the bulb related to students misconception that electrical current
Method is consumed in the bulb.
Participants
Procedure
Sixty-three undergraduate college students enrolled in introductory psy-
chology courses participated in this study. The data of 6 students were Participants were tested individually in an hour-long session. Partici-
eliminated from the study because the prior-knowledge inclusion criterion pants first completed the interview and the science questionnaire, followed
was not satisfied. Of the remaining participants, 34 were women and 23 by the Woodcock Passage Comprehension test. Participants were then
were men, with an age range of 18 28 years. Students were recruited from asked to read the two practice texts. Texts appeared one sentence at a time
the University of Minnesota and received extra course credit for partici- on a computer screen. Participants could move from one sentence to the
pating. The misconception group (as identified using the procedures de- next with a mouse click. Texts were interrupted at prespecified points with
scribed below) comprised 29 participants. The nonmisconception group a blank screen. When the blank screen appeared, students were instructed
comprised the remaining 28 participants. to think aloud about the preceding sentence. The experimenter provided
feedback after each text. Participants were instructed to read the third text
Materials on their own and to think aloud whenever the blank screen appeared. The
presentation of the third text was interrupted every three or four sentences,
Prior knowledge tests. A science questionnaire and an interview were for a total of five times. Two blank screens were used for control purposes,
used to identify students prior knowledge. A pilot study was conducted to whereas the remaining three blank screens followed the three target sen-
develop and validate the science questionnaire and the interview questions. tences. Participants responses were recorded. After completing reading,
In this study, 24 undergraduate psychology students were administered an participants were administered the Woodcock Reading Vocabulary test.
open-ended science questionnaire on electricity. The pilot questionnaire Finally, participants were asked to write down everything they could
consisted of 10 questions designed to elicit participants incorrect knowl- remember from the text about flashlights.
edge related to electricity. Questions were adapted from a longer instru-
ment used previously in applied research (Huffman & Michlin, 2001). Scoring
Participants responses to each question were scored as correct or incorrect.
All incorrect responses were analyzed further to identify common incorrect Prior knowledge tests. The interview questions and the science ques-
ideas about electricity. This analysis showed that a large number of tionnaire were scored dichotomously. Each correct response received a
participants had inaccurate knowledge related to the nature of electrical score of 1, whereas each incorrect response received a score of 0. The
current and simple electrical circuits. Particularly, 58% of the participants observed total scores for the interview questions ranged from 0 to 3
provided answers that indicated that electrical current is a substance that is (possible range 0 to 3), whereas the observed total scores for the science
238 KENDEOU AND VAN DEN BROEK

questionnaire ranged from 4 to 16 (possible range 0 to 16). The criterion for Types of Comprehension Processes
inclusion in the nonmisconception group required participants to score a 3
on the interview questions and an 11 or higher in the science questionnaire. Participants responses during the think-aloud procedure were
These participants had none of the three misconceptions related to the classified with respect to the five selected categories. A multivar-
concept of electrical current. The criterion for inclusion in the misconcep- iate analysis of variance (MANOVA) was conducted, with group
tion group required participants to score a 0 on the interview questions and (misconception, nonmisconception) as the independent variable
10 or lower on the science questionnaire. These participants had all three and frequency for each of the five response categories across
specific misconceptions related to the concept of electrical current. The participants as the dependent variables. The means are provided in
data from participants who scored a 1 or a 2 in the interview were Table 1. There were no significant differences between the mis-
eliminated from the study. conception and the nonmisconception groups in any of the five
Think-aloud protocols. Students responses during the think-aloud
categories, F(5, 51) 1.29, p .05 (for the follow-up univariate
procedure were transcribed. Each protocol was parsed into clauses. Each
tests, all Fs 3, all ps .05).
clause was categorized on the basis of two coding schemes. The first
coding scheme was adapted from Pritchard (1990). This coding scheme
A second MANOVA was performed, with each of the two
consisted of four categories and was used to identify the different processes subcategories of explanations, knowledge-based and text-based, as
readers engaged in during reading. Categories included understanding the dependent variables. The analysis included group (misconcep-
(readers expressed their understanding, e.g., I knew that. It makes sense), tion, nonmisconception) as the independent variable and the num-
uncertainty confusion (readers expressed their confusion, e.g., I am not ber of responses per category as the dependent variables. The
sure about that. I am just guessing here), explanations (readers provided means are provided in Table 1. There were no significant differ-
explanations that were based on their prior knowledge, textual information, ences between the misconception and the nonmisconception
or both prior knowledge and textual information, e.g., The electrons groups with respect to the frequency of knowledge-based or text-
collide with the atoms of the filament. That is actually resistance), and based explanations, F(2, 54) 0.11, p .05 (for the follow-up
paraphrases (readers made comments that captured the gist meaning of univariate tests, all Fs 1, all ps .05).
text sentences, e.g., The current is not used in the bulb). Responses that Knowledge-based explanations for all participants were further
did not fall into any of these categories were coded as other. This last
classified as valid or invalid. A MANOVA was performed, with
general category included responses such as repeating the sentence, eval-
group (misconception, nonmisconception) as the independent vari-
uations of the text, or associations that were not relevant to the text.
The second coding scheme was developed to further analyze the content
able and valid or invalid knowledge-based explanations as the
of explanations. First, each explanation was coded with respect to the role dependent variables. This analysis revealed a significant effect of
of readers prior knowledge. Responses that comprised information beyond group, F(2, 54) 3.68, p .05, 2 .12. As can be seen in Table
the information presented in the text were categorized as knowledge-based 1, readers in the misconception group included fewer valid
explanations, whereas responses that comprised information only pre- knowledge-based explanations than did readers in the nonmiscon-
sented in the text were categorized as text-based explanations. Second, to ception group, F(1, 55) 5.49, p .05, 2 .10. Readers in the
investigate possible content effects of prior knowledge, we further coded misconception group included more invalid, knowledge-based
knowledge-based explanations as valid or invalid on the basis of their explanations than did readers in the nonmisconception group, but
scientific accuracy. this difference was not statistically significant, F(1, 55) 1.25,
Two independent coders completed parsing and coding the think-aloud p .05.
protocols on the basis of the two coding schemes blind to students group
assignment. Twenty-five percent of the protocols were coded in common.
Agreement was 85%. Disagreements between coders were resolved Recall of the Text
through discussion. The responses of the participants in the recall procedure were
Recall protocols. Students written recall protocols were parsed into coded with respect to three categories (recall score, valid infer-
clauses. Each clause was matched to the text sentences according to a gist
criterion. Two additional categories were developed for inferences (see van
den Broek, 1994, for a review), statements that were not explicitly men-
tioned in the text but could be inferred on the basis of the text and prior Table 1
knowledge: Inferences that were scientifically correct were coded as valid, Means of the Types of Processes by Misconception and
whereas inferences that were scientifically incorrect were coded as invalid. Nonmisconception Groups
Two independent coders completed parsing and coding the protocols blind
to students group assignment. Twenty-five percent of the protocols were Misconception Nonmisconception
(N 29) (N 28)
coded in common. Agreement was 91%. Disagreements between coders
were resolved through discussion. Response category M SD M SD

Uncertaintyconfusion 2.07 1.57 1.36 1.47


Results Explanations 3.31 2.37 3.54 2.96
Text-based 1.24 1.24 1.21 1.25
Preliminary analyses showed that there were no significant Knowledge-based 2.07 1.81 2.32 2.00
Valid 0.58* 0.68 1.32* 1.54
differences in Woodcock reading comprehension score between Invalid 1.48 1.59 1.00 1.65
the misconception (M 532.53, SD 9.50) and nonmisconcep- Understanding 2.38 1.37 2.89 1.66
tion groups (M 537.53, SD 9.55), F(1, 55) 3.36, p .05. Paraphrase 3.62 2.63 3.32 2.01
As expected, the misconception group had a significantly lower Other 1.86 1.50 1.54 1.03
mean science score (M 7.89, SD 1.67) than did the nonmis- Note. Numbers indicate the average frequency for each response cate-
conception group (M 12.00, SD 1.94), F(1, 55) 73.01, p gory.
.01, 2 .57. * p .05.
EFFECTS OF READERS MISCONCEPTIONS 239

ences, invalid inferences). A MANOVA was performed, with ceptions. These results provide strong evidence that
group (misconception, nonmisconception) as the independent vari- misconceptions have an intrusive effect on both the quantity and
able and recall score, valid inferences, and invalid inferences as the the quality of students memory representation of text.
dependent variables. This analysis revealed a significant effect of Online and offline results are consistent in that they show
group, F(3, 53) 14.08, p .01, 2 .44. As can be seen in similar effects of readers misconceptions. In the case of
Table 2, there were significant differences between the miscon- knowledge-based inferences, readers misconceptions affected the
ception and the nonmisconception groups with respect to all three quality of knowledge-based explanations online and the quality of
variables. Readers with misconceptions recalled less textual infor- inferences offline. Indeed, the number of readers invalid,
mation after reading than did readers with no misconceptions, F(1, knowledge-based explanations generated during think-aloud was
55) 24.70, p .01, 2 .31. Readers with misconceptions significantly related to the number of invalid inferences included
generated more invalid inferences, F(1, 55) 23.31, p .01, in recall (r .35, p .01). Thus, the quality of individuals
2 .30, and fewer valid inferences, F(1, 55) 7.38, p .01, processing of the text was related to the quality of their represen-
2 .12, than did readers with no misconceptions. tation of the text.
To rule out the possibility that differences in recall and inference In this experiment, we used a think-aloud methodology because
generation were due to readers individual differences in reading we wanted to investigate online processing and to obtain a variety
comprehension ability, we performed a multivariate analysis of of students responses. Think-aloud procedures provide a rich
covariance (MANCOVA) with the Woodcock reading comprehen- source of data, but they also have several limitations. One limita-
sion score as the covariate, group (misconception, nonmisconcep- tion is that this methodology reveals only a subset of cognitive
tion) as the independent variable, and recall score, number of processes during reading, namely those of which readers are
invalid inferences, and number of valid inferences as the depen- aware. However, readers also engage in automatic, subconscious
dent variables. The data showed that group was a significant processes. For instance, when readers encounter information in the
predictor for readers recall, F(1, 54) 20.33, p .01, 2 .27, text that is inconsistent with information in the preceding text, they
invalid inferences, F(1, 54) 18.50, p .01, 2 .25, and valid slow down without necessarily being aware of doing so (Albrecht
inferences, F(1, 54) 4.89, p .05, 2 .08, over and above & OBrien, 1993; Myers, OBrien, Albrecht, & Mason, 1994). A
readers comprehension ability. second limitation of the think-aloud methodology is that it may
have altered readers natural processing of the text, resulting in
Discussion either better or worse processing. Because of these limitations, it is
important to obtain converging evidence by using a less intrusive
The purpose of this experiment was to investigate the effects of and more automatic measure (Magliano & Graesser, 1991). To do
readers misconceptions on the online processes and the offline so, in Experiment 2 we obtained reading-time measures for the
end result of reading in science texts. The online results showed target sentences. Reading times at word, sentence, or text levels
that readers with misconceptions and readers without misconcep- have been used extensively in psychological research as an indi-
tions engaged in the same types of processes during reading. cator of processing (Lorch & van den Broek, 1997; OBrien, 1995;
Readers expressed their understanding, uncertainty, or confusion Rapp, Gerrig, & Prentice, 2001; Zwaan & Singer, 2003).
and explained and paraphrased equally often regardless of whether
they had misconceptions. The content of these processes differed,
however, at points in the text where prior knowledge played a Experiment 2
direct role. At these points, readers in the misconception group In Experiment 2, we used a reading-time measure to investigate
generated significantly fewer valid, knowledge-based explanations the effects of misconceptions on the cognitive processes during
than did readers in the nonmisconception group. Thus, readers comprehension of science texts. Reading-time measures are unob-
misconceptions affected their attempts to explain information in trusive and reflect both conscious and subconscious processes. As
the text during reading. in Experiment 1, we also obtained offline measures.
The offline results showed that misconceptions intruded in With respect to the online effects of misconceptions, there are
readers memory for the text. Readers with misconceptions re- two possible scenarios for the anticipated results. One scenario is
called less textual information and generated more invalid infer- that readers actual processing of the text is affected by their
ences and fewer valid inferences than did readers with no miscon- having misconceptions. This scenario would be indicated if readers
with misconceptions read one or more of the three target sentences
or the entire text at a significantly different speed than do readers
Table 2 with no misconceptions. A second scenario, in line with the results
Mean Recall by Misconception and Nonmisconception Groups obtained from the first experiment, is that readers misconceptions
do not affect the actual processes during reading, although they
Misconception Nonmisconception may affect the content of those processes. This scenario would be
(N 29) (N 28) indicated if we observe no significant differences in reading times
Variable M SD M SD between the two groups of readers.
With respect to the offline effects, on the basis of the findings
Recall score 7.72*** 1.84 10.92*** 2.91 obtained in Experiment 1, we expected that readers misconcep-
Invalid inferences 1.48*** 1.05 0.39*** 0.56 tions affect readers offline text representation. As in Experiment
Valid inferences 0.83** 1.10 1.64** 1.16
1, such effects would involve the quantity of memory of the text
Note. Numbers indicate the average frequency for each variable. (i.e., how much readers remember) and the content of this memory
** p .01. *** p .001. (i.e., how accurate is the memory). This expectation assumes that
240 KENDEOU AND VAN DEN BROEK

the think-aloud methodology in the first experiment did not alter Table 3. There were no significant differences between the mis-
readers natural online processing in a way that affected the offline conception and the nonmisconception groups with respect to any
text representation. If we observe substantially different results for of the three sentences, F(3, 33) 0.33, p .05 (for the follow-up
the offline task in Experiment 2 than in Experiment 1, this would univariate tests, all Fs 1, all ps .05), or the total reading time,
indicate that the think-aloud task did alter the reading process and F(1, 35) 0.56, p .05. Thus, there was no evidence that readers
product. with misconceptions spent more time reading the target sentences
or the entire text than did readers in the nonmisconception group.
Method
Analysis of Recall Protocols
Participants
The responses of the participants during the recall procedure
Forty-three undergraduate college students enrolled in introductory psy- were coded with respect to three categories (recall score, valid
chology courses participated in this study. The data of 6 students were inferences, invalid inferences). A MANOVA was performed, with
eliminated from the study because the prior knowledge inclusion criterion
group (misconception, nonmisconception) as the independent vari-
was not satisfied. Of the remaining participants, 20 were women and 17
able and recall score, valid inferences, and invalid inferences as the
were men, with an age range of 18 26 years old. Students were recruited
from the University of Minnesota and received extra course credit for dependent variables. This analysis was significant, F(3, 33)
participating. The misconception group, as identified using the procedures 12.24, p .01, 2 .53. The data in Table 4 show significant
described in Experiment 1, comprised 18 participants. The nonmisconcep- differences between the misconception and the nonmisconception
tion group comprised the remaining 19 participants. groups with respect to all three variables. Specifically, readers with
misconceptions recalled less textual information after reading
Materials compared with readers with no misconceptions, F(1, 35) 7.20,
p .01, 2 .17. Readers with misconceptions generated more
The materials were the same as used in Experiment 1. invalid inferences, F(1, 35) 14.84, p .01, 2 .30, and fewer
valid inferences, F(1, 35) 18.75, p .01, 2 .35, compared
Procedure
with readers with no misconceptions.
The procedure was the same as in Experiment 1, except that participants To rule out the possibility that differences in recall and inference
were instructed to read silently one sentence at a time and the text was not generation were due to readers individual differences in reading
interrupted. Reading times for the target sentences and the entire text were comprehension ability, we performed a MANCOVA with Wood-
collected. cock reading comprehension score as the covariate, group (mis-
conception, nonmisconception) as the independent variable, and
Scoring recall score, number of invalid inferences, and number of valid
inferences as the dependent variables. The data showed that group
Prior knowledge tests. The interview and the science questionnaire
were scored as in Experiment 1. The reliability of the instrument fell within
was a significant predictor for readers invalid inferences, F(1,
moderate range (Cronbachs .65). 34) 15.08, p .01, 2 .31, and valid inferences, F(1, 34)
Reading times. The reading times for the three target sentences were 19.89, p .01, 2 .37, over and above readers comprehension
recorded. Reading times that were more than two standard deviations from ability. Group was only a marginally significant predictor for
the mean were replaced with two standard deviations. This method re- readers recall, F(1, 34) 3.99, p .05, 2 .11.
placed approximately 4.5% of the data.
Recall protocols. The recall protocols were scored as in Experiment 1. Discussion
Two independent coders coded 25% of the protocols in common. Agree-
ment was 93%. Disagreements between coders were resolved through The purpose of this experiment was to investigate readers
discussion. online processes during the reading of scientific texts, including
processes of which readers are not aware, and to obtain converging
Results evidence for the recall results in Experiment 1.
The online results demonstrate that readers with misconceptions
Preliminary analyses showed that there were no significant did not spend more time reading any of the sentences that explic-
differences in Woodcock reading comprehension scores between
the misconception (M 531.36, SD 7.98) and the nonmiscon-
ception groups (M 535.79, SD 7.88), F(1, 35) 2.88, p Table 3
.05. As expected, the misconception group had a significantly Mean Reading Times of the Target Sentences and Text by
lower mean science score (M 8.11, SD 2.02) than did the Misconception and Nonmisconception Groups
nonmisconception group (M 12.53, SD 1.47), F(1, 35)
58.12, p .01, 2 .62. Misconception Nonmisconception
(N 18) (N 19)

Analysis of Reading Times Variable M SD M SD

The reading times for the three target sentences and the total Sentence 11 4,471 1,627 4,552 1,648
reading time were entered into a MANOVA. The analysis included Sentence 14 3,432 981 3,189 1,281
Sentence 18 3,342 892 3,176 1,016
group (misconception, nonmisconception) as the independent vari- Total reading time 77,958 19,043 74,321 21,115
able and response times for the three sentences and the total
reading time as the dependent variables. The data are shown in Note. Numbers indicate the average time in milliseconds.
EFFECTS OF READERS MISCONCEPTIONS 241

Table 4 conceptions. Given that the two groups of readers did not differ in
Mean Recall by Misconception and Nonmisconception Groups their overall reading comprehension ability, as measured by the
Woodcock reading comprehension test, or in the total number of
Misconception Nonmisconception inferences they included during recall, these differences appear to
(N 18) (N 19)
be due directly to the readers prior knowledge.
Variable M SD M SD These results have important implications for our understanding
of the way in which misconceptions influence the comprehension
Recall score 8.17** 2.54 10.58** 2.89 of scientific texts. There is ample evidence that having miscon-
Invalid inferences 1.83*** 1.20 0.53*** 0.84
ceptions vis-a`-vis the content of a text leads to poor understanding
Valid inferences 0.17*** 0.38 1.39*** 1.11
and memory for the text (see Guzzetti et al., 1993, for a review).
Note. Numbers indicate the average frequency for each variable. Our results allow us to go beyond these findings and consider how
** p .01. *** p .001. the actual reading processes themselves, as they unfold during
reading of the text, are influenced by a reader having misconcep-
tions and lead to an impoverished mental representation of the text
itly conflicted with their knowledge or reading the entire text than after reading is completed. Readers with misconceptions proceed
did readers with no misconceptions. These results support the through a text at the same speed and with the same types of
conclusion from Experiment 1 that readers misconceptions do not processes as do readers without misconceptions. When the text
result in different types of processes during reading. The offline calls for it, they activate and integrate their background knowledge
results replicated the results from Experiment 1 that readers with the textual information as do readers without misconceptions
misconceptions intruded in their memory of the text. The fact that (see also Hannon & Daneman, 2001; Kintsch, 1988). But the
the offline results were consistent across the two experiments content of these knowledge-based inferences reflects the miscon-
indicates that the think-aloud methodology used in Experiment 1 ceptions, resulting in fewer valid explanations.
did not alter readers processing. Specifically, misconceptions exerted their influence on the ex-
planatory inferences that readers generated during reading. Such
inferences have been found to be crucial in understanding expos-
General Discussion
itory texts (Ohlsson, 2002) and can determine students success in
The aims of the present study were to investigate the online and other tasks such as problem-solving in science (Chi, de Leeuw,
offline effects of readers misconceptions on the comprehension of Chiu, & LaVancher, 1994). Indeed, they are deemed central to
scientific texts. With regards to the online processes that take place successful reading comprehension in current theoretical models.
as the reader proceeds through a text, we demonstrated that readers They provide the glue that provides coherence to a readers mental
with misconceptions engage in the same types of processes during representation of a text (e.g., Graesser, Singer, & Trabasso, 1994;
reading as do readers with no misconceptions but that the content Kintsch, 1988; van den Broek et al., 1999) and lead to a feeling of
of those processes differs. During the think-aloud procedure in satisfaction in the reader that he or she indeed understands a
Experiment 1, readers expressed their understanding, uncertainty, certain phenomenon or situation (Brewer, Chinn, & Samarapun-
and confusion and explained and paraphrased to the same degree gavan, 2000). The importance of explanatory inferences is also
regardless of whether they had misconceptions. At those points in reflected in the fact that many interventions are aimed at fostering
the text where prior knowledge plays an important role, as is the the skills necessary for and increasing the likelihood that readers
case for knowledge-based explanations, both groups generated will construct such inferences, for example, using either externally
equal numbers of inferences, but it is important to note that readers or internally generated questions (Pressley, 2000; van den Broek,
with misconceptions generated fewer valid explanations than did Tzeng, Risden, Trabasso, & Basche, 2001) or through self-
readers with no misconceptions. In Experiment 2, we obtained explanation reading training (McNamara, 2004). Given the impor-
reading times to verify the conclusion that misconceptions do not tance of such inferences, it is not surprising that we found that
alter the processes themselves. This methodology is less intrusive misconceptions in the end led to deficient mental representations
than a think-aloud task and, unlike think-aloud procedures, is by the readers. The invalid inferences generated during reading not
sensitive to automatic as well as strategic processes. Here, readers only found their way into this mental representation but also led to
with misconceptions did not differ from their counterparts without generally less consistent and internally coherent representations.
misconceptions in their reading speed for the text as a whole nor Thus, readers misconceptions appear to have an interference
for the sentences that contained information that directly contra- effect on memory for the text, resulting in both quantitative and
dicted their misconceptions. Thus, there is no evidence that readers qualitative problems. From the results of our study, we can con-
altered their processing of the text when their prior knowledge clude that an important part of this interference originates during
conflicted with the information in the text. reading, particularly during the generation of knowledge-based
The online processes in which readers engage as they read a text inferences, such as knowledge-based explanations.
form the basis for the offline outcome of their comprehension, so Research in reading and science education has shown consis-
one would expect that differences in online processes have impli- tently that readers with misconceptions are relatively unaware of
cations for the eventual memory representation of the text. Indeed, conflicts between their knowledge and the information in tradi-
the offline results in both experiments indicate that readers mis- tional expository text (Guzzetti et al., 1993). This finding stemmed
conceptions affect both the quantity and the quality of their mem- primarily from results obtained offline, in which students learning
ory for the text. Readers with misconceptions remembered less was ineffective as measured after reading using free recall, appli-
information from the text and included more inaccurate and fewer cation questions, or problem solving. We provided additional
accurate inferences in their recalls than did readers with no mis- evidence that during online reading of traditional expository text
242 KENDEOU AND VAN DEN BROEK

readers do not become aware of their misconceptions. This is quite gagement with the text or interest may interact with readers
startling: Here we have a situation where a reader realizes that a misconceptions given the relation between prior knowledge and
piece of information in the text needs explanation, recruits his or interest. Readers who have high knowledge in a domain also
her relevant background knowledge, and generates an explanation, tend to express high interest in the domain (Kintsch, 1980).
without realizing that the explanation actually contradicts the Other sources of individual differences, such as domain knowl-
to-be-explained text. Had readers detected the inconsistency be- edge, may be directly related to readers misconceptions. For
tween their knowledge and the text, they could have engaged in instance, it may be the case that readers with misconceptions
deeper processing in an attempt to establish coherence (Glenberg, also tend to have a lower level of domain knowledge than do
Wilkinson, & Epstein, 1983; Graesser et al., 1994; McNamara & readers without misconceptions, and conversely readers with a
Kintsch, 1996) or reconcile the inconsistent information (e.g., low level of domain knowledge may tend to have more mis-
Hakala & OBrien, 1995; Linderholm, Virtue, Tzeng, & van den conceptions than do readers with a higher level of domain
Broek, 2004). Failure to do so suggests that when average readers knowledge. Exploration of such interactions and interdependen-
generate explanatory inferences they have a tendency to do so in a cies in the future is important because it will reveal how
relatively shallow fashion (cf. Elshout-Mohr & van Daalen- comprehension processes vary as a function of reader charac-
Kapteijns, 2002; Otero, 2002; Rouet & Vidal-Abarca, 2002). teristics and text properties.
Our findings are consistent with research on knowledge incor- A thorough understanding of the online and offline effects of
poration versus compartmentalization. Incorporation is the inte- misconceptions on text comprehension is important for the design
gration of new information from the text with the readers existing of effective texts and instructional techniques to promote students
body of knowledge, whereas compartmentalization is the isolation learning from texts. Further, this understanding is necessary be-
of new information from the text from prior knowledge (Potts & cause readers with misconceptions will always be a reality in and
Peterson, 1985; Potts, Peterson, St. John, & Kirson, 1990; Potts, out of schools, and learning from text will always be a medium of
St. John, & Kirson, 1989). The evidence from our experiments independent lifelong learning.
suggests that our readers engaged in knowledge compartmental-
ization. Although they activated and used their prior knowledge
when needed during reading (e.g., knowledge-based explanations), References
they failed to incorporate textual information into their prior Afflerbach, P. (2002). Verbal reports and protocol analysis. In M. L.
knowledge. Kamil, P. B. Mosenthal, P. D. Pearson, & R. Barr (Eds.), Methods of
Such compartmentalization presents an important educational literacy research: The methodology chapters from the Handbook of
problem. How do educators foster incorporation rather than com- Reading Research, Volume 3 (pp. 87103). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
partmentalization? An important prerequisite would be that readers Albrecht, J. E., & OBrien, E. J. (1993). Updating a mental model:
are aware that there are inconsistencies between their prior knowl- Maintaining both local and global coherence. Journal of Experimental
edge and textual information. For example, one could construct Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 19, 10611070.
texts in such a way that the inaccuracies in ones background Alexander, P. A., Kulikowich, J. M., & Schulze, S. K. (1994). How
knowledge and its incompatibility with the textual information are subject-matter knowledge affects recall and interest. American Educa-
tional Research Journal, 31, 313337.
explicitly mentioned. Indeed, one such type of text, refutation text
Alvermann, D. E., & Hague, S. A. (1989). Comprehension of counterin-
(in which readers misconceptions are explicitly mentioned and tuitive science text: Effects of prior knowledge and text structure.
refuted and the correct conceptions explained in close textual Journal of Educational Research, 82, 197202.
proximity), has been found to be more effective in bringing about Alvermann, D. E., & Hynd, C. E. (1989). Effects of prior knowledge
conceptual change than is traditional expository text, as measured activation modes and text structure on nonscience majors comprehen-
by offline tasks such as recall and question answering (e.g., Dole, sion of physics. Journal of Educational Research, 83, 97102.
2000; Guzzetti et al., 1993). In the context of the present theoret- Alvermann, D. E., Smith, L. C., & Readence, J. E. (1985). Prior knowledge
ical framework, we can speculate that the benefits of refutation activation and the comprehension of compatible and incompatible text.
texts come about because they engage readers in different types of Reading Research Quarterly, 20, 420 436.
processes than traditional expository texts do, resulting in greater Baker, L. (1979). Comprehension monitoring: Identifying and coping with
text confusions. Journal of Reading Behavior, 11, 365374.
likelihood of incorporation instead of compartmentalization and,
Baker, L., & Anderson, R. I. (1982). Effects of inconsistent information on
hence, in more effective learning. text processing: Evidence from comprehension monitoring. Reading
In this article, we have provided evidence that one source of Research Quarterly, 27, 281294.
individual differences, readers misconceptions, has a profound Bartlett, F. C. (1932). Remembering: A study in experimental and social
effect on science text comprehension. Other individual differ- psychology. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
ences such as differences in working memory capacity or level Beck, I. L., McKeown, M. G., Sinatra, G. M., & Loxterman, J. A. (1991).
of engagement with the text may also play a role in science text Revising social studies text from a text-processing perspective: Evidence
comprehension and indeed may interact with readers miscon- of improved comprehensibility. Reading Research Quarterly, 26, 251
ceptions. With respect to working memory capacity, extensive 276.
evidence in the psychological literature has demonstrated that Bloomfield, L. A. (2001). How things work: The physics of everyday life.
New York: Wiley.
in cases where processing is demandingfor example, as with
Borges, A. T., Horizonte, B., & Gilbert, J. K. (1999). Mental models of
inconsistenciesperformance is much better for readers with electricity. International Journal of Science Education, 21, 95117.
large working memory capacity than for readers with a small Brewer, W. F., Chinn, C. A., & Samarapungavan, A. (2000). Explanations
capacity (e.g., Engle & Conway, 1998). These findings raise the in scientists and children. In F. C. Keil & R. A. Wilson (Eds.), Expla-
possibility that differences in working memory capacity may nation and cognition (pp. 279 298). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
interact with readers misconceptions. Likewise, level of en- Britton, B. K., & Gulgoz, S. (1991). Using Kintschs computational model
EFFECTS OF READERS MISCONCEPTIONS 243

to improve instructional texts: Effects of repairing inference calls on inferences while reading expository texts on science and technology.
recall and cognitive structures. Journal of Educational Psychology, 83, Scientific Studies of Reading, 2, 247269.
329 345. Graesser, A. C., Singer, M., & Trabasso, T. (1994). Constructing infer-
Britton, B. K., Van Dusen, L., Gulgoz, S., & Glynn, S. M. (1989). ences during narrative comprehension. Psychological Review, 101, 371
Instructional texts rewritten by five expert teams: Revisions and reten- 395.
tion improvements. Journal of Educational Psychology, 81, 226 239. Guzzetti, B. J., Snyder, T. E., Glass, G. V., & Gamas, W. S. (1993).
Carey, S. (1985). Conceptual change in childhood. Cambridge, MA: Promoting conceptual change in science: A comparative meta-analysis
Bradford. of instructional interventions from reading education and science edu-
Chi, M. T. H., Bassok, M., Lewis, M. W., Reimann, P., & Glaser, R. cation. Reading Research Quarterly, 28, 117159.
(1989). Self-explanations: How students study and use examples in Hakala, C. M., & OBrien, E. J. (1995). Strategies for resolving coherence
learning to solve problems. Cognitive Science, 13, 145182. breaks in reading. Discourse Processes, 20, 167185.
Chi, M. T. H., de Leeuw, N., Chiu, M. H., & LaVancher, C. (1994). Hannon, B., & Daneman, M. (2001). A new tool for measuring and
Eliciting self-explanations improves understanding. Cognitive Science, understanding individual differences in the component processes of
18, 439 477. reading comprehension. Journal of Educational Psychology, 93, 103
Chiesi, H., Spilich, G., & Voss, J. (1979). Acquisition of domain-related 128.
information in relation to high and low domain information. Journal of Heller, P. M., & Finley, F. N. (1992). Variable uses of alternative concep-
Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 18, 257273. tions: A case study in current electricity. Journal of Research in Science
Cote, N., & Goldman, S. R. (1999). Building representations of informa- Teaching, 29, 259 275.
tional text: Evidence from childrens think-aloud protocols. In H. van Huffman, D., & Michlin, M. (2001). The impact of CPU on high school
Oostendorp & S. R. Goldman (Eds.), The construction of mental repre- physics: Current electricity results. Minneapolis: University of Minne-
sentations during reading (pp. 169 193). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. sota, Center for Applied Research and Educational Improvement.
Cote, N., Goldman, S. R., & Saul, E. U. (1998). Students making sense of Hynd, C., & Guzzetti, B. J. (1998). When knowledge contradicts intuition:
informational text: Relations between processing and representation. Conceptual change. In C. Hynd (Ed.), Learning from text across con-
Discourse Processes, 25, 153. ceptual domains (pp. 139 164). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Diakidoy, I. N., & Kendeou, P. (2001). Facilitating conceptual change in Just, M. A., & Carpenter, P. A. (1980). A theory of reading: From eye
astronomy: A comparison of the effectiveness of two instructional fixations to comprehension. Psychological Review, 87, 329 354.
approaches. Learning and Instruction, 11, 120. Kendeou, P., Rapp, D. N., & van den Broek, P. (2004). The influence of
Diakidoy, I. N., Kendeou, P., & Ioannides, C. (2003). Reading about readers prior knowledge on text comprehension and learning from text.
energy: The effects of text structure in science learning and conceptual In R. Nata (Ed.), Progress in education (Vol. 13, pp. 189 209). Haup-
change. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 28, 335356. pauge, NY: Nova Science.
Diakidoy, I. N., Vosniadou, S., & Hawks, J. D. (1997). Conceptual change King, A. (1994). Guiding knowledge construction in the classroom: Effects
in astronomy: Models of the earth and the day/night cycle in American- of teaching children how to question and how to explain. American
Indian children. European Journal of Psychology of Education, 12, Educational Research Journal, 31, 338 368.
159 184. Kintsch, W. (1980). Learning from texts, levels of comprehension, or: Why
Dochy, F., Segers, M., & Buehl, M. M. (1999). The relation between anyone would read a story anyway. Poetics, 9, 8798.
assessment practices and outcomes of studies: The case of research on Kintsch, W. (1988). The role of knowledge in discourse comprehension: A
prior knowledge. Review of Educational Research, 69, 145186. construction integration model. Psychological Review, 95, 163182.
Dole, J. A. (2000). Readers, texts and conceptual change learning. Reading Kintsch, W. (1998). Comprehension: A paradigm for cognition. Cam-
and Writing Quarterly, 16, 99 118. bridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
Driver, R., Squires, A., Rushworth, P., & Wood-Robinson, V. (1994). Kintsch, W., & van Dijk, T. A. (1978). Toward a model of text compre-
Making sense of secondary science. New York: Routledge. hension and production. Psychological Review, 85, 363394.
Elshout-Mohr, M., & van Daalen-Kapteijns, M. (2002). Situated regulation Langston, M. C., & Trabasso, T. (1999). Modeling causal integration and
of scientific text processing. In J. Otero, J. A. Leon, & A. C. Graesser availability of information during comprehension of narrative texts. In
(Eds.), The psychology of science text comprehension (pp. 223252). H. van Oostendorp & S. R. Goldman (Eds.), The construction of mental
Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. representations during reading (pp. 29 69). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Engle, R. W., & Conway, A. R. A. (1998). Working memory and com- Leon, J., & Perez, O. (2001). The influence of prior knowledge on the time
prehension. In R. H. Logie & K. J. Gilhooly (Eds.), Working memory course of clinical diagnosis inferences: A comparison of experts and
and thinking (pp. 6791). East Sussex, England: Psychology Press. novices. Discourse Processes, 31, 187213.
Ericsson, K. A., & Simon, H. A. (1993). Protocol analysis: Verbal reports Linderholm, T., Gaddy, M., van den Broek, P., Mischinski, M., Crittenden,
as data. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. A., & Samuels, S. J. (2000). Effects of causal text revisions on more- and
Gentner, D., & Gentner, D. R. (1983). Flowing waters or teeming crowds: less-skilled readers comprehension of easy and difficult texts. Cognition
Mental models of electricity. In D. Gentner & A. L. Steves (Eds.), and Instruction, 18, 525556.
Mental models (pp. 99 129). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Linderholm, T., Virtue, S., Tzeng, Y., & van den Broek, P. (2004).
Glenberg, A. M., Wilkinson, A. C., & Epstein, W. (1983). The illusion of Fluctuations in the availability of information during reading: Capturing
knowing: Failure in a self-assessment of comprehension. Memory & cognitive processes using the landscape model. Discourse Processes, 37,
Cognition, 10, 597 602. 165186.
Goldman, S. R., & Bisanz, G. L. (2002). Toward a functional analysis of Lipson, M. (1982). Learning new information from text: The role of prior
scientific genres: Implications for understanding and learning processes. knowledge and reading ability. Journal of Reading Behavior, 14, 243
In J. Otero, J. A. Leon, & A. C. Graesser (Eds.), The psychology of 261.
science text comprehension (pp. 19 50). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Lorch, R. E. J., & van den Broek, P. (1997). Understanding reading
Goldman, S. R., & Varma, S. (1995). CAPing the construction-integration comprehension: Current and future contributions to cognitive science.
model of discourse comprehension. In C. A. Weaver, S. Mannes, & Contemporary Educational Psychology, 22, 213246.
C. R. Fletcher (Eds.), Discourse comprehension: Essays in honor of Magliano, J. P., & Graesser, A. C. (1991). A three-pronged method for
Walter Kinstch (pp. 337358). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. studying inference generation in literary text. Poetics, 20, 193232.
Graesser, A. C., & Bertus, E. L. (1998). The construction of causal Magliano, J. P., & Millis, K. K. (2003). Assessing reading skill with a
244 KENDEOU AND VAN DEN BROEK

think-aloud procedure and latent semantic analysis. Cognition and In- Rouet, J. F., & Vidal-Abarca, E. (2002). Mining for meaning: Cognitive
struction, 21, 251283. effects of inserted questions in learning from scientific text. In J. Otero,
Maria, K., & MacGinitie, W. (1987). Learning from texts that refute the J. A. Leon, & A. C. Graesser (Eds.), The psychology of science text
readers prior knowledge. Reading Research and Instruction, 26, 222238. comprehension (pp. 417 436). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
McNamara, D. S. (2004). SERT: Self-Explanation Reading Training. Dis- Royer, J. M., Carlo, M. S., Dufresne, R., & Mestre, J. (1996). The
course Processes, 38, 130. assessment of levels of domain expertise while reading. Cognition and
McNamara, D. S., & Kintsch, W. (1996). Learning from texts: Effects of Instruction, 14, 373 408.
prior knowledge and text coherence. Discourse Processes, 22, 247288. Schneider, W., Koerkel, J., & Weinert, F. (1989). Domain-specific knowl-
Means, M. L., & Voss, J. F. (1985). Star wars: A developmental study of edge and memory performance: A comparison of high- and low-aptitude
expert novice structures. Journal of Memory and Language, 24, 746 757. children. Journal of Educational Psychology, 81, 306 312.
Millis, K. K., Morgan, D., & Graesser, A. C. (1990). The influence of Shapiro, A. M. (2004). How including prior knowledge as a subject
knowledge-based inferences on the reading time of expository text. In variable may change outcomes of learning research. American Educa-
A. C. Graesser & G. H. Bower (Eds.), The psychology of learning and tional Research Journal, 41, 159 189.
motivation (Vol. 25, pp. 197212). New York: Academic Press. Smith, L. C., Readence, J. E., & Alvermann, D. E. (1984). Effects of
Myers, J. L., & OBrien, E. J. (1998). Accessing the discourse represen- activating prior knowledge on retention of expository text. In J. A. Niles
tation during reading. Discourse Processes, 26, 131157. & L. A. Harris (Eds.), Thirty-third yearbook of the National Reading
Myers, J. L., OBrien, E. J., Albrecht, J. E., & Mason, R. A. (1994). Conference (pp. 188 192). Rochester, NY: National Reading
Maintaining global coherence during reading. Journal of Experimental Conference.
Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 20, 876 886. Stocklmayer, S. M., & Treagust, D. F. (1996). Images of electricity: How
Noordman, L. G., & Vonk, W. (1992). Readers knowledge and control of do novices and experts model electric current? International Journal of
inferences in reading. Language and Cognitive Processes, 7, 373391. Science Education, 18, 163178.
Nussbaum, J., & Novak, J. D. (1976). An assessment of childrens concepts Trabasso, T., & Suh, S. (1993). Understanding text: Achieving explanatory
of the earth utilizing structured interviews. Science Education, 60, 535 coherence through on-line inferences and mental operations in working
550. memory. Discourse Processes, 16, 334.
OBrien, E. J. (1995). Automatic components of discourse comprehension. van den Broek, P. (1994). Comprehension and memory of narrative texts.
In E. P. Lorch & E. J. OBrien (Eds.), Sources of coherence in reading In M. A. Gernsbacher (Ed.), Handbook of psycholinguistics (pp. 539
(pp. 159 176). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. 588). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Ohlsson, S. (2002). Generating and understanding qualitative explanations. van den Broek, P., Tzeng, Y., Risden, K., Trabasso, T., & Basche, P.
In J. Otero, J. A. Leon, & A. C. Graesser (Eds.), The psychology of (2001). Inferential questioning: Effects on comprehension of narrative
science text comprehension (pp. 91128). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. texts as a function of grade and timing. Journal of Educational Psychol-
Osborne, R., & Freyberg, P. (1985). Learning in science: The implications ogy, 93, 521529.
of childrens science. Portsmouth, NH: Heinemann. van den Broek, P., Virtue, S., Everson, M., Tzeng, Y., & Sung, Y. (2002).
Otero, J. (2002). Noticing and fixing difficulties while understanding Comprehension and memory of science texts: Inferential processes and
science texts. In J. Otero, J. A. Leon, & A. C. Graesser (Eds.), The the construction of a mental representation. In J. Otero, J. A. Leon, &
psychology of science text comprehension (pp. 281307). Mahwah, NJ: A. C. Graesser (Eds.), The psychology of science text comprehension
Erlbaum. (pp. 131154). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Peeck, J., van den Bosch, A. B., & Kreupeling, W. J. (1982). Effect of van den Broek, P., Young, M., Tzeng, Y., & Linderholm, T. (1999). The
mobilizing prior knowledge on learning from text. Journal of Educa- landscape model of reading: Inferences and on-line construction of a
tional Psychology, 74, 771777. memory representation. In H. van Oosterndorp & S. R. Goldman (Eds.),
Perkins, D. N., & Simmons, R. (1988). An integrative model of miscon- The construction of mental representations during reading (pp. 7198).
ceptions. Review of Educational Research, 58, 303326. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Potts, G. R., & Peterson, S. B. (1985). Incorporation versus compartmen- Vosniadou, S., & Brewer, W. F. (1992). Mental models of the earth: A
talization in memory for discourse. Journal of Memory and Language, study of conceptual change in childhood. Cognitive Psychology, 24,
24, 107118. 535585.
Potts, G. R., Peterson, S. B., St. John, M. F., & Kirson, D. (1990). Vosniadou, S., & Brewer, W. F. (1994). Mental models of the day/night
Independent access to world knowledge and newly learned facts. In cycle. Cognitive Science, 18, 123183.
A. C. Graesser & G. H. Bower (Eds.), The psychology of learning and Voss, J. F., & Bisanz, G. L. (1985). Knowledge and the processing of
motivation (Vol. 25, pp. 213232). New York: Academic Press. narrative and expository texts. In B. K. Britton & J. B. Black (Eds.),
Potts, G. R., St. John, M. F., & Kirson, D. (1989). Incorporating new Understanding expository texts (pp. 173198). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
information into existing world knowledge. Cognitive Psychology, 21, Woloshyn, V. E., Paivio, A., & Pressley, M. (1994). Use of elaborative
303333. interrogation to help students acquire information consistent with prior
Pressley, M. (2000). What should comprehension instruction be the in- knowledge and information inconsistent with prior knowledge. Journal
struction of? In M. L. Kamil, P. B. Mosenthal, P. D. Pearson, & R. Barr of Educational Psychology, 86, 79 89.
(Eds.), Handbook of reading research (Vol. 3, pp. 545586). Mahwah, Woodcock, R. W. (1991). Woodcock Language Proficiency Battery
NJ: Erlbaum. Revised. Chicago: Riverside.
Pressley, M., & Afflerbach, P. (1995). Verbal protocols of reading: The Zwaan, R. A. (1999). Five dimensions of narrative comprehension: The
nature of constructively responsive reading. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. events indexing model. In S. R. Goldman, A. C. Graesser, & P. van den
Pritchard, R. (1990). The effects of cultural schemata on reading process- Broek (Eds.), Narrative comprehension, causality, and coherence (pp.
ing strategies. Reading Research Quarterly, 24, 273295. 93110). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Rapp, D. N., Gerrig, R. J., & Prentice, D. A. (2001). Readers trait-based Zwaan, R. A., & Brown, C. M. (1996). The influence of language profi-
models of characters in narrative comprehension. Journal of Memory ciency and comprehension skill on situation-model construction. Dis-
and Language, 45, 737750. course Processes, 21, 289 328.
Recht, D. R., & Leslie, L. (1988). Effect of prior knowledge on good and Zwaan, R. A., & Singer, M. (2003). Text comprehension. In A. C.
poor readers memory of text. Journal of Educational Psychology, 80, Graesser, M. A. Gernsbacher, & S. R. Goldman (Eds.), Handbook of
16 20. discourse processes (pp. 83121). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
EFFECTS OF READERS MISCONCEPTIONS 245

Appendix A

Sample Questions of the Science Questionnaire

1. Electrical current is: a) The wire that enters the radio.

a) The flow of chemical energy b) The wire that leaves the radio.

b) A substance stored in the battery c) Both wires have the same amount of electrical current.
c) The flow of electrons
3. Is there electrical current in the automobiles battery when the vehicle
d) The path of electricity is moving?

2. A radio is powered by a battery. Electrical current enters the radio a) Yes


flowing through one wire and leaves the radio through another. Which
wire has more electrical current? b) No

Appendix B

Expository Text: How Flashlights Work

1. There isnt much to a typical flashlight: 15. When the switch of the flashlight is turned off, it breaks the
circuit,
2. You flip a switch and out comes a beam of light.
16. and the electrical current is prevented from flowing through the
3. To understand how flashlights works, circuit.

4. you must understand how an electrical circuit works. 17. Without electrical current flowing the flashlight becomes dark.

5. The circuit of the flashlight contains one or more batteries, a 18. The electrical current is not consumed in the bulb.B3
light bulb, a switch,
19. What is consumed in the bulb is the chemical energy of the
6. and several metal strips that connect the components in a battery.
continuous loop.

7. When the switch of the flashlight is turned on,


Note. From How Things Work: The Physics of Everyday Life (p. 411), by
8. the chemical energy stored in the battery is transformed into
electrical energy. L. A. Bloomfield, 2001, New York: Wiley. Copyright 2001 by John Wiley
& Sons. Adapted with permission.
9. The electrical energy makes all free electrons move

10. from the negative terminal of the battery to the positive


B1
terminal. The fact that electrical current is the directed flow of free electrons
relates to readers preconception that electrical current is a substance stored
11. That directed flow of electrons is the electrical current.B1 in the battery.
B2
The fact that the collision of the electrons and the atoms in the
12. As electrical current flows through the bulbs filament,
filament causes the bulb to light relates to readers preconception that the
13. the electrons collide with the atoms of the filament. light in the bulb is the product of a chemical reaction.
B3
The fact that electrical current is not consumed in the bulb relates to
14. Thats why the filament becomes very hot and emits light.B2 readers preconception that electrical current is consumed in the bulb.

Received March 11, 2004


Revision received August 5, 2004
Accepted October 21, 2004

Você também pode gostar