Você está na página 1de 17

DOI 10.1515/revneuro-2012-0049 Rev. Neurosci.

2012; 23(5-6): 627643

Jarid Goodman, Kah-Chung Leong and Mark G. Packard*

Emotional modulation of multiple memory


systems: implications for the neurobiology
of post-traumatic stress disorder
Abstract: In lower animals and humans, stress/anxiety
can enhance dorsal striatal-dependent habit memory,
Introduction
at the expense of hippocampal-dependent cognitive
A major goal of the behavioral sciences is to identify the
memory. The present review considers the potential for
contributing factors in the development and persistence
this stress/anxiety-induced habit bias to explain some
of human psychopathologies. In the context of learning
aspects of post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD). In rats,
theory, researchers have developed various stimulus
anxiety induced by peripheral or intra-amygdala infu-
stimulus or stimulus-response (S-R) learning paradigms
sions of anxiogenic drugs can enhance habit memory and
that putatively generate experimental neuroses in
impair cognitive memory. In tasks in which both habit
lower animals and have suggested that learning in these
and cognitive memory processes may provide a learned
situations may reflect the development of neuroses in
solution, stress and drug-induced anxiety favors the use
humans as well (Mowrer, 1947; Wolpe, 1952; Eysenck,
of habit memory. The effect of stress and anxiety on the
1976). Building on these earlier models and considering
use of multiple memory systems in rats depends on the
the modern multiple brain systems approach to learn-
functional integrity of the basolateral amygdala. Thus,
ing and memory, recent theories have proposed that the
under robust emotional arousal, amygdala activation can
development and persistence of some psychopathologies
modulate the relative use of memory systems in a manner
may arise from the differential involvement of neuroana-
that favors habit memory. We propose a similar mecha-
tomically distinct memory systems (e.g., White, 1996;
nism may underlie the development and persistence of
McDonald et al., 2004; Packard, 2009). More specifically,
some PTSD symptoms. The traumatic memories of PTSD
it has been suggested that some psychological disorders,
patients can be deficient in hippocampus-dependent
particularly those characterized by strong habit-like
contextual or autobiographical aspects, and enhanced
behavioral features, may reflect a shift from the use of
in responding to trauma-related cues, which we suggest
a hippocampal-dependent cognitive memory system
may reflect increased involvement of the dorsal striatum.
towards a dorsal striatal-dependent habit memory
We briefly consider the potential role of a stress/anxiety-
system.
induced habit bias with regard to other psychopatholo-
In dual solution tasks that can be solved adequately
gies, including obsessive-compulsive disorder and drug
with either a habit- or cognitive-based learning system,
addiction.
pre-training, post-training or pre-retrieval drug-induced
anxiety biases animals towards the use of habit memory
Keywords: anxiety; brain; memory; stress; posttraumatic
(for a review, see Packard, 2009). This anxiety-induced
stress disorder.
habit bias may be mediated by a dynamic interplay among
the amygdala, hippocampus and dorsal striatum (Packard
*Corresponding author: Mark G. Packard, Department of and Wingard, 2004; Elliot and Packard, 2008; Wingard and
Psychology, Texas A&M University, College Station, TX 77843, USA, Packard, 2008; Packard and Gabriele, 2009). Specifically,
e-mail: mgp@psyc.tamu.edu in some learning situations that occur under high levels of
Jarid Goodman: Department of Psychology, Texas A&M University,
emotional arousal, activation of the amygdala can modu-
College Station, TX 77843, USA
Kah-Chung Leong: Department of Psychology, Texas A&M late memory in a manner that favors the use of dorsal str-
University, College Station, TX 77843, USA iatal-dependent habit memory over hippocampal-depend-
ent cognitive memory. Considering both the potential
role of anxiety and S-R learning in post-traumatic stress
disorder (PTSD), it is possible that interactions among the
amygdala, hippocampus, and dorsal striatum may under-

Brought to you by | provisional account


Unauthenticated | 132.203.227.61
Download Date | 7/16/14 7:08 PM
628 J. Goodman et al.: Multiple memory systems and post-traumatic stress disorder

lie some aspects of the disorder (Packard, 2009). In the nitive form of learning and memory mediated by a brain
present review, we further examine the stress-induced system that includes the hippocampus as a critical com-
habit bias as a potential model for the development, per- ponent, and a Hullian (Hull, 1943) or S-R habit form of
sistence and expression of some PTSD symptoms. learning mediated by a non-hippocampal-based memory
We begin by considering the literature on the emo- system. Subsequent neurobiological studies employing
tional modulation of multiple memory systems in lower dissociation methodology provide evidence that the hip-
animals, describing research indicating that stress/ pocampus and dorsal striatum mediate cognitive and
anxiety can facilitate dorsal striatal-dependent habit S-R habit forms of memory, respectively. For example, in
memory, and that this effect depends on a modulatory rats, lesions of the hippocampal system selectively impair
influence of the basolateral amygdala (BLA). In some the acquisition of cognitive tasks that require learning
learning situations, the stress/anxiety-induced bias relationships among multiple stimuli in both spatial (for
towards the use of habit memory may arise via an impair- a review, see OKeefe and Nadel, 1978) and non-spatial
ing effect on hippocampal-dependent memory. Recent (Bunsey and Eichenbaum, 1996) domains. In contrast,
evidence indicating that the stress-induced habit bias lesions of the caudate-putamen (i.e., dorsal striatum)
observed in lower animals may also exist in humans is selectively impair the acquisition of several S-R or habit-
briefly discussed. The symptomatology and diagnostic learning tasks (for a review, see Packard and Knowlton,
criteria for PTSD are summarized, and various models 2002).
of PTSD that implicate multiple memory systems are An early experiment in rats demonstrating a double
described. Considering both the animal literature and dissociation between the mnemonic functions of the hip-
the symptomatic features of PTSD, we propose that the pocampal system and dorsal striatum used two versions
stress/anxiety-induced habit bias observed in lower of a radial maze task. In the win-shift or cognitive version
animals may underlie some PTSD symptoms, namely of the task (Olton and Samuelson, 1976), each maze arm
the acquisition and expression of maladaptive behavio- is baited once during a daily session and rats learn to
ral responses to trauma-related cues. In this context, we forage among the various spatial locations and to not
review findings from relevant human behavioral and neu- return to those maze arms in which food has already been
roimaging experiments. It is important to note that a wide acquired. Acquisition of this task is severely impaired by
variety of brain structures have been implicated in PTSD lesions of the hippocampal system (Olton et al., 1979), but
(for a recent review, see Hughes and Shin, 2011) and that not by lesions of the dorsal striatum (Packard et al., 1989;
this review will focus selectively on the potential roles McDonald and White, 1994). In a win-stay or S-R habit
of the amygdala, hippocampus and dorsal striatum in version of the task, rats are reinforced for learning to
PTSD. Finally, whether the stress/anxiety-induced habit selectively approach illuminated maze arms. Acquisition
bias may influence the neurobiological and behavioral of this task is severely impaired by lesions of the dorsal
aspects of other human psychopathologies (e.g., drug striatum, but not by lesions of the hippocampal system
addiction and relapse, obsessive-compulsive disorder, (Packard et al., 1989). Similarly, in a dual-solution plus-
etc.) is briefly considered. maze task that can be acquired using either Tolmanian
place or Hullian response learning, neural inactivation
of the dorsal hippocampus selectively blocks the use of
place learning, whereas neural inactivation of the dor-
Multiple memory systems in the solateral striatum selectively blocks the use of response
mammalian brain learning (Packard and McGaugh, 1996).
Double dissociations between the roles of the hip-
The hypothesis that memory is organized in multiple pocampus and dorsal striatum in cognitive and habit
systems in the mammalian brain is supported by exten- memory are also observed following post-training intrac-
sive evidence from studies in lower animals and humans erebral drug injections (e.g., Packard and White, 1991;
(for reviews, see White and McDonald, 2002; Squire, Packard et al., 1994; Packard and Teather, 1997, 1998).
2004). Numerous theories describing the psychological Thus, post-training intra-hippocampal injections of
operating principles that may distinguish different forms dopaminergic agonists selectively facilitate memory in the
of memory have been proposed (e.g., Hirsh, 1974; OKeefe win-shift radial maze task described above, whereas
and Nadel, 1978; Mishkin and Petri, 1984; for a review, see similar injections into the dorsal striatum selectively facil-
Kesner, 1998). Several of these theories typically draw a itate memory in the win-stay radial maze task (Packard
distinction between a Tolmanian (Tolman, 1932) or cog- and White, 1991).

Brought to you by | provisional account


Unauthenticated | 132.203.227.61
Download Date | 7/16/14 7:08 PM
J. Goodman et al.: Multiple memory systems and post-traumatic stress disorder 629

Importantly, when considered across studies, double the anxiogenic drugs predominantly displayed response
dissociations between the mnemonic functions of the hip- learning (Packard and Wingard, 2004). A bias towards the
pocampus and the dorsal striatum in cognitive and habit use of response learning in the dual-solution water maze
memory have also been demonstrated in other mam- task is also observed if an anxiogenic dose of RS 79948
malian species, including monkeys and humans (for a 197 is peripherally injected prior to memory retrieval
review, see Packard, 2010). (Elliot and Packard, 2008).
Consistent with these findings, rats exposed to a
pre-training stress regimen consisting of restraint stress
and intermittent tail-shock also displayed a bias towards
Emotional arousal and the relative the use of dorsal striatal-dependent habit learning (Kim
use of multiple memory systems et al., 2001), and lower levels of trait anxiety positively
correlated with place recognition memory and with a pref-
In view of the evidence that the hippocampus and dorsal erence for the use of hippocampal-dependent place learn-
striatum mediate cognitive and habit memory respectively, ing (Hawley et al., 2011). Taken together, experiments in
studies have begun to focus on factors that can influence lower animals involving exposure to acute stress or drug-
the relative use of these two memory systems in various induced anxiety suggest that in some learning situations,
learning situations. In our laboratory we have investigated robust levels of emotional arousal can bias the brain
the hypothesis that robust emotional arousal induced by towards the use of a habit memory system.
stress or anxiety can influence the use of multiple memory
systems in rats. The results of several studies now indicate
that in learning situations in which both the hippocam-
pal and dorsal striatal memory systems can provide an Emotional modulation of cognitive
adequate learned solution, acute stress or drug-induced
anxiety can bias animals towards the use of dorsal stri-
and habit memory: role of the
atal-dependent habit memory. In one study (Packard and basolateral amygdala
Wingard, 2004) rats were trained in a dual-solution water
plus-maze task to swim from the same start position on We have recently investigated the BLA as a potential
each trial (i.e., the south arm), and reach a hidden escape neural site of action that mediates the modulatory influ-
platform that was always located in the same location ence of drug-induced anxiety on the relative use of multi-
(i.e., the west arm). In this task, making the same body ple memory systems. A possible role for this brain region
turn response and approaching the same spatial location is suggested by evidence that the BLA is involved in
are reinforced, and thus this task can be acquired using mammalian emotional behavior, including the ability of
either hippocampal-dependent place learning or dorsal various drugs to produce anxiety (e.g., Kluver and Bucy,
striatal-dependent response learning. Following acquisi- 1939; Nagy et al., 1979; Scheel-Kruger and Petersen, 1982;
tion, the use of these learning strategies can be assessed Sanders and Shekhar, 1991). In addition, the BLA exerts a
on a probe trial in which rats are started from the oppo- modulatory influence on memory processes occurring in
site start arm of the plus-maze (i.e., the north arm). In the several other brain structures, including the hippocam-
probe trial, rats that swim to the maze arm that contained pus and dorsal striatum (Packard et al., 1994, 1996;
the platform during training were designated place learn- Packard and Teather 1998; Roozendaal and McGaugh,
ers, whereas rats that made the same body turn response 1996, 1997). In an experiment investigating the effect of
that was consistently reinforced (in this example, a left pre-training injections of RS 79948197 directly into the
turn at the maze choice point) were designated response BLA, we observed that this treatment was sufficient to
learners. In order to assess the effect of drug-induced produce the same bias towards the use of response learn-
anxiety on the use of these two learning strategies, rats ing in a dual-solution plus-maze task that is produced by
received pre-training peripheral injections of yohimbine peripheral injections of the drug (Packard and Wingard,
or RS 79948197, two drugs that act as -2 adrenorecep- 2004). Thus, in a task in which both hippocampal-
tor antagonists and possess anxiogenic properties in rats dependent place learning and dorsal striatal-dependent
(e.g., Handley and Mithani, 1984; Guy and Gardner, 1985; response learning can provide an adequate solution,
White and Birkle, 2001). On a drug-free probe trial follow- peripheral or intra-BLA injections of an anxiogenic dose
ing task acquisition, vehicle-treated rats predominantly of RS 79948197 favors a facilitated use of response
displayed place learning. In contrast, rats treated with learning.

Brought to you by | provisional account


Unauthenticated | 132.203.227.61
Download Date | 7/16/14 7:08 PM
630 J. Goodman et al.: Multiple memory systems and post-traumatic stress disorder

One possible explanation for this modulatory influ- at the expense of a more cognitive learning strategy. For
ence of anxiogenic drugs in the dual-solution plus maze example, when trained to locate a win-card in a three-
task is that the drug activates BLA efferent projections and dimensional model of a room, humans exposed to an
directly enhances dorsal striatal-dependent memory pro- acute stressor (i.e., public speaking) were more likely to
cesses. Alternatively, it is possible that the drug activates associate a proximal cue with the location of the win-card
BLA efferents in a manner that impairs hippocampal- (i.e., a S-R strategy), as opposed to integrating the contex-
dependent memory processes, effectively allowing the tual elements of the room to locate the card (i.e., a spatial
dorsal striatum to guide the expression of learned behav- strategy; Schwabe et al., 2007). Subjects with higher
ior. In order to examine these two possibilities, rats were scores on a chronic stress questionnaire were also more
trained in single-solution plus-maze tasks that required likely to be S-R learners, as opposed to spatial learners, in
animals to employ either response or place learning. In a two-dimensional version of the win-card task (Schwabe
the single-solution water plus-maze tasks, the start points et al., 2008). Thus, consistent with the lower animal find-
varied across trials between north and south. For the ings described earlier, both acute and chronic stress may
place task, rats learned to approach the same spatial loca- shift humans towards the use of habit learning in dual
tion to reach the escape platform (i.e., the west arm), and solution tasks.
the body turn response at the choice point (i.e., the left or Consistent with these findings, emotional arousal
right) was equally reinforced across trials. Thus, response at the time of encoding enhances retention in a proba-
learning could not provide an adequate solution. For the bilistic classification weather prediction task (Steidl
response task, rats were required to always make the et al., 2006, 2011). In addition, human subjects exposed to
same body turn response (i.e., left) at the choice point to the socially-evaluated cold pressor test were more likely to
reach the escape platform, and the spatial location of the engage in habitual behavior as opposed to goal-directed
platform varied equally across trials. Post-training intra- behavior in a food-devaluation paradigm (Schwabe and
BLA injections of an anxiogenic dose of RS 79948197 Wolf, 2009). Importantly, learning in the weather predic-
enhanced acquisition of the response learning task and tion task and habitual behavior in the food-devaluation
impaired acquisition of the place learning task (Wingard paradigm depend, in part, upon the dorsal striatum
and Packard, 2008). Interestingly, the same pattern of (Knowlton et al., 1996; Poldrack et al., 1999; Tricomi et al.,
results in these two tasks was observed following post- 2009).
training neural inactivation of the dorsal hippocampus As suggested in lower animals, the stress/anxiety-
(Schroeder et al., 2002). Therefore, the bias towards the induced facilitation of habit memory in humans may
use of response learning produced by intra-BLA injections also stem from an impairment of hippocampal-depend-
of RS 79948197 was effectively mimicked by a reversible ent memory. Several studies in humans indicate that
lesion of the hippocampus. Taken together, these findings high levels of stress at the time of encoding or retrieval
suggest that intra-BLA administration of this anxiogenic impair learning in hippocampal-dependent memory tasks
drug impairs the modulatory influence on hippocampal- (Schwabe et al., 2009; Merz et al., 2010; Schwabe and
dependent cognitive memory, resulting in a reliance on Wolf, 2010; Thomas et al., 2010). Interestingly, subjects
dorsal striatal-dependent habit memory. Consistent with with lower scores in episodic memory tasks were more
this suggestion, neural inactivation of the BLA blocks both likely to be response learners in a dual-solution virtual
the impairing and enhancing memory modulatory effects maze task (Bohbot et al., 2011). The observation that
of peripheral injections of RS 79948197 on response and impaired hippocampal-dependent memory may result in
place learning, respectively (Packard and Gabriele, 2009). a greater use of an S-R strategy suggests that the stress-
induced facilitation of habit memory may arise indirectly,
i.e., through stress-induced impairments of the hippocam-
pal-dependent memory system. Consistent with this
Emotional arousal and multiple interpretation, stressful life events have been associated
memory systems in humans with reduced gray matter volume of the hippocampus
in humans (Papagni et al., 2011), and lower gray matter
Recent research suggests that the effects of emotional volume of the hippocampus has been associated with
arousal on multiple memory systems observed in lower the use of an S-R strategy in a dual-solution virtual maze
animals may also exist in humans. Specifically, in some (Bohbot et al., 2011).
learning situations, acute or chronic stress can bias Taken together, findings from investigating the effects
humans towards the use of a S-R habit-learning strategy, of emotional arousal on multiple memory systems in

Brought to you by | provisional account


Unauthenticated | 132.203.227.61
Download Date | 7/16/14 7:08 PM
J. Goodman et al.: Multiple memory systems and post-traumatic stress disorder 631

humans reflect the lower animal literature. In humans When considering the PTSD symptoms that relate
and lower animals, acute or chronic stress can produce a to memory processes, an interesting paradox emerges.
bias towards the use of striatal-dependent habit memory, Specifically, the DSM IV-TR suggests that PTSD patients
at the expense of hippocampal-dependent cognitive may potentially exhibit both enhanced and impaired
memory. The existence of a stress/anxiety-induced habit memory of the traumatic event. Enhancements of the
bias in humans provides further reason for considering traumatic memory become evident through the excep-
the phenomenon as a potential mechanism underlying tionally vivid and involuntary re-experiencing, or flash-
some human psychopathologies. backs, of the event. Such flashbacks are characterized
We now consider the hypothesis that a stress-induced as being triggered by trauma-related internal cues (e.g.,
bias towards the use of dorsal striatal-dependent habit anxiety) or external cues (e.g., loud noises) and as con-
memory may underlie some of the symptomatology of taining vivid perceptual features from the event, such as
PTSD. We begin with a review of PTSD symptomatology sounds, smells, and images. Importantly, when a patient
and a description of multiple memory systems theories experiences a flashback, he or she feels the event is actu-
of PTSD. Subsequently, we briefly review evidence from ally recurring, as if it were part of the present instead of
human neuroimaging studies indicating a role for the the past. These observations suggest that the version of
amygdala, hippocampus and dorsal striatum in PTSD the traumatic memory that is readily and involuntarily
symptoms. Finally, we consider the converging evidence invoked by cues fails to acknowledge the spatial or tem-
indicating a stress-induced bias towards the use of habit poral context in which the event originally took place.
memory and its implications for PTSD. This lack of contextual content may be viewed as one of
the many impairments commonly observed in traumatic
memories. Other impairments in traumatic memory
become evident through the patients inability to recall
Introduction to PTSD certain aspects of the event. Thus, amnesia, memory gaps
symptomatology and fragmentary recall of the traumatic event are rela-
tively common features in PTSD (van der Kolk and Fisler,
The Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Dis- 1995; Koss et al., 1996; Shalev et al., 1996; Tichenor et al.,
orders text revision (DSM IV-TR; American Psychiatric 1996; Mechanic et al., 1998; Yovell et al., 2003). Although
Association, 2000) classifies PTSD as an anxiety disorder a concomitant enhancement and impairment of traumatic
that may develop after experiencing a traumatic event. memory may be difficult to explain via a unitary model
According to the DSM IV-TR, the traumatic episode itself of memory, these seemingly antagonistic effects may
must have included actual or anticipated threats of death be explained more readily through a multiple memory
or serious injury to oneself or others, and the traumatic systems approach.
episode must have instilled within the patient intense
feelings of fear, helplessness or horror. In addition, the
patient must persistently re-experience the traumatic
episode. This re-experiencing may occur through intru- Multiple memory systems in PTSD
sive recollections of the event (taking the form of mental
images, thoughts, or perceptions), a sense that the trau- In view of the evidence reviewed earlier indicating that
matic event is actually recurring, emotional or physiolog- the mammalian brain contains multiple memory systems,
ical reactivity to internal or external cues that somehow the enhancement and impairment of traumatic memory
represent the traumatic event, or recurrent nightmares in PTSD may be explained by hypothesizing differen-
of the event. Importantly, these recollections often arise tial involvement of distinct memory systems. Thus, one
involuntarily. In fact, in many cases, a patients ability memory system may be highly active and lead to biased
to recall the event voluntarily proves to be impaired. encoding of certain features, and another memory system
The phenomenon of impaired recall falls under the fol- may be relatively inactive and fail to encode other features
lowing criterion: in order to make a PTSD diagnosis, a of the traumatic event. Interestingly, evidence that more
patient must display several avoidance behaviors; this than one memory system encode traumatic memories in
avoidance may include efforts to avoid stimuli that might PTSD arises from case studies involving traumatic brain
incite involuntary recollection, or may manifest itself as injury (TBI). TBI occurring during a traumatic event may
the inability to voluntarily recall important aspects of the produce both anterograde and retrograde amnesia for the
traumatic event. event. However, in some cases patients with TBI-induced

Brought to you by | provisional account


Unauthenticated | 132.203.227.61
Download Date | 7/16/14 7:08 PM
632 J. Goodman et al.: Multiple memory systems and post-traumatic stress disorder

amnesia may also develop PTSD-like symptoms, and these Interestingly, contemporary psychological theories
symptoms may prove sufficient for making a full (or at surrounding PTSD often implicate more than one memory
least partial) diagnosis of PTSD (Horton and Barrett, 1991; system to explain the development and persistence of
McMillan, 1991; Layton and Wardi-Zonna, 1995; Bryant, PTSD symptoms. For example, the dual representation
1996; McMillan, 1996; King, 1997; McGrath, 1997; Warden theory (Brewin et al., 1996) suggests the existence of a ver-
et al., 1997; Krikorian and Layton, 1998; Podoll et al., bally accessible memory (VAM) system and a situation-
2000a, b; Creamer et al., 2005; Bryant et al., 2009). Although ally accessible memory (SAM) system. The VAM system
TBI may prevent conscious recall of the traumatic event, a mediates memories that are easily retrieved through
patient may re-experience the trauma through recurrent voluntary recall. These memories often contain contex-
nightmares or through emotional and physiological reac- tual aspects of the event (i.e., time and place) and are
tivity to cues that in some manner represent the trauma. For usually integrated with other autobiographical memories.
example, in one case study, a 19-year-old male developed However, the VAM system can only encode information
PTSD symptoms after a motor vehicle accident that caused that is consciously attended to during the event. There-
TBI and consequent amnesia of the event (see Podoll fore, considering that a person amidst trauma may focus
et al., 2000a). Despite having no memory of the accident, his or her attention selectively on the immediate source of
the patient experienced recurrent nightmares involving the threat, the VAM system may fail to encode the contex-
two lights appearing like the headlights of an approach- tual aspects (i.e., time and place) surrounding the trauma.
ing motor vehicle and the feeling of a shock or concussion Unlike the VAM system, the SAM system is postulated to
going through the whole body. The patient would also mediate memories that fail to be recalled voluntarily. Mem-
experience intense psychological distress when driving ories mediated by the SAM system are instead triggered
past the location of the accident or when seeing another involuntarily through situational reminders (e.g., internal
accident on the road, and he avoided riding in vehicles or external cues). These memories may contain informa-
of the same make and model as the one he was driving tion from lower-level processes (e.g., sights, sounds and
during the crash. The patient displayed all these symptoms smells), the emotions felt during the experience (e.g., fear
without having any conscious memory of the accident. and helplessness), and the bodys initial responses to the
Case studies such as this suggest that although a traumatic trauma (e.g., a change in heart rate). Therefore, when the
memory may not be stored or recalled at a conscious level, SAM memory is later triggered by a situational reminder,
some remnants of the event may be preserved at a com- the patient re-experiences the perceptions, emotions and
paratively less conscious level. This view is consistent with bodily responses from the traumatic event. These features
the idea of at least two distinct memory systems: one medi- recalled through the SAM system culminate into a highly
ating conscious memory of the event (a cognitive, declara- distressing flashback.
tive, explicit system) and another mediating latent memory Similarly, Ehlers and Clark (2000) propose a cogni-
of the event (a habit, non-declarative, implicit system). tive model for PTSD that implicates two distinct systems.
Thus, TBI may impair the ability of the cognitive system to The authors suggest that, like other memories, trauma
encode and consciously recall the traumatic event, while memory may be encoded through either conceptual or
sparing the non-declarative/habit memory systems ability data-driven processing. Through conceptual processing,
to encode and store the memory at a relatively unconscious the mind encodes the meaning of the event and organizes
level (Layton and Wardi-Zonna, 1995). The unimpaired the memory within its appropriate context, allowing the
non-declarative/habit system may then in part subserve memory to be voluntarily retrieved at a later time. Accord-
the avoidance and emotional reactivity to trauma-related ing to the authors hypothesis, trauma victims that use
cues. Finally, it is also important to note that TBI-induced conceptual processing during the event do not develop
amnesia resembles the memory impairments observed in persistent PTSD. In contrast, through data-driven pro-
PTSD patients without TBI. For example, in both PTSD cessing the mind encodes the sensory impressions of the
patients with TBI and PTSD patients without TBI, volun- event, priming the individual to later readily respond to
tary recall of certain aspects proves to be impaired, while cues that resemble these impressions. Trauma victims
involuntary re-experiencing remains intact (e.g., Podoll that had used data-driven processing during the event fail
et al., 2000b; Yovell et al., 2003). Therefore, an impairment to recall the event voluntarily, yet show enhanced invol-
of the cognitive/declarative memory system and a concom- untary recall when cued. According to the model, more
itant preservation of the non-declarative/habit memory data-driven processing (relative to conceptual processing)
system may be relatively common processes underlying increases the chance that a trauma victim will develop
traumatic memories in PTSD. long-lasting PTSD symptoms. Consistent with this inter-

Brought to you by | provisional account


Unauthenticated | 132.203.227.61
Download Date | 7/16/14 7:08 PM
J. Goodman et al.: Multiple memory systems and post-traumatic stress disorder 633

pretation, several studies using analogues of traumatic Evidence the amygdala has a role
experiences (e.g., watching a traumatic film) found that
a tendency to use data-driven processing was associated in PTSD
with greater PTSD-like symptoms, most notably the devel-
In view of the role of the mammalian amygdala in stim-
opment of intrusive recollections (Halligan et al., 2002;
ulus-affect learning (for reviews, see Maren, 2001; White
Evans et al., 2007; Kindt et al., 2008; McKinnon et al.,
and McDonald, 2002; Davis, 2003; LeDoux, 2003) and in
2008; Ehlers et al., 2010; Laposa and Rector, 2012; Regam-
the emotional modulation of multiple memory systems
bal and Alden, 2012).
(for reviews, see Packard and Cahill, 2001; McGaugh,
There are several neurobiological models of PTSD
2004), it is perhaps not surprising that numerous studies
that incorporate more than one memory system (Layton
have implicated this brain structure in PTSD symptoma-
and Wardi-Zonna, 1995; Nadel and Jacobs, 1998; Brewin,
tology. Using various neuroimaging techniques, studies
2001; Layton and Krikorian, 2002; Rauch et al., 2006).
have consistently revealed increased activation of the
These neurobiological theories suggest, in part, that the
amygdala in PTSD subjects (Hughes and Shin, 2011). For
concomitant impairment and enhancement of traumatic
example, amygdala activation is increased in combat
memory observed in PTSD may be explained through dif-
veterans with PTSD in response to both combat-related
ferential involvement of the hippocampus and amygdala.
noises (Liberzon et al., 1999) and odors (Vermetten et al.,
For example, Layton and Krikorian (2002) propose that
2007), suggesting that the amygdala may be involved in
as a situation increases in emotional intensity the amyg-
the discrete-cued recall of the traumatic event. In another
dala becomes progressively more involved in the encod-
study, Vietnam veterans with PTSD listened to a script
ing process. At moderate levels of arousal, the amygdala
describing a traumatic event that the subject had actually
enhances hippocampal function, thus strengthening the
experienced during the war (Shin et al., 2004). Positron
consolidation of aspects of the event dependent on the
emission tomography scans revealed that the regional cer-
hippocampus (e.g., contextual aspects, integration of
ebral blood flow in the right amygdala maintained a posi-
the event into autobiographical memory, etc.). However,
tive correlation with the severity of PTSD symptoms. Using
at extreme levels of emotional intensity (such as those
similar techniques, several other studies have established
experienced under trauma) the amygdala inhibits hip-
positive correlations between amygdala activation and
pocampal function, thus leading to impairments in the
PTSD symptom severity (Rauch et al., 2000; Pissiota
consolidation of hippocampal-dependent features of
et al., 2002; Armony et al., 2005; Bryant et al., 2005;
the event. At the same time, the amygdala encodes and
Protopopescu et al., 2005; Dickie et al., 2008; Brunetti
consolidates its own version of the traumatic event,
et al., 2010). Conversely, there is evidence that in sub-
serving as the predominant locus of consolidation of
jects exposed to trauma, decreased amygdala activation
the traumatic memory. According to this hypothesis, the
may actually be associated with a failure to develop PTSD
traumatic memory mediated by the amygdala remains
(Britton et al., 2005; Osuch et al., 2008). Taken together,
noncontexual and highly emotional (two characteris-
the evidence suggests that the amygdala is likely to be
tics that are common to PTSD flashbacks). It should be
involved in the development, and potentially the expres-
noted that this view of the mnemonic role of the amyg-
sion, of PTSD symptoms. As the amygdaloid nuclei project
dala in PTSD is consistent with extensive research in
to several brain structures related to different types of
lower animals indicating that this brain structure has a
learning and memory (McGaugh, 2004), it may also be
role in fear conditioning (e.g., Davis, 1992; Maren, 2001;
reasonable to infer that heightened amygdala activation
LeDoux, 2003). In addition, neurobiological models of
during and after a traumatic event may modulate memory
PTSD (e.g., Layton and Krikorian, 2002) remain con-
via an interaction with other brain structures as well.
gruent with contemporary psychological theories. For
example, both VAM vs. SAM and conceptual vs. data-
driven theories of PTSD may potentially be understood
as complex interactions between hippocampal-depend-
ent and amygdala-dependent memory processes (Brewin
Evidence the hippocampus has
and Holmes, 2003). Lastly, it has been hypothesized that a role in PTSD
a failure to inhibit distressing flashbacks and avoidance
behaviors in PTSD may reflect an impairment of execu- As mentioned previously, several studies report impair-
tive control resulting from hypofunction of the prefrontal ments in the traumatic memories of PTSD patients (e.g.,
cortex (Aupperle et al., 2012). van der Kolk and Fisler, 1995; Koss et al., 1996; Shalev

Brought to you by | provisional account


Unauthenticated | 132.203.227.61
Download Date | 7/16/14 7:08 PM
634 J. Goodman et al.: Multiple memory systems and post-traumatic stress disorder

et al., 1996; Tichenor et al., 1996; Mechanic et al., 1998; In addition to reduced hippocampal volume, neu-
Yovell et al., 2003). These impairments include a lack of roimaging studies reveal decreased activation of the
temporal or spatial context, failure to integrate the event hippocampus in patients with PTSD (for reviews, see
into autobiographical memory, inability to intentionally Francati et al., 2007; Hughes and Shin, 2011). Typically,
recall important aspects of the traumatic event, and frag- these experiments measure hippocampal activation while
mented or disorganized recall of the traumatic memory. the subject performs a hippocampal-dependent memory
Also, relative to healthy controls, PTSD patients tend to task. For example, researchers trained individuals with
perform poorly on declarative memory tasks (for a brief PTSD and healthy individuals who had not been exposed
review, see Bremner, 2006). Considering their character- to trauma on a virtual version of the Morris water maze
istics, these memory impairments may reflect a functional (Astur et al., 2006). While performing the task, the PTSD
decline of hippocampal-dependent cognitive memory pro- group showed no significant activation in the hippocam-
cesses in PTSD (Bremner, 2006). There are several lines of pus, whereas individuals without PTSD showed a robust
research in support of this idea that indicate abnormali- increase in hippocampal activation. Moreover, a negative
ties of the hippocampus in PTSD patients. For example, correlation between hippocampal activation and sever-
several neuroimaging studies suggest that patients with ity of PTSD symptoms was observed (Astur et al., 2006).
PTSD have a reduced hippocampal volume (for a review, It should be noted that, although reduced hippocampal
see Bremner, 2007; and also Bonne et al., 2001; Carrion et activation is often observed, some studies have reported
al., 2001; De Bellis et al., 2001; Fennema-Notestine et al., increases in hippocampal activation in PTSD patients
2002; Pederson et al., 2004; Golier et al., 2005). Moreover, (Werner et al., 2008; Whalley et al., 2009). The reason for
neuroimaging studies have established a negative corre- this discrepancy remains uncertain, but it may depend on
lation between hippocampal volume and the severity of the features of the particular learning task or the analyses
PTSD symptoms (Gilbertson et al., 2002; Villarreal et al., used for the scans (Francati et al., 2007; Hughes and Shin,
2002; Bremner et al., 2003). 2011).
In view of extensive evidence that behavioral stress-
ors and stress hormones can be detrimental to hip-
pocampal structure and morphology (Watanabe et al.,
1992; Gould et al., 1998; Kim and Diamond, 2002), it
Evidence the dorsal striatum has
is possible that the traumatic event and the distress it a role in PTSD
may engender reduces the volume of the hippocampus.
However, there is also evidence that a prior reduction As described earlier, there is extensive evidence from
in hippocampal volume may serve as a risk factor for studies in lower animals and humans indicating that
developing PTSD (e.g., Pitman et al., 2006). Indeed, the dorsal striatum has a selective role in S-R/habit
whether the reduction in hippocampal volume pre- learning and memory. In PTSD, the learning of behav-
cedes or follows the traumatic event and the develop- ioral responses to trauma-related cues may be a form
ment of PTSD symptoms remains debatable (Bremner, of S-R learning. PTSD patients may learn in part to
2001; Pitman, 2001). Results in favor of the latter view associate discrete stimuli/cues surrounding the trauma
(i.e., that traumatic stress reduces hippocampal volume) (e.g., loud noises) with the trauma itself and have sub-
came from one study that measured hippocampal gray sequently learned to respond to the cues as if the cues
matter volume in healthy human subjects at two dif- themselves posed a serious threat. This automatic/habit-
ferent time points, separated by a three-month interval ual responding to cues without regard to the spatial or
(Papagni et al., 2011). The number of stressful life events temporal context in which they occur is characteristic
that a participant experienced during the three-month of both PTSD symptomatology and S-R (or habit) learn-
interval was positively correlated with a reduction in ing. Indeed, the idea that S-R learning may be associ-
gray matter volume of the right hippocampus, suggest- ated with PTSD pathology is not a new concept. In the
ing that the stressful life events preceded and contrib- 1980s, drawing from Mowrers earlier (1960) two-factor
uted to the reduction in hippocampal volume. Experi- learning theory, researchers attempted to describe the
ments in animals yield potentially similar results (Kim development of PTSD as involving two distinct learning
and Diamond, 2002). For example, rats subjected to phases, one characterized by classical conditioning and
21 days of chronic restraint stress showed a 3% reduc- the other by instrumental learning (for a review, see Foa
tion in hippocampal gray matter volume relative to non- et al., 1989). In the initial phase, a trauma victim learns
stressed controls (Lee et al., 2009). to associate spatially or temporally proximal sounds,

Brought to you by | provisional account


Unauthenticated | 132.203.227.61
Download Date | 7/16/14 7:08 PM
J. Goodman et al.: Multiple memory systems and post-traumatic stress disorder 635

smells and images (i.e., the conditioned stimuli; CSs) Evidence the noradrenergic system
with the central threat (i.e., the unconditioned stimulus;
US) so that when the person later encounters the previ- has a role in PTSD
ously neutral CSs he or she re-experiences the tremen-
In lower animals, the effect of anxiety on the relative use
dous fear and anxiety originally elicited by the US. This
of memory systems can be produced by drugs that increase
may be viewed as a representation of Pavlovian classi-
norepinephrine release and hence can activate adreno-
cal conditioning. In the second phase, the trauma victim
receptors in the BLA (Packard, 2009). Consistent with
learns to react to the CSs through maladaptive condi-
the hypothesis that this anxiety-mediated habit bias may
tioned responses, treating the CSs like USs, avoiding
underlie some PTSD symptoms, converging evidence indi-
them at any cost. It is this second phase of Mowrers two-
cates that PTSD patients have increased basal norepineph-
factor learning theory that may be viewed as a represen-
rine levels and show exaggerated noradrenergic responses
tation of S-R learning. Despite some explanatory power,
to trauma-related stimuli (for reviews, see Pitman and Dela-
conditioning theories of PTSD have been criticized in
hanty, 2005; Strawn and Geracioti, 2008). Pitman (1989)
part due to their inability to explain more complex PTSD
advanced a pathophysiological model of PTSD suggesting
symptoms, such as emotional numbness, hyperarousal
that stress hormones released during and after a traumatic
and memory impairment (Foa et al., 1989; Brewin and
event may lead to an overconsolidation of the traumatic
Holmes, 2003). However, S-R learning may nonetheless
memory, precipitating the intrusive recollections and con-
operate alongside other learning processes and in part
ditioned emotional responses observed in PTSD. Aside
mediate the learning of behavioral responses to trauma-
from the consolidation of traumatic experiences, it has
related cues (Ehlers and Clark, 2000; Pitman et al., 2000;
also been suggested that the retrieval of traumatic memo-
Bryant, 2005).
ries may depend on noradrenergic mechanisms (Pitman,
Consistent with the neurobehavioral findings in
1989; Pitman and Delahanty, 2005). Considering the puta-
lower animals and humans implicating the dorsal stria-
tive role of the noradrenergic system in PTSD, several
tum in S-R habit learning and with the idea that S-R
studies have investigated the treatment efficacy of antiadr-
learning may underlie some PTSD symptoms, recent neu-
energic agents (for reviews, see Pitman et al., 2006; Stein,
roimaging studies indicate that the dorsal striatum has a
2006; Strawn and Geracioti, 2008). For example, treatment
role in PTSD symptomatology. For example, researchers
with the -adrenoreceptor blocker propranolol in the acute
played personalized auditory scripts to traumatized train
aftermath of trauma or after the development of the disor-
drivers and asked them to re-imagine the traumatic event
der has been shown to reduce PTSD symptoms (Famularo
as if it were actually recurring (Nardo et al., 2011). Sub-
et al., 1988; Pitman et al., 2002; Vaiva et al., 2003; Brunet et
sequent single-photon emission computed tomography
al., 2008; Krauseneck et al., 2010). Considering the effects
analyses revealed increased regional cerebral blood flow
of norepinephrine on the relative use of memory systems
in the caudate and putamen of train drivers with PTSD,
(Packard, 2009), it is tempting to speculate that exagger-
relative to train drivers without PTSD. An additional
ated norepinephrine release during and after a traumatic
study using script-driven imagery established a positive
event may increase amygdala activation, thus mobilizing
correlation between regional cerebral blood flow in the
a shift from a hippocampal-dependent memory system
right putamen and self-reported intensity of the flash-
towards a dorsal striatal-dependent system. Disrupting
back (Osuch et al., 2001). Script-driven activations of the
the noradrenergic system with drugs like propranolol may
caudate head and putamen have also been correlated
prevent or reverse this shift to habit memory.
positively with the severity of PTSD symptoms (Mickle-
borough et al., 2011). Other findings indicate increased
activation of the caudate and putamen in PTSD patients
subjected to electrical stimulation or heat pain (Geuze
et al., 2007; Linnman et al., 2011; Mickleborough et al.,
The stress-induced habit bias:
2011). In addition to increased activation, it should be implications for PTSD
noted that some neuroimaging studies of PTSD patients
report decreased activation in regions of the dorsal stria- Research in lower animals described earlier indicates
tum as well (e.g., Lucey et al., 1997; Lindauer et al., 2004). that anxiety-induced impairment of the hippocampal-
In summary, there is a developing and increasing body dependent memory system enhances dorsolateral stri-
of evidence indicating that the dorsal striatum may be atal-dependent habit memory and that this enhancement
involved in PTSD symptoms. depends on the integrity of the basolateral amygdala. We

Brought to you by | provisional account


Unauthenticated | 132.203.227.61
Download Date | 7/16/14 7:08 PM
636 J. Goodman et al.: Multiple memory systems and post-traumatic stress disorder

propose that a similar mechanism may in part underlie monly observed in PTSD, because footshock treatment may
the development, persistence and expression of some not elicit the same high levels of stress or the prolonged
PTSD symptoms. More specifically, we suggest that during nature of traumatic experiences. Despite these inefficien-
a traumatic event the amygdala becomes highly active and cies, auditory fear conditioning may nevertheless serve as
modulates memory of the experience by impairing modu- a useful model for understanding other major PTSD symp-
latory influence on the hippocampus, essentially allowing toms, in particular the learning of emotional and behavioral
the dorsal striatum to play a prominent role in encoding responses to trauma-related cues (Foa et al., 1992; Rasmu-
and consolidating trauma-related cues. The prominence sson and Charney, 1997; Siegmund and Wotjak, 2006; for
of the dorsal striatal-dependent S-R learning system a recent review of PTSD animal models, see Berardi et al.,
may manifest in part as enhanced learning of malada- 2012). Consistent with the dorsal striatum having a role
ptive avoidance behaviors in response to these cues. In in PTSD, pre-training or post-training lesions delivered
addition, considering that pre-test anxiety also induces to the dorsal striatum in rats impaired conditioned freez-
a habit bias in lower animals (Elliot and Packard, 2008), ing behavior in response to an auditory cue, while having
we suggest that the anxiety-driven retrieval of traumatic no effect on freezing behavior in response to the context
memory may similarly engage the dorsal striatum to guide (Ferreira et al., 2003, 2008). Thus, dorsal striatal lesions
the automatic-like avoidance behaviors observed in PTSD selectively impaired learning to freeze in response to a dis-
patients. Support for these ideas comes from human neu- crete cue (i.e., S-R learning). Post-training amphetamine
roimaging research and neurobehavioral studies in lower infused directly into the dorsal striatum enhanced freez-
animals suggesting potential roles for the amygdala, hip- ing in both auditory and contextual versions of the task
pocampus and dorsal striatum in PTSD symptomatology. (White and Salinas, 2003). Interestingly, it has been dem-
As described previously, a recurrent finding among onstrated more recently that an amygdala-dorsal striatum
PTSD patients is an impaired hippocampus. The hip- system is necessary for auditory fear conditioning (Ferreira
pocampal impairments commonly observed in PTSD et al., 2008). A pre-training unilateral lesion to the central
include reductions in volume, lower activation levels and amygdaloid nucleus and contralateral lesion to the dorsal
relatively poor performance in hippocampal-dependent striatum impaired conditioned freezing, while ipsilateral
memory tasks. Likewise, common deficits observed in lesions had no effect. Lastly, the contralateral lesions did
traumatic memory may reflect hippocampal impairment. not affect conditioned freezing in a contextual version of
Although the mechanism underlying these impairments the task, suggesting that the amygdala-dorsal striatum
remains uncertain, contributing factors may include the system may mediate conditioned freezing selectively to
high levels of stress experienced both during and after the discrete cues (i.e., stimulus-response learning). Taken
traumatic episode (Bremner, 2001). Interestingly, in lower together, these experiments suggest that the acquisition of
animals, stress-induced impairments of hippocampal behavioral responses to fear-conditioned cues may depend
long-term potentiation or neurogenesis have been asso- on an indirect pathway from the amygdala to the dorsal
ciated with the use of an S-R strategy in dual solution striatum (Ferreira et al., 2008). Considering the parallels
tasks (Kim et al., 2001; Ferragud et al., 2010). Similarly, in between auditory fear conditioning and some PTSD symp-
healthy human subjects, lower hippocampal volume and tomatology, we might also speculate that the development
poor performance in episodic memory tasks have been of avoidance behaviors to trauma-related cues in PTSD
associated with an S-R strategy in a dual solution virtual may depend on an amygdala-dorsal striatum system.
maze (Bohbot et al., 2007, 2011). With this evidence, it is
tempting to speculate that trauma-induced impairments
in hippocampal structure and function may promote a
habit bias in PTSD patients. The stress-induced habit
The recent neuroimaging evidence we have summa-
rized indicates that the dorsal striatum may have a role in
bias: implications for other
PTSD symptoms. Further evidence such a role exists may psychopathologies
be gleaned from fear conditioning experiments in lower
animals. It should be noted that auditory fear conditioning In addition to PTSD, other human psychopathologies
fails to mimic all the symptoms of PTSD (e.g., emotional may similarly reflect a stress-induced facilitation of the
numbness, memory impairment, recurrent nightmares, dorsal striatal-dependent habit memory system. Consid-
etc.). In addition, we might speculate that this model fails ering both the role of anxiety and the habit-like nature of
to replicate the hippocampal memory impairment com- compulsions in obsessive-compulsive disorder (American

Brought to you by | provisional account


Unauthenticated | 132.203.227.61
Download Date | 7/16/14 7:08 PM
J. Goodman et al.: Multiple memory systems and post-traumatic stress disorder 637

Psychiatric Association, 2000), the stress-mediated habit (White, 1996). In a similar hypothesis, initial drug-taking
bias observed in lower animals may also underlie at least behavior may depend on a goal-directed learning system
some symptoms of the disorder (McDonald et al., 2004). mediated by the dorsomedial region of the striatum and
More specifically, the anxiety arising from obsessions or gradually shift to a habit-learning system mediated by the
stressful/traumatic life events may lead to the compul- dorsolateral striatum (Everitt and Robbins, 2005; Schwabe
sive behavior observed in obsessive-compulsive disorder et al., 2011). Consistent with the stress-induced habit bias
patients, and this process may depend on an interaction observed in lower animals, emotional arousal or stress may
between the amygdala, hippocampus and dorsal striatum. facilitate a shift to the use of the habit memory system in
When exposed to distressing life events or obsessions, drug addicts or reinstate drug cravings in rehabilitated
the amygdala may become highly active and, through users (Packard and Wingard, 2004; Schwabe et al., 2011).
its inputs to different learning and memory structures, In humans, major stressful life events have been associated
modulate which behavioral strategy becomes employed with increased drug abuse (Newcomb and Bentler, 1988;
to relieve the anxiety. In this situation, a highly active Kaplan and Johnson, 1992; Harrison et al., 1997; Chilcoat
amygdala may favor the use of a habit learning strategy and Breslau, 1998). Acute stressors can also increase alcohol
mediated by the dorsal striatum, as opposed to a more consumption (Higgins and Marlatt, 1975). Drug abusers
cognitive strategy, which would be mediated in part by the often report that stress and negative affective states are
hippocampus. In a recent study, researchers trained obses- common factors contributing to their relapse (Marlatt and
sive-compulsive disorder patients on a set of instrumental Gordon, 1980; Wallace, 1989), and successfully coping with
learning tasks and found that relative to healthy individu- stress is associated with the prevention of relapse in absti-
als they demonstrated deficient knowledge of action-out- nent drug-users (Marlatt and Gordon, 1980; Shiffman, 1982).
come relationships and were more likely to rely on habit- Thus, although the present review has focused on
like responses to the stimuli (Gillan et al., 2011). These emotional modulation of multiple memory systems in
results may represent the impairment of a cognitive, goal- PTSD, a stress-induced bias towards the use of habit
directed learning system and consequent reliance on the memory may have implications for the neurobiology of
habit memory system in obsessive-compulsive disorder. In several psychopathologies.
addition, a number of studies report that environmental
factors contributing to the development and persistence
of obsessive-compulsive disorder symptoms may include Conclusion
stressful or traumatic life events (Happle, 2005; Sasson et
al., 2005; Cromer et al., 2007a,b; Tolin et al., 2010; Landau The effect of stress/anxiety on the relative use of memory
et al., 2011). Lastly, several neuroimaging studies have pro- systems in the mammalian brain is partly characterized
vided evidence of abnormal structure and function of the by greater dorsolateral striatal-dependent habit learning,
amygdala, hippocampus and regions of the dorsal stria- to the detriment of hippocampus-dependent memory.
tum in obsessive-compulsive disorder patients (for recent Moreover, this stress/anxiety-induced habit bias appears to
reviews, see Del Casale et al., 2011; Milad and Rauch, 2012). depend on a functionally-intact basolateral amygdala. The
In drug abuse, the acquisition and subsequent relapse idea that a similar mechanism in part underlies the devel-
of drug-seeking behaviors may be attributed to aberrant opment, persistence and expression of avoidance behaviors
or maladaptive functioning of different memory systems in PTSD receives some support from neuroimaging and fear
(White, 1996; Di Chiara, 1999; Everitt and Robbins, 2005; conditioning experiments. Future research should inves-
Schwabe et al., 2011). Whereas the initial acquisition of drug- tigate the exact role of the dorsal striatum in PTSD symp-
taking behavior may depend on a hippocampal-dependent tomatology, in particular its potential role in the learning
memory system, extended drug use may shift the control of of avoidance behaviors in response to trauma-related cues.
drug-taking behavior to a dorsal striatal-dependent system, Received May 14, 2012; accepted July 5, 2012; previously published
increasing the rigid and habit-like nature of these behaviors online September 22, 2012

References
American Psychiatric Association (2000). Diagnostic and Statistical Armony, J.L., Corbo, V., Clement, M.H., and Brunet, A. (2005). Amygdala
Manual for Mental Disorders, 4th edition, text revision, response in patients with acute PTSD to masked and unmasked
(Washington, DC, USA: American Psychiatric Association). emotional facial expressions. Am. J. Psych. 162, 19611963.

Brought to you by | provisional account


Unauthenticated | 132.203.227.61
Download Date | 7/16/14 7:08 PM
638 J. Goodman et al.: Multiple memory systems and post-traumatic stress disorder

Astur, R.S., St Germain, S.A., Tolin, D., Ford, J., Russell, D., and correlates of generalized emotional response. Neuroscience
Stevens, M. (2006). Hippocampus function predicts severity of 168, 670679.
post-traumatic stress disorder. Cyberpsychology and Behavior Bryant, R.A. (1996). Posttraumatic stress disorder, flashbacks, and
9, 234240. pseudomemories in closed head injury. J. Trauma. Stress 9,
Aupperle, R.L., Melrose, A.J., Stein, M.B., and Paulus, M.P. (2012). 621629.
Executive function and PTSD: disengaging from trauma. Bryant, R.A. (2005). Conceptually driven psychosocial approaches
Neuropharmacology 62, 686694. of acute stress reactions. CNS Spectrums 10, 116122.
Berardi, A., Trezza, V., and Campolongo, P. (2012). Modeling specific Bryant, R.A., Felmingham, K.L., Kemp, A.H., Barton, M., Peduto,
phobias and post-traumatic stress disorder in rodents: the A.S., Rennie, C., Gordon, E., and Williams, L.M. (2005). Neural
challenge to convey both cognitive and emotional features. networks of information processing in posttraumatic stress
Rev. Neurosci., in press. disorder: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Biol.
Bohbot, V.D., Lerch, J., Thorndycraft, B., Iaria, G., and Zijdenbos, Psychiatry 58, 111118.
A.P. (2007). Gray matter differences correlate with Bryant, R.A., Creamer, M., ODonnell, M., Silove, D., Clark, C.R., and
spontaneous strategies in a human virtual navigation task. McFarlane, A.C. (2009). Post-traumatic amnesia and the nature
J. Neurosci. 27, 1007810083. of post-traumatic stress disorder after mild traumatic brain
Bohbot, V.D., Gupta, M., Banner, H., and Dahmani, L. (2011). Caudate injury. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 15, 862867.
nucleus-dependent response strategies in a virtual navigation Bunsey, M. and Eichenbaum, H. (1996). Conservation of
task are associated with lower basal cortisol and impaired hippocampal memory function in rats and humans. Nature 379,
episodic memory. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 96, 173180. 255257.
Bonne, O., Brandes, D., Gilboa, A., Gomori, J.M., Shenton, M.E., Carrion, V.G., Weems, C.F., Eliez, S., Patwardhan, A., Brown, W.,
Pitman, R.K., and Shalev, A.Y. (2001). Longitudinal MRI study Ray, R.D., Reiss, A.L. (2001). Attenuation of frontal asymmetry
of hippocampal volume in trauma survivors with PTSD. Am. J. in pediatric posttraumatic stress disorder. Biol. Psychiatry 50,
Psych. 158, 12481251. 943951.
Bremner, J.D. (2001). Hypotheses and controversies related Creamer, M., ODonnell, M.L., and Pattinson, P. (2005). Amnesia,
to effects of stress on the hippocampus: an argument for traumatic brain injury, and posttraumatic stress disorder: a
stress-induced damage to the hippocampus in patients with methodological inquiry. Behav. Res. Ther. 43, 13831389.
posttraumatic stress disorder. Hippocampus 11, 7581. Cromer, K.R., Schmidt, N.B., and Murphy, D.L. (2007a). An
Bremner, J.D. (2006). The relationship between cognitive and brain investigation of traumatic life events and obsessive-
changes in posttraumatic stress disorder. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. compulsive disorder. Behav. Res. Ther. 45, 16831691.
1071, 8086. Cromer, K.R., Schmidt, N.B., and Murphy, D.L. (2007b). Do traumatic
Bremner, J.D. (2007). Neuroimaging in posttraumatic stress disorder events influence the clinical expression of compulsive
and other stress-related disorders. Neuroimaging Clinics of hoarding? Behav. Res. Ther. 45, 25812592.
North America 17, 523538. Davis, M. (1992). The role of the amygdala in fear and anxiety. Ann.
Bremner, J.D., Vythilingam, M., Vermetten, E., Southwick, S.M., Rev. Neurosc. 15, 353375.
McGlashan, T., Nazeer, A., Khan, S., Vaccarino, L.V., Soufer, R., De Bellis, M.D., Hall, J., Boring, A.M., Frustaci, K., and Moritz, G.
Garg, P.K., et al. (2003). MRI and PET study of deficits in (2001). A pilot longitudinal study of hippocampal volumes in
hippocampal structure and function in women with childhood pediatric maltreatment-related posttraumatic stress disorder.
sexual abuse and posttraumatic stress disorder. Am. J. Psych. Biol. Psychiatry 50, 305309.
160, 924932. Del Casale, A., Kotzalidis, G.D., Rapinesi, C., Serata, D., Ambrosi, E.,
Brewin, C.R. (2001). A cognitive neuroscience account of Simonetti, A., Pompili, M., Ferracuti, S., Tatarelli, R., and
posttraumatic stress disorder and its treatment. Behav. Res. Girardi, P. (2011). Functional neuroimaging in obsessive-
Ther. 39, 373393. compulsive disorder. Neuropsychobiology 64, 6185.
Brewin, C.R. and Holmes, E.A. (2003). Psychological theories of Di Chiara, G. (1999). Drug addiction as dopamine-dependent
posttraumatic stress disorder. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 23, 339376. associative learning disorder. Eur. J. Pharmacol. 375, 1330.
Brewin, C.R., Dalgleish, T., and Joseph, S. (1996). A dual Dickie, E.W., Brunet, A., Akerib, V., and Armony, J.L. (2008). An
representation theory of post traumatic stress disorder. fMRI investigation of memory encoding in PTSD: influence of
Psychol. Rev. 103, 670686. symptom severity. Neuropsychologia 46, 15221531.
Britton, J.C., Phan, K.L., Taylor, S.F., Fig, L.M., and Liberson, I. Ehlers, A. and Clark, D.M. (2000). A cognitive model of
(2005). Corticolimbic blood flow in posttraumatic stress posttraumatic stress disorder. Behav. Res. Ther. 38, 319345.
disorder during script-driven imagery. Biol. Psychiatry 57, Ehlers, A., Suendermann, O., Boellinghaus, I., Vossbeck-Elsebusch,
832840. A., Gamer, M., Briddon, E., Martin, M.W., and Glucksman, E.
Brunet, A., Orr, S.P., Tremblay, J., Robertson, K., Nader, K., and (2010). Heart rate responses to standardized trauma-related
Pitman, R.K. (2008). Effect of post-retrieval propranolol on pictures in acute posttraumatic stress disorder. Int. J.
psychophysiologic responding during subsequent script- Psychophysiol. 78, 2734.
driven traumatic imagery in post-traumatic stress disorder. Elliot, A. and Packard, M.G. (2008). Intra-amygdala administration
J. Psychiatr. Res. 42, 503506. of an anxiogenic drug prior to retrieval biases rats towards the
Brunetti, M., Sepede, G., Mingoia, G., Catani, C., Ferretti, A., use of habit memory. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 90, 616623.
Merla, A., Del Gratta, C., Romani, G.L., and Babiloni, C. Evans, C., Ehlers, A., Mezey, G., and Clark, D.M. (2007). Intrusive
(2010). Elevation response of human amygdala to neutral memories in perpetrators of violent crime: emotions and
stimuli in mild post traumatic stress disorder: neural cognitions. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 75, 134144.

Brought to you by | provisional account


Unauthenticated | 132.203.227.61
Download Date | 7/16/14 7:08 PM
J. Goodman et al.: Multiple memory systems and post-traumatic stress disorder 639

Everitt, B.J. and Robbins, T.W. (2005). Neural systems of Halligan, S.L., Clark, D.M., and Ehlers, A. (2002). Cognitive
reinforcement for drug addiction: from actions to habits to processing, memory, and the development of PTSD symptoms:
compulsion. Nat. Neurosci. 8, 14811489. two experimental analogue studies. J. Behav. Ther. Exp.
Eysenck, H.J. (1976). The learning theory model of neurosis a new Psychitary 33, 7389.
approach. Behav. Res. Ther. 14, 251267. Handley, S.L. and Mithani, S. (1984). Effects of alpha-adrenoceptor
Famularo, R., Kinscherff, R., and Fenton, T. (1988). Propranolol agonists and antagonists in a maze-exploration model of
treatment for childhood posttraumatic stress disorder, acute fear-motivated behaviour. Naunyn Schmiedebergs Arch.
type. A pilot study. Am. J. Dis. Child. 142, 12441247. Pharmacol. 327, 15.
Fennema-Notestine, C., Stein, M.B., Kennedy, C.M., Archibald, S.L., Happle, R. (2005). Trichotemnomania: obsessive-compulsive
and Jernigan, T.L. (2002). Brain morphometry in female victims habit of cutting or shaving the hair. J. Am. Acad. Dermatol. 52,
of intimate partner violence with and without posttraumatic 157159.
stress disorder. Biol. Psychiatry 52, 10891101. Harrison, P.A., Fulkerson, J.A., and Beebe, T.J. (1997). Multiple
Ferragud, A., Haro, A., Sylvain, A., VelazqueSSanchez, C., substance use among adolescent physical and sexual abuse
Hernandez-Rabaza, V., and Canales, J.J. (2010). Enhanced victims. Child Abuse Neglect 21, 529539.
habit-based learning and decreased neurogenesis in the Hawley, W.R., Grisson, E.M., and Dohanich, G.P. (2011). The
adult hippocampus in a murine model of chronic social stress. relationships between trait anxiety, place recognition
Behav. Brain Res. 210, 134139. memory, and learning strategy. Behav. Brain Res. 216,
Ferreira, T.L., Moreira, K.M., Ikeda, D.C., Bueno, O.F.A., and Oliveira, 525530.
M.G.M. (2003). Effects of dorsal striatum lesions in tone fear Higgins, R.L. and Marlatt, G.A. (1975). Fear of interpersonal
conditioning and contextual fear conditioning. Brain Res. 987, evaluation as a determinant of alcohol consumption in male
1724. social drinkers. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 84, 644651.
Ferreira, T.L., Shammah-Lagnado, S.J., Bueno, O.F.A., Moreira, K.M., Hirsh, R. (1974). The hippocampus and contextual retrieval of
Fornari, R.V., and Oliveira, M.G.M. (2008). The indirect information from memory: A theory. Behav. Biol. 12, 421444.
amygdala-dorsal striatum pathway mediates conditioned Horton, A.M. and Barrett, D. (1991). Post-traumatic stress disorder
freezing: insights on emotional memory networks. masking head injury: a case report. Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol.
Neuroscience 153, 8494. 6, 195.
Foa, E.B., Steketee, G., and Rothbaum, B.O. (1989). Behavioral/ Hughes, K.C. and Shin, L.M. (2011). Functional neuroimaging studies
cognitive conceptualizations of post-traumatic stress disorder. of post-traumatic stress disorder. Expert Rev. Neurother. 11,
Behavior Therapy 20, 155176. 275285.
Foa, E.B., Zinbarg, R., and Rothbaum, B.O. (1992). Uncontrollability Hull, C.L. (1943). Principles of Behavior. (New York:
and unpredictability in posttraumatic-stress-disorder an Appleton-Century-Crofts).
animal-model. Psychol. Bull. 112, 218238. Kaplan, H.B. and Johnson, R.J. (1992). Relationships between
Francati, V., Vermetten, E., and Bremner, J.D. (2007). Functional circumstances surrounding initial illicit drug use and
neuroimaging studies in posttraumatic stress disorder: escalation of use: moderating effects of gender and early
review of current methods and findings. Depress. Anxiety 24, adolescent experiences. In: Vulnerability to Drug Abuse.
202218. M. Glantz and R. Pickens, eds. (Washington, DC: American
Geuze, E., Westenberg, H.G.M., Jochims, A., de Kloet, C.S., Bohus, Psychological Association), pp. 200358.
M., Vermetten, E., and Schmahl, C. (2007). Altered pain Kesner, R.P. (1998). Neurobiological views of memory. In: The
processing in veterans with posttraumatic stress disorder. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory. J.L. Martinez and R.P
Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 64, 7685. Kesner, eds. (San Diego: Academic Press), pp.361416.
Gilbertson, M.W., Shenton, M.E., Ciszewski, A., Kasai, K., Lasko, Kim, J.J. and Diamond, D.M. (2002). The stressed hippocampus,
N.B., Orr, S.P., and Pitman, R.K. (2002). Smaller hippocampal synaptic plasticity and lost memories. Nat. Rev. 3, 453462.
volume predicts pathologic vulnerability to psychological Kim, J.J., Lee, H.J., Han, J.S., and Packard, M.G. (2001). Amygdala
trauma. Nat. Neurosci. 5, 12421247. is critical for stress-induced modulation of hippocampal
Gillan, C.M., Papmeyer, M., Morein-Zamir, S., Sahakian, B.J., long-term potentiation and learning. J. Neurosci. 21,
Fineberg, N.A., Robbins, T.W., and de Wit, S. (2011). Disruption 52225228.
in the balance between goal-directed behavior and habit Kindt, M., van den Hout, M., Arntz, A., and Drost, J. (2008). The
learning in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Am. J. Psychiatry influence of data-driven versus conceptually-driven processing
168, 718726. on the development of PTSD-like symptoms. J. Behav. Ther.
Golier, J.A., Yehuda, R., De Santi, S., Segal, S., Dolan, S., and Exp. Psychiatry 39, 546557.
de Leon, M.J. (2005). Absence of hippocampal volume King, N.S. (1997). Post-traumatic stress disorder and head injury as
differences in survivors of the Nazi Holocaust with and without a dual diagnosis: islands of memory as a mechanism.
posttraumatic stress disorder. Psychiatry Res. 139, 5364. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 62, 8284.
Gould, E., Tanaput, P., McEwen, B.S., Flugge, G., and Fuchs, E. Kluver, H. and Bucy, P.C. (1939). Preliminary analysis of the
(1998). Proliferation of granule cell precursors in the dentate functions of the temporal lobes in monkeys. Arch. Neurol.
gyrus of adult monkeys is diminished by stress. Proc. Nat. Psychiat. 42, 9791000.
Acad. Sci. USA 95, 31683171. Knowlton, B.J., Mangels, J.A., and Squire, L.R. (1996). A neostriatal
Guy, A.P. and Gardner, C.R. (1985). Pharmacological characterisation habit learning system in humans. Science 273, 13991402.
of a modified social interaction model of anxiety in the rat. Koss, M.P., Figueredo, A.J., Bell, I., Tharan, M., and Tromp, S.
Neuropsychobiology 13, 194200. (1996). Traumatic memory characteristics: a cross-validated

Brought to you by | provisional account


Unauthenticated | 132.203.227.61
Download Date | 7/16/14 7:08 PM
640 J. Goodman et al.: Multiple memory systems and post-traumatic stress disorder

meditational model of response to rape among employed McDonald, R.J., Devan, B.D., and Hong, N.S. (2004). Multiple
women. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 105, 421432. memory systems: the power of interactions. Neurobiol. Learn.
Krauseneck, T., Padberg, F., Roozendaal, B., Grathwohl, M., Weis, Mem. 82, 333346.
F., Hauer, D., Kaufmann, I., Schmoeckel, M., and Schelling, McGaugh, J.L. (2004). The amygdala modulates the consolidation
G. (2010). A beta-adrenergic antagonist reduces traumatic of memories of emotionally arousing experiences. Ann. Rev.
memories and PTSD symptoms in female but not in male Neurosci. 27, 128.
patients after cardiac surgery. Physiol. Med. 40, 861869. McGrath, J. (1997). Cognitive impairment associated with
Krikorian, R. and Layton, B.S. (1998). Implicit memory in post-traumatic stress disorder and minor head injury: a case
posttraumatic stress disorder with amnesia for the traumatic report. Neurophyschol. Rehabilit. 7, 231239.
event. J. Neuropsychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 10, 359362. McKinnon, A.C., Nixon, R.D.V., and Brewer, N. (2008). The influence
Landau, D., Iervolino, A.C., Pertusa, A., Santo, S., Singh, S., and of data-driven processing on perceptions of memory quality
Mataix-Cols, D. (2011). Stressful life events and material and intrusive symptoms in children following traumatic events.
deprivation in hoarding disorder. J. Anxiety Disord. 25, Behav. Res.Ther. 46, 766775.
192202. McMillan, T.M. (1991). Post-traumatic stress disorder and severe
Laposa, J.M. and Rector, N.A. (2012). The prediction of intrusions head injury. Br. J. Psychiatry 159, 431433.
following an analogue traumatic event: peritraumatic cognitive McMillan, T.M. (1996). Post-traumatic stress disorder following
processes and anxiety-focused rumination versus rumination minor and severe closed head injury; 10 single cases. Brain
in response to intrusions. J. Behav. Ther. Exp. Psychiatry 43, Injury 10, 749758.
877883. Mechanic, M.B., Resick, P.A., and Griffin, M.G. (1998). A comparison
Layton, B. and Krikorian, R. (2002). Memory mechanisms in of normal forgetting, psychopathology and information-
posttraumatic stress disorder. J. Neuropsychiatry Clin. processing models of reported amnesia for recent sexual
Neurosci. 14, 254261. trauma. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 66, 948957.
Layton, B.S. and Wardi-Zonna, K. (1995). Posttraumatic stress Merz, C.J., Wolf, O.T., and Hennig, J. (2010). Stress impairs retrieval
disorder with neurogenic amnesia for the traumatic event. of socially relevant information. Behav. Neurosci. 124,
Clinical Neuropsychologist 9, 210. 288293.
LeDoux, J. (2003). The emotional brain, fear, and the amygdala. Cell. Mickleborough, M.J.S., Daniels, J.K., Coupland, N.J., Kao, R.,
Mol. Neurobiol. 23, 727738. Williamson, P.C., Lanius, U.F., Hegadoren, K., Schore, A.,
Lee, T., Jarome, T., Li, S.J., Kim, J.J., and Helmstetter, F.J. (2009). Densmore, M., Stevens, T., et al. (2011). Effects of trauma-
Chronic stress selectively reduces hippocampal volume in rats: related cues on pain processing in posttraumatic stress
a longitudinal magnetic resonance imaging study. Neuroreport disorder: an fMRI investigation. J. Psychiatry Neurosci. 36,
20, 15541558. 614.
Liberzon, I., Taylor, S.F., Amdur, R., Jung, T.D., Chamberlain, K.R., Milad, M.R. and Rauch, S.L. (2012). Obsessive-compulsive disorder:
Minoshima, S., Koeppe, R.A., and Fig, L.M. (1999). Brain beyond segregated cortico-striatal pathways. Trends Cogn. Sci.
activation in PTSD in response to trauma-related stimuli. Biol. 16, 4351.
Psychiatry 45, 817826. Mishkin, M. and Petri, H.L. (1984). Memories and habits: Some
Lindauer, R.J.L., Booij, J., Habraken, J.B.A., Uylings, H.B.M., Olff, implications for the analysis of learning and retention. In:
M., Carlier, I.V.E., den Heeten, G.J., van Eck-Smit, B.L.F., and Neuropsychology of Memory. L.R. Squire and N. Butters, eds.
Gersons, B.P.R. (2004). Cerebral blood flow changes during (New York: Guilford), pp.287296.
script-driven imagery in police officers with posttraumatic Mowrer, O.H. (1947). On the dual nature of learning a
stress disorder. Biol. Psychiatry 56, 853861. re-interpretation of conditioning and problem-solving.
Linnman, C., Zeffiro, T.A., Pitman, R.K., and Milad, M.R. (2011). Harvard Educ. Rev. 17, 102148.
An fMRI study of unconditioned responses in post-traumatic Mowrer, O.H. (1960). Learning theory and behavior (New York:
stress disorder. Biol. Mood Anxiety Disord. 1, 8. Wiley).
Lucey, J.V., Costa, D.C., Adshead, G., Deahl, M., Busatto, G., Nadel, L. and Jacobs, W.J. (1998). Traumatic memory is special. Curr.
Gacinovic, S., Travis, M., Pilowsky, L., Ell, P.J., Marks, I.M., Direct. Psychological Sci. 7, 154157.
et al. (1997). Brain blood flow in anxiety disorders: OCD, panic Nagy, J. Zambo, K., and Decsi, L. (1979). Anti-anxiety action of
disorder with agoraphobia, and post-traumatic stress disorder diazepam after intra-amygdaloid application in the rat. Neurop-
on 99m TcHMPAO single photon emission tomography (SPET). harmacology 18, 573576.
Br. J. Psychiatry 171, 346350. Nardo, D., Hgberg, G., Flumeri, F., Jacobsson, H., Larsson, S.A.,
Maren, S. (2001). Neurobiology of pavlovian fear conditioning. Ann. Hllstrm, T., and Pagani, M. (2011). Self-rating scales
Rev. Neurosci. 24, 897931. assessing subjective well-being and distress correlate with
Marlatt, G.A. and Gordon, J.R. (1980). Determinants of relapse: rCBF in PTSD-sensitive regions. Psychol. Med. 41, 25492561.
Implications for the maintenance of behavioral change. In: Newcomb, M.D. and Bentler, P.M. (1988). Impact of adolescent
Behavioral Medicine: Change Health Lifestyles. P. Davidson, drug use and social support on problems of young adults: a
and S. Davidson, eds. (New York: Brunner, Mazel), longitudinal study. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 97, 6475.
pp. 410452. OKeefe, J. and Nadel, L. (1978). The Hippocampus as a Cognitive
McDonald, R.J. and White, N.M. (1994). Parallel information Map. (Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press).
processing in the water maze: Evidence for independent Olton, D.S. and Samuelson, R.J. (1976). Remembrance of places
memory systems involving dorsal striatum and hippocampus. passed spatial memory in rats. J. Exp. Psych. Animal Behav.
Behav. Neural Biol. 61, 260270. Proc. 2, 97116.

Brought to you by | provisional account


Unauthenticated | 132.203.227.61
Download Date | 7/16/14 7:08 PM
J. Goodman et al.: Multiple memory systems and post-traumatic stress disorder 641

Olton, D.S., Becker, J.T., and Handelmann, G.E. (1979). Pederson, C.L., Maurer, S.H., Kaminski, P.L., Zander, K.A.,
Hippocampus, space, and memory. Behav. Brain Sci. 2, Peters, C.M., Stokes-Crowe, L.A., and Osborn, R.E. (2004).
313322. Hippocampal volume and memory performance in a
Osuch, E.A., Benson, B., Geraci, M., Podell, D., Herscovitch, P., community-based sample of women with posttraumatic stress
McCann, U.D., and Post, R.M. (2001). Regional cerebral blood disorder secondary to child abuse. J. Trauma Stress 17, 3740.
flow correlated with flashback intensity in patients with Pissiota, A., Frans, O., Fernandez, M., von Knorring, L., Fischer,
posttraumatic stress disorder. Biol. Psychiatry 50, 246253. H., and Fredrikson, M. (2002). Neurofunctional correlates of
Osuch, E.A., Willis, M.W., Bluhm, R., Ursano, R.J., and Drevets, W.C. posttraumatic stress disorder: a PET symptom provocation
(2008). Neurophysiological responses to traumatic reminders study. Eur. Arch. Psychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 252, 6875.
in the acute aftermath of serious motor vehicle collisions using Pitman, R.K. (1989). Post-traumatic stress disorder, hormones, and
[15O]-H2O positron emission tomography. Biol. Psychiatry 64, memory. Biol. Psychiatry 26, 221223.
327335. Pitman, R.K. (2001). Hippocampal diminution in PTSD: more (or
Packard, M.G. (2009). Anxiety, cognition, and habit: a multiple less?) than meets the eye. Hippocampus 11, 7374.
memory systems perspective. Brain Res. 1293, 121128. Pitman, R.K. and Delahanty, D.L. (2005). Conceptually driven
Packard, M.G. (2010). Role of basal ganglia in habit learning and pharmacologic approaches to acute trauma. CNS Spectrums
memory: Rats, monkeys, and humans. In: Handbook of Basal 10, 99106.
Ganglia Structure and Function. H. Steiner and K.Y. Tseng, eds. Pitman, R.K., Shalev, A.Y., and Orr, S.P. (2000). Posttraumatic stress
(Elsevier/Academic Press), pp.561567. disorder: emotion, conditioning, and memory. In: The New
Packard, M.G. and Cahill, L. (2001). Affective modulation of multiple Cognitive Neurosciences. M.S. Gazzaniga, ed. (Cambridge, MA:
memory systems. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 11, 752756. MIT Press), pp.11331147.
Packard, M.G. and Gabriele, A. (2009). Peripheral anxiogenic drug Pitman, R.K., Sanders, K.M., Zusman, R.M., Healy, A.R., Cheema,
injections differentially affect cognitive and habit memory: role F., Lasko, N.B., Cahill, L., and Orr, S.P. (2002). Pilot study of
of basolateral amygdale. Neuroscience 164, 457462. secondary prevention of posttraumatic stress disorder with
Packard, M.G. and Knowlton, B.J. (2002). Learning and memory propranolol. Biol. Psychiatry 51, 189192.
functions of the Basal Ganglia. Ann. Rev. Neurosci. 25, Pitman, R.K., Gilbertson, M.W., Gurvits, T.V., May, F.S., Lasko, N.B.,
563593. Metzger, L.J., Shenton, M.E., Yehuda, R., and Orr, S.P. (2006).
Packard, M.G. and McGaugh, J.L. (1996). Inactivation of Clarifying the origin of biological abnormalities in PTSD
hippocampus or caudate nucleus with lidocaine differentially through the study of identical twins discordant for combat
affects expression of place and response learning. Neurobiol. exposure. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1071, 242254.
Learn. Mem. 65, 6572. Podoll, K., Kunert, H.J., and Sa, H. (2000a). Reexperiencing
Packard, M.G. and Teather, L.A. (1997). Double dissociation nightmares of posttraumatic stress disorder in a patient with
of hippocampal and dorsal striatal memory systems by neurogenic amnesia for the traumatic event. Neurol. Psychiat.
post-training intracerebral injections of 2-amino-phosphon- Brain Res. 8, 3336.
opentanoic acid. Behav. Neurosci. 111, 543551. Podoll, K., Kunert, H.J., and Sa, H. (2000b). Posttraumatische
Packard, M.G. and Teather, L.A. (1998). Amygdala modulation Belastungsstrung bei neurogener Amnesie r das
of multiple memory systems: hippocampus and caudate- traumatische Ereignis. Fortschr Neurol Psychiat. 68, 458467.
putamen. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 69, 163203. Poldrack, R.A., Prabhakaran, V., Seger, C.A., and Gabrieli, J.D.E.
Packard, M.G. and Wingard, J.C. (2004). Amygdala and emotional (1999). Striatal activation during acquisition of a cognitive
modulation of the relative use of multiple memory systems. skill. Neuropsychology 13, 564574.
Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 82, 243252. Protopopescu, X., Pan, H., Tuescher, O., Cloitre, M., Goldstein, M.,
Packard, M.G. and White, N.M. (1991). Dissociation of hippocampus Engelien, W., Epstein, J., Yang, Y.H., Gorman, J., LeDoux, J.,
and caudate nucleus memory systems by post-training intrac- et al. (2005). Differential time courses and specificity of
erebral injection of dopamine agonists. Behav. Neurosci. 105, amygdala activity in posttraumatic stress disorder subjects
295306. and normal control subjects. Biol. Psychiatry 57, 464473.
Packard, M.G., Hirsh, R., and White, N.M. (1989). Differential effects Rasmusson, A.M. and Charney, D.S. (1997). Animal models of
of fornix and caudate nucleus lesions on two radial maze relevance to PTSD. Psychobiol. Posttraum. Stress Dis. 821,
tasks: evidence for multiple memory systems. J. Neurosci. 9, 332351.
14651472. Rauch, S.L., Whalen, P.J., Shin, L.M., McInerney, S.C., Macklin, M.L.,
Packard, M.G., Cahill, L., and McGaugh, J.L. (1994). Amygdala Lasko, N.B., Orr, S.P., and Pitman, R.K. (2000). Exaggerated
modulation of hippocampal-dependent and caudate nucleus- amygdala response response to masked facial stimuli in
dependent memory processes. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 91, posttraumatic stress disorder: a functional MRI study. Biol.
84778481. Psychiatry 47, 769776.
Packard, M.G., Introini-Collison, I.B., and McGaugh, J.L. (1996). Rauch, S.L., Shin, L.M., and Phelps, E.A. (2006). Neurocircuitry
Stria terminalis lesions attenuate the memory enhancement models of posttraumatic stress disorder and extinction: human
produced by intra-caudate nucleus injections of oxotremorine. neuroimaging research Past, present, and future. Biol.
Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 675, 278282. Psychiatry 60, 376382.
Papagni, S.A., Benetti, S., Arulanantham, S., McCrory, E., McGuire, Regambal, M.J. and Alden, L.E. (2012). The contribution of threat
P., and Mechelli, A. (2011). Effects of stressful life events probability estimates to reexperiencing symptoms: a
on human brain structure: a longitudinal voxel-based prospective analog study. J. Behav. Ther. Exp. Psychiatry 43,
morphometry study. Stress 14, 227232. 947951.

Brought to you by | provisional account


Unauthenticated | 132.203.227.61
Download Date | 7/16/14 7:08 PM
642 J. Goodman et al.: Multiple memory systems and post-traumatic stress disorder

Roozendaal, B. and McGaugh, J.L. (1996). The memory modulatory Strawn, J.R. and Geracioti, T.D. (2008). Noradrenergic dysfunction
effects of glucocorticoids depend on an intact stria terminalis. and the psychopharmacology of posttraumatic stress disorder.
Brain Res. 709, 243250. Depress. Anxiety 25, 260271.
Roozendaal, B. and McGaugh, J.L. (1997). Basolateral amygdala Thomas, K.G.F., Laurance, H.E., Nadel, L., and Jacobs, J. (2010).
lesions block the memory-enhancing effect of glucocorticoid Stress-induced impairment of spatial navigation in females.
administration in the dorsal hippocampus of rats. Eur. J. South African Journal of Psychology 40, 3243.
Neurosci. 9, 7683. Tichenor, V., Marmar, C.R., Weiss, D.S., Metzler, T.J., and
Sanders, S.K. and Shekhar, A. (1991). Blockade of GABA-A receptors Ronfeldt, H.M. (1996). The relationship of peritraumatic
in the region of the anterior basolateral amygdala of rats elicits dissociation and posttraumatic stress: findings in female
increases in heart rate and blood pressure. Brain Res. 567, Vietnam theater veterans. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 64,
101110. 10541059.
Sasson, Y., Dekel, S., Nacasch, N., Chopra, M., Zinger, Y., Amital, Tolin, D.F., Meunier, S.A., Frost, R.O., and Steketee, G. (2010).
D., and Zohar, J. (2005). Posttraumatic obsessive-compulsive Course of compulsive hoarding and its relationship to life
disorder: a case series. Psychiatry Res. 135, 145152. events. Depress. Anxiety 27, 829838.
Scheel-Kruger, J. and Petersen, E.N. (1982). Anticonflict effect of the Tolman, E.C. (1932). Purposive behavior in animals and men. (New
benzodiazepines mediated by a GABAergic mechanism in the York: Appleton-Century Crofts).
amygdala. Eur. J. Pharmacol. 82, 115116. Tricomi, E., Balleine, B.W., and ODoherty, J.P. (2009). A specific role
Schroeder, J.A., Wingard, J., and Packard, M.G. (2002). Post-training for posterior dorsolateral striatum in human habit learning.
reversible inactivation of the dorsal hippocampus reveals Eur. J. Neurosci. 29, 22252232.
interference between multiple memory systems. Hippocampus Vaiva, G., Ducroz, F., Jezequel, K., Averland, B., Lestavel, P.,
12, 280284. Brunet, A., and Marmar, C.R. (2003). Immediate treatment
Schwabe, L. and Wolf, O.T. (2009). Stress prompts habit behavior in with propranolol decreases posttraumatic stress disorder two
humans. J. Neurosci. 29, 71917198. months after trauma. Biol. Psychiatry 54, 947949.
Schwabe, L. and Wolf, O.T. (2010). Learning under stress impairs van der Kolk, B.A. and Fisler, R. (1995). Dissociation and the
memory formation. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 93, 183188. fragmentary nature of traumatic memories: overview and
Schwabe, L., Oitzl, M., Philippsen, C., Richter, S., Bohringer, A., exploratory study. J. Trauma. Stress 2, 505525.
Wippich, W., and Schchinger, H. (2007). Stress modulates the Vermetten, E., Schmahl, C., Southwick, S.M., and Bremner, J.D.
use of spatial and stimulus-response learning strategies in (2007). Positron tomographic emission study of olfactory
humans. Learn. Mem. 14, 109116. induced emotional recall in veterans with and without combat-
Schwabe, L., Dalm, S., Schchinger, and Oitzl, M. (2008). Chronic related posttraumatic stress disorder. Psychopharmacol. Bull.
stress modulates the use of spatial and stimulus-response 40, 830.
learning strategies in mice and man. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. Villarreal, G., Hamilton, D.A., Petropoulos, H., Driscoll, I., Rowland,
90, 495503. L.M., Griego, J.A., Kodituwakku, P.W., Hart, B.L., Escalona, R.,
Schwabe, L., Bhringer, A., and Wolf, O.T. (2009). Stress disrupts and Brooks, W.M. (2002). Reduced hippocampal volume and
context-dependent memory. Learn. Mem. 16, 110113. total white matter volume in posttraumatic stress disorder.
Schwabe, L., Dickinson, A., and Wolf, O.T. (2011). Stress, habits, and Biol. Psychiatry 52, 119125.
drug addiction: a psychoneuroendocrinological perspective. Wallace, B.C. (1989). Psychological and environmental
Exp. Clin. Psychopharmacol. 19, 5363. determinants of relapse in crack cocaine smokers. J. Subst.
Shalev, A.Y., Peri, T., Canetti, L., and Schreiber, S. (1996). Predictors Abuse Treat. 6, 95106.
of PTSD in injured trauma survivors: a prospective study. Am. J. Warden, D.L., Labbate, L.A., Salazar, A.M., Nelson, R., Sheley, E.,
Psychiatry 153, 219225. Staudenmeier, J., and Martin, E. (1997). Posttraumatic
Shiffman, S. (1982). Relapse following smoking cessation: a stress disorder in patients with traumatic brain injury and
situational analysis. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 50, 7186. amnesia for the event? J. Neuropsychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 9,
Shin, L.M., Orr, S.P., Carson, M.A., Rauch, S.L., Macklin, M.L., 1822.
Lasko, N.B., Peters, P.M., Metzger, L.J., Dougherty, D.D., Watanabe, Y., Gould, E., and McEwen, B. (1992). Stress induces
Cannistraro, P.A., et al. (2004). Regional cerebral blood flow atrophy of apical dendrites of hippocampal CA3 pyramidal
in the amygdala and medial prefrontal cortex during traumatic neurons. Brain Res. 588, 341345.
imagery in male and female Vietnam veterans with PTSD. Arch. Werner, N.S., Meindl, T., Engel, R.R., Rosner, R., Riedel, M.,
Gen. Psychiatry 61, 168176. Reiser, M., and Fast, K. (2008). Hippocampal function during
Siegmund, A. and Wotjak, C. (2006). Toward an animal model associative learning in patients with posttraumatic stress
of posttraumatic stress disorder. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1071, disorder. J. Psychiatr. Res. 43, 309318.
324334. Whalley, M.G., Rugg, M.D., Smith, A.P.R., Dolan, R.J., and Brewin,
Squire, L.R. (2004). Memory systems of the brain: a brief history C.R. (2009). Incidental retrieval of emotional contexts in
and current perspective. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 82, 171177. post-traumatic stress disorder and depression: an fMRI study.
Steidl, S., Mohi-uddin, S., and Anderson, A.K. (2006). Effects of Brain Cogn. 69, 98107.
emotional arousal on multiple memory systems: evidence from White, N.M. (1996). Addictive drug as reinforcers: multiple partial
declarative and procedural learning. Learn. Mem. 13, 650658. actions on memory systems. Addiction 91, 921950.
Steidl, S., Razik, F., and Anderson, A.K. (2011). Emotion enhanced White, D.A. and Birkle, D.L. (2001). The differential effects of
retention of cognitive skill learning. Emotion 11, 1219. prenatal stress in rats on the acoustic startle reflex under

Brought to you by | provisional account


Unauthenticated | 132.203.227.61
Download Date | 7/16/14 7:08 PM
J. Goodman et al.: Multiple memory systems and post-traumatic stress disorder 643

baseline conditions and in response to anxiogenic drugs. Wingard, J.C. and Packard, M.G. (2008). The amygdala and
Psychopharmacology (Berl.) 154, 169176. emotional modulation of competition between cognitive and
White, N.M. and McDonald, R.J. (2002). Multiple parallel memory habit memory. Behav. Brain Res. 193, 126131.
systems in the brain of the rat. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 77, Wolpe, J. (1952). Experimental neuroses as learned behaviour. Br. J.
125184. Psychology 43, 243268.
White, N.M. and Salinas, J.A. (2003). Mnemonic functions of dorsal Yovell, Y., Bannett, Y., and Shalev, A.Y. (2003). Amnesia for traumatic
striatum and hippocampus in aversive conditioning. Behav. events among recent survivors: a pilot study. CNS Spectrums,
Brain Res. 142, 99107. 89, 676685.

Brought to you by | provisional account


Unauthenticated | 132.203.227.61
Download Date | 7/16/14 7:08 PM

Você também pode gostar