Você está na página 1de 11

Journal of Psychosomatic Research 58 (2005) 61 71

Comparative sensitivity of a simulated driving task to self-report,


physiological, and other performance measures during
prolonged wakefulness
J. Todd Arnedt*, M. Ainsley C. Geddes, Alistair W. MacLean
Department of Psychology, Queens University, Kingston, ON, Canada
Received 28 April 2003; accepted 12 May 2004

Abstract

Objectives: The objectives of this study were to compare (1) the on motorway and urban routes. Results: In Study 1, speed vari-
sensitivity of simulated driving to self-report measures, nocturnal ability, tracking variability, and driving off the road on the driving
sleep latency tests (SLTs), and an auditory vigilance task and (2) simulator had comparable sensitivity to d V on the auditory vigilance
urban and motorway driving. Methods: Healthy males 18 to 35 task. In Study 2, driving performance was consistently worse on the
years maintained wakefulness for one night and were tested at 2400, motorway route. Conclusion: The driving simulator was equally
0230, 0500 and 0730 h. In Study 1 (n = 11), the SLTs were followed sensitive to another performance measure during prolonged
by auditory vigilance and simulated driving tasks; in Study 2 wakefulness and impairments were greater with motorway driving.
(n = 18), the SLTs were preceded and followed by simulated driving D 2005 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.

Keywords: Prolonged wakefulness; Sleepiness; Driving; Simulated driving; Sensitivity

Introduction age, male gender, higher education level and annual mileage
driven, and more time spent on motorway roads [68].
The contribution of sleepiness to large-scale disasters, Clearly, there exists a need to develop measures adequate
errors in the workplace, and motor vehicle accidents has to detect the presence of sleepiness. Currently, introspective
been increasingly recognized and studied [13]. Conditions estimates and electrophysiological measures, such as the
that have been associated with an increased risk for sleep- Multiple Sleep Latency Test [9], are the primary instruments
related accidents include shift work, the use of alcohol or used for this purpose. However, Dinges and Kribbs [10]
other drugs, and sleep disorders, in particular, obstructive have argued that measures of functional capacity might be
sleep apnea, narcolepsy, and chronic insomnia [4]. Esti- more suitable for detecting the consequences of increased
mates of the number of automobile crashes attributable to sleepiness. A number of tests are well known to be sensitive
sleepiness range widely, but there is converging evidence to conditions provoking sleepiness, such as sleep loss, and
that from 16% to 23% are sleep-related [2,5]. Among the the characteristics of such tests have been widely described.
demographic and driving behaviour characteristics associa- These characteristics include long duration [11,12], no
ted with increased risk for fall-asleep crashes are younger knowledge of results [13], complex yet uninteresting [14],
monotonous [15], familiar [13], and experimenter paced
[14,16]. These features have been used for the development
of a number of performance tests, including vigilance [11],
* Corresponding author. Sleep and Chronophysiology Laboratory, simple [17] and choice [18] reaction time, logical reasoning
Department of Psychiatry, University of Michigan, 2101 Commonwealth
Blvd., Suite D, Ann Arbor, MI 48105. Tel.: 734 764 1234; fax: 734 764
[19], and memory [20].
1229. Broadbent [21] has suggested that a battery of tests
E-mail address: tarnedt@med.umich.edu (J.T. Arnedt). assessing a variety of task-relevant characteristics should be

0022-3999/04/$ - see front matter D 2005 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.jpsychores.2004.05.002
62 J.T. Arnedt et al. / Journal of Psychosomatic Research 58 (2005) 6171

the approach of choice when assessing complex perform- vigilance task under conditions likely to produce perform-
ance. Such tasks are generally well tolerated by student ance decrements.
subjects; however, patients frequently indicate that they lack
face validity and may therefore be less inclined to give their
best performance or may refuse to participate altogether. In Methods
addition, these tests frequently have large practice effects,
making them impractical in clinical settings [22,23], and Participants
their relative sensitivity to conditions causing performance
decrements in comparison with other measures of sleepiness Eleven male participants recruited from the Introductory
is unknown. The ideal performance measure would have Psychology Subject Pool (mean age = 20.4 years, S.D. = 2.2,
high face validity, involve a highly practised skill, and range 1825; mean Epworth Sleepiness Scale [32] score 8.6,
assess a number of relevant aspects of functional capacity. S.D. = 4.7, range 218), who gave their informed consent,
For these reasons, a driving simulator provides a possible experienced one night of prolonged wakefulness. Inclusion
alternative to the use of a test battery. Driving simulators of criteria were a regular sleep schedule, neither extreme
various levels of sophistication have been used to examine evening nor morning type [33], good health, the absence of
the consequences of a variety of conditions, including sleep medications that affect daytime sleepiness or the sleep/wake
deprivation [24,25], alcohol ingestion [24,26,27], and sleep cycle, and willingness to comply with the conditions of the
disorders [28,29]. experiment. One participant had an Epworth Sleepiness
The York Driving Simulator (York Computer Technolo- Scale score of 18, which exceeds the clinical cutoff for
gies, Kingston, ON, Canada) is a low-cost, portable driving excessive daytime sleepiness [32]. Participants were asked to
simulator that offers a flexible data output, allowing for the maintain a regular sleep schedule, with bedtime between
measurement of a number of driving-related constituent 2300 and 0100 h and waking between 0700 and 0900 h, for
skills, such as vehicle control (e.g., steering and pedal the week prior to the study and to refrain from the use of
control) and on-the-road measures [e.g., lane position caffeine for 24 h and alcohol and other drugs for 48 h prior to
(tracking) and speed deviation]. Such a device could prove the study. Compliance with the protocol was confirmed using
valuable in a clinical setting to provide objective information sleep logs and an activity/substance use diary. Consistent
regarding a patients ability to operate a motor vehicle safely. with the policies of the Department of Psychology, partic-
Currently, most clinicians rely on self-report and physiolog- ipants earned a 1% increase to their class mark in
ical measures, such as the Multiple Sleep Latency Test, to Introductory Psychology for each 1 h of participation in
guide such decisions. There seems considerable potential, departmental research activities, up to a maximum of 5%.
however, for the use of performance measures to provide a Accordingly, for this study, they were awarded an extra 5% to
more objective and relevant assessment. Driving perform- their class mark. Of 14 participants who volunteered for the
ance on the York Driving Simulator has previously been study, two did not meet the inclusion criteria and one became
found to deteriorate following partial sleep deprivation (4.5-h ill on the test night for reasons unrelated to the experiment.
time in bed) and to ameliorate with slow-release caffeine
[30]. In addition, minimal practice effects on this simulator Dependent measures
were found in a sample of young participants with little
driving experience [31]. We sought to determine the degree to Self-report
which the York Driving Simulator is sensitive to the effects of The Stanford Sleepiness Scale [34] and a modified form
prolonged wakefulness in comparison with other self-report, of the Stanford Scale [35] were used to assess subjective
physiological, and behavioural measures of sleepiness. sleepiness. The latter test yields two factors provisionally
This paper describes two studies. The purpose of the first described as, respectively, ban energic or activating factorQ
study was to compare the sensitivity of the York Driving (Factor 1; range 013) and a factor brelated to conscious-
Simulator to other measures known to be sensitive to ness, sleepiness, and a loss of control over remaining awakeQ
sleepiness and to decrements in performance. The aim of the (Factor 2; range 011; Ref. [35], p. 38). Higher scores on
second study was to compare the decrements occurring both factors indicate higher levels of subjective sleepiness.
while driving under urban and motorway conditions to
evaluate more closely the degree to which task character- Physiological
istics affect simulator performance. Repeated nocturnal SLTs were conducted consistent with
procedures outlined for the Multiple Sleep Latency Test [9].
Continuous monitoring of electroencephalogram (EEG),
Study 1 electrooculogram (EOG), and electromyogram (EMG) was
conducted for each test. The test was terminated after 2 min
The aim of this study was to compare the sensitivity of of Stage 2 sleep, the first epoch of any other stage of sleep, or
the York Driving Simulator to self-report measures, repeated after 20 min without sleep onset. The resulting records were
nocturnal sleep latency tests (SLTs), and a 30-min auditory scored independently by two scorers, and any discrepancies
J.T. Arnedt et al. / Journal of Psychosomatic Research 58 (2005) 6171 63

not resolved by consultation were decided by a third scorer.


Electrophysiological activity was also recorded while
participants performed a 30-min auditory vigilance task,
and records were scored in 30-s epochs using standard
procedures [36]. A score of 30 min was given to participants Urban Route
who showed no signs of sleep onset on this task. Consistent
with guidelines for the Multiple Sleep Latency Test, the
dependent variable for both physiological tests is the latency
from lights out to the first epoch scored as Stage 1 sleep.

Performance
The auditory vigilance task [37] required participants to
listen to a series of blocks of 10 tones of constant pitch
Motorway Route
(1000 Hz) with a fixed intertone interval of 5 s. Noise tones
had a 1.0-s duration, while signal tones were 1.2-s long.
Each had an 82-dB SPL intensity and was played against a
background noise of 32 dB SPL. The signal tones occurred
once in each block of 10 tones and were randomly
interspersed among the nine noise tones. Participants were
instructed to sit comfortably and press a microswitch with Fig. 1. The urban and motorway routes of the York Driving Simulator.
the preferred hand when they thought they heard a signal
tone. The dependent variables reported here are the widely
used signal detection parameters d prime (d V) and Beta ( b). The dependent variables on the simulated driving task
The former is typically interpreted as an individuals included tracking, measured as the deviation (in m) of the
intrinsic ability to discriminate signal from noise tones, with centre of the vehicle from the centre of the right hand
higher scores representing greater sensitivity to signal tones. lane; tracking variability, the standard deviation of track-
On the other hand, b is an index of an individuals decision ing; speed deviation, calculated as the difference in speed
criterion, or degree of caution, used to respond to a signal. (in km/h) of the vehicle from the speed limit for that
Higher b scores indicate a more cautious response style. stretch of road; speed variability, the standard deviation of
The York Driving Simulator runs on a personal computer speed deviation; and off-road events, measured as the
system, and an electronic interface provides driver inputs to number of occasions on which the vehicle left the
the software (steering, brakes, and accelerator). Simple line simulated road.
graphics are used to portray objects and to induce a sense of
apparent motion by using real-time perspective generation. Procedure
The simulator presents a forward view from the drivers seat
of either an urban or a motorway road scene (Fig. 1), with Prior to the experiment, participants attended two train-
standard lane markings and signs and signals appropriate ing sessions. At each training session, they completed the
for the specified road environment. The participant navi- self-report assessments and were given a 15-min practice
gates within the computer driving environment interactively trial on the auditory vigilance task and a 20-min trial on the
just as one would navigate a car in the real world, while driving simulator. At the first training session, the partic-
being told to obey all road signs and traffic signals and to ipants also had electrodes attached as they would for the
maintain the car in the appropriate lane on the road. The SLTs. Prior to the first test session on the experimental
participants drove in a simulated night environment and night, participants were given a further 30-min practice trial
were instructed to drive as far as they could in 30 min while on the driving simulator and a further demonstration of the
keeping the cars position in the middle of the right hand auditory vigilance task.
lane and obeying traffic lights, speed restrictions, stop signs, Test sessions took place at 2400, 0230, 0500, and 0730 h;
and other road signs. Each 30-min drive consisted of an each session took about 90 min to complete. The
area of urban driving followed immediately by motorway participants came to the laboratory about 2 h prior to the
driving, each section taking approximately 15 min. first session to be prepared for continuous recording of EEG
Vehicle and road dimensions in the York Driving (C3/A2, O2/A1), referential EOG, and submental EMG
Simulator are measured in simulator units, where 1 ft activity. They remained awake under supervision in the
(0.3048 m) = 100 simulator units (su). The simulated car laboratory, indulging in quiet activities between test
was 600 su in width and 1000 su in length. The entire sessions. At each test session, the SLT was given first
simulated road is 3000 su wide; each lane on the two-lane followed by the two performance tests (auditory vigilance
road is 1100 su wide, and 400 su on either side is devoted to and simulated driving tests); the order of the latter two tests
the road shoulder. was counterbalanced across test sessions. Self-report sleepi-
64 J.T. Arnedt et al. / Journal of Psychosomatic Research 58 (2005) 6171

ness ratings were obtained prior to each SLT and each Table 1
Mean and standard error of the mean (S.E.M.) on the self-report,
performance test.
physiological, auditory vigilance, and simulated driving measures across
the four test times in Study 1
Test time 2400 h 0230 h 0500 h 0730 h
Results
Self-report measures
Stanford Mean 3.5 4.2 5.0 5.4
In general, the data were analysed using repeated Sleepiness S.E.M. 0.4 0.4 0.3 0.3
measures analysis of variance, in which time of testing Scale
(2400, 0230, 0500, and 0730 h) was extracted as a within- Modified Stanford Mean 2.0 3.2 5.1 6.3
subject factor. For the Stanford and Modified Stanford Scale: Factor 1 S.E.M. 0.9 0.9 0.9 0.8
Modified Stanford Mean 9.0 10.7 12.1 12.5
Sleepiness Scales, a second within-subject factor was also
Scale: Factor 2 S.E.M. 1.0 0.8 0.2 0.2
extracted corresponding to the time of administration (prior Physiological measures
to the SLT and the two performance tests). In the case of Sleep latency on Mean 13.8 6.4 4.4 2.4
the auditory vigilance and the simulated driving tasks, the SLT (min) S.E.M. 1.9 1.6 1.6 0.5
time-on-task was extracted by analysing the data in 5-min Sleep latency Mean 28.0 23.8 22.1 14.4
during the S.E.M. 1.4 3.2 3.2 3.4
blocks. Where appropriate, HuynhFeldt [38] corrections
Auditory
for lack of sphericity were applied, and the data were Vigilance
further analysed using orthogonal polynomials or the Test (min)
Tukey Honestly Significant Difference Test. The level of Auditory vigilance task
significance for all analyses was set at .05. dV Mean 2.63 2.02 1.65 1.17
S.E.M. 0.28 0.27 0.28 0.24
b Mean 3.36 3.84 3.54 3.28
Sensitivity of tests to experimental conditions S.E.M. 0.53 0.46 0.53 0.52
Simulated driving task
The summary statistics for all of the measures at the Tracking Mean 0.30 0.43 0.46 0.52
four test sessions are reported in Table 1. deviation from S.E.M. 0.09 0.06 0.09 0.09
lane centre (m)
Tracking Mean 0.46 0.55 0.58 0.64
Self-report variability (m) S.E.M. 0.03 0.03 0.06 0.06
With increasing time awake, participants reported Deviation from Mean 1.1 0.0 1.2 0.3
themselves as increasingly sleepy on the Stanford Sleepi- posted speed S.E.M. 1.1 1.6 0.6 1.2
ness Scale [ F(3,30) = 16.23, P = .0001] and on both Factors (km/h)
Variability of Mean 5.6 7.6 7.8 8.9
1 [ F(3,30) = 15.07, P b.0005] and 2 of the Modified
speed deviation S.E.M. 0.8 1.2 1.3 1.1
Stanford Scale [ F(3,30) = 8.58, P = .003]. There was also (km/h)
a small, but statistically significant, difference between Off-road incidents Mean 0.2 0.9 1.0 1.5
reports of subjective sleepiness [Stanford Sleepiness Scale: (number/5 min) S.E.M. 0.1 0.3 0.3 0.4
F(2,20) = 26.33, P = .0001; Modified Stanford Scale:
Factor 1: F(2,20) = 20.00, P b.0005; Factor 2: F(2,20) =
12.26, P b .0005] before the SLT and the performance tests, With increasing time awake on the simulated driving
but no difference in sleepiness ratings between the two task, the drivers drove increasingly to the left of the centre
performance tests. For example, for the Stanford Sleepiness of their lane [ F(3,30) = 6.81, P b.005], the variability of
Scale, the mean values before the SLT and the auditory their tracking increased [ F(3,30) = 9.54, P b.005], the
vigilance and the simulated driving tasks were 4.1, 4.8, and speed variability increased [ F(3,30) = 4.96, P b.009], and
4.7, respectively. the number of times that they drove off the road increased
linearly [ F(1,9) = 7.70, P = .022]. With increasing time-on-
Physiological measures task during each session, drivers again tracked to the left
On the SLT, latency to Stage 1 sleep decreased with of the lane centre [ F(5,50) = 7.78, P = .01], and tracking
increasing time awake [ F(3,30) = 15.80, P b.0005], as was variability increased [ F(5,50) = 16.72, P b.005]. At the
true also for the auditory vigilance task [ F(3,30) = 6.31, beginning of the task, drivers tended to drive below the
P = .002]. speed limit, but, as task duration increased, they increased
their speed until they were driving above the posted limit
Performance [ F(5,50) = 5.04, P b.03].
On the auditory vigilance task, there was a decline in d V
with increasing time awake [ F(3,30) = 15.70, P = .0001] Comparative sensitivity
and time-on-task [ F(5,50) = 9.05, P = .0001]. Beta did not
change significantly with either increasing time awake To compare the various dependent measures on a
[ F(3,30) = 0.49, P N.05] or time-on-task [ F(5,30) = 0.94, common scale, Cohens standardised difference measure
P N.05]. [39] was used. This was defined as (mean at other
J.T. Arnedt et al. / Journal of Psychosomatic Research 58 (2005) 6171 65

times mean at 2400 h)/pooled standard deviation. As Discussion


suggested by Cohen [39], a standardised difference of 0.2
was considered small, 0.5 was considered medium, and While the driving simulator was not the most sensitive
0.8 was considered a large difference. The results are test, it did appear to reach a level of sensitivity sufficient
shown in Fig. 2. to warrant further investigation. These findings are
considered in more detail in the General Discussion below.
However, it was first necessary to resolve an important
confound in the present study. An examination of the
driving simulator data for time-on-task effects indicates that
the greatest change in performance occurred during the
second half of the 30-min test. This happened to be the time
when participants were driving on the motorway route.
There is thus a confound between driving conditions and
time-on-task. To determine which of these factors is
responsible for the decline in performance, a second study
was carried out as described below.

Study 2

The aim of the second study was to eliminate the


confound of route and task length by counterbalancing the
order of presentation of urban and motorway routes.

Methods

Participants

Participants were recruited from the Introductory Psy-


chology Subject Pool. Twenty healthy males, who agreed to
comply with the requirements of the study, gave their
informed consent to participate. Inclusion criteria were a
valid drivers license, a regular sleep schedule, a moderate
to good sleeper [40], neither extreme morning nor evening
type ([33]; mean 48.9, S.D. = 8.7, range 3262), no evidence
of excessive daytime sleepiness ([32]; mean 6.8, S.D. = 2.3,
range 412), and no medications with sleep-altering
qualities. One participant included in the sample had an
Epworth score above the clinical cutoff for excessive
daytime sleepiness [32]. Of the original 20 participants,
two participants were excluded from the analyses because of
failure to comply with the instructions, resulting in a final
sample of 18 participants (mean age = 21.9 years, S.D. = 3.5,
range 1932 years).
Participants were instructed to refrain from using alcohol
and other drugs for 48 h and caffeine for 24 h prior to the
experimental night. They were also asked to maintain a
Fig. 2. Cohens standardised differences (Cohens d) at each test session regular sleep schedule, retiring between 2300 and 0100 h
(2400, 0230, 0500, and 0730 h) for self-report (SSS, MSS Factor 1, and and rising between 0700 and 0900 h, beginning 48 h prior
MSS Factor 2), physiological (Stage 1 latency on the SLT and auditory to testing. Compliance with the conditions was assessed
vigilance task), and performance (d V on the auditory vigilance task, mean
using sleep logs and an activity/substance use diary.
tracking, tracking variability, speed variability, and off-road occurrences on
the simulated driving task) measures. Cohens d was calculated at each test Participants were either paid CAD$30 or awarded a 5%
session using the following formula: (mean at other times mean at 2400 addition to their overall mark in the Introductory Psychol-
h)/pooled standard deviation. ogy course for participation.
66 J.T. Arnedt et al. / Journal of Psychosomatic Research 58 (2005) 6171

Dependent measures Statistical analyses

Self-report In general, the data from the four test sessions were
The self-report variables were the same as those used in analysed using split plot analyses of variance, with the
Study 1. wearing of the Magellan Monitor (Magellan, no Magellan)
extracted as a between-subjects source of variance, and
Physiological test session (2400, 0230, 0500, and 0730 h) extracted as
The SLT procedure was as described in Study 1. a within-subjects source of variance. For the self-report
Electrophysiological recordings were also made during the measures, two analyses were conducted. The first ex-
simulated driving task; however, these data will not be tracted the time of questionnaire administration (before
reported here. As in Study 1, the dependent variable was the and after the driving sessions and before the SLT) as an
latency to Stage 1 sleep. additional within-subjects factor to examine self-reported
changes in sleepiness with prolonged wakefulness. The
second analysis extracted route (motorway and urban) and
Performance time of administration (before and after the driving
The York Driving Simulator was used to assess driving sessions) as additional within-subjects factors. For the
performance. The urban and motorway routes were similar simulated driving task, route (motorway and urban) and
to those used previously. As in Study 1, the dependent the six 5-min blocks of the task were extracted as within-
variables were mean tracking, tracking variability, speed subjects effects.
deviation, speed variability, and the number of off-road Univariate F ratios were evaluated using the Huynh
occurrences. Feldt correction factor for sphericity [38]. Orthogonal
polynomials and Bonferonni-adjusted t tests were used
Procedure where appropriate. The level of significance for all analyses
was set at .05.
Forty-eight hours prior to testing, the participants came
to the Sleep Laboratory for a 30-min training session on the
driving simulator, 15 min each on the urban and motorway
routes. A second similar training session took place at the Results
beginning of the experimental night.
On the experimental night, participants arrived at the Effects of prolonged wakefulness
Sleep Laboratory between 2100 and 2130 h, where
electrodes were applied for continuous monitoring of Self-report
EEG (C3/A2, O2/A1), EOG, and EMG activity. Following With increasing time awake, participants rated them-
training, four 90-min test sessions took place at 2400, selves as more sleepy on all three self-report sleepiness
0230, 0500, and 0730 h. For each test session, the measures: the Stanford Sleepiness Scale [ F(3,48) = 86.35,
participants drove for 30 min on either the urban or P b.001] and Factors 1 [ F(3,48) = 40.69, P b.001] and 2
motorway route of the driving simulator, underwent the [ F(3,48) = 36.68, P b.001] of the Modified Stanford Scale.
SLT, and then drove the other route for another 30 min. Post hoc analyses indicated that self-reported sleepiness
The order of the urban and motorway routes was counter- ratings on all three scales increased significantly from
balanced across participants. Participants completed the 2400 to 0730 h, with significant differences between
Stanford Sleepiness and the Modified Stanford Scales ratings at each successive test time, except between 0500
before and after each simulated drive, and before each and 0730 h ( P b.001). There were also significant time
SLT. During breaks, the participants engaged in quiet effects on the Stanford Sleepiness Scale [ F(2,32) = 21.71,
activities in the laboratory under supervision. At the P b.001] and on Factors 1 [ F(2,32) = 12.78, P b.001] and
conclusion of testing, the participants were debriefed and 2 [ F(2,32) = 47.27, P b.001] of the Modified Stanford
remunerated for their participation. Scale. In all cases, ratings were significantly higher before
As part of a protocol separate from this study, 10 the SLT and after the driving task than before the driving
randomly selected participants wore a behavioural device, task ( P b.01). Sleepiness ratings before the SLT for Factor
the Magellan Monitor, on their nondominant wrist 2 of the Modified Stanford Scale were also significantly
throughout the experimental night, except during the higher than after the simulated driving task ( P = .013).
SLT. The monitor emitted vibratory stimuli, on average, There were no significant TimeMagellan interactions or
every 45 s, with a minimum and maximum interstimulus three-way interactions involving test, time, and Magellan,
interval of 30 and 60 s, respectively. The participants were but a significant TestMagellan interaction was found for
required to respond to the stimuli as quickly as possible by Factor 2 of the Modified Stanford Scale [ F(3,49) = 3.41,
flexing their index finger to activate a piezoelectric P b.05], and TestTime interactions were evident for both
response sensor. the Stanford Sleepiness Scale [ F(6,96) = 2.92, P b.05] and
J.T. Arnedt et al. / Journal of Psychosomatic Research 58 (2005) 6171 67

Factor 2 of the Modified Stanford [ F(6,96) = 3.65, Table 2


Mean and standard error of the mean (S.E.M.) for self-report and driving
P b.01].
measures on the urban and motorway routes
Route type Urban Motorway Cohens d
Physiological measures
On the SLT, sleep onset latency to Stage 1 decreased Self-report measures
Stanford Mean 3.9 4.0 0.10
with increasing time awake [ F(3,48) = 26.97, P b.001].
Sleepiness Scale S.E.M. 0.2 0.2
There were no significant main effects of Magellan or Modified Stanford Mean 9.1 9.4 0.15
Test  Magellan interactions. Scale: Factor 1 S.E.M. 0.4 0.5
Modified Stanford Mean 3.1 3.3 0.11
Driving performance Scale: Factor 2 S.E.M. 0.4 0.4
Simulated driving task
There were no main effects of wearing the Magellan
Mean tracking (m) Mean 0.13 0.20 1.35
monitor on any of the dependent variables of the driving S.E.M. 0.01 0.01
task. Consistent with the findings from Study 1, with Tracking variability (m) Mean 0.48 0.61 0.50
increasing time awake, the participants drifted to the left of S.E.M. 0.01 0.01
the centre of the lane [ F(3,48) = 10.14, P b.001], tracking Speed deviation (km/h) Mean 1.43 5.97 1.15
S.E.M. 0.16 0.41
became more variable [ F(3,48) = 16.97, P b.001], speed
Variability of speed Mean 6.27 12.25 1.66
variability increased [ F(3,48) = 21.80, P b.001], and off- deviation (km/h) S.E.M. 0.13 0.21
road incidents increased linearly [ F(1,16) = 4.85, P b.05]. Off-road incidents Mean 0.20 0.46 0.28
Post hoc analyses indicated that mean tracking, tracking (number/5 min) S.E.M. 0.04 0.05
variability, and speed variability were worse at the 0730-h
test time relative to both the 2400- and 0230-h test times
( P b.003). Additionally, mean tracking and speed variability Driving performance
were worse at 0730 h compared with the test at 0500 h The significant differences in driving parameters between
( P b.008). There was a significant TestMagellan interac- the urban and motorway routes are shown in Fig. 3.
tion only for speed variability [ F(3,48) = 3.78, P b.05], Performance was significantly worse on the motorway
which revealed worse performance at the 0730-h test time route for all five simulated driving variables: mean tracking
in the group wearing the device. Performance on the [ F(1,16) = 8.35, P = .01], tracking variability [ F(1,16) =
following dependent variables also deteriorated with increas- 26.23, P b.001], mean speed deviation from the posted
ing time-on-task: mean tracking [ F(5,80) = 5.17, P = .001], speed signs [ F(1,16) = 70.38, P b.001], speed variability
tracking variability [ F(5,80) = 10.95, P b.001], speed varia- [ F(1,16) = 148.33, P b.001], and the number of off-road
bility [ F(5,80) = 3.55, P b.01], and off-road occurrences incidents [ F(1,16) = 15.47, P = .001]. Across the six 5-min
[ F(5,80) = 3.50, Pb.05]. Mean tracking [ F(15,240) = 2.73, blocks of the driving task, off-road events [ F(5,80) = 2.53,
P b.01], tracking variability [ F(15,240) = 4.31, P b.001], P b.05] increased more rapidly on the motorway route. There
speed variability [ F(15,240) = 4.27, P b.001], and off-road were no Route  Test or Magellan  Route interactions, but a
events [ F(15,240) = 2.74, P b.05] deteriorated more rapidly significant three-way interaction involving Magellan, route,
during the 30-min driving task as the night progressed. There and block was found for off-road incidents [ F(5,80) = 2.66,
were no significant Magellan  Block interactions, but speed P b.05].
deviation [ F(15,240) = 2.69, P b.01] and speed variability
[ F(15,240) = 1.71, P = .05] deteriorated more rapidly across
the 30-min driving task at the later test times for participants General Discussion
wearing the Magellan monitor.
The primary objective of the first study was to evaluate
Effects of route type on measures of driving performance the sensitivity of a face valid measure of driving ability, the
York Driving Simulator, relative to measures that are widely
Descriptive data for the self-report measures and the used and known to be sensitive to the effects of sleep loss.
simulated driving task with effect sizes (Cohens d) are The second study was conducted to clarify an important
presented in Table 2. confound in the first study, as well as to examine more
closely the degree to which task characteristics (in
Self-report particular, monotony) affect simulated driving performance
Ratings on the three self-report sleepiness measures did in a manner consistent with other performance measures.
not differ by route type, but ratings were consistently higher
after the driving task than before: Stanford Sleepiness Scale Sensitivity of the York Driving Simulator
[ F(1,16) = 80.30, P b.001] and Factors 1 [ F(1,16) = 29.90,
P b.001] and 2 [ F(1,16) = 94.69, P b .001]. There were no By placing the various sleepiness measures on a common
significant two- or three-way interactions involving Magel- metric in Study 1, it was possible to compare their relative
lan, route, and time. sensitivity to the experimental manipulation. The findings
68 J.T. Arnedt et al. / Journal of Psychosomatic Research 58 (2005) 6171

Fig. 3. Comparison of overall driving performance on urban and motorway routes of the York Driving Simulator. Results for the five dependent measures of the
driving taskmean tracking, tracking variability, speed deviation, speed variability, and off-road incidentsare plotted as meansFS.E.M.s.

were generally consistent with the model of sleepiness sensitive than the other performance measure, d V on the
outlined by Carskadon and Dement [41]. Sleep latency on auditory vigilance task. These findings too would be
the SLT was the most sensitive measure, presumably due, in predicted by the model of Carskadon and Dement [41].
large part, to the fact that the test is conducted in an The auditory vigilance task was administered while the
environment that is explicitly devoid of external stimula- participant sat upright in a chair and had to make a simple
tionthe lights are dimmed, the participant is supine, noise motor response; in contrast, the driving task required
levels are low, room temperature is constantand the sustained attention to a more complex stimulus and the
participant is instructed to try to fall asleep. As such, the manipulation of steering wheel and pedals.
SLT is the most direct measure of physiological or basal
sleepiness. The four parameters of the York Driving Self-report and performance measures
Simulator that showed significant deterioration with increas-
ing time awake in both studiesmean tracking, tracking As expected, self-reported sleepiness increased monot-
variability, speed variability, and off-road eventswere onically across the night of prolonged wakefulness in both
considerably less sensitive than the SLT and slightly less studies. These findings are consistent with several studies
J.T. Arnedt et al. / Journal of Psychosomatic Research 58 (2005) 6171 69

showing increased subjective sleepiness during the night the same circadian phase, but altering the length of
assessed in experimentally induced sleep deprivation studies wakefulness prior to testing.
(e.g., Ref. [42]), constant routine protocols (e.g., Ref. [43]), In addition to the deficits observed across the night, there
and studies of shift workers in a variety of industries were characteristic changes in driving performance within
[44,45]. In addition, it has been shown repeatedly that the 30-min driving task. With increasing time-on-task,
alertness follows a self-sustained circadian pattern reaching participants again drifted towards the middle of the road,
its nadir in the early morning around 0500 h [46,47]. In- and their tracking became more variable. These findings are
terestingly, although objective performance was consistently consistent with previous studies showing deviations in
worse on the motorway route of the driving task, there were lateral position as a result of prolonged driving [60,61].
no significant differences in self-reported sleepiness Although the driving task used in the present studies was
between the motorway and urban routes. This finding considerably shorter (30 min) in duration than those in the
suggests that the degree of impairment associated with latter two studies (2 h), it is likely that time-on-task fatigue
sleepiness may be underrecognized, which has implications was a significant contributor to the observed deterioration in
for decisions about ones capacity to operate safely a motor performance. Others have noted that fatigue is manifested
vehicle. The inability to appreciate fully the dangers more rapidly in driving simulators than in actual driving
associated with drowsy driving has also been highlighted conditions (e.g., Ref. [55]), and participants in the present
by other researchers [48]. study rated themselves as significantly sleepier after the
Consistent with previous studies [49,50], latency to Stage driving task than before. From a practical perspective, a test
1 sleep onset declined linearly across the night on the SLT. that is sensitive to detecting the decrements related to
The decline in dV on the auditory vigilance task with sleepiness and of a relatively short duration is more likely to
increasing time awake is consistent with previous findings be preferred by both clinicians and patients.
and has typically been interpreted as indicating a reduction Contrary to the present findings, Lenne et al. [25,55]
in the intrinsic capacity to discriminate a signal [5153]. have found that participants drifted towards the outer edge
Naitoh [54] has cautioned against a simple interpretation of rather than towards the midline of the road with increasing
the signal detection measures dV and b and has questioned task duration. Their simulated driving task may have been
the appropriateness of applying signal detection theory to more alerting, however, because participants were required
sleep loss studies. He argues that independent evidence, to perform a secondary reaction time task while driving, and
such as physiological measures, is necessary to ensure bthat there were other simulated cars on the road. In a more
the subject observe and attend to the stimuliQ (Ref. [54], monotonous driving task without other simulated cars, the
p. 360). Because no such evidence was gathered in this sleepy driver may approach the road midline in an effort to
study, the findings may indicate either a true reduction of avoid an off-road incident. Stewart and MacLean [62] have
intrinsic capacity or a loss of attentional capacity. also observed that when other cars are introduced, partici-
pants drive towards the outer edge of the road, perhaps, to
Simulated driving performance during reduce the likelihood of a collision with another vehicle.
prolonged wakefulness
Effects of route type on driving performance
Simulated driving performance deteriorated progres-
sively across the night in both studies. More specifically, The results from Study 2 demonstrated that driving
with increasing time awake, participants drove closer to the performance was consistently worse on the motorway route.
middle of the road, tracking and speed became more More specifically, participants ability to maintain a
variable, and the number of off-road incidents increased. consistent road position and a constant speed was more
This overall decline in driving ability during periods of variable, their speed deviated more from the posted speed
increased sleepiness is consistent with the findings from limit, and they drove off the motorway road more
both simulated driving [55] and nondriving performance frequently. These findings may have been due to the greater
studies conducted during the circadian trough of alertness monotony of the motorway route, which has few turns and
[56,57]. The findings from the present studies are also no road signs or stop lights. In addition to being consistent
consistent with the well-identified nighttime peak for with predictions, the deterioration in driving performance is
sleepiness-related motor vehicle accidents [8,58,59] and supported by accident data demonstrating a 2025% higher
with the occurrence of several large-scale catastrophic incidence of sleep-related motor vehicle accidents on
events during the early morning hours [3]. One significant motorway than on urban routes [2,48]. Studies of the effects
shortcoming with the current study is that the experimental of sleep loss on performance have consistently shown that
design did not permit a systematic analysis of the relative monotonous tasks are more sensitive to the effects of
contribution of circadian and homeostatic influences to the sleepiness than are intrinsically more interesting tasks
observed decrements in performance across the night. This [11,13,15,63]. Moreover, the greater decline in performance
is an important issue, and one that could be addressed in on the motorway route is consistent with the model of
future studies experimentally by, e.g., testing participants at Carskadon and Dement [41] of the constructs underlying
70 J.T. Arnedt et al. / Journal of Psychosomatic Research 58 (2005) 6171

sleepiness because physiological sleepiness is theoretically [3] Mitler MM, Carskadon MA, Czeisler CA, Dement WC, Dinges DF,
Graeber RC. Catastrophes, sleep, and public policy: consensus report.
unmasked in the absence of alerting factors [64]. The results
Sleep 1988;11:100 9.
of the second study, in particular, suggest that simulated [4] Lyznicki JM, Doege TC, Davis RM, Williams MA. Sleepiness, driving,
motorway routes may be more sensitive to the decrements in and motor vehicle crashes Council on Scientific Affairs, American
performance observed with sleepiness than urban routes are Medical Association [see comments]. JAMA 1998;279:1908 13.
and may therefore be more appropriate for assessing [5] Garbarino S, Nobili L, Beelke M, De Carli F, Ferrillo F. The
driving-related impairments associated with sleepiness. contributing role of sleepiness in highway vehicle accidents. Sleep
2001;24:203 6.
Finally, half of the participants in Study 2 were required [6] Maycock G. Sleepiness and driving: the experience of UK car drivers.
to respond to the Magellan Monitor during the driving task, J Sleep Res 1996;5:229 37.
which could have resulted in better performance in this [7] McCartt AT, Ribner SA, Pack AI, Hammer MC. The scope and nature
group due to increased arousal. There were, however, no of the drowsy driving problem in New York state. Accid Anal Prev
1996;28:511 7.
main effects of the Magellan on driving performance, and
[8] Pack AI, Pack AM, Rodgman E, Cucchiara A, Dinges DF, Schwab
the only significant two-way interaction found worse CW. Characteristics of crashes attributed to the driver having fallen
performance in the group wearing the device. Thus, the asleep. Accid Anal Prev 1995;27:769 75.
Magellan seemed to have little direct effect on arousal level [9] Carskadon MA, Dement WC, Mitler MM, Roth T, Westbrook PR,
while driving. Comparing the concordance of the Magellan Keenan S. Guidelines for the Multiple Sleep Latency Test (MSLT): a
data with the other measures of sleepiness will reveal standard measure of sleepiness. Sleep 1986;9:519 24.
[10] Dinges DF, Kribbs NB. Performing while sleepy: effects of exper-
whether this device might serve as an on-board real-time imentally-induced sleepiness. In: Monk TH, editor. Sleep, sleepiness
indicator of a drivers level of alertness. and performance. Chicester7 Wiley, 1991. pp. 97 128.
In summary, the findings from both investigations [11] Wilkinson RT. The effect of lack of sleep on visual watch-keeping. Q J
suggest that the York Driving Simulator is sensitive to the Exp Psychol 1960;12:36 40.
decrements in performance evident with increasing time [12] Wilkinson RT. Effects of up to 60 hours sleep deprivation on different
types of work. Ergonomics 1964;7:175 86.
awake, and it appears to have at least comparable [13] Wilkinson RT. Interaction of lack of sleep with knowledge of results,
sensitivity under these conditions to an auditory vigilance repeated testing, and individual differences. J Exp Psychol 1961;62:
task. Moreover, consistent with what has been demon- 263 71.
strated with other performance measures, increasing the [14] Williams HL, Lubin A, Goodenough JJ. Impaired performance with
acute sleep loss. Psychol Monogr 1959;73:1 26.
monotony of the task produces an even more marked
[15] Hockey GRJ. Changes in attention allocation in a multicomponent
deterioration in driving performance. The simulated task under loss of sleep. Br J Psychol 1970;61:473 80.
driving task offers advantages over self-report, physio- [16] Williams HL, Lubin A. Speeded addition and sleep loss. J Exp
logical, and other performance measures in the assessment Psychol 1967;73:313 7.
of driving performance, such as providing a more face [17] Dinges DF, Powell JE. Microcomputer analyses of performance on a
valid measure of driving and using a highly practised portable, simple visual RT task during sustained operations. Behav
Res Methods Instrum Comput 1985;17:652 5.
skill. We have recently shown that performance impair- [18] Wilkinson RT, Houghton D. Portable four-choice reaction time test
ments on the York Driving Simulator following prolonged with magnetic memory tape. Behav Res Methods Instrum Comput
wakefulness are equivalent to the decrements produced by 1975;7:441 6.
levels of alcohol intoxication that are illegal for operating [19] Baddeley AD. A 3-min reasoning test based on grammatical
a motor vehicle [65,66]. These findings suggest that such transformation. Psychon Sci 1968;10:341 2.
[20] Sternberg S. Memory scanning: mental processes revealed by reaction
a driving task may have clinical utility in the assessment time experiments. Am Sci 1969;57:421 57.
of driving ability in patients who are at an increased risk [21] Broadbent DE. Performance and its measurement. Br J Clin
for sleepiness-related accidents. Pharmacol 1984;18:5S 9S.
[22] Monk TH. Circadian rhythms in subjective activation, mood, and
performance efficiency. In: Kryger MH, Roth T, Dement WC,
editors. Principles and practice of sleep medicine, 2nd ed. Philadelphia
Acknowledgments (PA)7 Saunders, 1994. pp. 321 30.
[23] Van Dongen HP, Dinges DF. Circadian rhythms in fatigue, alertness,
The support of the Ontario Ministry of Transportation and performance. In: Kryger MH, Roth T, Dement WC, editors.
Principles and practice of sleep medicine, 3rd ed. Philadelphia (PA)7
(Grant No. 9505) and of Queens University is gratefully
Saunders, 2000. pp. 391 9.
acknowledged, as is the technical assistance of J. Brown, S. [24] Fairclough SH, Graham R. Impairment of driving performance caused
Ferguson, and M. York. by sleep deprivation or alcohol: a comparative study. Hum Factors
1999;41:118 28.
[25] Lenne MG, Triggs TJ, Redman JR. Interactive effects of sleep
deprivation, time of day, and driving experience on a driving task.
References Sleep 1998;21:38 44.
[26] Gawron VJ, Ranney TA. The effects of alcohol dosing on driving
[1] Dinges DF. An overview of sleepiness and accidents. J Sleep Res performance on a closed course and in a driving simulator. Ergonomics
1995;4:4 14. 1988;31:1219 44.
[2] Horne JA, Reyner LA. Sleep related vehicle accidents. BMJ 1995; [27] Roehrs T, Beare D, Zorick F, Roth T. Sleepiness and ethanol effects on
310:565 7. simulated driving. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 1994;18:154 8.
J.T. Arnedt et al. / Journal of Psychosomatic Research 58 (2005) 6171 71

[28] Findley L, Unverzagt M, Guchu R, Fabrizio M, Buckner J, Suratt PM. [47] Monk TH. Subjective ratings of sleepinessthe underlying circadian
Vigilance and automobile accidents in patients with sleep apnea or mechanisms. Sleep 1987;10:343 53.
narcolepsy. Chest 1995;108:619 24. [48] Horne JA, Reyner LA. Driver sleepiness. J Sleep Res 1995;4:23 9.
[29] George CFP, Boudreau AC, Smiley A. Comparison of simulated [49] Sugerman JL, Walsh JK. Physiological sleep tendency and ability to
driving performance in narcolepsy and sleep apnea patients. Sleep maintain alertness at night. Sleep 1989;12:106 12.
1996;19:711 7. [50] Walsh JK, Sugerman JL, Muehlbach MJ, Schweitzer PK. Physio-
[30] De Valck E, Cluydts R. Slow-release caffeine as a countermeasure to logical sleep tendency on a simulated night shift: adaptation and
driver sleepiness induced by partial sleep deprivation. J Sleep Res effects of triazolam. Sleep 1991;11:251 64.
2001;10:203 9. [51] Deaton M, Tobias JS, Wilkinson RT. The effect of sleep deprivation on
[31] Arnedt JT, Acebo C, Seifer R, Carskadon MA. Assessment of a signal detection parameters. Q J Exp Psychol 1971;23:449 52.
simulated driving task for sleep research. Sleep 2001;24:A413. [52] Horne JA, Anderson NR, Wilkinson RT. Effects of sleep deprivation
[32] Johns MW. A new method for measuring daytime sleepiness: the on signal detection measures of vigilance: implications for sleep
Epworth Sleepiness Scale. Sleep 1991;14:540 5. function. Sleep 1983;6:347 58.
[33] Horne JA, Ostberg O. A self-assessment questionnaire to determine [53] Wilkinson RT. Sleep deprivation: performance tests for partial and
morningnesseveningness in human circadian rhythms. Int J Chrono- selective sleep deprivation. In: Abt LA, Riess BF, editors. Progress in
biol 1976;4:97 110. clinical psychology: dreams and dreaming, vol. 8. New York7 Grune
[34] Hoddes E, Dement W, Zarcone V. Quantification of sleepiness: a new and Stratton, 1968. pp. 28 43.
approach. Psychophysiology 1973;10:431 6. [54] Naitoh P. Signal detection theory as applied to vigilance perform-
[35] MacLean AW, Fekken CG, Saskin P, Knowles JB. Psychometric ance of sleep-deprived subjects. Sleep 1983;6:359 61.
evaluation of the Stanford Sleepiness Scale. J Sleep Res 1992;1: [55] Lenne MG, Triggs TJ, Redman JR. Time of day variations in driving
35 9. performance. Accid Anal Prev 1997;29:431 7.
[36] Rechtschaffen A, Kales A. A manual of standardized terminology, [56] Dawson D, Reid K. Fatigue, alcohol and performance impairment
techniques, and scoring system for sleep stages of human subjects. Los [letter]. Nature 1997;388:235.
Angeles7 UCLA Brain Information Service/Brain Research Institute, [57] Gillberg M, Kecklund G, Akerstedt T. Relations between performance
1968. and subjective ratings of sleepiness during a night awake. Sleep 1994;
[37] Cairns J, Knowles JB, MacLean AW. Effect of varying the time of 17:236 41.
sleep on sleep, vigilance, and self-rated activation. Psychophysiology [58] Summala H, Mikkola T. Fatal accidents among car and truck drivers:
1982;19:623 8. effects of fatigue, age, and alcohol consumption. Hum Factors 1994;
[38] Huyhn H, Feldt LS. Estimation of the box correction factor for degrees 36:315 26.
of freedom from sample data in randomised block and split-plot [59] Schwing RC. Exposure-controlled highway fatality rates: temporal
designs. J Educ Stat 1976;1:69 82. patterns compared to some explanatory variables. Alcohol Drugs Driv
[39] Cohen J. Statistical power for the behavioral sciences, 2nd ed. New 1989/1990;5/6:275 85.
York7 Academic Press, 1988. [60] DeWaard D, Brookhuis KA. Assessing driver status: a demonstration
[40] Monroe LJ. Psychological and physiological differences between experiment on the road. Accid Anal Prev 1991;23:297 307.
good and poor sleepers. J Abnorm Psychol 1967;72:255 64. [61] Dureman EI, Boden C. Fatigue in simulated car driving. Ergonomics
[41] Carskadon MA, Dement WC. The Multiple Sleep Latency Test: what 1972;15:299 308.
does it measure? Sleep 1982;5:S6772. [62] Stewart CP, MacLean AW. Effect of dynamic vehicles in simulated
[42] Froberg JE. Twenty-four-hour patterns in human performance, sub- driving during prolonged wakefulness. Sleep 1998;21:231.
jective and physiological variables and differences between morning [63] Thiffault P, Bergeron J. Monotony of road environment and driver
and evening active subjects. Biol Psychol 1977;5:119 34. fatigue: a simulator study. Accid Anal Prev 2003;35:381 91.
[43] Dijk D-J, Duffy JF, Czeisler CA. Circadian and sleep/wake dependent [64] Dinges DF. The nature of sleepiness: causes, contexts, and consequences.
aspects of subjective alertness and cognitive performance. J Sleep Res In: Stunkard AJ, Baum A, editors. Perspectives in behavioral medicine.
1992;1:112 7. Hillsdale (NJ)7 Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 1989. pp. 147 79.
[44] 2kerstedt T. Shift work and disturbed sleep/wakefulness. Sleep Med [65] Arnedt JT, Wilde GJS, Munt PW, MacLean AW. Simulated driving
Rev 1998;2:117 28. performance following prolonged wakefulness and alcohol consump-
[45] Torsvall L, 2kerstedt T. Sleepiness on the job: continuously measured tion: separate and combined contributions to impairment. J Sleep Res
EEG changes in train drivers. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 2000;9:233 41.
1987;66:502 11. [66] Arnedt JT, Wilde GJS, Munt PW, MacLean AW. How do prolonged
[46] 2kerstedt T. Work hours, sleepiness and the underlying mechanisms. wakefulness and alcohol compare in the decrements they produce on a
J Sleep Res 1995;4:15 22. simulated driving task? Accid Anal Prev 2001;33:337 44.

Você também pode gostar