Você está na página 1de 12

American Journal of Epidemiology Vol. 185, No.

7
The Author 2017. Published by Oxford University Press on behalf of the Johns Hopkins Bloomberg School of DOI: 10.1093/aje/kww135
Public Health. All rights reserved. For permissions, please e-mail: journals.permissions@oup.com. Advance Access publication:
March 3, 2017

Systematic Reviews and Meta- and Pooled Analyses

Risk Factor Models for Neurodevelopmental Outcomes in Children Born Very


Preterm or With Very Low Birth Weight: A Systematic Review of Methodology
and Reporting

Louise Linsell*, Reem Malouf, Joan Morris, Jennifer J. Kurinczuk, and Neil Marlow
* Correspondence to Louise Linsell, National Perinatal Epidemiology Unit, Nufeld Department of Population Health, University of
Oxford, Old Road Campus, Headington, Oxford OX3 7LF, United Kingdom (e-mail: louise.linsell@npeu.ox.ac.uk).

Initially submitted January 7, 2016; accepted for publication March 3, 2016.

The prediction of long-term outcomes in surviving infants born very preterm (VPT) or with very low birth weight
(VLBW) is necessary to guide clinical management, provide information to parents, and help target and evaluate
interventions. There is a large body of literature describing risk factor models for neurodevelopmental outcomes in
VPT/VLBW children, yet few, if any, have been developed for use in routine clinical practice or adopted for use in
research studies or policy evaluation. We sought to systematically review the methods and reporting of studies
that have developed a multivariable risk factor model for neurodevelopment in surviving VPT/VLBW children. We
searched the MEDLINE, Embase, and PsycINFO databases from January 1, 1990, to June 1, 2014, and identied
78 studies reporting 222 risk factor models. Most studies presented risk factor analyses that were not intended to
be used for prediction, conrming that there is a dearth of specically designed prognostic modeling studies for
long-term outcomes in surviving VPT/VLBW children. We highlight the strengths and weaknesses of the research
methodology and reporting to date, and provide recommendations for the design and analysis of future studies
seeking to analyze risk prediction or develop prognostic models for VPT/VLBW children.

data reporting; development; preterm infants; prognosis; research methodology; risk factors; systematic reviews;
very low birth weight

Abbreviations: VLBW, very low birth weight; VPT, very preterm.

Prematurity and its associated neonatal morbidity have a We reviewed and summarized the methods and reporting in
pervasive effect on neurodevelopment, leading to a number 78 studies that were recently included in a systematic review
of conditions, including cerebral palsy, visual and auditory of risk predictors for neurodevelopmental outcomes in surviv-
decits, impairments in motor and cognitive function, and be- ing children born at or before 32 weeks gestation (VPT) or
havioral problems (1). Early identication of factors that me- with a birth weight less than or equal to 1,250 g (very low
diate long-term outcomes is necessary to guide the clinical birth weight (VLBW)) (24). Studies were included if the aim
management of children born preterm, provide information to (or one of the aims) was to perform a multivariable (>2 vari-
parents, and help develop, target, and evaluate interventions. ables) risk factor analysis for neurodevelopmental outcomes
A large body of literature reports on risk factor models for in this population, in one or more of the following domains:
neurodevelopmental outcomes in very preterm (VPT) chil- motor skills, cognition, hearing, vision, or behavior. In this ar-
dren (32 weeks gestation), yet few, if any, have been devel- ticle, we report the main elements of study design, model de-
oped for use in routine clinical practice or adopted for use in velopment, reporting, and validation (if performed) of the 78
research studies or policy evaluation. Our aim in this article is studies included in the review. The ndings are then discussed
to describe and review the conduct and reporting of these within the framework of recommended approaches for risk
studies over the last 2 decades and provide methodological factor analysis and reporting advocated by experts advising on
recommendations for future research in this area. prediction modeling in the medical and statistical literature.

601 Am J Epidemiol. 2017;185(7):601612


602 Linsell et al.

Despite the recent publication of reporting guidelines for stud- Data extraction and reporting
ies developing and validating risk prediction models (5), there
is no equivalent central resource that provides guidance for All articles identied by the search strategies were screened
the design and analysis of studies seeking to perform a risk on title and abstract for denite exclusions and duplicates
factor analysis or develop a prognostic model. (screen 1). For the remaining articles, the full text was re-
trieved and the eligibility criteria were applied (screen 2). The
2 screens were initially performed by the rst author (L.L.),
METHODS but if there was uncertainty about the eligibility of an article,
it was screened independently by the second author (R.M.). If
Methods for the systematic review of risk predictors for a decision could not be reached, it was referred to the rest of
poor neurodevelopment in VPT and VLBW children have the author review team (J.J.K., N.M., and J.M.). Non-English
previously been published in a protocol (http://www.crd.york. articles included in the review were fully translated. Multiple
ac.uk/PROSPERO/; registration number CRD42014006943) articles based on the same cohort of children underwent a pa-
and in 3 published review articles on motor, cognitive, and be- nel review (L.L., R.M., and N.M.). Studies reporting on the
havioral outcomes (24). They are described in brief below. same outcome domain (cognitive, motor, behavior, hearing,
or vision) at the same age of assessment (<5 years or 5
Search strategy years) were assessed on relevance to the review, and only 1
article was selected for data extraction. For all articles eligible
Three electronic search strategies were devised in the for inclusion, both reviewers (L.L. and R.M.) independently
MEDLINE, Embase, and PsycINFO databases (available with completed a full data extraction form in a customized Micro-
the protocol) using the National Institutes of Health Medical soft Access 2010 database (Microsoft Corporation, Redmond,
Subject Headings (see Web Appendix 1, available at http:// Washington). These were cross-validated for discrepancies
aje.oxfordjournals.org/). The searches identied journal arti- and referred to the rest of the author review team if agreement
cles published between January 1, 1990, and June 1, 2014, could not be reached. If critical information was missing or
presenting a multivariable risk prediction model for a neurode- unclear, the corresponding author was contacted once by
velopmental outcome assessed at or after the age of 18 months e-mail for clarication.
in VPT/VLBW children. No language restrictions were made. Summary statistics were provided for each item of data
The bibliographies of all articles included for data extraction extracted (listed in Tables 13), and the results from this re-
were hand-searched for further eligible articles. view are presented in accordance with the Preferred Report-
ing Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses
(PRISMA) guidelines (Web Appendix 2) (9). If more than 1
Eligibility criteria
model was presented in an article, we selected the rst mod-
Articles were included in the review if they satised the el reported to summarize model characteristics to avoid the
following eligibility criteria: 1) they contained original data; overrepresentation of studies presenting multiple models.
2) the study population was born after January 1, 1990; 3) the
study population had a gestational age less than or equal to RESULTS
32 weeks (VPT) or a birth weight less than or equal to
1,250 g (VLBW) and was not a highly select group (based on The search strategy retrieved 44,500 articles (Figure 1),
other clinical criteria); and 4) one of the authors objectives and 2,284 articles were screened on full text, applying the
was to perform a multivariable risk factor analysis (>2 vari- full set of eligibility criteria. Ninety-one articles (from 48 co-
ables) of a neurodevelopmental outcome in survivors as- hort populations) containing multivariable risk factor anal-
sessed at or after 18 months of age. yses were eligible for inclusion. Following panel review
A birth weight cutoff of 1,250 g was used, instead of the (L.L., R.M., and N.M.), a further 13 articles were excluded
ofcial denition of VLBW (1,500 g), to exclude the sub- because they reported on the same outcome domain at the
set of more mature but extremely growth-restricted children same age of assessment in the same cohort as another article
included in the typical 1,500-g VLBW cohort, which can with a more relevant objective. The remaining 78 articles, re-
cause heterogeneity and lead to confounding bias when porting 222 risk prediction models for a neurodevelopmental
examining the relationship between risk factors and out- outcome in surviving VPT/VLBW children, were included
comes (6). Explanatory prognostic factor studies which in- in the data extraction (1087).
vestigated the causal pathway between a single prognostic
factor and an outcome (ideally adjusted for confounders) Study design
and estimated effect size were not included in the review. In
these types of studies, other risk factors are included based The design characteristics of the 78 studies that were in-
on the change in the regression coefcient of the prognostic cluded for data extraction are shown in Table 1. The main
factor under study, whereas in multivariable outcome pre- study design was cohort (91%), and there were 5 randomized
diction models, risk factors are included in the model based controlled trial populations (51, 52, 55, 60, 69), 1 case-
on their ability to predict the outcome. Current guidelines control study (53), and 1 cross-sectional study (31). Of the 71
recommend not combining these 2 distinct types of studies, cohorts followed up prospectively, half (n = 36) were ascer-
as their objectives and model-building strategies differ and tained from all live births in a geographically dened region
when synthesized could lead to biased results (7, 8). and half (n = 35) were recruited from neonatal intensive care

Am J Epidemiol. 2017;185(7):601612
Review of Risk Factor Models for VPT/VLBW Children 603

Table 1. Designs of Studies Presenting a Multivariable Risk Factor Table 1. Continued


Model for a Neurodevelopmental Outcome in Very Preterm and Very
Low Birth Weight Children Included in a Systematic Review, No. of % of Studies
Study Design Characteristic
Studies (n = 78)
19902014
Age of assessment, years
No. of % of Studies
Study Design Characteristic 1.52 34 45
Studies (n = 78)
35 21 27
Study design
612 17 22
Cohort 71 91
RCT 5 6.4 Other 6 8

Case-control 1 1.3 Sample sizea 219 407,632


(141461)
Cross-sectional 1 1.3
Abbreviations: BW, birth weight; ELBW, extremely low birth
Participant selection
weight; EPT, extremely preterm; GA, gestational age; IQR, interquar-
Live births 36 46 tile range; NICU, neonatal intensive care unit; RCT, randomized con-
Admissions to NICU 35 45 trolled trial; VPT, very preterm.
a
RCT participants 5 6.4 Values are presented as median (interquartile range) and range.

Selected from a database 1 1.3


Not clear 1 1.3
Research center selection unit admissions. Forty-ve percent (n = 35) of the studies re-
Single center 35 45 cruited subjects from a single research center, and 79%
All centers in a geographically 30 38 (n = 62) of studies extracted risk factor and outcome data pro-
dened region spectively. Overall, 32% (n = 25) of studies were based on a
Multicenter (nonrandom) 13 17 cohort study of all live births in a geographically dened re-
Data extraction gion and followed up the subjects prospectively.
Fifty-ve percent (n = 43) of studies dened the study co-
Prospective 62 79
hort using gestational age only, and 21% (n = 16) used birth
Retrospective 13 17 weight only; the remaining studies used some combination of
Not clear 3 4 the two. The most common age of assessment was 1824
Data collected as part of routine months (45%; n = 34), and no studies followed up partici-
follow-up pants beyond the age of 12 years. The median sample size
No 57 73 was 219 (interquartile range, 141461), and 11 studies had
Yes 21 27 more than 1,000 participants (11, 12, 14, 1618, 24, 25, 54,
Continent of study 69, 82). Only 1 published article mentioned the issue of statis-
tical power, and it referred to the number of events per vari-
Europe 46 59
able in the modeling process (39).
North America 17 22
Australia and New Zealand 12 15
Asia 2 2.6 Model development
International 1 1.3
Starting years of recruitment
The median number of risk factor models presented per
study was 2, and 25% of studies presented 4 or more models.
19901995 25 32
A summary of the model-building techniques used is pre-
19962000 33 42 sented in Table 2 (for the rst model presented in each article).
20012010 20 26 The median number of candidate risk factors considered at the
GA and BW inclusion criteria outset was 17 (range, 351). Seventy-two percent (n = 56) of
VPT (32 weeks) 24 31 studies provided a rationale for their choice of candidate fac-
tors or used a comprehensive list with wide coverage (20
EPT (29 weeks) 19 24
factors), but 28% (n = 22) of studies with fewer than 20 candi-
ELBW (1,000 g) 16 21 date factors gave no rationale at all. The derivation and coding
Other GA/BW combination 19 24 of outcomes and risk factors were described clearly in 85%
Exclusion criteria (n = 66) of studies, and outcomes were generally measured
Congenital anomalies 34 44 using comprehensive, well-validated tests or assessed using
Severity of disability 22 28
standardized diagnostic criteria, though information on blind-
ing to previous medical history at assessment was frequently
Multiple births 7 9
not reported. Sixty-nine percent (n = 54) of studies categor-
Table continues ized some or all of the risk factors that were measured on a
continuous scale. In many cases there was a clinical rationale,

Am J Epidemiol. 2017;185(7):601612
604 Linsell et al.

Table 2. Development of the First Model Presented in Each Study Included in a Systematic Review of Risk Factor
Models for Neurodevelopmental Outcomes in Very Preterm and Very Low Birth Weight Children, 19902014

Model Development No. of Studies % of Studies (n = 78)

No. of candidate risk factors at outset


Cleara 17 (1124) 351
Unclear 7 9
Rationale given for candidate risk factors
Yes 29 37
No, but wide coverage (20) 27 35
No 22 28
Treatment of continuous risk factors
All left as continuous 17 22
Some categorized 33 42
All categorized 21 27
No continuous risk factors 1 1
Unclear 6 8
Coding of risk factors and outcome clearly described 66 85
Statistical model
Linear/logistic regression 74 95
Multinomial/ordinal logistic regression 2 3
Multilevel modeling 1 1
Unclear 1 1
Method of initial screening of candidate risk factors
No screeningall included 33 43
P value from univariable model 22 28
P value from multivariable model 6 8
Other 7 9
Unclear 10 13
Model-building strategy
All candidates/screened candidate risk factors included 30 39
Stepwise selection 27 35
Backward selection 9 12
Forward selection 4 5
Other 3 4
Unclear 5 6
Any interaction terms tted 2 3
Any nonlinear terms tted 0 0
Any factors forced into the nal multivariable model 15 19
Multicollinearity of risk factors mentioned/discussed 11 14
Model t/assumptions checked 15 19
No. of risk factors in nal multivariable model
Cleara 5 (49) 025
Unclear 1 1
a
Values are presented as median (interquartile range) and range.

but the use of arbitrary thresholds for no reason was also a (n = 22) only included candidate factors with a P value below
widespread practice. a set threshold in a univariable test of association with the
Forty-three percent (n = 33) of studies entered all of the outcome, and 8% (n = 6) screened variables using multivari-
candidate variables into the multivariable model; 28% able analysis. The most popular method of model-building

Am J Epidemiol. 2017;185(7):601612
Review of Risk Factor Models for VPT/VLBW Children 605

Table 3. Reporting and Model Validation in Studies Included in a Systematic Review of Risk Factor Models for
Neurodevelopmental Outcomes in Very Preterm and Very Low Birth Weight Children, 19902014

Model Reporting and Validation No. of Studies % of Studies (n = 78)

Comparison of those lost versus not lost to follow-up


No 21 30
Yes 48 70
Not applicableall were followed up 9
Missing data presented for each variable
No 51 67
Yes 19 25
Some variables 6 8
Not applicablenone missing 2
No. of participants included in nal model reported 24 31
Point estimates of model coefcients reported
No 8 10
Yesall variables 57 73
Signicant variables only 7 9
Selected variables only 6 8
CIs/SEs of model coefcients reported
No 11 14
Yesall variables 56 72
Signicant variables only 5 6
Selected variables only 6 8
P value/signicance level of model coefcients reported
No 5 6
Yesall variables 55 71
Signicant variables only 11 14
Selected variables only 7 9
Any statistical validation performed 4 5
If yes, type of validation
Discrimination 4
Split sample 1
Published in format for clinical use 2 3

Abbreviations: CI, condence interval; SE, standard error.

after initial screening (or no screening) was to simply include presented the amount of missing data for each variable included
all factors (39%; n = 30) regardless of statistical signicance, in the nal model, and only 31% (n = 24) reported the number
and the second-most popular method was stepwise selection of infants included in the nal model. Although 52 (67%)
(35%; n = 27). The median number of risk factors included in studies reported point estimates, condence intervals or
the nal prediction model was 5 (interquartile range, 49). standard errors, and the P value or signicance level of all
model coefcients included in the nal multivariable model,
Reporting and model validation the remaining studies only did so for selected variables or
failed to report them at all.
A summary of reporting and model validation (if per- Four studies (12, 32, 42, 72) assessed the performance of the
formed) is presented in Table 3. Generally the reporting of nal model using statistical validation techniques. All 4 re-
study attrition and missing data was quite poor, making it ported the area under the receiver operating characteristic curve,
difcult to assess how representative of the original re- which measures the discriminatory ability of the model to dif-
cruited study sample the analyzed children were. A compari- ferentiate between individuals by severity of outcome. One
son of baseline characteristics between children lost to study (42) produced a decision tree/algorithm based on their
follow-up and those assessed was not performed by 30% model, and another (12) produced a Web-based tool with which
(n = 21) of studies. Furthermore, only 25% (n = 19) of studies to predict either death or neurodevelopmental impairment for

Am J Epidemiol. 2017;185(7):601612
606 Linsell et al.

44,500 studies identified in


MEDLINE, Embase, and PsycINFO 12,217 duplicates
(January 1, 1990June 1, 2014)

32,283 screened on title and


29,999 excluded
abstract (screen 1)

2,284 screened on full text


(screen 2)

2,193 children excluded


621 born before 1990
100 not original data
549 not 32 weeks or 1,250 g
39 highly select clinical group
263 assessed before 18 months
372 objective not risk factor analysis
222 single prognostic factor study
27 univariable or bivariable analysis only
91 articles containing a multi-
variable risk prediction model
13 excluded: same outcome
domain and age of assessment in
same cohort population

78 total articles with data extracted


28 with motor outcomes
31 with cognitive outcomes
15 with behavioural outcomes
3 with visual outcomes
0 with hearing outcomes
27 with composite outcomes

Figure 1. Search and selection process used to locate published studies that developed a multivariable risk factor model for neurodevelopmen-
tal outcomes in very preterm and very low birth weight children, 19902014.

use in clinical practice (https://neonatal.rti.org/OTEstimator/). prospective cohort study of all live births in all research cen-
In this study, the model was developed by randomly splitting ters in a geographically dened region. The strength of this
the data set into a development set (70%) and a validation set design is that the sample represents the source population
(30%), and the model was tested in the validation set. and exposure to risk factors can be measured prior to the oc-
currence of an outcome (88). Studies based in a single re-
DISCUSSION search center, while convenient, have less generalizability
due to between-center differences in service provision, re-
This review has highlighted some strengths and weakness ferral, and management practices and the socioeconomic/
in the research methodology and reporting of multivariable ethnic characteristics of the local population. In addition,
risk factor models for neurodevelopmental outcomes in the neonatal intensive care unit populations are less representa-
VPT/VLBW population in the published literature. Recom- tive because they tend to have a different case-mix than the
mendations for future studies, based on the latest literature general VPT/VLBW population; they contain a higher pro-
in the eld, are discussed below in relation to the ndings of portion of infants with poorer outcomes due to referrals of
the review. These guidelines are intended as an aid for re- higher-risk infants from lower-level centers. Data from ran-
searchers planning risk factor studies and are not intended domized controlled trials can be used to study prognosis,
as a prescribed checklist. provided that the treatment allocation is included as a pre-
dictor variable, though such studies may have reduced gen-
Study design eralizability because of restricted trial eligibility criteria.
There was a lack of consistency in the gestational age and
Selection of participants. The optimal study design for birth weight criteria used nationally and internationally to
prognostic research in the VPT/VLBW population is a dene cohorts, with some studies using prematurity or birth

Am J Epidemiol. 2017;185(7):601612
Review of Risk Factor Models for VPT/VLBW Children 607

weight alone and others using a combination of both. Before comes and risk factors should be described clearly in sufcient
the routine use of ultrasound, cohorts were generally dened detail if the model is to be correctly interpreted or applied by
by birth weight due to the unreliable measurement of gesta- other researchers.
tional age. Although there has been a shift away from using Coding and modeling of continuous variables. The fac-
birth weightdened cohorts in the last 2 decades, even tors retained in the nal model and the value of their coef-
studies conducted more recently have used varying criteria. cients are strongly inuenced by the coding and methods
The typical VLBW cohort can be a fairly heterogeneous used to model continuous and categorical variables (95).
group due to the inclusion of more mature but extremely Many studies included in the review categorized some or all
growth-restricted infants, and it is recommended that epide- of the continuous risk predictors used in the modeling, often
miologic studies of immature newborns be based on gesta- without a clear rationale. The arbitrary categorization of con-
tional age rather than the birth weight criterion (6). tinuous predictors should be avoided, because this results in a
Sample size and number of events per variable. It is loss of information and statistical power. It also results in the
generally recommended that there be a minimum of 10 out- classication of individuals who are close to but on either side
come events per predictor variable when using logistic re- of the chosen cutpoint as having very different levels of risk
gression models for risk factor analysis (89), though the (96). Using cutpoints that are data-driven, such as the sample
evidence supporting this rule is weak, and some advocate median, are problematic if a model is transported to a different
the view that a minimum of 20 events per variable generally study population (97). Assessing whether the relationship be-
eliminates bias when many low-prevalence predictors are tween a continuous predictor and an outcome is linear or non-
included (90, 91). Simulation studies have shown that as the linear is important, yet few studies reported doing this. If
number of events per variable declines below 10, there is an a nonlinear relationship exists, then categorization or tting of
increase in bias and variability, unreliable condence inter- a nonlinear termfor example, a quadratic termmay be a
val coverage, and problems with model convergence (92). reasonable option. The use of splines and fractional polyno-
Overtting is a particular problem when small samples are mials could be also be considered in larger samples (95).
used for risk prediction modeling, which can lead to the Selection of risk predictors. There is no overall consen-
models performance being overoptimistic in the data set sus on the best strategy with which to select variables for in-
from which it was developed (93). The median sample size clusion in a risk factor model; however, some approaches
of studies included in the review was 219, and the median are not recommended. This includes screening candidate
number of risk factors in the nal models was 5, which factors using univariable tests of association with the out-
means that, on average, studies lacked the power to detect come, as the correlation with other risk factors is not con-
associations for any outcome with an event rate less than trolled for. This can result in the rejection of important risk
20%. It is difcult to calculate a suitable sample size for risk factors that only become predictive after adjustment for
prediction studies, and risk factor analysis is often a second- other factors (92, 98). Nearly half of the studies included all
ary aim when planning a cohort study; however, it is advis- candidate variables, but many started with fewer than 20
able to not stray too far below the recommended number of variables at the outset, so it is likely that some preselection
events per variable. process was applied but not reported. It is important to re-
port any procedure used to reduce the number of candidate
Model development variables in sufcient detail so that the degree of coverage
(of factors considered) can be assessed.
Denitions of outcomes and risk factors. The measure- We caution against the use of automated variable selec-
ment and assessment of outcomes were generally clearly de- tion processes, such as forward selection, backward elimin-
ned and robust in the studies included in this review, though ation, or stepwise approaches, as they are data-driven and
it would be helpful to have more international agreement and ignore clinical plausibility. This can lead to poorly perform-
standardizationfor example, in the diagnostic criteria used ing models with biased regression coefcients (93, 99,
by studies to dene cerebral palsy, which are available (94). 100). Some important risk factors that are well-established
There was more consistency across studies in the tests used to in the literature may not always be statistically signicant in
measure motor and cognitive outcomes than there was for a particular data set, but it is advisable to include these in
tests used for the other outcome domains; however, studies the development process. The preferred approach is to start
did not always use the same cutpoints for impairment, and with the full model and eliminate factors one by one (taking
some used continuous scores, which made it difcult to com- both statistical signicance and clinical relevance into con-
pare ndings. Few studies reported whether assessments were sideration), because this avoids overtting and selection
blinded to previous medical history. For risk factor variables, bias and provides correct estimates of standard errors (101).
some conditions such as bronchopulmonary dysplasia and in- The level of signicance has a major effect on the number
traventricular hemorrhage were dened quite consistently of variables retained; a 1% level will almost always result
across studies, whereas other factors such as sepsis and necro- in a more parsimonious model than a 5% level. However,
tizing enterocolitis varied in denition or were not clearly de- starting with the full model can be problematic when there
ned at all. It is recommended that outcomes be assessed are a large number of candidate factors, as is usually the
prospectively using comprehensive, well-validated tests or case when predicting neurodevelopmental outcomes in the
using standard diagnostic criteria with a strict protocol, VPT/VLBW population, due to the vast amount of data
blinded to previous medical history. The denitions of out- usually collected during the neonatal period and following

Am J Epidemiol. 2017;185(7):601612
608 Linsell et al.

discharge. Six studies (12, 30, 45, 47, 52, 85) in the review and results in a loss of power, and therefore are the preferred
adopted a sequential multivariable approach to prediction, approaches for internal validation (88, 105).
tting candidate factors in stages according to the time
frame in which they occurred or according to themes, such
Strengths and limitations of the review
as clinical and sociodemographic factors. This approach
seems reasonable, given that prognosis in these children is a The search lter used in this review was intentionally broad
complex and dynamic process, with environmental factors at the expense of precision in order to capture all studies
potentially superseding the inuence of early biological fac- reporting risk factor analyses, which resulted in a large num-
tors as the child grows up. ber of articles retrieved. This approach is recommended for
reviews in elds in which clinical prediction models are
Reporting and model validation largely underdeveloped, rather than a more specic search
lter, which would have had a high false-negative rate, po-
Attrition and missing data. One area that could easily be tentially leading to many articles being missed (106). No lan-
improved is the reporting of study ascertainment and attrition. guage restrictions were imposed and no further articles were
While this was excellent in some studies, it was lacking in identied in the hand search of bibliographies of all studies
many others, which made it difcult to determine how repre- included, so it is unlikely that there were any major omis-
sentative the study populations were. In prospective cohort sions. Some studies included in the review were published
studies where children are followed up at multiple time points, before the proliferation of comprehensive guidelines on the
the numbers and reasons for exclusions and dropouts at each conduct and reporting of research studies (http://www.
stage should be clearly reported in a ow diagram. In retro- equator-network.org/) and may not reect the standards of
spective studies, the number of infants assessed for inclusion more current work. Furthermore, many of them preceded the
should be reported, in addition to the number selected for in- publication of the large body of literature that now exists on
clusion, which is important for assessing the risk of selection risk prediction modeling. Specic reporting guidelines for
bias. There is evidence of selective dropout in studies of pre- this eld have only just emerged (5).
term children, with those lost to follow-up being more likely
to be severely impaired or to have mothers with a lower level
of education (102104); therefore, it is important to report and Conclusion
assess the potential impact of dropout on the results. While the This systematic review of 78 published articles reporting
majority of studies reported that a comparison of children lost on multivariable risk factor models for neurodevelopmental
versus not lost to follow-up had been conducted, the data relat- outcomes in surviving VPT/VLBW children has revealed
ing to this were not always fully reported in the article (or not some shortcomings in methodology and reporting that could
provided as supplemental material). If further participants are be improved in future studies, and has conrmed that there is
excluded from the nal model due to missing data, this num- a dearth of properly designed and well-conducted prognostic
ber should be reported and the representativeness of the anal- modeling studies in this eld. Modeling long-term outcomes
ysis population should be assessed. in such a heterogeneous population is challenging, often with
Model reporting and validation. Some studies only re- the existence of multiple impairments within the same indi-
ported the results for selected variables in the nal model or vidual and with multiple risk factors acting sequentially over
only provided P values or regression coefcients with no con- time. In the 3 published reviews of risk factors for cognitive
dence intervals. The absolute minimum data that should be and motor impairment and behavioral problems that were
reported are regression coefcients with condence intervals based on these studies, the evidence for most risk factors was
for all factors retained in the nal model, but it is also helpful mixed or unclear (24). This may be due to the difculty of
to report the results of important intermediate stages of model modeling prognosis in this population, but it may also be due
development (as supplemental material if space is not permit- to differences in study design, study population, methodo-
ted). If authors wish to go further and develop a prognostic logical quality, and lack of standardization of measures. The
model, then its performance can be assessed for calibration ndings and recommendations of this critical review should
(comparing observed and predicted outcomes for groups), ac- be used as a basis for the design and analysis of future studies
curacy (comparing the observed and predicted outcomes for seeking to develop multivariate risk factor or prognostic mod-
individuals), and discrimination (ability to distinguish between els in this population.
individuals at low and high risk of developing an outcome).
These tests can be carried out on the data used to derive the
model but should ideally be carried out on new data, either
from the same source (internal validation) or from an inde- ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
pendent source (external validation) to evaluate the transport-
ability of the model. Methods of internal validation include Author afliations: National Perinatal Epidemiology
split-sample (splitting the sample randomly into a training Unit, Nufeld Department of Population Health, University
set for model development and a test set for model valid- of Oxford, Oxford, United Kingdom (Louise Linsell, Reem
ation), cross-validation (developing a model in each set and Malouf, Jennifer J. Kurinczuk); Queen Mary University of
testing it on the other set), and bootstrapping (resampling with London, Centre for Environmental and Preventive
replacement). The latter 2 resampling methods are more Medicine, Barts and The London School of Medicine and
effective than the split-sample approach, which is inefcient Dentistry, London, United Kingdom (Joan Morris); and

Am J Epidemiol. 2017;185(7):601612
Review of Risk Factor Models for VPT/VLBW Children 609

Institute of Womens Health, University College London, DevelopmentThird Edition. J Pediatr. 2013;163(3):
London, United Kingdom (Neil Marlow). 680685.e3.
This work was supported by National Institute for Health 12. Ambalavanan N, Carlo WA, Tyson JE, et al. Outcome
Research doctoral research fellowship NIHR-DRF-2012- trajectories in extremely preterm infants. Pediatrics. 2012;
130(1):e115e125.
05-206.
13. Andrews WW, Cliver SP, Biasini F, et al. Early preterm birth:
We thank Nia Wyn Roberts, Outreach Librarian at the association between in utero exposure to acute inammation
Bodleian Health Care Libraries (Oxford, United Kingdom), and severe neurodevelopmental disability at 6 years of age.
for her input and expertise during the search phase of the Am J Obstet Gynecol. 2008;198(4):466.e1466.e11.
review. 14. Arnaud C, Daubisse-Marliac L, White-Koning M, et al.
The funder had no role in the study design, data Prevalence and associated factors of minor neuromotor
collection, data analysis, manuscript preparation, or dysfunctions at age 5 years in prematurely born children: the
publication decisions. This article presents independent EPIPAGE study. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2007;161(11):
research funded by the National Institute for Health 10531061.
Research (NIHR), United Kingdom. The views expressed 15. Ayoubi JM, Audibert F, Boithias C, et al. Perinatal factors
affecting survival and survival without disability of extreme
are those of the authors and not necessarily those of the
premature infants at two years of age. Eur J Obstet Gynecol
National Health Service, the NIHR, or the United Kingdom Reprod Biol. 2002;105(2):124131.
Department of Health. 16. Beaino G, Khoshnood B, Kaminski M, et al. Predictors
Conict of interest: none declared. of the risk of cognitive deciency in very preterm infants:
the EPIPAGE prospective cohort. Acta Paediatr. 2011;
100(3):370378.
17. Beaino G, Khoshnood B, Kaminski M, et al. Predictors of
cerebral palsy in very preterm infants: the EPIPAGE
REFERENCES prospective population-based cohort study. Dev Med Child
Neurol. 2010;52(6):e119e125.
1. Saigal S, Doyle LW. An overview of mortality and sequelae 18. Bolisetty S, Dhawan A, Abdel-Latif M, et al. Intraventricular
of preterm birth from infancy to adulthood. Lancet. 2008; hemorrhage and neurodevelopmental outcomes in extreme
371(9608):261269. preterm infants. Pediatrics. 2014;133(1):5562.
2. Linsell L, Malouf R, Morris J, et al. Prognostic factors for 19. Brvaut-Malaty V, Busuttil M, Einaudi MA, et al.
poor cognitive development in children born very preterm or Longitudinal follow-up of a cohort of 350 singleton infants
with very low birth weight: a systematic review. JAMA born at less than 32 weeks of amenorrhea: neurocognitive
Pediatr. 2015;169(12):11621172. screening, academic outcome, and perinatal factors. Eur J
3. Linsell L, Malouf R, Morris J, et al. Prognostic factors for Obstet Gynecol Reprod Biol. 2010;150(1):1318.
cerebral palsy and poor motor development in children born 20. Charkaluk ML, Truffert P, Fily A, et al. Neurodevelopment
very preterm or with very low birth weight: a systematic of children born very preterm and free of severe disabilities:
review. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2016;58(6):554569. the Nord-Pas de Calais Epipage cohort study. Acta Paediatr.
4. Linsell L, Malouf R, Johnson S, et al. Prognostic factors for 2010;99(5):684689.
behavioural problems and psychiatric disorders in children 21. Cooke RW. Perinatal and postnatal factors in very preterm
born very preterm or with very low birth weight: a systematic infants and sequent cognitive and motor abilities. Arch Dis
review. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 2016;37(1):88102. Child Fetal Neonatal Ed. 2005;90(1):F60F63.
5. Collins GS, Reitsma JB, Altman DG, et al. Transparent 22. Davis NM, Ford GW, Anderson PJ, et al. Developmental
Reporting of a multivariable prediction model for Individual coordination disorder at 8 years of age in a regional cohort of
Prognosis Or Diagnosis (TRIPOD): the TRIPOD statement. extremely-low-birthweight or very preterm infants. Dev Med
J Clin Epidemiol. 2015;68(2):134143. Child Neurol. 2007;49(5):325330.
6. Arnold CC, Kramar MS, Hobbs CA, et al. Very low birth 23. de Waal CG, Weisglas-Kuperus N, van Goudoever JB, et al.
weight: a problematic cohort for epidemiologic studies of Mortality, neonatal morbidity and two year follow-up of
very small or immature neonates. Am J Epidemiol. 1991; extremely preterm infants born in the netherlands in 2007.
134(6):604613. PLoS One. 2012;7(7):e41302.
7. Van den Berg T, Heymans MW, Leone SS, et al. Overview 24. Delobel-Ayoub M, Arnaud C, White-Koning M, et al.
of data-synthesis in systematic reviews of studies on outcome Behavioral problems and cognitive performance at 5 years of
prediction models. BMC Med Res Methodol. 2013;13:42. age after very preterm birth: the EPIPAGE study. Pediatrics.
8. Riley RD, Hayden JA, Steyerberg EW, et al. Prognosis 2009;123(6):14851492.
Research Strategy (PROGRESS) 2: prognostic factor 25. Delobel-Ayoub M, Kaminski M, Marret S, et al. Behavioral
research. PLoS Med. 2013;10(2):e1001380. outcome at 3 years of age in very preterm infants: the
9. Moher D, Liberati A, Tetzlaff J, et al. Preferred Reporting EPIPAGE study. Pediatrics. 2006;117(6):19962005.
Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses: the 26. Dewey D, Creighton DE, Heath JA, et al. Assessment of
PRISMA statement. BMJ. 2009;339:b2535. developmental coordination disorder in children born with
10. Aarnoudse-Moens CS, Weisglas-Kuperus N, Duivenvoorden extremely low birth weights. Dev Neuropsychol. 2011;36(1):
HJ, et al. Neonatal and parental predictors of executive 4256.
function in very preterm children. Acta Paediatr. 2013; 27. Doyle LW; Victorian Infant Collaborative Study Group.
102(3):282286. Outcome at 5 years of age of children 23 to 27 weeks gestation:
11. Adams-Chapman I, Bann CM, Vaucher YE, et al. rening the prognosis. Pediatrics. 2001;108(1):134141.
Association between feeding difculties and language delay 28. Emsley HC, Wardle SP, Sims DG, et al. Increased
in preterm infants using Bayley Scales of Infant survival and deteriorating developmental outcome

Am J Epidemiol. 2017;185(7):601612
610 Linsell et al.

in 23 to 25 week old gestation infants, 19904 compared years in the EPICure study. J Am Acad Child Adolesc
with 19849. Arch Dis Child Fetal Neonatal Ed. 1998; Psychiatry. 2010;49(5):453463.e1.
78(2):F99F104. 45. Johnson S, Hollis C, Kochhar P, et al. Autism spectrum
29. Farooqi A, Hgglf B, Sedin G, et al. Impact at age 11 years disorders in extremely preterm children. J Pediatr. 2010;
of major neonatal morbidities in children born extremely 156(4):525531.e2.
preterm. Pediatrics. 2011;127(5):e1247e1257. 46. Johnson S, Wolke D, Hennessy E, et al. Educational
30. Fily A, Pierrat V, Delporte V, et al. Factors associated with outcomes in extremely preterm children: neuropsychological
neurodevelopmental outcome at 2 years after very preterm correlates and predictors of attainment. Dev Neuropsychol.
birth: the population-based Nord-Pas-de-Calais EPIPAGE 2011;36(1):7495.
cohort. Pediatrics. 2006;117(2):357366. 47. Jones KM, Champion PR, Woodward LJ. Social competence
31. Ford RM, Neulinger K, OCallaghan M, et al. Executive of preschool children born very preterm. Early Hum Dev.
function in 79-year-old children born extremely preterm or 2013;89(10):795802.
with extremely low birth weight: effects of biomedical 48. Kiechl-Kohlendorfer U, Ralser E, Pupp Peglow U, et al.
history, age at assessment, and socioeconomic status. Arch Early risk predictors for impaired numerical skills in
Clin Neuropsychol. 2011;26(7):632644. 5-year-old children born before 32 weeks of gestation. Acta
32. Franz AR, Pohlandt F, Bode H, et al. Intrauterine, early Paediatr. 2013;102(1):6671.
neonatal, and postdischarge growth and neurodevelopmental 49. Leversen KT, Sommerfelt K, Ronnestad A, et al. Predicting
outcome at 5.4 years in extremely preterm infants after neurosensory disabilities at two years of age in a national
intensive neonatal nutritional support. Pediatrics. 2009; cohort of extremely premature infants. Early Hum Dev.
123(1):e101e109. 2010;86(9):581586.
33. Gianni ML, Picciolini O, Vegni C, et al. Twelve-month 50. Leversen KT, Sommerfelt K, Ronnestad A, et al. Prediction
neurofunctional assessment and cognitive performance at 36 of neurodevelopmental and sensory outcome at 5 years in
months of age in extremely low birth weight infants. Norwegian children born extremely preterm. Pediatrics.
Pediatrics. 2007;120(5):10121019. 2011;127(3):e630e638.
34. Goyen TA, Lui K. Developmental coordination disorder in 51. Lowe J, MacLean PC, Shaffer ML, et al. Early working
apparently normal schoolchildren born extremely preterm. memory in children born with extremely low birth weight:
Arch Dis Child. 2009;94(4):298302. assessed by object permanence. J Child Neurol. 2009;24(4):
35. Hack M, Taylor HG, Schluchter M, et al. Behavioral 410415.
outcomes of extremely low birth weight children at age 8 52. Marston L, Peacock JL, Calvert SA, et al. Factors affecting
years. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 2009;30(2):122130. vocabulary acquisition at age 2 in children born between 23
36. Hansen BM, Hoff B, Uldall P, et al. Perinatal risk factors of and 28 weeks gestation. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2007;49(8):
adverse outcome in very preterm children: a role of initial 591596.
treatment of respiratory insufciency? Acta Paediatr. 2004; 53. Matsuda Y, Kouno S, Hiroyama Y, et al. Intrauterine
93(2):185189. infection, magnesium sulfate exposure and cerebral palsy in
37. Helderman JB, OShea TM, Kuban KC, et al. Antenatal infants born between 26 and 30 weeks of gestation. Eur J
antecedents of cognitive impairment at 24 months in Obstet Gynecol Reprod Biol. 2000;91(2):159164.
extremely low gestational age newborns. Pediatrics. 2012; 54. Mercier CE, Dunn MS, Ferrelli KR, et al.
129(3):494502. Neurodevelopmental outcome of extremely low birth weight
38. Hk-Wikstrand M, Hrd AL, Niklassn A, et al. Early infants from the Vermont Oxford Network: 19982003.
postnatal growth variables are related to morphologic and Neonatology. 2010;97(4):329338.
functional ophthalmologic outcome in children born preterm. 55. Messinger D, Lambert B, Bauer CR, et al. The relationship
Acta Paediatr. 2010;99(5):658664. between behavior ratings and concurrent and subsequent
39. Hlmstrom GE, Kllen K, Hellstrm A, et al. mental and motor performance in toddlers born at extremely
Ophthalmologic outcome at 30 months corrected age of a low birth weight. J Early Interv. 2010;32(3):214233.
prospective Swedish cohort of children born before 27 weeks 56. Mikkola K, Ritari N, Tommiska V, et al.
of gestation: the Extremely Preterm Infants in Sweden study. Neurodevelopmental outcome at 5 years of age of a national
JAMA Ophthalmol. 2014;132(2):182189. cohort of extremely low birth weight infants who were born
40. Howard K, Roberts G, Lim J, et al. Biological and in 19961997. Pediatrics. 2005;116(6):13911400.
environmental factors as predictors of language skills in very 57. Moore T, Johnson S, Hennessy E, et al. Screening for autism
preterm children at 5 years of age. J Dev Behav Pediatr. in extremely preterm infants: problems in interpretation. Dev
2011;32(3):239249. Med Child Neurol. 2012;54(6):514520.
41. Iijima S, Arai H, Ozawa Y, et al. Clinical patterns in 58. Neubauer AP, Voss W, Kattner E. Outcome of extremely
extremely preterm (22 to 24 weeks of gestation) infants in low birth weight survivors at school age: the inuence of
relation to survival time and prognosis. Am J Perinatol. perinatal parameters on neurodevelopment. Eur J Pediatr.
2009;26(6):399406. 2008;167(1):8795.
42. Janssen AJ, Nijhuis-van der Sanden MW, Akkermans 59. Orchinik LJ, Taylor HG, Espy KA, et al. Cognitive outcomes
RP, et al. A model to predict motor performance in for extremely preterm/extremely low birth weight children in
preterm infants at 5 years. Early Hum Dev. 2009; kindergarten. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2011;17(6):
85(9):599604. 10671079.
43. Janssen AJ, Nijhuis-van der Sanden MW, Akkermans RP, 60. Peralta-Carcelen M, Bailey K, Rector R, et al. Behavioral and
et al. Inuence of behaviour and risk factors on motor socioemotional competence problems of extremely low birth
performance in preterm infants at age 2 to 3 years. Dev Med weight children. J Perinatol. 2013;33(11):887892.
Child Neurol. 2008;50(12):926931. 61. Pin TW, Eldridge B, Galea MP. Motor trajectories from 4 to
44. Johnson S, Hollis C, Kochhar P, et al. Psychiatric disorders 18 months corrected age in infants born at less than 30 weeks
in extremely preterm children: longitudinal nding at age 11 of gestation. Early Hum Dev. 2010;86(9):573580.

Am J Epidemiol. 2017;185(7):601612
Review of Risk Factor Models for VPT/VLBW Children 611

62. Potharst ES, Houtzager BA, van Sonderen L, et al. Prediction 79. Treyvaud K, Ure A, Doyle LW, et al. Psychiatric outcomes at
of cognitive abilities at the age of 5 years using age seven for very preterm children: rates and predictors.
developmental follow-up assessments at the age of 2 and 3 J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2013;54(7):772779.
years in very preterm children. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2012; 80. Trittmann JK, Nelin LD, Klebanoff MA. Bronchopulmonary
54(3):240246. dysplasia and neurodevelopmental outcome in extremely
63. Potharst ES, Van Wassenaer-Leemhuis AG, Houtzager BA, preterm neonates. Eur J Pediatr. 2013;172(9):11731180.
et al. Perinatal risk factors for neurocognitive impairments in 81. Vincer MJ, Allen AC, Joseph KS, et al. Increasing
preschool children born very preterm. Dev Med Child prevalence of cerebral palsy among very preterm infants: a
Neurol. 2013;55(2):178184. population-based study. Pediatrics. 2006;118(6):
64. Sann L, Bourgeois J, Stephant A, et al. A study of 490 e1621e1626.
premature babies born before 31 weeks gestation age. 82. Vohr BR, Wright LL, Poole WK, et al. Neurodevelopmental
Mortality and development before 2 years. Rev Franc outcomes of extremely low birth weight infants <32 weeks
Gynecol Obstet. 1999;94(6):500510. gestation between 1993 and 1998. Pediatrics. 2005;116(3):
65. Sann L, Bourgeois J, Stephant A, et al. Outcome of 249 635643.
infants of less than 29 weeks gestational age. Arch Pediatr. 83. Voss W, Jungmann T, Wachtendorf M, et al. Long-term
2001;8(3):250258. cognitive outcomes of extremely low-birth-weight infants:
66. Sansavini A, Guarini A, Savini S. Linguistic and cognitive the inuence of the maternal educational background. Acta
delays in very preterm infants at 2 years: general or Paediatr. 2012;101(6):569573.
specic delays? Rev Logoped Foniatr Audiol. 2011; 84. Wong HS, Huertas-Ceballos A, Cowan FM, et al.
31(3):133147. Evaluation of early childhood social-communication
67. Schlapbach LJ, Aebischer M, Adams M, et al. Impact of difculties in children born preterm using the Quantitative
sepsis on neurodevelopmental outcome in a Swiss national Checklist for Autism in Toddlers. J Pediatr. 2014;164(1):
cohort of extremely premature infants. Pediatrics. 2011; 2633.e1.
128(2):e348e357. 85. Wood NS, Costeloe K, Gibson AT, et al. The EPICure study:
68. Schmidhauser J, Caisch J, Rousson V, et al. Impaired motor associations and entecedents of neurological and
performance and movement quality in very-low-birthweight developmental disability at the 30 months of age following
children at 6 years of age. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2006; extremely preterm birth. Arch Dis Child Fetal Neonatal Ed.
48(9):718722. 2005;90(2):F134F140.
69. Schmidt B, Asztalos EV, Roberts RS, et al. Impact of 86. Zanudin A, Burns Y, Gray PH, et al. Perinatal events and
bronchopulmonary dysplasia, brain injury, and severe motor performance of children born with ELBW and
retinopathy on the outcome of extremely low-birthweight nondisabled. Pediatr Phys Ther. 2013;25(1):3035.
infants at 18 months: results from the trial of indomethacin 87. Zanudin A, Gray PH, Burns Y, et al. Perinatal factors in
prophylaxis in preterms. JAMA. 2003;289(9):11241129. non-disabled ELBW school children and later performance.
70. Spittle AJ, Treyvaud K, Doyle LW, et al. Early emergence of J Paediatr Child Health. 2013;49(1):E62E67.
behavior and social-emotional problems in very preterm infants. 88. Moons KG, Royston P, Vergouwe Y, et al. Prognosis and
J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2009;48(9):909918. prognostic research: what, why, and how? BMJ. 2009;338:
71. Stahlmann N, Rapp M, Herting E, et al. Outcome of b375.
extremely premature infants at early school age: 89. Peduzzi P, Concato J, Kemper E, et al. A simulation study of
health-related quality of life and neurosensory, cognitive, and the number of events per variable in logistic regression
behavioral outcomes in a population-based sample in analysis. J Clin Epidemiol. 1996;49(12):13731379.
northern Germany. Neuropediatrics. 2009;40(3):112119. 90. Ogundimu EO, Altman DG, Collins GS. Adequate sample
72. Stephens BE, Bann CM, Watson VE, et al. Screening for size for developing prediction models is not simply related
autism spectrum disorders in extremely preterm infants. to events per variable. J Clin Epidemiol. 2016;76:
J Dev Behav Pediatr. 2012;33(7):535541. 175182.
73. Stoelhorst GM, Martens SE, Rijken M, et al. Behaviour at 2 91. van Smeden M, de Groot JA, Moons KG, et al. No
years of age in very preterm infants (gestational age <32 rationale for 1 variable per 10 events criterion for binary
weeks). Acta Paediatr. 2003;92(5):595601. logistic regression analysis. BMC Med Res Methodol.
74. Stoelhorst GM, Rijken M, Martens SE, et al. Developmental 2016;16:163.
outcome at 18 and 24 months of age in very preterm children: 92. Harrell FE Jr, Lee KL, Mark DB. Multivariable prognostic
a cohort study from 1996 to 1997. Early Hum Dev. 2003; models: issues in developing models, evaluating assumptions
72(2):8395. and adequacy, and measuring and reducing errors. Stat Med.
75. Taylor HG, Klein N, Anselmo MG, et al. Learning problems 1996;15(4):361387.
in kindergarten students with extremely preterm birth. Arch 93. Steyerberg EW, Eijkemans MJC, Harrell FE, et al.
Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2011;165(9):819825. Prognostic modeling with logistic regression analysis: in
76. Taylor HG, Klein N, Drotar D, et al. Consequences and risks search of a sensible strategy in small data sets. Med Decis
of <1000-g birth weight for neuropsychological skills, Making. 2001;21(1):4556.
achievement, and adaptive functioning. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 94. Surveillance of Cerebral Palsy in Europe. Surveillance of
2006;27(6):459469. cerebral palsy in Europe: a collaboration of cerebral palsy
77. Tommiska V, Heinonen K, Kero P, et al. A national two year surveys and registers. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2000;42(12):
follow up study of extremely low birthweight infants born in 816824.
19961997. Arch Dis Child Fetal Neonatal Ed. 2003;88(1): 95. Royston P, Sauerbrei W. Multivariable Model-Building: A
F29F35. Pragmatic Approach to Regression Analysis Based on
78. Toome L, Varendi H, Mannamaa M, et al. Follow-up study Fractional Polynomials for Modelling Continuous
of 2-year-olds born at very low gestational age in Estonia. Variables. Chichester, United Kingdom: John Wiley &
Acta Paediatr. 2013;102(3):300307. Sons Ltd.; 2008.

Am J Epidemiol. 2017;185(7):601612
612 Linsell et al.

96. Royston P, Altman DG, Sauerbrei W. Dichotomising Survival Analysis. New York, NY: Springer-Verlag;
continuous predictors in multiple regression: a bad idea. Stat 2001.
Med. 2006;25(1):127141. 102. Wolke D, Shne B, Ohrt B, et al. Follow-up of preterm
97. Altman DG, Royston P. The cost of dichotomising children: important to document drop-outs. Lancet. 1995;
continuous variables. BMJ. 2006;332(7549):1080. 345(8947):447.
98. Sun GW, Shook TL, Kay GL. Innapropriate use of 103. Johnson S, Fawke J, Hennessy E, et al. Neurodevelopmental
bivariable analysis to screen risk factors for use in disability through 11 years of age in children born before
multivariable analysis. J Clin Epidemiol. 1996;49(8): 26 weeks of gestation. Pediatrics. 2009;124(2):
907916. e249e257.
99. Steyerberg EW, Eijkemans MJ, Harrell FE Jr, et al. 104. Tin W, Fritz S, Wariyar U, et al. Outcome of very preterm
Prognostic modelling with logistic regression analysis: a birth: children reviewed with ease at 2 years differ from those
comparison of selection and estimation methods in small data followed up with difculty. Arch Dis Child Fetal Neonatal
sets. Stat Med. 2000;19(8):10591079. Ed. 1998;79(2):F83F87.
100. Austin PC, Tu JV. Automated variable selection methods for 105. Steyerberg EW. Clinical Prediction Models. New York, NY:
logistic regression produced unstable models for predicting Springer Publishing Company; 2008.
acute myocardial infarction mortality. J Clin Epidemiol. 106. Geersing GJ, Bouwmeester W, Zuithoff P, et al. Search lters
2004;57(11):11381146. for nding prognostic and diagnostic prediction studies
101. Harrell FE. Regression Modelling Strategies: With in Medline to enhance systematic reviews. PLoS One. 2012;
Application to Linear Models, Logistic Regression and 7(2):e32844.

Am J Epidemiol. 2017;185(7):601612

Você também pode gostar