Você está na página 1de 14

Copyright 1994 by the American Psychological Association, Inc.

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology


1994, Vol. 66, No. 1, 127-140 0022-3514/94/S3.00

Depression, Working Models of Others, and Relationship Functioning


Katherine B. Carnelley, Paula R. Pietromonaco, and Kenneth Jaffe

Two studies examined depressives' working models of others and the relative contribution of these
models and depression to relationship functioning. Respondents reported on their childhood rela-
tionships, adult attachment style, and relationship functioning. Study 1 compared mildly depressed
and nondepressed college women, and Study 2 compared married women recovering from clinical
depression with nondepressed married women. Mildly depressed college women evidenced greater
preoccupation and fearful avoidance in romantic relationships than did nondepressed women; re-
covering depressed women evidenced greater fearful avoidance. In both studies, relationship func-
tioning was predicted more strongly by adult attachment style than by depression status. Among
college women, positive experiences with mother also were linked to better relationship functioning;
however, attachment style and depression status mediated this effect.

Several theorists (Brown & Harris, 1978; Coates & Wortman, warmth from their caregiver are likely to develop a working
1980; Coyne, 1976; Lewinsohn, 1974; Paykel & Weissman, model of the self as lovable and competent and a working model
1973) have suggested that interpersonal processes function to of others as reliable, consistent, and warm. In contrast, children
promote and maintain depression. Interpersonal views of de- who have experienced rejection and coldness are likely to de-
pression emphasize that depressed individuals have difficulty velop a working model of the self as unlovable and incompetent
interacting with others and obtaining social reinforcement and a working model of others as unreliable, cold, and distant;
(Lewinsohn, 1974) or support (Coyne, 1976), which further ex- furthermore, they are likely to experience depression and anxi-
acerbate the depressed person's symptoms. Other theorists (Ab- ety (Bowlby, 1973). Such working models are thought to influ-
ramson, Seligman, & Teasdale, 1978; Beck, 1976) have focused ence expectations and interpretations of others' behavior from
on the role of maladaptive thought processes in promoting de- childhood through adulthood (Bowlby, 1973; Bretherton, 1985;
pression. Both cognitive and interpersonal processes appear to Sroufe & Fleeson, 1986) and to create a cognitive push toward
contribute to depression, yet depression researchers (e.g., Ham- reconstructing the patterns of early attachment relationships in
men, Ellicott, Gitlin, & Jamison, 1989; Pietromonaco, Rook, & later adult relationships (Hazan & Shaver, 1987; Shaver, Hazan,
Lewis, 1992; Siegel & Alloy, 1990) are only beginning to exam- &Bradshaw, 1988).
ine how these processes work together. Depressives' difficulties in their adult relationships may be
We propose that attachment theory (Bowlby, 1969, 1973, linked to their working models of others. For example, depres-
1980, 1988) integrates cognitive and interpersonal approaches sives may expect others to reject them and unintentionally elicit
to depression and thus provides a framework for understanding behavior that confirms their view that they are unworthy of an-
depressives' thoughts and behavior in their relationships. From other's love. The present research therefore sought (a) to iden-
an attachment perspective, children's experiences with a pri- tify the content of depressives' working models of others and (b)
mary caregiver form the basis for their mental representations, to examine the extent to which working models of others versus
or "internal working models" (Bowlby, 1973), of the self and depression contribute to dissatisfaction and conflict in adult ro-
others (Bowlby, 1973; Bretherton, 1985; Sroufe & Fleeson, mantic relationships. Wefirstbriefly review the recent literature
1986). Thus, children who have experienced consistency and on working models of others and then discuss their link to de-
pression and relationship functioning.

Working Models of Others


Katherine B. Carnelley, Institute for Social Research, University of
Michigan, Ann Arbor; Paula R. Pietromonaco, Department of Psychol- Working models of others fall into two general categories: se-
ogy, University of Massachusetts at Amherst; Kenneth Jaffe, Baystate cure and insecure. Ainsworth and her colleagues (Ainsworth,
Medical Center, Springfield, Massachusetts. Blehar, Waters, & Wall, 1978) found that infants differed in
Katherine B. Carnelley and Paula R. Pietromonaco's names are or- their response to separation and reunion with their mothers.
dered alphabetically. Secure infants were willing to approach the mother after a sep-
This research was supported in part by a grant from Baystate Medical aration and to maintain proximity. Avoidant infants resisted
Center. We thank Anita Barbee, Ronnie JanorF-Bulman, George Lev- contact with the mother and actively refrained from interacting
inger, Sally Powers, Alice Rossi, Phil Shaver, Rick Tessler, and several
with her. Anxious-ambivalent infants sought proximity to the
anonymous reviewers for helpful comments on a draft of the article.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Kath- mother but, at the same time, expressed anger toward her and
erine B. Carnelley, Institute for Social Research, Box 1248, University were difficult to comfort. Each style is thought to reflect a
of Michigan, Ann Arbor, Michigan 48106-1248, or to Paula R. Pietro- different mental representation, or working model of attach-
monaco, Department of Psychology, Tobin Hall, University of Massa- ment figures (Bowlby, 1973), that guides the infant's percep-
chusetts, Amherst, Massachusetts 01003. tions and behavior (Main, Kaplan, & Cassidy, 1985).
127
128 K. CARNELLEY, P. PIETROMONACO, AND K. JAFFE

Hazan and Shaver (1987; Shaver et al., 1988) have suggested Furthermore, depressives appear to hold a negatively biased
that adults' beliefs about relationships with romantic partners view of strangers with whom they have shared personal infor-
parallel the categories found for children. According to Hazan mation (Pietromonaco et al., 1992; Rook, Pietromonaco, &
and Shaver's (1987) typology, adults with a secure style are com- Lewis, 1993).
fortable with intimacy and are able to give help to others, those From an attachment perspective, depressives' problematic
with an avoidant style are less trusting of others and uncomfort- adult relationships derive from early negative experiences with
able with intimacy, and those with an anxious-ambivalent style a primary caregiver in which they learned that they were un-
worry about abandonment and feel unable to get as close to worthy of love and learned to expect future attachment figures
others as they would like. Adults who differ in their attachment to respond in a similar rejecting or inconsistent fashion. Several
style also differ in their beliefs about relationships, their emo- studies indicate that depressives hold a negative view of their
tional reactions to relationships, and their satisfaction with re- relationship with parents. Depression is associated with strict-
lationships (Carnelley & Janoff-Bulman, 1992; Collins & Read, ness, inconsistent love, and a perceived lack of nurturance from
1990; Feeney & Noller, 1990; Kirkpatrick & Davis, in press; parents (Blatt, Wein, Chevron, & Quinlan, 1979; McCranie &
Pietromonaco & Carnelley, in press; Simpson, 1990). Just as Bass, 1984; Schwarz & Zuroff, 1979), parental overprotec-
infants' attachment styles are thought to reflect their experi- tiveness (Richman & Flaherty, 1987), and loss of a parent
ences with caregivers, adults' attachment styles also are thought (Brown & Harris, 1978).
to reflect different early experiences with caregivers and there-
fore different mental representations of others.
Depression and Relationship Functioning
Bartholomew and Horowitz (1991) recently refined Hazan
and Shaver's (1987) typology by examining two distinct dimen- The intimate relationships of depressed individuals typically
sions: esteem for self (positive vs. negative) and esteem for others are distressed (e.g., Coleman & Miller, 1975; Gotlib & Whiffen,
(positive vs. negative). Bartholomew and Horowitz demon- 1989; Kahn, Coyne, & Margolin, 1985; Kowalik & Gotlib,
strated that secure individuals have a positive model of both 1987; Ruscher & Gotlib, 1988). In Gotlib and Hooley's (1988)
the self and others, whereas preoccupied (anxious-ambivalent) thorough review of this literature, they concluded that de-
individuals have a negative model of self and a positive model of pressed people and their spouses experience troubled commu-
others. Furthermore, avoidants fell into one of two categories: nication, negative affect, conflict, and tend to convey mixed
dismissing or fearful. Dismissing individuals, who indicated messages (e.g., a positive statement may be delivered in a nega-
that they did not need intimate relationships, held a positive tive tone). This conclusion is based on many studies that used a
model of self and a negative model of others. Fearful individu- variety of methodologies including self-report interviews (e.g.,
als, who indicated both a desire and fear of intimacy, held a Weissman & Paykel, 1974) and direct behavioral observations
negative model of the self and of others. (e.g., Hinchliffe, Hooper, & Roberts, 1978).
Depression may lead to poor relationship functioning by
Depression and Working Models of Others affecting both depressives' perceptions and interactions in close
relationships. For example, depression is likely to bias interper-
Most studies of depression have emphasized depressives' sonal perceptions in a negative direction and thus lead de-
mental representations of self. These studies indicate that de- pressed individuals to experience a decline in marital satisfac-
pressed people, compared with nondepressed people, describe tion. In addition, spouses of the depressed may experience
themselves more negatively (e.g., Beck, 1967; Pietromonaco, greater marital distress because they feel burdened by the de-
1985), expect more negative events and fewer positive events to pressed partner's needs and negative statements (Coyne, Kes-
happen to them (e.g., Pietromonaco & Markus, 1985; Pietro- sler, & Tal, 1987), and they may convey their irritation to the
monaco & Rook, 1987;Pyszczynski, Holt, & Greenberg, 1987), partner (cf. Coyne, 1976). As a consequence, depressed part-
and recall more negative and less positive personal information ners may become dissatisfied with the relationship and even
(e.g., Derry & Kuiper, 1981; MacDonald & Kuiper, 1984; Teas- more depressed (cf. Coyne, 1976). Thus, the causal link be-
dale, Taylor, & Fogarty, 1980). tween depression and marital distress is likely to involve a com-
Few studies have directly examined depressives' mental rep- plex, reciprocal process (Barnett & Gotlib, 1988; Gotlib &
resentations of others. Some evidence suggests that depressives Hooley, 1988). Although relationship difficulties may result
may have a more positive view of others than of themselves, from depression, they also may promote or exacerbate depres-
whereas other evidence suggests that depressives hold a negative sive symptoms (Coates & Wortman, 1980; Coyne, 1976).
view of both themselves and others. On the one hand, depres- Whether depression precedes or follows relationship difficulties,
sives, in contrast with nondepressives, have been shown to be it is likely to affect subsequent functioning in the relationship.
more optimistic about another person's control over a positive Studies of depression and marriage have described the prob-
outcome (Golin, Terrell, & Johnson, 1977; Golin, Terrell, lems experienced by depressed individuals in their relationships
Weitz, & Drost, 1979; Martin, Abramson, & Alloy, 1984) and but have not explored the beliefs and expectations that de-
to be less negative in their thoughts about others than about pressed people have about close relationships that may promote
themselves (Bargh & Tota, 1988; Pietromonaco & Markus, marital distress. The present studies not only examine the work-
1985; Pyszczynski et al., 1987). On the other hand, depressives ing models of others that may promote behaviors that un-
are sometimes more pessimistic than are nondepressives in their dermine depressives' relationships but also explore the relative
judgments of both themselves and others (Alloy & Ahrens, contribution of working models of others and depression to re-
1987; Pietromonaco & Rook, 1987; Pyszczynski et al., 1987). lationship functioning.
DEPRESSION AND WORKING MODELS 129

Hypothesized Linkages tive. The literature just reviewed suggests that some depressives
may be more likely to hold a negative model of others, and some
General Model depressives may be more likely to hold a positive model of oth-
ers. The present research examines which style is most charac-
The literature just reviewed suggests that childhood experi- teristic of depressives' models of close others (e.g., romantic
ences with parents, working models of attachment in adult- partners).
hood, and depression status are likely to be key variables in pre- The third hypothesis predicted that attachment style would
dicting individuals' perceptions and behavior in their close rela- be more strongly associated with relationship functioning than
tionships. Figure 1 shows the hypothesized links among these would depression. Nevertheless, we also examined the alterna-
variables. We propose that less positive (or more negative) child- tive possibility that depression negatively biases individuals'
hood experiences with a primary caregiver lead to an insecure perceptions of their relationships and thus contributes equally
working model of adult attachment. Furthermore, these child- or more than attachment style to relationship functioning.
hood experiences are apt to influence the likelihood of depres- The fourth hypothesis concerned whether adult attachment
sion in adulthood (e.g., Blatt et al, 1979). Both adult attach- style or depression mediates the link between childhood experi-
ment style and depression may result from less positive child- ences and relationship functioning. If people significantly revise
hood experiences, and they also may exert a reciprocal their mental models of relationships on the basis of their expe-
influence on each other. The curved line in the model indicates riences with people other than parents (e.g., their romantic part-
that these two constructs are correlated. Thus, a particular ners), then as individuals age or become partners in long-term
adult attachment style may increase (or decrease) vulnerability relationships, early experiences with a primary caregiver may
to depression, but depression also may affect individuals' per- become less central to working models of others, whereas expe-
ceptions of their attachment style. The model also includes the riences with one's romantic partner may become more central.
possibility that adult attachment style or depression may influ- If this is the case, then adult attachment style, which reflects
ence relationship functioning and that either of these variables individuals' perceptions of their interactions with adult roman-
may mediate the link between childhood experiences and rela- tic partners, should predict relationship functioning above and
tionship functioning. beyond that predicted by childhood experiences. Similarly, de-
Figure 1 also indicates that a strong link (illustrated by the pression may mediate the link between childhood experiences
solid line) exists between attachment style and relationship and relationship functioning because more recent negative ex-
functioning, whereas a weaker link (illustrated by the dotted periences may be incorporated into depressives' negative model
line) exists between depression status and relationship function- of self, and they may take precedence over earlier negative expe-
ing. This assumption follows from attachment theory, which riences with a primary caregiver. Thus, we expected adult at-
suggests that depressives' insecure working models are respon- tachment style, and possibly depression status, to mediate the
sible for relationship difficulties, and from the fact that depres- link between experiences with parents and relationship func-
sives' relationship difficulties often persist even after recovery tioning (Hypothesis 4).
(e.g., Bothwell & Weissman, 1977; Paykel & Weissman, 1973).
We conducted two studies to examine these hypotheses.
Study 1 included a sample of mildly depressed and nonde-
Specific Hypotheses pressed college women, some of whom were currently in a stable
dating relationship. Study 2 included a sample of married
The first set of hypotheses examined the assumption that
women who were recovering from a clinical depression and a
childhood experiences are associated with both adult attach-
comparison group of nondepressed women from the same com-
ment style and depression. We expected less positive childhood
munity. We focused on women because depression is far more
experiences to be associated with an insecure adult attachment
likely among women than men (e.g., Lewinsohn, Hoberman, &
style (Hypothesis la) and greater vulnerability to depression
Rosenbaum, 1988) and because relationships appear to carry
(Hypothesis lb).
special significance for women (Chodorow, 1978; Miller, 1986).
The second hypothesis concerned the link between adult at- In both studies, women described their relationship with their
tachment style and depression. Because depressives typically mother and father while they were growing up, their style of
hold a negative view of self, we predicted that depressives should relating to a romantic partner, and evaluated their romantic re-
be characterized by either a preoccupied style or a fearful style, lationship along several dimensions (e.g., satisfaction, quality of
depending on whether their model of others is positive or nega- exchanges, and style of conflict resolution).

Study 1
Adult Attachment
Style Method
Childhood

Relationship
Experiences Functioning Subjects
with Parents
An initial sample of 204 undergraduate women participated in the
Depression
study for course credit. All women completed the Beck Depression In-
ventory (BDI; Beck, 1967) at a large prescreening session for psychology
Figure 1. Hypothesized associations among predictors undergraduates and again, approximately 4-10 weeks later, during the
of relationship functioning. study session. Women who scored 9 or above at both times formed the
130 K. CARNELLEY, P. PIETROMONACO, AND K. JAFFE

mildly depressed group (n = 73); those who scored 3 or under at both attachment descriptions and chose the one that best described their feel-
times formed the nondepressed group (n = 90). The final sample in- ings in romantic relationships:
cluded only these 163 women who had stable scores across the two ses- Fearful. I am somewhat uncomfortable getting close to others. I want
sions. These women ranged in age from 17 to 48 (M = 20.4 years, SD = emotionally close relationships, but I find it difficult to trust others com-
3.20). The mean BDI score was 14.7 for the depressed women and 2.2 pletely, or to depend on them. I sometimes worry that I will be hurt if I
for the nondepressed women. Of the final sample, 101 women reported allow myself to become too close to others.
that they were currently involved in a stable dating relationship. The Preoccupied. I want to be completely emotionally intimate with oth-
average length of these relationships was 19.99 months. ers, but I often find that others are reluctant to get as close as I would
like. I am uncomfortable being without close relationships, but I some-
times worry that others don't value me as much as I value them.
Materials and Procedure Secure. It is relatively easy for me to become emotionally close to
others. I am comfortable depending on others and having others depend
Respondents completed all questionnaires during the study session.
on me. I don't worry about being alone or having others not accept me.
Working models of parents. We assessed a variety of important char-
Dismissing. I am comfortable without close emotional relationships.
acteristics of the parent-child relationship that were suggested by at-
It is very important to me to feel independent and self-sufficient, and I
tachment theory. First, the Mother, Father, Peer Scale (MFP; see Ricks,
prefer not to depend on others or have others depend on me.
1985; Carnelley & Janoff-Bulman, 1992) assessed, separately for
mother and father, whether the parent encouraged independence or was Second, respondents completed a 51 -item multidimensional measure
overprotective (e.g., "When I was a child, my mother encouraged me of their adult romantic attachment style. Twenty-two items came from
to make my own decisions.") and whether the parent was accepting or Brennan and Shaver's (in press) measure, which includes several dimen-
rejecting (e.g., "My father gave me the feeling that he liked me as I was; sions that are relevant to attachment theory and that specify the attri-
he didn't feel he had to make me over into someone else."). MFP items butes of each style (e.g., proximity seeking, self-reliance, and ambiva-
about relationships with peers were not relevant to our central hypoth- lence). We also included 10 items concerning closeness, disclosure,
eses and therefore are not discussed here. trust, and anxiety from Collins and Read's (1990) attachment scale and
additional items constructed by us in order to extend the domains
Items from the Family Background Scale (FBS; Latty-Mann & Davis,
tapped by previous measures. All items were selected for their face va-
1989), the Parental Bonding Instrument (PBI; Parker, Tupling, &
lidity and assessed one of nine theoretically relevant dimensions: prox-
Brown, 1979), and other items developed by the authors measured the
imity seeking (e.g., "After even a brief separation I eagerly look forward
extent to which parents were warm (e.g., "My mother was very loving
to seeing my partner"), closeness (e.g., "I find it relatively easy to get
and warm."); reliable (e.g., "No matter how busy my mother was, she
close to a romantic partner."), disclosure (e.g., "I feel comfortable shar-
always made me feel wanted."); sensitive (e.g., "My father was very at-
ing my private thoughts and feelings with romantic partners."), com-
tentive to my needs and responsive to my concerns."); controlling (e.g.,
pulsive self-reliance (e.g., "I would rather take care of myself than de-
"My father was demanding and strict."); and had a role-reversed rela-
pend on a romantic partner."), defensiveness (e.g., "I sometimes wonder
tionship with their daughter (e.g., "My father relied on me to see him
if I need romantic partners more than I let on."), ambivalence (e.g., " I
through stressful times"). Respondents rated all of the above subscales
often have troublefiguringout whether I'm truly in love with my part-
on a 7-point scale ranging from disagree strongly to agree strongly.
ner or not"), trust/confidence in partners (e.g., "I find it easy to trust
To simplify our measure of the quality of parental relationships, we romantic partners."), jealousy/fear of abandonment (e.g., "When my
constructed a composite measure of positive experiences with mother partner pays attention to other people, I can't help feeling jealous."), and
(a = .95; 32 items) and with father (a = .94; 32 items). Positive experi- anxious clinging (e.g., "More than anything I want my partner to return
ences included items concerning acceptance, encouragement of inde- my love."). Although the subscales were included to tap a variety of
pendence, warmth, sensitivity, and reliability. Furthermore, we in- theoretically relevant dimensions, they served an exploratory function.
cluded measures that focused on negative experiences with parents: Alpha reliabilities for the subscales ranged from .33 to .84. Further in-
controlling mother (a = .83; 5 items), controlling father (a = .77; 5 formation on the subscales is available on request.
items), role-reversed mother-daughter relationship (a = .84; 5 items),
and role-reversed father-daughter relationship (a = .79; 5 items). As an We constructed two composite measures of adult working models of
additional measure of negative experiences, we also included a categor- attachment on the basis of a direct discriminant function analysis in
ical item concerning whether each parent had a problem with alcohol which respondents' ratings of the nine attachment dimensions were
or other drugs that interfered with family harmony or holding a steady used to predict membership in three attachment groups (secure, preoc-
job (FBS; Latty-Mann & Davis, 1989); respondents selected one of the cupied, and fearful); we excluded the dismissing-avoidant group be-
following options: neither parent, my mother, my father, or both par- cause few respondents selected it. Other researchers (Bartholomew &
ents. Two dummy variables were created for the analyses: Mother has a Horowitz, 1991; Brennan, Shaver, & Tobey, 1991) have found that men
problem with alcohol or drugs (0 = no and 1 = yes), and father has a are much more likely than women to fall into the dismissing-avoidant
problem with alcohol or drugs (0 = no and 1 = yes). category. Thus, it is not surprising that we found only a few women who
We included separate measures of positive and negative experiences selected the dismissing-avoidant prototype.
with parents because previous work (Rook, 1984) has shown that nega- Prior probabilities of classification were 41% fearful, 16% preoccu-
tive versus positive contact with relatives and friends contribute inde- pied, and 43% secure. Two discriminant functions were calculated on
pendently to well-being. Furthermore, the subscales for role reversal and the basis of 191 cases, with a combined x2( 18, N = 191)= 133.43, p <
parents' problems with alcohol or drugs generally were not correlated .0001. After removing the first function, the second function remained
with the subscales tapping positive experiences. In addition, we did not a significant predictor, x 2 (8, N = 191) = 49.96, p < .0001. The first
combine the three subscales for negative experiences because they were function accounted for 65% of the between-group variability, the second
not correlated with each other. for 35%. Thefirstfunction distinguished between fearful avoidants and
Working models of romantic partners. Because researchers are still the other two groups, whereas the second function separated the preoc-
assessing the best way to measure working models of romantic partners cupied respondents from the other two groups. The two discriminant
(see Brennan & Shaver, in press), we relied on both a prototype measure functions overall correctly classified 71.2% of respondents into the three
and a multi-item measure to construct our attachment measure. First, attachment styles. Function 1 (fearful avoidance) correlated positively
respondents read the following (Bartholomew & Horowitz, 1991) four with compulsive self-reliance, defensiveness, and ambivalence and neg-
DEPRESSION AND WORKING MODELS 131

atively with comfort with closeness and disclosure. Function 2 (preoc- Table 1
cupation) correlated positively with anxious clinging, proximity seek- Correlations Among the Predictor Variables: Study 1
ing, jealousy/fear of abandonment, and negatively with trust. The fear-
ful avoidance (a = .94; 29 items) and preoccupation (a = .89; 22 items) Measure Avoidance Preoccupation Depression
scores were formed by averaging the scores on the subscales associated
with the two functions. These two reliable subscales constituted our pri- Mother
Positive experiences -.29**** -.23** -.33****
mary measure of working models of romantic relationships (adult at-
Negative experiences
tachment style).1 Controlling .16* .21** .19*
We relied on the multi-item measure of attachment because recent Roles reversed .14 .09 .07
work (Brennan & Shaver, in press) has suggested that such measures Alcohol or drug
are more powerful predictors of relationship satisfaction, sexual and problem .08 -.01 -.00
drinking behavior, and eating disorders than are attachment prototype Father
ratings. We would expect the avoidant and preoccupied scores derived Positive experiences -.26*** -.23** -.23**
from the multi-item measure to be more powerful than the single-item Negative experiences
ratings because they capture a greater range of beliefs and because they Controlling .27**** .12 .17*
Roles reversed .08 .02 .04
are based on more items.
Alcohol or drug
Depression status. Because women had been preselected on the basis problem .05 .02 .06
of low or high BD1 scores (3 or less and 9 or greater) at two time points, Depression status .37**** .38****
BDI scores were not continuous (i.e., no women had scores in the range
of 4 to 8). Thus, women were classified as mildly depressed or nonde- Note. Significance levels are two-tailed. Sample size ranges from 160-
pressed, and depression status was analyzed as a dichotomous variable. 163; the variation reflects missing data.
Relationship functioning. We assessed three components of relation- *p<.05. **p<.0\. ***p<.00l. ****p<.0001.
ship functioning: overall satisfaction, quality of interactions with the
partner, and conflict resolution style. Subjects who were in a current
relationship rated a single satisfaction item ("How satisfied are you with
your current relationship?) on a 5-point scale ranging from not at all to negative experiences with mother and with father), adult attach-
extremely. ment style (scores on fearful avoidance and preoccupation), and
To determine the quality of interactions with the partner, respondents depression status (depressed or nondepressed). According to
rated on a 7-point scale the extent to which 14items(a = .88) described our model, childhood experiences are linked to later adult at-
their interactions (adapted from Schuster, Kessler, & Aseltine, 1990). tachment style. Consistent with Hypothesis la, we found that
Examples of positive exchanges are "Your partner really listens to what
women who reported less positive childhood experiences with
you have to say" and "Your partner comforts you when you feel bad."
Examples of negative exchanges are "Your partner makes too many de-
their mother and a more controlling mother were characterized
mands on you" and "Your partner criticizes you." Negative items were by greater avoidance and preoccupation (i.e., more insecurity).
reversed; a high score reflects more positive exchanges.2 Furthermore, women who reported less positive experiences
To determine how respondents typically handled conflict in their re- with their father were characterized by greater avoidance and
lationships, we administered the Conflict Style Inventory (CSI; Levinger preoccupation, and those who reported a more controlling fa-
& Pietromonaco, 1989). The CSI includes subscales for collaborating ther were characterized by greater avoidance. The correlations
("I share the problem with my partner so that we can work it through also support Hypothesis lb, which concerned the link between
together"), compromising ("I try to find a compromise solution"), and childhood experiences with parents and depression. Depressed
contending ("I try to get my way whenever I can"). These subscales were women were more likely than nondepressed women to report
combined into a single reliable scale (a = .85; 15 items) as an indicator less positive childhood experiences.
of constructive conflict resolution style. Higher scores indicate con-
structive strategies (i.e., more collaborating, more compromising, and The model also assumes that adult attachment style and de-
less contending).3 pression are correlated. Table 1 indicates that depressed women
were more likely than nondepressed women to endorse avoidant
Results and preoccupied attachment beliefs about their romantic rela-
The model depicted in Figure 1 suggests that childhood ex-
periences with parents contribute to a person's adult attach- 1
Our subscales generally were associated in the expected manner
ment style and depression status, which, in turn, affect relation- with summary scores based on our limited set of items from previous
ship functioning. We first examine the assumptions of the measures (Brennan & Shaver, in press; Collins & Read, 1990). Further
model by presenting correlations among the different predictor details regarding these comparisons are available on request.
variables; then, we examine the relative contribution of child- 2
Because positive and negative exchanges may have differential
hood experiences with mother or father (working models of par- effects (Rook, 1984), we also analyzed these measures separately. We
ents), adult attachment style (working models of romantic part- found parallel results for positive and negative exchanges and therefore
ners), and depression status to relationship functioning. Given combined them to simplify the presentation of the results.
3
the large number of analyses conducted, we focus on only effects The CSI also includes a subscale that taps avoidance of conflict ("I
significant at p < .01. For informational purposes, however, any put off talking about an issue about which we disagree") and accommo-
dation ("If we disagree, I adjust my views toward those of my partner").
effects significant at p< .05 are noted in the tables. Because these strategies are not clearly constructive or destructive and
because they did not correlate well with the other strategies, we excluded
Correlations Among the Predictor Variables them from our overall measure. Analyses of the avoiding-accommodat-
Table 1 shows the zero-order correlations among the predic- ing subscale that paralleled those reported in the Results section for the
tor variables, including experiences with parents (positive and constructive conflict style scale did not yield any significant effects.
132 K. CARNELLEY, P. PIETROMONACO, AND K. JAFFE

tionships; thisfindingsupports the second hypothesis. Thus, the


pattern of associations among the predictor variables fits our

9.68*
7.18*

4.65*
6.58*

20.68*

18.38*
5.06*
'o to
c
&,
initial expectations. Furthermore, the relatively low magnitude x; ON m
of the correlations suggests that these variables also are distinct s
from each other.
1 &

.10

.18

.13
.04
g 2 p

.06
c p p

cha
re atta
Prediction of Relationship Functioning

.39***
.29**
To assess the relative contributions of early childhood experi-

21*
.20*

24*
a

25*
o

.15
OO

12
.07
o

Ste
ences with mother and with father, adult romantic attachment
style, and depression to relationship functioning, we performed
3
a series of hierarchical multiple regressions; subjects in these 2
analyses included only women who were involved in a stable co a.
dating relationship. Because our model assumes that childhood 1

ssi
2-
experiences are the initial determinant of later relationship ex- Q.

periences, the parent variable (e.g., positive experiences with Q

Step 1:
Beta
mother or positive experiences with father) always was entered
into the regressions at Step 1. To test alternative possibilities I I I ".III *. I I I

regarding the contribution of adult attachment style versus de-


pression status (nondepressed or depressed) to relationship
functioning (see Hypothesis 3), we entered these variables in
two separate orders. In one set of regressions, the adult attach-
ment variables (fearful avoidance and preoccupation) were en-
n
.2 S 81
S v
tered at Step 2 and the depression status dummy variable at rt
Step 3. In a second set of regressions, the order of these two
2
variables was reversed. Separate regressions were performed for
each of the three relationship-functioning variables (i.e., rela- >.:= -s
tionship satisfaction, quality of interactions, and constructive
conflict resolution style). We first report the results of the re- o
gressions that included the experiences-with-mother variables
and then report those that included the experiences-with-father
variables. "3 |
k,
Positive experiences with mother. Table 2 shows the results of 3 o
ion stat

the regressions, including the standardized beta weights and the


variance accounted for at each step (R2). Positive childhood ex- "--1 j 00
periences with the mother significantly predicted relationship
UBl|

a a; o o
functioning on all of the measures. More positive childhood ex- 3 o
-
'ore

periences with the mother were linked to better relationship


functioning, including greater satisfaction, more positive
1
Bet
tep
red

exchanges, and a more constructive style of conflict reso- C/7 a.


B
lution (i.e., more compromising and collaborating and less ca j
contending).
ent styl

ti.
rN
Consistent with Hypothesis 3, adult attachment style ap- -5 Q.

peared to be the strongest predictor of relationship functioning. E w5 1 t^


rjmq
O O

x: r
When the attachment variables were entered before depression 2
status, adult attachment style was linked to relationship func- ~S 2
tioning on all of the measures, even after taking into account sv
Ste

I I I \i IMS
the quality of childhood experiences with the mother. Although
the adult attachment variables together contributed signifi-
cantly, the results for the individual predictors indicate that 18
most of these effects were accounted for by scores on the avoid- ON
aB ill
ance subscale. The standardized beta weights, shown in Table 3 x:
v
2, indicate that women who scored high on avoidance experi- u IS
enced less satisfaction in their relationships, less supportive ex- 3
4J C
lea!

changes with their partner, and a less constructive conflict reso- a; v


"a 1
lution style. The effect for the preoccupied subscale also oc- a.

curred on two measures, but it was weaker (/? < .05); women -5
"II ?
Hierar

high in preoccupation were less satisfied with their relationship X! 5 V


and reported less supportive exchanges with their partner. 2 o.
DEPRESSION AND WORKING MODELS 133

In contrast with the findings for adult attachment style, de- Positive and negative experiences with father. Neither positive
pression status, when entered last, was not a significant predic- nor negative childhood experiences with father significantly
tor of the relationship-functioning variables, although it did add predicted any aspect of relationship functioning.
marginally (/? < .05) to the prediction of constructive conflict
resolution. When depression status was entered before attach-
ment style, depression was significantly associated with less re- Discussion
lationship satisfaction. (Depression also was associated with less Overall, these findings are consistent with our model. Early
supportive exchanges and a less constructive conflict resolution positive experiences with mother predicted relationship func-
style, but these effects did not meet the p < .01 criterion.) Even tioning, but this effect was attenuated when either attachment
when depression status was entered before the attachment vari- style or depression status was added as the second predictor.
ables, however, adult attachment style still significantly pre- Thus, it may be that experiences with mother are represented
dicted the three relationship-functioning measures, including within individuals' working models of relationships and in the
the satisfaction measure, which was also predicted by depres- working models of self that are associated with depression and
sion. Furthermore, attachment style, when entered after depres- that early experiences influence relationship functioning
sion status, also accounted for more of the variance than did through working models that also incorporate more recent life
depression status and thereby remained the strongest predictor experiences. Furthermore, attachment style was the most con-
of relationship functioning. sistent predictor of relationship functioning and generally pre-
Hypothesis 4 suggests that adult attachment style mediates dicted relationship functioning better than depression.
the link between childhood experiences and relationship func- Although it is possible that the link between positive experi-
tioning; the regression analyses provide evidence for this idea. ences with mother and relationship functioning reflects a posi-
Note that the beta weights for positive experiences with mother tive response bias (i.e., people who rated their mother positively
decreased in magnitude when attachment style was added to also rated their relationships positively), we think this explana-
the equation in Step 2 for constructive conflict resolution and tion is unlikely given that positive experiences with father were
became nonsignificant for satisfaction and quality of exchanges. not associated with relationship functioning. This finding is
This decrease in magnitude from Step 1 to Step 2 suggests that consistent with the idea that experiences with one's primary
attachment style mediates the link between childhood experi- caregiver (usually the mother) and the working models that de-
ences with mother and relationship functioning (see Baron & velop from such experiences influence later relationship func-
Kenny, 1986). Thus, the influence of childhood experiences tioning.
with mother may be linked indirectly to current romantic rela-
tionship functioning through working models of adult attach- Thesefindingsalso indicate that mildly depressed women are
ment. characterized by an insecure attachment style. The mildly de-
pressed group, in contrast with the nondepressed group, evi-
Similar to thefindingsfor attachment style, depression status denced greater preoccupation and greater fearful avoidance,
also appears to mediate the link between experiences with par- which suggests that they held a negative model of self and both
ents and relationship functioning. The regressions in which de- positive and negative models of others. Thisfindingis consistent
pression status was entered before attachment style indicate with previous work (e.g., Bargh & Tota, 1988; Pietromonaco
that the beta weights for positive experiences with mother de- & Markus, 1985) that suggests that mildly depressed students,
creased in magnitude for quality of exchanges and constructive compared with nondepressed students, are not necessarily
conflict resolution style (although they still remained signifi- more negative in their thoughts about others.
cant) and became nonsignificant for satisfaction. Thus, consis-
tent with Hypothesis 4, both attachment style (i.e., working Study 1 focused on women who were in a stable dating rela-
models of romantic relationships) and depression status appear tionship; it is not clear whether thesefindingswould generalize
to mediate the link between experiences with parents and rela- to women who are in longer term marital relationships. Another
tionship functioning.4 limitation is that the college women were mildly depressed (or
dysphoric) and are not necessarily more vulnerable to severe
Negative experiences with mother. Parallel analyses were per- clinical depression (cf. Gotlib, 1984). Thus, these college
formed with negative experiences with mother as predictors of women may differ in some ways from women who have been
relationship functioning. The three negative subscales (control- diagnosed as having a major depressive disorder. To address
ling mother, role reversal with mother, and mother who had a these concerns, Study 2 examined whether thesefindingsgener-
problem with alcohol or drugs) rarely predicted current rela- alized to women who had experienced a clinical depression and
tionship functioning. One isolated effect was found: Women who were married.
who reported having a controlling mother were less likely to
evidence a constructive conflict resolution style in their roman-
tic relationship(0 = .35,/?<.001 atStep l,.FofchangeatStep 4
1 = 4.42, p < .01). Together, negative experiences with mother Our model dictated the order in which we entered the variables into
the regression equations. Nevertheless, to control for the possibility that
accounted for about 13% of the variance, but adult attachment
depression distorted reports of childhood experiences and working
style accounted for another 13% of the variance in conflict res- models of attachment, we also performed a set of regressions entering
olution style over and above the effect of negative experiences depression at Step 1. Although depression significantly predicted the
with mother (F of change at Step 2 = 8.08, p < .001), and de- relationship-functioning variables in these regressions, the effects for
pression status accounted for an additional 4% of the variance childhood experiences with mother and adult attachment followed the
(F of change at Step 3 = 5.72, p < .05). same pattern as in the primary analyses and remained significant.
134 K. CARNELLEY, P. PIETROMONACO, AND K. JAFFE

Study 2 1976). The DAS is a standard measure of relationship adjustment in


married couples.
Method
Subjects Results
Twenty-five married women who were recovering from depression af- Correlations Among the Predictor Variables
ter receiving outpatient psychiatric care at Baystate Medical Center in
Springfield, Massachusetts, and 23 married nondepressed women par- Table 3 shows the correlations among the predictor variables.
ticipated in Study 2. The mean age of the women was 40 years (SD = 9 In contrast with the Study 1findingsfor college students, we did
years). not find an association between less positive childhood experi-
Recovering depressed women were eligible if they met the following ences with parents and an insecure adult attachment style (Hy-
criteria: (a) married or living with the same partner for at least 7 pothesis la) among the older sample of married women who
months, (b) previously diagnosed with a major depressive disorder while participated in Study 2. The only significant association was
in treatment, (c) either currently did not meet the Diagnostic and Sta- in the direction opposite our prediction: Women who had an
tistical Manual ofMental Disorders (3rd ed., rev.; DSM-III-R; Ameri- alcoholic mother were less likely to be preoccupied. As in Study
can Psychiatric Association, 1987) criteria for major depressive disorder 1, childhood experiences with parents were associated with de-
or dysthymia or were judged by the therapist to be partially remitted;
pression status (Hypothesis 1 b). Women who had more positive
(d) between the ages of 18 and 65; (e) had no history of organic brain
disorder, mania, hypomania, or psychosis; (f) had no alcohol or drug childhood experiences with their mother (p < .0001) and with
abuse within the past 12 months; and (g) were not suffering from a ter- their father (p = .011) were less likely to have experienced a
minal illness. Except for two women whose therapists judged them to clinical depression.
be partially remitted, all recovering depressed women did not currently Depression status also was associated with adult attachment
meet the DSM-III-R criteria for major depressive disorder. style in the predicted manner. Table 3 indicates that recovering
Control respondents (i.e., nondepressed women) were recruited from depressed women were more likely to report fearful avoidance
the nonexecutive staff of Baystate Medical Center. All control respon- than were the nondepressed women; however, unlike their col-
dents met the following criteria: (a) married or living with the same lege counterparts in Study 1, they were not significantly more
partner for at least 7 months, (b) between the ages of 18 and 65, (c) no preoccupied. In contrast with the Study 1findingthat mildly
evidence of current or past psychiatric disorder, (d) no evidence of se- depressed women showed greater fearful avoidance and preoc-
vere physical illness, and (e) a BDI score of less than 12. All nonde-
cupation, this finding suggests that experiencing a clinical de-
pressed women had BDI scores of 9 or less (the standard cutpoint for
nondepression) except for two women who had borderline scores of 10 pression may be linked specifically to fearful avoidance (i.e., a
and 11. (Results from analyses excluding the two women with slightly negative view of the self and of others).
higher BDI scores were virtually identical to those reported in the re-
sults section.) On average, recovering depressed women scored higher Prediction ofRelationship Functioning
on the BDI than did nondepressed women (Ms = 7.92 and 4.87), F(l,
45) = 4.66, p < .05, but the means for both groups fell within the non- As in Study 1, we performed a series of hierarchical multiple
depressed range (i.e., scores of 9 or less). regressions to assess the relative contributions of quality of pa-
Participants had been married an average of 13.7 years, had an aver- rental relationships, adult attachment style, and depression sta-
age of 2.4 children, had some college experience, had an average annual tus to relationship functioning. The experiences-with-parents
family income of approximately $40,000, and were predominantly
White. Recovering depressed and nondepressed respondents did not
differ significantly in length of marriage, annual income, education, or
number of children. Table 3
Correlations Among the Predictor Variables: Study 2
Procedure
Measure Avoidance Preoccupation Depression
Respondents were recruited using a two-stage procedure. Therapists
asked eligible clients to participate in a study of marriage and family Mother
life. If clients were interested, the therapist requested their permission Positive experiences -.29* -.05 -.56***
to be contacted by a researcher to schedule an interview. Clients who Negative experiences
agreed to be contacted were told that an interviewer would call within Controlling .18 .09 .28
Roles reversed .11 -.20 .05
the next few days to arrange a time to be interviewed. The therapist Alcohol or drug
emphasized that participation was entirely voluntary and unconnected problem -.10 -.39** -.21
to their treatment. The nondepressed respondents were recruited from Father
the staff at the hospital by advertising in the hospital newsletter. Inter- Positive experiences -.17 -.23 -.38*
ested employees also were contacted by phone to schedule an interview. Negative experiences
Interviews took place at Baystate Medical Center and lasted approxi- Controlling .28 .21 .29*
mately 2 hrs. At the beginning of the interview, respondents answered Roles reversed .20 -.01 -.22
questions about their background and then completed the question- Alcohol or drug
problem .24 .11 .21
naire. At the end of the session, the interviewer thanked respondents for
Depression status .41** .19
participating and paid them $30. The procedure and materials were the
same as in Study 1 except that all relationship questions were answered Note. Significance tests are two-tailed. Sample size ranges from 44-
in reference to the respondent's spouse, and the relationship satisfaction 47; the variation reflects missing data.
measure was replaced by the Dyadic Adjustment Scale (DAS; Spanier, *p<.05. **/?<.01. ***p<.0001.
DEPRESSION AND WORKING MODELS 135

variable always was entered at Step 1, followed by the adult at- depressed college women scored higher on average on the BDI
tachment scores at Step 2 and depression status (nondepressed than did the recovering depressed women, but we cannot deter-
or recovering from depression) at Step 3. As in Study 1, we also mine whether any of the college women would have met the
performed a second set of regressions in which depression status DSM-HI-R criteria for depression. We did have access to the
was entered before adult attachment. recovering depressed women's earlier BDI scores that were
Table 4 shows the regression results, including the standard- taken when they were diagnosed with a clinical depression. The
ized regression coefficients and the percentage of variance ac- average BDI score for the recovering depressed women when
counted for (R2) at each step. In contrast with thefindingsfrom they met the DSM-III-R criteria for depression was 25.3, fully
Study 1, positive experiences with mother were not associated 10 points higher than the average of 14.7 for the college student
with relationship functioning. Additional analyses including depressed sample, which suggests that the sample of recovering
negative experiences with the mother and positive and negative women previously had experienced more severe depression
experiences with the father also indicated that these childhood than the currently depressed college students. Thus, greater
experiences were not significantly associated with any of the fearful avoidance may be associated with vulnerability to a clin-
current relationship-functioning variables. ical depression. The possibility remains, however, that the
The findings for the adult attachment style variables, how- different patterns in working models arose from other differ-
ever, were consistent with those of Study 1; these variables were ences between the two samples, such as differences in marital
the strongest predictors of relationship functioning. Women status, age, or income.
who were more avoidant or more preoccupied showed lower One limitation of Study 2 is that we were unable to include
marital adjustment and reported less supportive exchanges with women at the height of their depressive episode; the women in
their spouse. In addition, women who were more avoidant were our study had partially recovered from their depression and no
less likely to evidence a constructive conflict resolution style. longer met the criteria for clinical depression. Despite this fact,
Table 4 indicates that these associations emerged whether the the recovering depressed women still differed from the nonde-
attachment style variables were entered into the equation before pressed sample in their working models of others. Whether
or after depression status. Whereas depression status contrib- these differences are stronger during the peak of a depression or
uted weakly to the prediction of relationship functioning in whether they persist after full recovery remains to be deter-
Study 1, it did not predict relationship functioning in Study 2, mined. Other work (cf. Barnett & Gotlib, 1988) suggests, how-
whether entered before or after attachment style.5 ever, that the nature of working models of others may not
As in Study 1, depression status was measured as a dichoto- change after recovery from depression. For example, interper-
mous variable, whereas attachment style was measured as a sonal dependency and introversion, which are viewed as vulner-
continuous variable. The restriction of range for depression sta- ability factors to depression, are found to endure even after full
tus may have masked its contribution to relationship function- recovery from depression. Compared with nondepressed sub-
ing. Because women in Study 2 also had completed the BDI, we jects, remitted depressives reported low social self-confidence
were able to reanalyze the data using BDI scores as a continuous and high emotional dependency (Hirschfeld, Klerman, Clay-
measure of depression status. Although these scores also were ton, & Keller, 1983; Hirschfeld, Klerman, Clayton, Keller, &
somewhat limited in range (0-22) and 78.7% of the sample Andersen, 1984; Pilowsky & Katsikitis, 1983; Reich, Noyes,
scored 9 or below, the results of these analyses paralleled those Hirschfeld, Coryell, & O'Gorman, 1987). If interpersonal de-
using the dichotomous scores; depression did not contribute pendency is similar to working models of attachment, then in-
significantly to the prediction of any of the relationship-func- secure attachment styles, like interpersonal dependency, may
tioning variables. persist after recovery from a depressive episode. Thus, the pos-
sibility that attachment style is a stable predisposing factor to
depression that does notfluctuategreatly with depressed mood
Discussion deserves further investigation.
Consistent with Study 1, attachment style was the strongest A second limitation is that we did not include a group of non-
predictor of relationship functioning. The results from both depressed individuals who showed other psychiatric symptoms.
studies suggest that individuals' mental models of others impor- It is possible that a fearful avoidant style is linked to disorders
tantly influence romantic relationship functioning for both dat- other than depression. We would hypothesize, however, that
ing relationships (as found in Study 1) and longer term marital different insecure working models are likely to lead to different
relationships (Study 2). In contrast with Study 1, positive expe- emotional consequences (see Bartholomew, 1990). For exam-
riences with mother did not predict marital functioning. It may ple, whereas individuals with a fearful avoidant style may be
be that early attachment experiences with parents become less more likely to become depressed, those with a dismissing-
salient or influential in determining the quality of marital rela- avoidant style (i.e., a positive view of self and a negative view of
tionships, which carry their own lengthy history of experience. others) may be more likely to develop antisocial behavior pat-
In contrast with thefindingsfrom Study 1, depression status terns. Indeed, individuals with a dismissing style evidence a high
was linked specifically to fearful avoidance (i.e., negative view
of self and of others) and not to preoccupation (i.e., negative 5
Again, we performed another set of regressions entering depression
view of self and positive view of others) in Study 2. One possible status at Step 1 (see Footnote 4). Depression status did not significantly
reason for this difference is that the recovering depressed predict any of the relationship-functioning variables; attachment style
women had experienced a more severe depression than the de- showed the same effects as in the primary analyses and remained the
pressed college women. At the time of the study, however, the best predictor of relationship functioning.
136 K. CARNELLEY, P. PIETROMONACO, AND K. JAFFE

'o c degree of overt hostility (Kobak & Sceery, 1988). Nevertheless,


ityl ^ JS
o
the specificity of a fearful avoidant style to depression remains
to be determined.
c
I
B x: General Discussion
ed bef ore at

u
The present research sought to integrate cognitive and inter-
personal approaches to depression by examining depressives'
&I internal working models of attachment and their connection to
n functioning in close romantic relationships. Consistent with at-
D
3 tachment theory, we found that depression was associated with
an insecure working model of attachment. College women who
ions

!l I I were mildly depressed (or dysphoric) evidenced greater preoc-


O,
cupation with attachment and fearful avoidance of attachment
than did nondepressed college women, whereas women who
a were recovering from a clinical depression evidenced greater
fearful avoidance, but not greater preoccupation, than did non-
depressed women. We also found that both mildly depressed
women (Study 1) and recovering depressed women (Study 2)
o were less likely to have experienced positive relationships with
their parents.
Thesefindingsinvite further longitudinal investigation to de-
l termine whether particular working models of others increase
vulnerability to depression or whether a depressed mood alters
1 individuals' views of others. One limitation of thesefindingsis
3 '-5 .^ that they are based on retrospective reports and therefore may
fc '55 &'5
be biased by the respondent's current mood and current rela-
1 tionship experiences. Being in a depressed mood may lead indi-
viduals to recall more negative experiences from their child-
hood and about their relationships than they would if they were
o c
not experiencing such a mood state. Although some evidence
o
(Gotlib, Mount, Cordy, & Whiffen, 1988) suggests that depres-
sc o . o sives' reports of their childhood histories are stable over time
and do notfluctuatewith mood, the precise nature of the causal
o
link between working models of others and depression remains
a O to be determined.
S
^O Hi
Our findings hint that an insecure working model is linked
foi

to vulnerability to depression. In particular, a fearful avoidant


Step 3:
Beta
ioni

model (i.e., negative view of self and of others), which distin-


guished between recovering depressed women and nonde-
IJU

pressed women, is likely to be a vulnerability factor for depres-


sion. Individuals with a fearful avoidant model are more likely
ent

&2 O Tt OO i to have experienced abusive and troubled relationships with


H Co pop I
.5 their parents (cf. Brennan, Shaver, & Tobey, 1991). Similarly,
Ut;

. our data indicate that women who had experienced a clinical


c< "a * depression (and who were more likely to show fearful avoid-
I I o V
ance) also reported less positive experiences with their parents.
ing

Thus, these early experiences with parents may form the basis
for a negative model of others (and of self ),which, in turn, may
increase vulnerability to depression. An important goal for fu-
ture work will be to determine the extent to which internal
res SW

2 '*
li working models of attachment are a cause or a consequence of
3 s depression.
5 p
These findings are consistent with other work (Beck, 1983;
lea;

<<
3 4> *-* Q. O 5v" u u - "Q, O ^ > aj *- 'g Birtchnell, 1984; Blatt, Quinlan, Chevron, MacDonald, & Zur-
f||3|21o|l3|51||S ofF, 1982) suggesting that interpersonal cognitions based on past
experience contribute to depression. Blatt (Blatt et al., 1982),
Hierar

5,0H < O . Q ga.<o-Q <i


x> . D a Si V for example, assumed that individuals whose childhood experi-
ences lead them to develop a dependent style or a self-critical
DEPRESSION AND WORKING MODELS 137

style are at increased risk for depression. A dependent style re- others. As a consequence, they may choose partners who will
flects an excessive dependency on others for love, support, and confirm their negative expectations, or they may behave in a
self-affirmation, whereas a self-critical style reflects an excessive way that encourages their partners to confirm their negative ex-
concern with achievement. These styles appear conceptually pectations (cf. Sroufe & Fleeson, 1986; Swann, 1987). Thus, a
similar to a preoccupied model of attachment and an avoidant woman with a preoccupied working model may remain in a
model of attachment. Beck (1983) also has proposed socio- relationship with an avoidant man who confirms her view of
tropic versus autonomous personality styles that appear to par- others while at the same time he increases her distress (Collins
allel Blatt's styles and insecure models of attachment. All of & Read, 1990; Kirkpatrick & Davis, in press).
these personality styles are thought to predispose individuals to Depressives' expectations of others also appear to influence
depression. An important step for future work will be to identify their understanding of others' responses and lead them to nega-
differences and overlap among these styles (see Barnett & Got- tively bias their perceptions of another's supportive attempts
lib, 1988) and the conditions under which they predict the onset (Pietromonaco et al., 1992; Rook et al., 1993). An important
of depression (e.g., Hammen et al., 1989). challenge for future work will be to identify the processes that
The results of this research also indicate that an adult's ro- link depressives' cognitions about relationships to their own and
mantic attachment style is more closely linked to current rela- their partners' social behavior. More generally, such work will
tionship functioning than is depression status. Although our further researchers' understanding of the link between social
cross-sectional data cannot uncover causal links, it is possible cognitive components of personality (e.g., working models of
that disruptions in depressives' relationships derive more from attachment) and close relationships.
their mental models of others than from the depressed mood We also found that adult attachment style predicted relation-
itself. One alternative explanation, however, is that depression ship functioning better than did positive childhood experiences
contributed less strongly to relationship functioning than did with one's mother. Although college women's experiences with
attachment style because it was measured as a categorical vari- their mothers did predict relationship functioning, these expe-
able and therefore had a more restricted range than did adult riences were mediated by women's adult attachment style.
attachment style, which was measured as a continuous variable. These adult working models may be more powerful predictors
Although future work will need to rule out this possibility, it because they are likely to incorporate not only early childhood
seems likely that the depression status measure was fairly pow- experiences but also later experiences with attachment figures
erful given that the two groups of women were preselected pre- (including romantic partners) and, in particular, experiences
cisely because they had extremely different BDI scores or be- with the current partner. This idea is supported by the findings
cause they had or had not experienced a clinical depression. from Study 2, which indicated that adult attachment style, but
Furthermore, analyses of the Study 2 data using a continuous not childhood experiences, predicted current relationship func-
measure of depression yielded the same pattern of results as tioning among married women. In contrast with the college
those using the dichotomous measure. women who participated in Study 1, the women in Study 2 were
A second alternative explanation is that the relationship- older and had a longer history with their romantic partner (i.e.,
functioning items overlapped more with the attachment items spouse). Thesefindingsare consistent with Hazan and Shaver's
than with the depression items. This explanation seems unlikely (1987) finding that childhood experiences were less associated
to account for our findings because the majority of items on with adult attachment style for older respondents than for youn-
these measures differ considerably. For example, items on the ger respondents. Thus, with increasing time and experience,
DAS concern a variety of topics, including how often the couple childhood relationships may become less influential, whereas
disagrees about specific issues (e.g., finances, major decisions, later adult relationships become more influential in determin-
and household tasks), how often the couple considers divorce, ing working models of adult attachment and relationship func-
happiness in the relationship, and feelings about the future of tioning. This idea agrees with Bowlby's (1973) assumption that
the relationship. In contrast, the attachment items focus on working models are not simply reflections of early life experi-
conceptually different domains, such as emotional closeness ences but rather are revised on the basis of new experiences
(e.g., "I like to be as emotionally close as possible with my part- throughout the life span. The fact that depression status also
ner."), physical proximity (e.g., "After even a brief separation, I appeared to mediate the impact of experiences with parents for
eagerly look forward to seeing my partner."), and fear of aban- the Study 1 sample further suggests that current life experiences
donment (e.g., "I often worry my partner might leave me for that may be incorporated into depressives' negative models of
someone else."). self also are important determinants of relationship function-
Given the strong contribution of attachment style to relation- ing.
ship functioning, it will be important for interpersonal theories One question raised by the present research is whether work-
of depression (e.g., Coyne, 1976; Lewinsohn, 1974) to specify ing models of adult attachment, which are strongly linked to
how cognitive processes and, in particular, internal working functioning in close relationships, also are implicated in social
models of attachment contribute to depressives' social diffi- perception in general. Individuals' working models of attach-
culties. For example, Coyne (1976) has proposed that it is ment may contribute to their perceptions and behavior not only
difficult to provide support to a depressed person and that the in their close relationships but also in their more casual rela-
responses of others to a depressed person can serve to maintain tionships with strangers, acquaintances, and co-workers. Most
or exacerbate depressive symptoms. An attachment perspective studies of social perception (cf. Fiske & Taylor, 1991) have not
suggests that depressives' negative working models of relation- taken into account how individuals' past experiences with oth-
ships lead them to expect rejection and a lack of support from ers, such as attachment figures, might influence their percep-
138 K. CARNELLEY, P. PIETROMONACO, AND K. JAFFE

tions and evaluations of others. Recent work by Andersen and Birtchnell, J. (1984). Dependence and its relationship to depression.
Cole (1990) suggests, however, that representations of signifi- British Journal of Medical Psychology, 5 7, 215-225.
cant others can affect individuals' perceptions of a new person. Blatt, S. J., Quinlan, D., Chevron, E., McDonald, C , & Zuroff, D.
Closer attention to the link between existing working models of (1982). Dependency and self-criticism: Psychological dimensions of
others and ongoing social perception is likely to provide a depression. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 50, 113-
124.
clearer understanding of the interplay between personality and
social processes. Blatt, S. J., Wein, S. J., Chevron, E., & Quinlan, D. M. (1979). Parental
representations and depression in normal young adults. Journal of
Since Hazan and Shaver's (Hazan & Shaver, 1987; Shaver et Abnormal Psychology, 88, 388-397.
al., 1988) original work, a growing number of researchers have Bothwell, S., & Weissman, M. M. (1977). Social impairments four years
documented a link between working models of attachment and after an acute depressive episode. American Journal ofOrthopsychia-
relationship functioning. Ourfindingsfurther refine knowledge try, 7,231-237.
about this link. The present studies suggest that working models Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment and loss: Vol. 1. Attachment. New York:
of adult attachment and childhood experiences are more closely Basic Books.
linked for younger individuals than for older individuals and Bowlby, J. (1973). Attachment and loss: Vol. 2. Separation: Anxiety and
that working models are strongly linked to relationship func- anger. New York: Basic Books.
tioning in both age groups and in both depressed and nonde- Bowlby, J. (1980). Attachment and loss: Vol. 3. Loss. New York: Basic
pressed individuals. A challenge for future work will be to de- Books.
termine the extent to which working models of attachment Bowlby, J. (1988). A secure base: Parent-child attachment and healthy
affect and are affected by interactions between romantic part- human development. New York: Basic Books.
ners. Brennan, K. A., & Shaver, P. R. (in press). Dimensions of adult attach-
ment and the dynamics of romantic relationships. Personality and
Social Psychology Bulletin.
References Brennan, K. A., Shaver, P. R., & Tobey, A. E. (1991). Attachment styles,
gender, and parental problem drinking. Journal of Social and Per-
Abramson, L. Y., Seligman, M. E. P., & Teasdale, J. (1978). Learned sonal Relationships, 8. 451-466.
helplessness in humans: Critique and reformulation. Journal of Ab- Bretherton, I. (1985). Attachment theory: Retrospect and prospect. In
normal Psychology, 87, 49-74. I. Bretherton & E. Waters (Eds.), Growing points in attachment the-
Ainsworth, M. D. S., Blehar, M. C , Waters, E., & Wall, S. (1978). Pal- ory and research. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child
terns of attachment: A psychological study of the strange situation. Development, 50(1-2, Serial No. 209).
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Brown, G. W., & Harris, T. O. (1978). Social origins of depression. Lon-
Alloy, L. B., & Ahrens, A. H. (1987). Depression and pessimism for the don: Tavistock and New York: Free Press.
future: Biased use of statistically relevant information in predictions Carnelley, K. B., & Janoff-Bulman, R. (1992). Optimism about love
for self versus other. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, relationships: General vs. specific lessons from one's personal experi-
366-378. ences. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 9, 5-20.
American Psychiatric Association. (1987). Diagnostic and statistical Chodorow, N. (1978). The reproduction of mothering: Psychoanalysis
manual of mental disorders (Rev. 3rded.). Washington, DC: Author. and the sociology of gender Berkeley: University of California Press.
Andersen, S. M., & Cole, S. W. (1990). "Do I know you?": The role of Coates, D., & Wortman, C. B. (1980). Depression maintenance and
significant others in general social perception. Journal of Personality interpersonal control. In A. Baum & J. Singer (Eds.), Advances in
and Social Psychology, 59, 384-399. environmental psychology (Vol. 2, pp. 149-182). Hillsdale, NJ: Erl-
Bargh, J., & Tota, M. E. (1988). Context-dependent automatic process- baum.
ing in depression: Accessibility of negative constructs with regard to Coleman, R. E., & Miller, A. G. (1975). The relationship between de-
self but not others. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, pression and marital maladjustment in a clinic population: A
925-939. multitrait multimethod study. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
Barnett, P. A., & Gotlib, I. H. (1988). Psychosocial functioning and de- Psychology, 43, 647-651.
pression: Distinguishing among antecedents, concomitants, and con- Collins, N. L., & Read, S. J. (1990). Adult attachment, working models,
sequences. Psychological Bulletin, 104, 97-126. and relationship quality in dating couples. Journal of Personality and
Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator-mediator vari-
Social Psychology, 58, 644-663.
able distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, strate-
Coyne, J. C. (1976). Toward an interactional description of depression.
gic, and statistical considerations. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychiatry, 39, 28-40.
Psychology, 51, 1173-1182.
Coyne, J. C , Kessler, R. C , Tal, M., Turnbull, J., Wortman, C. B., &
Bartholomew, K. (1990). Avoidance of intimacy: An attachment per-
Greden, J. F. (1987). Living with a depressed person. Journal of Con-
spective. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 7, 147-178.
Bartholomew, K., & Horowitz, L. M. (1991). Attachment styles among sulting and Clinical Psychology, 55, 347-352.
young adults: A test of a four-category model. Journal of Personality Derry, P. A., & Kuiper, N. A. (1981). Schematic processing and self-
and Social Psychology, 61, 226-244. reference in clinical depression. Journal ofAbnormal Psychology, 90,
Beck, A. T. (1967). Depression: Clinical, experimental, and theoretical 286-297.
aspects. New \brk: Harper & Row. Feeney, J. A., & Noller, P. (1990). Attachment style as a predictor of
Beck, A. T. (1976). Cognitive therapy and the emotional disorders. New adult romantic relationships. Journal of Personality and Social Psy-
\brk: International Universities Press. chology, 58, 281-291.
Beck, A. T. (1983). Cognitive therapy of depression: New perspectives. Fiske, S. T, & Taylor, S. E. (1991). Social cognition. New York: McGraw-
In P. J. Clayton & J. E. Barrett (Eds.), Treatment of depression: Old Hill.
controversies and new approaches (pp. 265-284). New York: Raven Golin, S., Terrell, F., & Johnson, B. (1977). Depression and the illusion
Press. of control. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 86,440-442.
DEPRESSION AND WORKING MODELS 139

Golin, S., Terrell, E, Weitz, J., & Drost, P. (1979). The illusion of control of dependency and self-criticism: Implications for depression. Jour-
in depressed patients. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 88, 454-457. nal of Abnormal Psychology, 93, 3-8.
Gotlib, I. H. (1984). Depression and general psychopathology in univer- Miller, J. (1986). Toward a new psychology of women (2nd ed.). Boston:
sity students. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 93, 19-30. Beacon Press.
Gotlib, I. H., & Hooley, J. M. (1988). Depression and marital distress: Parker, G., Tupling, H., & Brown, L. B. (1979). A parental bonding
Current status and future directions. In S. Duck (Ed.), Handbook of instrument. British Journal of Medical Psychology, 52, 1-10.
personal relationships (pp. 543-570). New York: Wiley. Paykel, E. S., & Weissman, M. M. (1973). Social adjustment and de-
Gotlib, I. H., Mount, J. J., Cordy, N. I., & Whiffen, V. F. (1988). De- pression: A longitudinal study. Archives of General Psychiatry, 28,
pressed mood and perceptions of early parenting: A longitudinal in- 659-663.
vestigation. British Journal of Psychiatry, 152, 24-27. Pietromonaco, P. R. (1985). The influence of affect on self-perception
Gotlib, I. H., & Whiffen, V. E. (1989). Depression and marital function- in depression. Social Cognition, 3, 121 -134.
ing: An examination of specificity and gender differences. Journal of Pietromonaco, P. R., & Carnelley, K. B. (in press). Gender and working
Abnormal Psychology, 98, 23-30. models of attachment: Consequences for perceptions of self and ro-
Hammen, C , Ellicott, A., Gitlin, M., & Jamison, K. R. (1989). Socio- mantic relationships. Personal Relationships.
tropy/autonomy and vulnerability to specific life events in patients Pietromonaco, P. R., & Markus, H. (1985). The nature of negative
with unipolar depression and bipolar disorders. Journal of Abnormal thoughts in depression. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
Psychology, 98, 154-160. 48, 799-807.
Hazan, C , & Shaver, P. (1987). Romantic love conceptualized as an Pietromonaco, P. R., & Rook, K. S. (1987). Decision style in depression:
attachment process. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, The contribution of perceived risks versus benefits. Journal of Person-
511-524. ality and Social Psychology, 52, 399-408.
Hinchliffe, M., Hooper, D., & Roberts, F. J. (1978). The melancholy Pietromonaco, P. R., Rook, K. S., & Lewis, M. (1992). Accuracy in
marriage. New \brk: Wiley. perceptions of interpersonal interactions: Effects of dysphoria, friend-
ship, and similarity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63,
Hirschfeld, R. M. A., Klerman, G. L., Clayton, P. J., & Keller, M. B.
247-259.
(1983). Personality and depression: Empirical findings. Archives of
Pilowsky, I., & Katsikitis, M. (1983). Depressive illness and dependency.
General Psychiatry, 40, 993-998.
Ada Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 66, 11-14.
Hirschfeld, R. M., Klerman, G. L., Clayton, P. J., Keller, M. B., & An-
Pyszczynski, T, Holt, K., & Greenberg, J. (1987). Depression, self-fo-
dersen, N. C. (1984). Personality and gender-related differences in de-
cused attention, and expectancies for positive and negative future life
pression. Journal of Affective Disorders, 7, 211-221.
events for self and others. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol-
Kahn, J., Coyne, J. C , & Margolin, G. (1985). Depression and marital
ogy, 52,994-1001.
disagreement: The social construction of despair. Journal of Social
Reich, J., Noyes, R., Hirschfeld, R., Coryell, W., & O'Gorman, T.
and Personal Relationships, 2, Ml-Ad 1.
(1987). State and personality in depressed and panic patients. Ameri-
Kirkpatrick, L. A., & Davis, K. E. (in press). Attachment style, gender,
can Journal of Psychiatry, 144, 181-187.
and relationship stability: A longitudinal analysis. Journal of Person-
Richman, J. A., & Flaherty, J. A. (1987). Adult psychosocial assets and
ality and Social Psychology.
depressive mood over time: Effects of internalized childhood attach-
Kobak, R. R., & Sceery, A. (1988). Attachment in late adolescence:
ments. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 175, 703-712.
Working models, affect regulation, and representations of self and
Ricks, M. H. (1985). The social transmission of parental behavior: At-
others. Child Development, 59, 135-146.
tachment across generations. Monographs of the Society for Research
Kowalik, D. L., & Gotlib, I. H. (1987). Depression and marital interac-
in Child Development, 50(1-2, Serial No. 209).
tion: Concordance between intent and perception of communication.
Rook, K. S. (1984). The negative side of social interaction: Impact on
Journal ofAbnormal Psychology, 96, 127-134.
psychological well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol-
Latty-Mann, H., & Davis, K. (1989). Relationship style development
ogy, 46, 1097-1108.
study questionnaire. Unpublished measure, University of South Car-
Rook, K. S., Pietromonaco, P. R., & Lewis, M. A. (1993). When are
olina.
depressives distressing to others and vice-versa?: Effects offriendship,
Levinger, G., & Pietromonaco, P. (1989). Conflict Style Inventory. Un- similarity, and interaction task. Manuscript submitted for publica-
published scale, University of Massachusetts, Amherst. tion.
Lewinsohn, P. M. (1974). A behavioral approach to depression. In R. J. Ruscher, S. M., & Gotlib, I. H. (1988). Marital interaction patterns of
Friedman & M. Katz (Eds.), The psychology of depression: Contem- couples with and without a depressed partner. Behavior Therapy, 19,
porary theory and research (pp. 112-139). Washington, DC: Win- 455-470.
ston-Wiley. Schuster, T. L., Kessler, R. C , & Aseltine, R. H., Jr. (1990). Supportive
Lewinsohn, P. M., Hoberman, H. M., & Rosenbaum, M. (1988). A interactions, negative interactions, and depressed mood. American
prospective study of risk factors for unipolar depression. Journal of Journal of Community Psychology, 18, 423-438.
Abnormal Psychology, 97, 251-264. Schwarz, J. C , & Zuroff, D. C. (1979). Family structure and depression
MacDonald, M. R., & Kuiper, N. A. (1984). Self-schema decision con- in female college students: Effects of parental conflict, decision-mak-
sistency in clinical depressives. Journal of Social and Clinical Psy- ing power, and inconsistency of love. Journal of Abnormal Psychology,
chology, 2, 264-272. 88, 398-406.
Mam, M., Kaplan, N., & Cassidy, J. (1985). Security in infancy, child- Shaver, P., Hazan, C , & Bradshaw, D. (1988). Love as attachment: The
hood, and adulthood: A move to the level of representation. Mono- integration of three behavioral systems. In R. J. Sternberg & M.
graphs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 50(1-2, Se- Barnes (Eds.), The psychology of love (pp. 68-99). New Haven, CT:
rial No. 209). Yale University Press.
Martin, D. J., Abramson, L. Y, & Alloy, L. B. (1984). Illusion of control Siegel, S. J., & Alloy, L. B. (1990). Interpersonal perceptions and conse-
for self and others in depressed and nondepressed college students. quences of depressive-significant other relationships: A naturalistic
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 46, 125-136. study of college roommates. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 99,
McCranie, E. W., & Bass, J. D. (1984). Childhood family antecedents 361-373.
140 K. CARNELLEY, P. PIETROMONACO, AND K. JAFFE

Simpson, J. A. (1990). Influence of attachment styles on romantic rela- Teasdale, J. D., Taylor, R., & Fogarty, S. J. (1980). Effects of induced
tionships. Journal ofPersonality and Social Psychology, 59, 971 -980. elation-depression on the accessibility of memories of happy and un-
Spanier, G. B. (1976). Measuring dyadic adjustment: New scales for happy experiences. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 18, 339-346.
assessing the quality of marriage and similar dyads. Journal of Mar- Weissman, M. M, & Paykel, E. S. (1974). The depressed woman: A
riage and the Family, 38, 15-28. study ofsocial relationships. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Sroufe, L. A., & Fleeson, J. (1986). Attachment and the construction of
relationships. In W. Hartup & Z. Rubin (Eds.), Relationships and
development (pp. 51-71). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Received April 17, 1992
Swann, W. B., Jr. (1987). Identity negotiation: Where two roads meet. Revision received June 24, 1993
Journal ofPersonality and Social Psychology, 53, 1038-1051. Accepted July 1, 1993

Você também pode gostar