Você está na página 1de 8

AMERICAN JOURNAL OF HUMAN BIOLOGY 19:8895 (2007)

Original Research Article

Studies of Human Physique and Sexual Attractiveness: Sexual


Preferences of Men and Women in China
BARNABY J. DIXSON,1 ALAN F. DIXSON,2* BAOGUO LI,3 AND M.J. ANDERSON1
1
Department of Conservation and Research for Endangered Species, Zoological Society of San Diego,
San Diego, California
2
School of Biological Sciences, Victoria University of Wellington, Wellington, New Zealand
3
College of Life Sciences, and Key Laboratory Resource Biology and Biotechnology in
Western China, Ministry of Education, Northwest University, Xian, China

ABSTRACT Men and women at Northwest University (n 631), Xian, China, were asked
to rate the attractiveness of male or female gures manipulated to vary somatotype, waist-to-
hip ratio (WHR), secondary sexual traits, and other features. In study 1, women rated the aver-
age masculine somatotype as most attractive, followed by the mesomorphic (muscular), ecto-
morphic (slim), and endomorphic (heavily built) somatotypes, in descending order of preference.
In study 2, the amount and distribution of masculine trunk (chest and abdominal) hair were
altered progressively in a series of front-posed gures. Women rated gures with no or little
trunk hair as most attractive. Study 3 assessed the attractiveness of front-posed male gures
which varied only in length of their nonerect penis. Numerical ratings for this trait were low,
but moderate lengthening of the penis (22% or 33% above average) resulted in a signicant
increase in scores for attractiveness. In study 4, Chinese men rated the attractiveness of back-
posed female images varying in waist-to-hip ratio (WHR from 0.51.0). The 0.6 WHR gure was
most preferred, followed by 0.7, while gures with higher ratios (0.9 or 1.0) were signicantly
less attractive. Study 5 rated the attractiveness of female skin color: men expressed a marked
preference for images which were lighter in color, as compared to images of average or darker
skin colors. These results, the rst of their kind reported for a Chinese population, support the
view that sexual selection has inuenced the evolution of human physique and sexual attrac-
tiveness in men and women. Am. J. Hum. Biol. 19:8895, 2007. ' 2006 Wiley-Liss, Inc.

There is increasing evidence that sexual with higher levels of 17-b-estradiol and
selection has inuenced the evolution of greater reproductive potential in women
human morphology, especially via sexual pref- (Jasienska et al., 2004). Women with higher
erences for features which signal female re- follicular-phase levels of estrogen are also
productive potential or masculine strength judged to be facially more attractive by men
and tness. In women, for example, the waist- (Law-Smith et al., 2006). Measurements of the
to-hip ratio (WHR) may provide an honest sig- body mass index (BMI) of women are also cor-
nal of fat distribution in relation to repro- related with female attractiveness; the rela-
ductive health (Singh, 1993, 2002; Singh and tive importance of BMI and WHR in this
Young, 1995). A low female WHR (0.7) is respect was debated by various authors (e.g.,
judged as most attractive by men in some cul- Tovee et al., 1999; Streeter and McBurney,
tures (e.g., North America: Singh and Young, 2003; Schutzwohl, 2006).
1995), but not in all human populations (e.g., Skin color is another trait which may be
the Matsigenka of Peru: Yu and Shepard, related to female attractiveness. Darwin (1871)
1998; the Hadza of Tanzania: Wetsman and noted that women in various parts of the world
Marlowe, 1998). Recent work on the Hadza
conrmed the importance of a low WHR in *Correspondence to: Alan F. Dixson, School of Biological
female attractiveness, provided that side- Sciences, Victoria University of Wellington, PO Box 600,
posed images are used which allow men to Wellington, New Zealand. E-mail: alan.dixson@vuw.ac.nz
Received 17 January 2006; Revision received 5 August
include judgments of the shape and size of the 2006; Accepted 8 August 2006
female buttocks (Marlowe et al., 2005). Nar- Published online in Wiley InterScience (www.interscience.
row waists and large breasts are correlated wiley.com). DOI 10.1002/ajhb.20584

V
C 2006 Wiley-Liss, Inc.
SEXUAL PREFERENCES IN CHINA 89

tend to have lighter skin than men. He quoted signicant role in visual display either within,
Schweinfurth, for example, concerning the or between, the sexes (Short, 1980; Diamond,
Monbuttoos of Africa: Like all her race she 1997; Potts and Short, 1999). It may play some
had a skin several shades lighter than her hus- role in female sexual preference, although
bands, being something of the colour of half- with some exceptions (Stulhofer, 2006), the
roasted coffee. Some evidence to support evidence for this remains largely anecdotal.
effects of sexual selection on female skin color The current study was undertaken in order
has been obtained (van den Berghe and Frost, to obtain information on the sexual preferences
1986; Frost, 1988; Symons, 1995). Female skin of young men and women in a Chinese popula-
color changes with age and reproductive condi- tion. Questionnaires were used to measure
tion (e.g., in pregnant and multiparous female preferences for images which varied in
women), so that it might provide men with vis- masculine physique (somatotype), distribution
ual cues of female age and reproductive condi- of chest and abdominal hair, and penile size, as
tion (Symons, 1995). However, few cross-cul- well as mens preferences for female waist-to-
tural data have been collected to assess mens hip ratio and skin color. The results, which are
preferences for female skin color. the rst of their kind obtained for a Chinese
Where masculine physique is concerned, population, are discussed in relation to studies
there is evidence that taller men achieve conducted in other parts of the world.
greater reproductive success (Pawlowski et al.,
2000), and that a broad chest, narrow waist,
METHODS
and muscular torso are rated as attractive by
women (Mealey, 1997; Lynch and Zellner, 1999; Subjects
Maisey et al., 1999). These traits may have This study was conducted at Northwest
played an important role in human evolution, University, in the city of Xian, Shaanxi Prov-
given the requirement for men to compete with ince, China. Students were asked to partici-
one another, to provide protection for female pate by lling out a questionnaire and rating
partners, and to assure competence in hunting various images (see below) for sexual attrac-
and other physically demanding survival skills tiveness. A total of 631 subjects participated
(Buss, 2003; Bramble and Lieberman, 2004). in the study (311 men and 320 women).
Men are more hirsute than women, and in a va-
riety of nonhuman primate species, reproduc-
Questionnaires
tively mature males exhibit androgen-depend-
ent capes of hair (Dixson, 1998). Sexual dimor- Each questionnaire had a cover sheet
phism in the distribution of trunk (chest and (written in Mandarin Chinese) for demographic
abdominal) hair in human beings might play data, including the respondents sex, age, and
some role in masculine sexual attractiveness. marital status. Participation was voluntary,
Human beings are unique among primates in and at no time were respondents asked to give
being bipedal, so that features on the chest and their names. Women were asked to rate images
abdomen are displayed more prominently to of males, varying in somatotype, distribution of
conspecics. Masculine trunk hair was rated as body hair, and size of genitalia. To produce
highly sexually attractive by women in studies these images, photographs of back-posed and
conducted in the UK (Dixson et al., 2003). front-posed men were scanned from Sheldon
Women also rated more hirsute male images as et al. (1954). Men were asked to rate the attrac-
being older than those lacking chest hair. Cross- tiveness of images of women, varying in WHR
cultural studies consistently show that women and skin color. Each image was manipulated
prefer to marry men who are older than them- using Photoshop version 7.0 in order to stand-
selves (Buss, 2003). However, there are very few ardize images for height, posture, and skin
cross-cultural data on the potential attractive- color. Skin color was matched to that of a Chi-
ness of masculine trunk hair, and this trait may nese sample by scanning photographs into the
vary considerably between human populations. computer and using these as the basis for color-
The assumption of an upright gait also ing images. Where front-posed images were
means that human genitalia are displayed in used, the faces were blocked out, as our studies
a different way, and perhaps more promi- did not concern facial stimuli.
nently, than in typically quadrupedal mam-
mals. A number of authors commented on the
relatively large size of the human penis, for Study 1. Study 1 assessed the preferences of
example, and the possibility that it plays some women for four back-posed male gures of

American Journal of Human Biology DOI 10.1002/ajhb


90 B.J. DIXSON ET AL.

varying somatotype (ectomorphic, endomor- and 15 units of contrast) to produce two


phic, mesomorphic, and average). images which were darker than the standard
image, and two that were lighter in color.
Data analysis and statistics
Study 2. Study 2 assessed womens preferen-
ces for front-posed images of males differing In studies 13, a repeated-measures analy-
in levels of hirsuteness of the trunk (chest and sis of variance (ANOVA) was applied to deter-
abdomen). Five images were presented, which mine whether attractiveness ratings for the
varied only in degree of trunk hair (from none various stimuli exhibited signicant differen-
to pronounced hirsuteness). ces. Post hoc Scheffe tests were then used to
test for signicant differences in attractive-
ness ratings in pairs of stimuli, but only if the
Study 3. Study 3 examined womens prefer- initial ANOVA had yielded a signicant result
ences for ve male images varying only in (P < 0.05). In studies 4 and 5, w2 tests were
length of the (nonerect) penis. In one gure, used to compare observed vs. expected scores
the penis was its original size (i.e., the same for preferences expressed by men for the vari-
size as the original photograph used to model ous female images.
the image in the computer). In the remaining
four images, penile length was modeled to be RESULTS
either smaller or larger than the original size. A total of 320 women answered the ques-
We had intended to alter penile lengths to be tionnaire: 68% of them were 20 years or less
80%, 120%, 130%, and 140% of original size. in age, and the remainder were aged 2130
In the event, measurement of the actual years. Only one of these women was married.
images conrmed that they were 78%, 122%, The ages of male respondents (n311) were
133%, and 143% of original size, respectively. distributed as follows: <20 years old 44%;
In all three studies, women rated each of 2130 years 54.5%; 3140 years 1%; >40
the stimulus images on a six-point scale where years 0.5%. Six of these men (i.e., 2%) were
0 unattractive, 1 only slightly attractive, married. Hence our sample consisted predomi-
2 mildly attractive, 3 moderately attrac- nantly of young and unmarried university stu-
tive, 4 very attractive, and 5 extremely dents in their late teens or twenties.
attractive. The scale appeared next to each
image, as in previous studies (Dixson et al., Study 1: Womens ratings of masculine
2003). somatotypes
ANOVA showed a signicant effect for wom-
Study 4. Study 4 examined mens preferen- ens ratings of the attractiveness of the four
ces for back-posed images of the same women, male gures (F[3,957] 293, P < 0.0001). The
differing only in waist-to-hip ratios. Using average somatotype was judged as most
Photoshop, six images were produced (WHR attractive, followed by the mesomorphic soma-
0.5, 0.6, 0.7, 0.8, 0.9, and 1.0). These images totype and the ectomorphic image. The endo-
were presented in random order on the same morphic somatotype was clearly rated as the
page of the questionnaire; respondents were least attractive image (Fig. 1). Post hoc
asked to select only the image that they found Scheffe tests revealed the endomorphic image
most attractive sexually. On a subsequent to be signicantly less attractive than any of
page, the same stimuli were presented again, the other three somatotypes (P < 0.001 for
but respondents were asked to select the one each paired comparison). The male image
they found most attractive for a long-term depicting an average somatotype was rated as
relationship. signicantly more attractive than the meso-
morphic gure (P < 0.001), and the meso-
morph achieved a higher rating than the ecto-
Study 5. Study 5 assessed male preferences morphic image, but only slightly so (P < 0.05;
for variations in female skin color. Five images Fig. 1).
of an identical back-posed female gure Study 2: Womens ratings of male images
(WHR, 0.8) were used. One standard image varying in hirsuteness
had skin typical for a Chinese woman. Skin
tone was altered (using Adobe Photoshop) in a ANOVA showed that the ve male (meso-
stepwise fashion (by 10 units of brightness morphic) gures differing only in amounts of

American Journal of Human Biology DOI 10.1002/ajhb


SEXUAL PREFERENCES IN CHINA 91

Fig. 2. Womens mean ratings ( SEM) for attrac-


tiveness of front-posed male gures which vary only in
hirsuteness of trunk (chest and abdomen). None, no
trunk hair; Max, pronounced hirsuteness. *P < 0.05,
***P < 0.001. [Color gure can be viewed in the online
issue, which is available at www.interscience.wiley.
com.]
Fig. 1. Womens mean ratings ( SEM) for sexual
attractiveness of back-posed male gures of four differ-
ent somatotypes: ENDO, endomorphic; ECTO, ectomor- ings were mean 1.87 and mean 1.84 for
phic; MESO, mesomorphic; AVER, average body build. images in which the penis was 22% or 33%
*P < 0.05, ***P < 0.001. [Color gure can be viewed in longer in the original, respectively (it should
the online issue, which is available at www.interscience.
wiley.com.] be recalled that a rating of 1 slightly attrac-
tive, and 2 mildly attractive). All images
used in study 3 depicted the same mesomor-
trunk (chest and abdominal) hair varied sig- phic front-posed gure, lacking trunk hair,
nicantly in their attractiveness to women and were identical except for their genitalia.
(F[4,1276] 76.69, P < 0.0001). Most attrac- The image depicting an unaltered penile
tive was the image lacking any trunk hair, length and lacking trunk hair also formed
with a progressive decline in attractiveness as part of study 2, on masculine hirsuteness. In
more hair was added to the image (Fig. 2). The that study, this gure scored a mean score of
image with the most chest hair was rated as 2.48, whereas in the study on penile size the
signicantly less attractive than any of the identical image was rated as signicantly less
other four images (P < 0.001 for each paired attractive, with a mean score of 1.38. An
comparison). unpaired t-test revealed that this difference
was signicant (P < 0.0001).
Study 3: Penile size and attractiveness
Study 4: Mens ratings of female WHR
ANOVA revealed an overall effect of penile
length on attractiveness ratings (F[4,1276] The majority of men rated the female image
29.59, P < 0.0001). Women rated the gure depicting a WHR of 0.6 as most attractive sex-
with the shortest penis (reduced to 78% of nor- ually, and most attractive for a potentially
mal length) as least attractive, followed by the long-term relationship. Thus 163 and 148
original and largest images, with the interme- men, respectively, rated the 0.6 WHR image
diate (22% and 33%) lengths yielding the as most attractive (Fig. 4), compared to the
highest ratings (Fig. 3). However, none of the total of 52 expected by chance (w2 test P <
images was rated as being moderately attrac- 0.001). The second most favored choice was
tive (score 3) on average. The highest rat- the 0.7 WHR image, signicantly so in the

American Journal of Human Biology DOI 10.1002/ajhb


92 B.J. DIXSON ET AL.

Fig. 3. Womens preferences for images of male g-


ures varying only in length of (nonerect) penis (15 in
order of increasing size). Data are means ( SEM).
***P < 0.001. [Color gure can be viewed in the online
issue, which is available at www.interscience.wiley.
com.]

case of a prospective long-term relationship


(P 0.001). The high WHR images (0.9 and
1.0) were consistently rated as less attractive
than expected (P < 0.001), as was the
extremely low WHR (0.5) image (P < 0.001 in
Fig. 4B). Ratings for the 0.8 WHR image did
not differ from those expected by chance.
Study 5: Mens ratings of female skin color
There was a striking effect of skin color on
mens ratings of attractiveness of the ve Fig. 4. Mens preferences for back-posed female g-
back-posed female images (Fig. 5). The two ures varying in WHR from 0.51.0. A: Subjects were
images with lighter-than-average skin colors asked to choose most sexually attractive image. B: Sub-
jects were asked to choose most attractive image for
were chosen as most attractive by 272 (87%) of potentially long-term relationship. Dashed line, ex-
the 311 men who lled out the questionnaire. pected number of males preferring a given image; histo-
By contrast, the two darker-than-average grams, observed scores. **P < 0.001, ***P < 0.001 (w2
female images were preferred by 15 (5%) of test). [Color gure can be viewed in the online issue,
which is available at www.interscience.wiley.com.]
the men, and the average image was chosen
by the remaining 24 (8%). The observed pref-
erences for the ve images differed signi- versity students); thus, only 4 of our subjects
cantly (P < 0.001) from those expected by were over 30 years of age, and only 7 of them
chance in every case (Fig. 5). were married. However, having acknowledged
DISCUSSION these limitations, our results provide some use-
ful comparisons and contrasts with the nd-
The studies reported here are, to our knowl- ings of similar studies conducted in the US,
edge, the rst of their kind conducted in Europe, and elsewhere.
China. It should be apparent that they refer to Chinese women rated images of average or
a limited sample of the population (631 uni- mesomorphic (i.e., muscular) masculine body

American Journal of Human Biology DOI 10.1002/ajhb


SEXUAL PREFERENCES IN CHINA 93

In this study, Chinese women showed a


clear preference for male images lacking hair
on the chest and abdomen. This result con-
trasts with that obtained in the UK, where
women rated hirsute male images as highly
attractive and also as being older than images
of men lacking trunk hair. Clearly it would be
incorrect to attribute a generalized function to
masculine trunk hair, as a signal of hormonal
(androgenic) status or reproductive maturity.
Very few human populations have been exam-
ined, and the expression of this trait probably
differs signicantly in different parts of the
world. Chinese men may be less hirsute than
men of some European populations, for exam-
ple, although it is exceedingly difcult to
obtain quantitative information on this ques-
tion.
Body depilation is also practiced by some
men, as reported by Boroughs et al. (2005) for
a sample 118 male university students in the
Fig. 5. Mens preferences for back-posed female g- US (Florida). Over half (63.61%) of these men
ures varying in skin color. Average, a gure whose color regularly removed body hair, especially from
is typical of Chinese women. Two gures were lighter in the groin, chest, and abdomen (Boroughs
color, and two gures were darker than average.
Dashed line, expected number of males preferring a et al., 2005).
given image; histograms, observed scores. ***P < 0.001 To our knowledge, no previous question-
(w2 test). [Color gure can be viewed in the online issue, naire study measured womens preferences for
which is available at www.interscience.wiley.com.]
male images which differ in genital morphol-
ogy. Many authors drew attention to the rela-
types as most attractive. Next preferred was tively large size of the human penis, and to
the slimly built (ectomorphic) male physique, the fact that it is more prominently displayed,
with the endomorphic (obese) male scoring due to the upright posture and bipedal gait of
lowest for attractiveness. These results are human beings (Short, 1980; Diamond, 1991,
similar to those obtained in the UK and Sri 1997; Jolly, 1999; Potts and Short, 1999; Miller,
Lanka (Dixson et al., 2003) which showed that 2000; Graslund, 2005). It was suggested that
women rate images of endomorphic males as penile size may inuence attractiveness of the
unattractive. The preferred (mesomorphic and human male, via visual as well as tactile cues
average) male physiques incorporate features, (e.g., Diamond, 1997; Jolly, 1999; Potts and
such as a broad chest and narrow waist, which Short, 1999; Miller, 2000). Our current nd-
were identied as attractive features for ings, from China, indicate that women rate
women in a number of studies (e.g., Mealey, the attractiveness of male images differently
1997; Schultze et al., 1991; Lynch and Zellner, if the length of the (nonerect) penis is altered.
1999; Maisey et al., 1999). Signals of mascu- The lowest ratings were for the image in which
line strength and health may have been penile length was reduced, but the greatest
selected for early in human evolution, because length increase (43%) was rated as less
the possessors of such traits offered tangible attractive than smaller increases (22% and
benets to women. Men who were better able 33%; Fig. 5). The comparatively low scores
to protect their female partners (and off- given by Chinese women may reect a cultural
spring), to succeed in physical contests with or experiential effect (e.g., ambivalence to view-
other males, and to undertake rigorous activ- ing such images or lack of sexual experience).
ities (such as endurance hunting) may have In studies conducted in Cameroon and the US
experienced signicantly greater reproductive (California), we noted that women gave higher
success (Buss, 2003). By contrast, obese or ratings, although the overall pattern was the
very slim men may have less stamina and same; the lowest ratings were for extremes
may be less healthy than those having a more (small or large) of penile length (Dixson et al.,
athletic physique (Katzmarzyk et al., 1998; unpublished data). The mechanisms controlling
Bolunchuk et al., 2000). these choices are currently unknown. It might

American Journal of Human Biology DOI 10.1002/ajhb


94 B.J. DIXSON ET AL.

be argued, for example, that images showing a tiveness. The results of the current study sup-
longer penis are more attractive because they port the notion that lower WHRs are more
signal some degree of sexual arousal. However, attractive to Chinese males than images with
it would be difcult to explain the lessened a high (0.9 or 1.0) WHR. Whether WHR alone
attractiveness of the largest image on that ba- or BMI is the critical variable in making such
sis. The women viewed all images simultane- judgments remains to be determined.
ously (arranged in random order). It is possible The evolution of lighter skin color in women,
that viewing and rating each image separately as compared to men, may have been inuenced
might have inuenced their results. Setting by sexual selection (Darwin, 1871). Changes in
aside cultural and methodological speculations, pigmentation (e.g., affecting the skin of the face
it remains the case that very little is known and abdomen) may occur in women as they
about how important women judge penis size to mature and pass through successive pregnan-
be. In a recent study conducted in Croatia, cies (Wong and Ellis, 1984; Sodhi and Sausker,
Stulhofer (2006) reported that, in a sample of 1988). Male preferences for lighter skin tones
556 sexually experienced women, 75% rated might be selected for because skin color pro-
penile length or girth as being either vides cues concerning female age and reproduc-
somewhat important or very important for tive potential (Symons, 1995). In the current
their judgment of partners. We believe that it is study, Chinese males expressed marked prefer-
time to set prudery aside and conduct more ences for images of women with the lightest skin
research on this topic. tones. Ethnographic comparisons conrm that
Men in our study population showed pro- such preferences exist in various human popula-
nounced preferences for back-posed female tions (van den Berghe and Frost, 1986; Frost,
images, dependent on their WHR or skin tone. 1988). Our ndings support this conclusion.
A WHR of 0.6 was judged as most attractive, However, it is important to extend such studies
with 0.7 as the next most favored image, espe- to a wide range of human populations, because
cially when judging attractiveness for a poten- human skin pigmentation varies so greatly in
tially long-term relationship. By contrast, different parts of the world, and because cul-
images with a high WHR (0.9 or 1.0) were tural biases may also inuence judgments of
rated as signicantly less attractive by Chi- attractiveness where this trait is concerned.
nese men. Singh (1993, 2002) argued that a
low WHR (0.7) typically reects a healthy and LITERATURE CITED
fertile reproductive status in women, based on Bolunchuk WW, Siders WA, Lykken GI, Lukaski HC. 2000.
the distribution of subcutaneous and intra-ab- Association of dominant somatotype of men with body
dominal fat reserves. Support for this view structure, function during exercise, and nutritional
comes from studies which reported correla- assessment. Am J Hum Biol 12:167180.
Boroughs M, Cafri G, Thompson JK. 2005. Male body depi-
tions between female waist size, breast size, lation: prevalence and associated features of body hair
and levels of 17-b-estradiol in relation to removal. Sex Roles 52:637644.
reproductive health (Jasienska et al., 2004). Bramble DM, Lieberman DE. 2004. Endurance running
However, cross-cultural studies revealed male and the evolution of Homo. Nature 432:345352.
Buss DM. 2003. The evolution of desire, 2nd ed. New York:
preferences for a higher WHR in some cases Basic Books.
(e.g., among the Matsigenka of Peru: Wu and Darwin C. 1871. The descent of man and selection in rela-
Shepard, 1998; and the Hadza of Tanzania: tion to sex. London: John Murray.
Wetsman and Marlowe, 1998). A more recent Diamond J. 1991. The rise and fall of the third chimpanzee.
London: Vintage.
study of the Hadza by Marlowe et al. (2005) Diamond J. 1997. Why is sex fun? The evolution of human
showed that preferences for higher WHR sexuality. London: Weidenfeld and Nicholson.
diminished when men were asked to rate side- Dixson AF. 1998. Primate sexuality: comparative studies of
posed images which varied in female buttock the prosimians, monkeys, apes and human beings.
Oxford: Oxford University Press.
size. Whether WHR or body mass index (BMI: Dixson AF, Halliwell G, East R, Wignarajah P, Anderson
weight scaled for height) is the most impor- MJ. 2003. Masculine somatotype and hirsuteness as
tant factor inuencing mens ratings of female determinants of sexual attractiveness to women. Arch
images remains a controversial issue (Tassi- Sex Behav 32:2939.
Frost P. 1988. Human skin color: a possible relationship
nary and Hansen, 1998; Manning et al., 1999; between its sexual dimorphism and its social perception.
Tovee et al., 1999, 2001; Henns, 2000; Singh, Perspect Biol Med 32:3858.
2002; Streeter and McBurney, 2003). Because Graslund B. 2005. Early humans and their world. New
York: Routledge.
WHR and BMI tend to be positively corre- Henns R. 2000. WHR and female attractiveness. Evidence
lated, it has proven difcult to tease apart the from photographic stimuli and methodological considera-
two variables in judgments of female attrac- tions. Perspect Indiv Diff 28:501513.

American Journal of Human Biology DOI 10.1002/ajhb


SEXUAL PREFERENCES IN CHINA 95
Jasienska G, Ziomkiewicz A, Ellison PT, Lipson SF, Thune Short RV. 1980. The origins of human sexuality. In: Repro-
I. 2004. Large breasts and narrow waists indicate high duction in mammals, volume 8. Austin CR, Short RV,
reproductive potential in women. Proc R Soc Lond [Biol] editors. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. p 133.
271:12131217. Singh D. 1993. Body shape and womens attractiveness.
Jolly A. 1999. Lucys legacy: sex and intelligence in human Hum Nat 4:297321.
evolution. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Singh D. 2002. Female mate value at a glance: relationship
Katzmarzyk PT, Malina RM, Song TMK, Bouchard C. 1998. of waist-to-hip ratio to health fecundity and attractive-
Somatotype and indicators of metabolic tness in youth. ness. Neuroendocrinol Lett [Suppl] 23:8191.
Am J Hum Biol 10:341350. Singh D, Young RK. 1995. Body weight, waist-to-hip ratio,
Law-Smith MJ, Perret DI, Jones BC, Cornwell RE, Moore breasts, and hips: role in judgements of female attrac-
FR, Feinberg DR, Boothroyd LG, Durrani SJ, Stirrat MJ, tiveness and desirability for relationships. Ethol Socio-
Whiten S, Pitman RM, Hillier SG. 2006. Facial appear- biol 16:483507.
ance is a cue to oestrogen levels in women. Proc R Soc Sodhi VK, Sausker WF. 1988. Dermatoses of pregnancy.
Lond [Biol] 273:135140. Am Fam Physician 37:131138.
Lynch SM, Zellner DA. 1999. Figure preference in two Streeter SA, McBurney DH. 2003. Waist-to-hip ratio and
generations of men: the use of gure drawings illus- attractiveness: new evidence and a critique of a critical
trating differences in muscle mass. Sex Roles 40:833 test. Evol Hum Behav 24:8898.
843. Stulhofer A. 2006. How (un)important is penis size for
Maisey DS, Vale ELE, Cornellisen PL, Tovee MJ. 1999. women with heterosexual experience? Arch Sex Behav
Characteristics of male attractiveness for women. Lancet 35:56.
353:1500. Symons D. 1995. Beauty is in the adaptions of the beholder:
Manning JT, Trivers RL, Singh D, Thornhill R. 1999. The the evolutionary psychology of human female sexual
mystery of female beauty. Nature 399:214215. attractiveness. In: Sexual nature, sexual conict. Abram-
Marlowe FW, Apicella CL, Reed D. 2005. Mens preferences son PR, Pinkerton SD, editors. Chicago: Chicago Univer-
for womens prole waist to-hip ratio in two societies. sity Press. p 80118.
Evol Hum Behav 25:371378. Tassinary LG, Hansen KA. 1998. A critical test of the WHR
Mealey L. 1997. Bulking up: the roles of sex and sexual ori- hypothesis of female sexual attractiveness. Psychol Sci
entation as attempts to manipulate sexual attractive- 9:150155.
ness. J Sex Res 34:223228. Tovee MJ, Maisey DS, Emery JL, Cornellisen PL. 1999. Vis-
Miller GF. 2000. The mating mind: how sexual choice ual cues to female sexual attractiveness. Proc R Soc Lond
shaped the evolution of human nature. New York: Dou- [Biol] 266:211218.
bleday. Tovee MJ, Brown JE, Jacobs D. 2001. Maternal waist-to-
Pawlowski B, Dunbar RIM, Lipowicz A. 2000. Tall men hip ratio does not predict child gender. Proc R Soc Lond
have more reproductive success. Nature 403:156. [Biol] 268:10071010.
Potts P, Short RV. 1999. Ever since Adam and Eve. The evo- van der Berghe PL, Frost P. 1986. Skin color preference,
lution of human sexuality. Cambridge: Cambridge Uni- sexual dimorphism and sexual selection: a case of gene
versity Press. culture co-evolution? Ethnic Rac Stud 9:87113.
Schultze S, Knussman R, Christiansen K. 1991. Male sex Wetsman A, Marlowe FW. 1998. How universal are prefer-
role identication and body build. Homo 42:203215. ences for female waist-to-hip ratios? Evidence from the
Schutzwohl A. 2006. Judging female gures: a new meth- Hadza from Tanzania. Evol Hum Behav 20:228229.
odological approach to male attractiveness of female Wong R, Ellis CN. 1984. Physiologic skin changes in preg-
waist-to-hip ratio. Biol Psychol 71:223229. nancy. J Am Acad Dermatol 10:929943.
Sheldon WH, Dupertuis CW, McDermott E. 1954. Atlas of Yu W, Shepard GH. 1998. Is beauty in the eye of the
men. New York: Harpers. beholder? Nature 396:321322.

American Journal of Human Biology DOI 10.1002/ajhb

Você também pode gostar