Você está na página 1de 16

See

discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.net/publication/24098742

A Consumer Values Orientation for


Materialism and Its Measurement

Article in Journal of Consumer Research February 1992


DOI: 10.1086/209304 Source: RePEc

CITATIONS READS

1,060 10,872

2 authors, including:

Marsha L. Richins
University of Missouri
51 PUBLICATIONS 7,311 CITATIONS

SEE PROFILE

All content following this page was uploaded by Marsha L. Richins on 17 March 2015.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file. All in-text references underlined in blue are added to the original document
and are linked to publications on ResearchGate, letting you access and read them immediately.
A Consumer Values Orientation for
Materialism and Its Measurement: Scale
Development and Validation
MARSHA L. RICHINS
SCOTT DAWSON*

This article reviews the construct and measurement of materialism and concludes
that materialism is appropriately conceptualized as a consumer value. The devel-
opment of a values-oriented materialism scale with three componentsacquisition
centrality, acquisition as the pursuit of happiness, and possession-defined success
is described. In validation tests high scorers (compared with low scorers) desired a
higher level of income, placed greater emphasis on financial security and less on
interpersonal relationships, preferred to spend more on themselves and less on
others, engaged in fewer voluntary simplicity behaviors, and were less satisfied with
their lives.

A merica is a consumer society, and many treatises


have described the dominance of consumption
motives among Americans (see, e.g., Bredemeier and
would be too great (Gaines 1990). Malcolm Forbes was
well known for his commitment to a lavish material
life-style (Hirschman 1990), and Leona Helmsley com-
Toby 1960; deTocqueville [1835] 1954;Wachtel 1983). mitted crimes to increase her wealth. Others, however,
In such discussions of consumption and materialism, are content with far less in life, choosing low-paying
authors tend to speak of consumers as an undifferen- charitable or social service work instead of higher-pay-
tiated group, acting individually, perhaps, but guided ing professions (Henkoff 1989). Some relinquish all
equally by the same consuming desire for goods. Hence, personal possessions to enter religious life or join a
Cushman (1990, p. 600) describes the post-World War commune.
II consumer as "yearn[ing] to acquire and consume," Although it is often useful to treat materialism as a
Fox and Lears (1983, p. xii) see Americans as engaged cultural or structural variable for purposes of comparing
in "a ceaseless pursuit of the 'good life' " through con- cultures or examining institutions within a culture of
sumption, and Fromm (1967, p. 179) notes that "con- consumption (e.g., Inglehart 1981), much is to be gained
temporary man has an unlimited hunger for more and by examining individual differences in materialism as
more goods." well. For instance, research that seeks to identify factors
While consumer goods do play an important role in that contribute to individual materialism may provide
American culture, these analyses obscure differences insight into the roots of materialism at a cultural level.
among individuals. For some, possessions are essential Examining materialism at the individual level also per-
to their lives and identities. For example. Lisa Labnon, mits the study of interactions between materialism and
a 30-year-old New Hampshire woman, became home- various marketing activities such as advertising. Finally,
less when she lost her job and her condominium was many of the hypotheses about materialism advanced in
repossessed. She refused to sell her Mercedes and mink the literature are more easily tested at an individual
coat, however, because the loss in image and self-esteem than a cultural level.
This article describes a scale to measure materialism
among individuals. Prior to reporting scale develop-
*Marsha L. Richins is associate professor of marketing, University ment, the notion of materialism is elaborated and prior
of Missouri, Columbia, MO 65211. Scott Dawson is associate pro- attempts to measure this construct are reviewed.
fessor of marketing at Portland State University, Portland, OR 97207.
The authors thank Russell Belk for his helpful comments at various
stages of the research and on a previous draft of this article, Terri
Root-Shaffer for her contributions in early stages of the project, and
The Notion of Materialism
Todd Mooradian for his assistance in data collection. This research The terms "materialism" and "materialistic" are used
was completed while the first author was at the University of Mas-
sachusetts. freely in ordinary conversation and by writers, fre-
quently without definition. Materialism originally re-
303
1992 by JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH. Inc. Vol. 19 December 1992
All rightsreserved.0O93-530l/93/19O3-OO0l$2.0O
304 JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH

ferred to the philosophical notion that nothing exists comes a fever that consumes all the potential energy it
except matter and its movements (see, e.g., Lange [1865] can get access to."
1925). In popular usage materialism more often refers
to a "devotion to material needs and desires, to the Acquisition as the Pursuit of Happiness. One ofthe
neglect of spiritual matters; a way of life, opinion, or reasons that possessions and their acquisition are so
tendency based entirely upon material interests," as de- central to materialists is that they view these as essential
fined in the Oxford English Dictionary. Rassuli and to their satisfaction and well-being in life. Belk (1984,
Hollander (1986, p. 10) describe materialism as "a p. 291) notes that "at the highest levels of materialism
m i n d - s e t . . . an interest in getting and spending," and . . . possessions assume a central place in a person's
Belk (1984, p. 291) defines it as "the importance a con- life and are believed to provide the greatest sources of
sumer attaches to worldly possessions." Mukerji (1983, satisfaction and dissatisfaction." In a like manner. Ward
p. 8), relying on Polanyi (1944) and Sahlins (1976), re- and Wackman (1971, p. 426) describe materialism as
fers to materialism as "a cultural system in which ma- "an orientation emphasizing possessions and money
terial interests are not made subservient to other social for personal happiness and social progress." The view-
goals" and material self-interest is preeminent. When point that pleasure or self-satisfaction is the goal of high
a large portion of a society avidly desires to consume consumption levels is frequently presented in the lit-
goods for reasons that economists have traditionally erature (e.g., Campbell 1987; Heilbroner 1956; Wachtel
defined as nonutilitarian (e.g., status seeking, novelty), 1983). While most individuals are probably involved
a "consumer culture" is said to exist (e.g., Belk 1988; to some extent in the pursuit of happiness, it is the pur-
Rassuli and Hollander 1986). suit of happiness through acquisition rather than
through other means (such as personal relationships,
Many issues concerning materialism have been con- experiences, or achievements) that distinguishes ma-
sidered in the literature, including causes and conse- terialism.
quences of materialism, the behaviors and personality
characteristics of materialists, and moral considerations Possession-defined Success. Materialists tend to judge
(see Belk 1983 and Fournier and Richins 1991 for de- their own and others' success by the number and quality
scriptions of some of these themes). The following dis- of possessions accumulated. Rassuli and Hollander
cussion describes only those themes that have consis- (1986, p. 5) describe members of a consumer society as
tently appeared when theorists have defined materialism evaluating others and themselves in terms of their con-
itself (as opposed to describing related issues such as suming life-styles, and in Heilbroner's (1956, p. 23)
the personal or moral consequences of materialism).' analysis, acquisitive, materialistic people value posses-
Acquisition Centrality. Materialists place possessions sions "for the money they cost rather than by the sat-
and their acquisition at the center of their lives. Daun isfactions they yield." Du Bois (1955) and others have
(1983) describes materialism as a life-style in which a noted that materialists consider material well-being as
high level of material consumption functions as a goal evidence of success and proof of right-mindedness, an
and serves as a set of plans. Materialism thus lends attitude consistent with religious teachings at the time
meaning to life and provides an aim for daily endeavors. ofthe Industrial Revolution (Weber [1930] 1958). The
According to Bredemeier and Toby (1960), materialists value of possessions stems not only from their ability
worship things, and the pursuit of possessions takes the to confer status (Veblen [1899] 1953) but from their
place of religion in structuring their lives and orienting ability to project a desired self-image and identify one
their behaviors. Csikszentmihalyi and Rochberg-Halton as a participant in an imagined perfect life (Campbell
(1981, p. 231) note the dominance materialism can 1987). Materialists view themselves as successful to the
achieve in one's life when they say of some materialists extent they can possess products that project these de-
that "consumption for the sake of consumption be- sired images.
The notions concerning materialism expressed in
theoretical writings are also held by lay persons. Four-
'The following discussion relies on the materialism literature in a
variety of disciplines. Because the characterizations of materialism nier and Richins (1991) compared popular and theo-
in the literature are predominantly unfavorable, this discussion is retical notions of materialism and found that the three
necessarily weighted toward the negative. However, it is not the au- themes described above were strongly represented in
thors' view that all aspects of materialism are inevitably bad. For ordinary consumers' conceptions of materialism.
instance, the desire for goods on the part of employees may cause
them to work harder or longer, enhancing their incomes and standard
of living. High levels of consumption by consumers can increase the Instrumental and Terminal Materialism
wealth of business institutions, increasing these firms' ability to make
capital improvements and invest in research and development, which Many religious and social critics have condemned
in turn leads to greater productivity, technological breakthroughs, materialism as inherently bad. Rochberg-Halton (1986;
and again, higher living standards. In addition, while the literature
assigns to materialists a number of characteristics that are viewed Csikszentmihalyi and Rochberg-Halton 1978, 1981),
negatively in contemporary Western society, its bias leads it to be however, pointed out that possessions can be a positive
silent on the potential positive characteristics of materialists such as influence in one's life and proposed two forms of ma-
a strong motivation to succeed and self-sufficiency. terialism based on the purposes of consumption.
MATERIALISM AND ITS MEASUREMENT 305

When objects act "as essential means for discovering minal materialism (Rochberg-Halton 1986, pp. 181,
and furthering personal values and goals of life," the 183-184).
materialism is a potentially harmless form labeled Given the difficulties of operationalizing the instru-
"instrumental materialism" (Csikszentmihalyi and mental/terminal forms and the value judgments inher-
Rochberg-Halton 1978, p. 8). When consumption fur- ent in Rochberg-Halton's conception of "good" and
thers no goal beyond possession itself, the materialism "bad" materialism, the distinction between instrumen-
is regarded as a more dangerous form labeled tal and terminal materialism will not be maintained for
"terminal." our purposes.
Unfortunately, Rochberg-Halton's dichotomy is dif-
ficult to use, and his analysis contains contradictions. MEASURES OF MATERIALISM AND
For instance, Csikszentmihalyi and Rochberg-Halton RELATED CONSTRUCTS
(1978, p. 8) note that, in terminal materialism, "we
reduce our ultimate goals to the possession of things," Empirical research on materialism has been scant
yet the authors often mention goals of terminal mate- until very recently; however, a variety of materialism
rialism beyond possession. They note that, in terminal measures have occasionally been mentioned in the lit-
materialism, people use possessions to generate the envy erature. With the exception of Belk's (1984) work, none
and admiration of others or to achieve status. This sug- of the measures appear to have involved application of
gests that these states (being envied, having status) rather commonly accepted standards for scale development
than the acquisition of possessions constitute the ulti- (Churchill 1979; Nunnally 1978). As practiced in prior
mate goals of terminal materialism. research, approaches to measuring materialism can be
divided into two types: those that infer materialism from
In addition, the analysis of instrumental and terminal
measures of related constructs and those that purport
materialism is incomplete and the constructs difficult to measure materialism more directly through the use
to operationalize. For example, it is unclear whether of attitude scales. Table 1 describes the measures in
the terms instrumental and terminal materialism refer each category.
to an individual difference variable (in which case in-
dividuals might be classified as primarily terminal or Materialism has often been assessed by measuring
instrumental in orientation) or serve simply as descrip- related constructs and using this to infer the level of
tions of specific behaviors or motives. Furthermore, materialism. Dickins and Ferguson (1957), for instance,
there is little guidance in making determinations of in- assessed materialism by the kinds of wishes expressed
strumental versus terminal materialism. Instrumental by children and the kinds of jobs they desire when they
materialism is described as possessing "a sense of di- grow up.
rectionality, in which a person's goals themselves may A few authors have inferred the presence of materi-
be cultivated through transactions with the object," its alism from scores on early personality-test batteries
purpose is "the fuller unfolding of human life," and it (e.g., Burdsal 1975; Justice and Birkman 1972). More
is "context-related." In terminal materialism, "there is recently Belk (1984, 1985) has developed personality-
no sense of reciprocal interaction in the relation between trait measures specifically designed to infer the presence
the object and the goal" (Csikszentmihalyi and Roch- of materialism. His work has been more fruitful than
berg-Halton 1981, p. 231). Except in extreme cases, it earlier approaches because he examined the theoretical
linkages between specific personality traits (envy, pos-
may be difficult to determine whether the conditions
sessiveness, and nongenerosity) and materialism and
for instrumental materialism are being met. used psychometric principles to develop his measures.
Essentially, the classification as instrumental or ter- A limitation of the Belk scales has been inconsistent
minal appears to rest on a value judgment. Instrumental and often low reports of scale reliability. In 12 separate
materialism "involves the cultivation of objects as es- data collections in which reliability was reported in the
sential means for discovering and furthering goals" literature, coefficient alpha for the individual person-
(Csikszentmihalyi and Rochberg-Halton 1981, p. 231), ality scales ranged from .09 to .81 with a median reli-
but only certain kinds of "acceptable" goals are deemed ability of .54; a measure summed across the three scales,
instrumental. Through his use of examples, it appears frequently used as an indicator of materialism, had a
that Rochberg-Halton considers a relationship with an median reliability of .62.
object to be instrumental if it involves self-actualization, The most sustained effort to measure materialism
the development of stronger family or friendship ties, has been carried out by Inglehart (e.g., 1981), who has
or the development and expression of aspects of the self attempted to identify postmaterialistic societies in
that he approves of. In his analysis, valuing a tool that which individuals emphasize such values as belonging
allows one to build model planes and fly them in com- and self-expression instead of material possessions. In
petitions involves instrumental materialism. Owning his surveys, administered primarily in Europe, he lists
an expensive car to impress others and feel better about 12 goals and classifies respondents as possessing ma-
one's self or buying a second home to spend more terialist or postmaterialist values by the social goals
time with one's mistress and her child represent ter- they choose as most important. A problem with this
306 JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH

TABLE 1
MEASURES OF MATERIALISM REPORTED IN EARLIER STUDIES

Study" Subjects How measured'' Reliability

I. Measures that infer materialism


from related constructs:
Dickins and Ferguson (1957) Ohiidren aged 7-8 and Content analysis of responses to five open-ended
11-12 questions: "If you could make three vi/ishes and
they would all come true, what would you wish
for?"
Justice and Birkman (1972) Employed adults, prison Subscale of the Birkman vocational interest and
inmates attitude survey; materialism inferred from true-
false questions concerning social perceptions
and self-image
Bengston and Lovejoy (1973) Three-generation families Materialism/humanism factor scores based on .78
rankings of 16 values: "finances,"
"possessions," "service"
Burdsal(1975) College students, military Materialistic motivations inferred from factor scores
personnel on Cattell's motivational analysis test
Jackson, Ahmed, and Heapy
(1976) Adults and college Acquisitiveness subscale of the six-dimensional Approximately .80
students in several achievement scale; includes Likert scale,
cultures semantic differential, and adjective checklist
items
lngiehart(1981) Adults in Europe and the Materialist and postmaterialist goals; 12 goals
United States ranked by importance: "maintain a stable
economy," "try to make our cities and
countryside more beautiful"
Belk(1984) College students, adults Personality traits of envy, nongenerosity, and Subscales .09-.81;
possessiveness; 24 Likert scale items: "I am entire .48-.73
bothered when I see people who buy anything
they want," "I don't like to lend things, even to
good friends," "I tend to hang on to things I
should probably throw out"
II. Attitude measures of
materialism:
Campbell (1969) College students, adults Materialism; eight items, forced-choice format: "If
things were such that everybody in the world had
stereophonic record players and champagne,
wars would probably be obsolete"
Wackman, Reale, and Ward
(1972) Adolescents Materialism; 5 items, Likert scale format: "It's really
true that money can buy happiness"
iVIoschis and Churchill (1978) Adolescents Materialism; 6 items, adaptation of Wackman et al. 53- 71
(1972)
De Young (1985-1986) Adults Nonmaterialism; four items with five-point scales: .78
"do not evaluate everything in dollars," "get
more pleasure from the non-material"
Richins(1987) Materialism; six items, two subscales, Likert scale .73, .61
Adults
format: "It is important to me to have really nice
things"
Heslin, Johnson, and Blake
(1989)"= Materialism subscale of the spender scales; six .76''
Students
items, Likert scale format

NOTE.Ellipses indicate that data are unavailable.


Where a scale has been used in more than one study, the source with the greatest amount of scale information is reported.
"Entry includes a description of the scale followed by sample items.
"Scale development is in progress.
"Measure is a test-retest correlation; all other reliabilities are Cronbach's alpha.

materialism measure for consumer-behavior research influences on day-to-day consumption choices. In ad-
is that the goals Inglehart lists are distant from most dition, this approach does not directly measure the
consumers' daily concerns, cannot be easily affected complex, multidimensional nature of materialism and,
by individual action, and are not likely to have large because of its ordinal level of measurement, does not
MATERIALISM AND ITS MEASUREMENT 307

assess individual differences in the strength of material istence" and, further, that a value "has a transcendental
values. quality to it, guiding actions, attitudes, judgments, and
Finally, some authors have used attitude measures comparisons across specific objects and situations and
that assume a more direct assessment of materialism. beyond immediate goals to more ultimate goals" (p.
These measures usually involve Likert scale response 18). Recall that materialistic consumers are said to make
formats and are described in Table 1. With the excep- a religion out of things (Bredemeier and Toby 1960),
tion of the materialism component of the spender scales they believe that possession of things is the ultimate
(Heslin, Johnson, and Blake 1989), still in the process source of happiness (Belk 1984), and materialism or-
of development, none of these measures has been rig- ganizes their lives to such an extent that it creates a life-
orously tested. style (Daun 1983). These descriptions fit Rokeach's and
others' characterizations of values. Defining material-
ism as a value is consistent with the notion that mate-
MATERIALISM AS A CONSUMER rialism reflects the importance a person places on pos-
VALUE sessions and their acquisition as a necessary or desirable
form of conduct to reach desired end states, including
The preceding review shows that materialism has happiness. (For more discussion of the value conception
been measured in a variety of waysby measuring per- of materialism, see Fournier and Richins 1991; Richins
sonality traits, by examining the importance of various and Dawson 1990.) Our conception of materialism,
social goals, and by assessing attitudes. All the existing then, is that it is a value that guides people's choices
measures seem to suffer from at least one of two im- and conduct in a variety of situations, including, but
portant limitations. First, many of the measures do not not limited to, consumption arenas. With respect to
possess adequate levels of reliability for use in anything consumption, materialism will influence the type and
except exploratory research. This is not surprising, per- quantity of goods purchased. Beyond consumption,
haps, given the difficulty of measuring a complex con- materialism will influence the allocation of a variety of
struct like materialism. resources, including time. A materialist, for instance,
Second, the construct validity of many of the mea- might choose to work longer hours and earn more
sures has not been established. Because none of the money instead of using that time for leisure activities.
measures except Belk's have involved the psychometric
procedures of construct definition, scale refinement, and All this suggests that those who place a high value on
validity assessment, they are of limited usefulness. material possessions and their acquisition will behave
Measures that infer materialism from scores on other differently from those who place a lower value on things.
variables such as personality traits or social goals require To the extent behavior patterns associated with mate-
further scrutiny. Peter (1981, p. 134) notes that a valid rialism arefixedand pervasive, there may be personality
construct measure should assess "only the character- traits associated with materialism (see, e.g., Fromm
istics of the construct it is purported to assess" and 1976). Belk's (1984) measures reflect some of the traits
should not be "contaminated with elements from the most commonly said to be associated with materialism.
domain of other constructs" (see also Nunnally 1978). A number of writers have adopted a value conception
Thus, using measures of personality traits to infer ma- of materialism. This is clearest among those who have
terialism may be inappropriate unless materialism is dealt with materialism at a cultural level. Mukerji
itself viewed as a personality trait. (1983), Fox and Lears (1983), and others describe ma-
To determine the appropriate measurement approach terialism and the culture of consumption as a value
for materialism, it is necessary to examine the nature system. Content-analytic studies designed to identify
of the construct itself. As suggested in the literature re- trends in materialism as reflected in advertising and
view, theoretical and popular notions indicate that ma- popular literature (e.g., Belk 1987; Spiggle 1986) have
terialism represents a mind-set or constellation of at- spoken of "material values," and consumer behavior
titudes regarding the relative importance of acquisition texts frequently describe materialism as a dominant
and possession of objects in one's life. For materialists, American value (e.g., Engel, Blackwell, and Miniard
possessions and their acquisition are at the forefront of 1990; Hawkins, Best, and Coney 1989; Mowen 1990).
personal goals that dictate "ways of life." They value In consumer behavior, values most frequently have
possessions and their acquisition more highly than most been measured with ranking scales such as those de-
other matters and activities in life. The organizing veloped by Rokeach (1973) and Kahle (see Kahle,
function of acquisition goals among materialists, the Beatty, and Homer 1986) in which respondents are pre-
centrality of acquisition-related activities to their lives, sented with a (sometimes large) set of end states or be-
and the prioritizing of possessions vis-a-vis other things haviors and asked to rank them according to their im-
in life suggests that materialism is a value. portance. Although this approach is useful in identifying
Rokeach (1973, p. 5) defined a value as "an enduring value configurations by revealing the relative impor-
belief that a specific mode of conduct or end-state of tance an individual or a cultural group places on various
existence is personally or socially preferable to an op- behaviors or end states, ranking methods are limited in
posite or converse mode of conduct or end-state of ex- several ways. First, the information gained about any
308 JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH

one particular value is very superficial. To know that emphasis on possessions is the essence of materialism
an individual ranks "warm relationships with others" (e.g., Belk 1984; Bredemeier and Toby 1960; Mukerji
fourth in a given set does not reveal very much about 1983).
the role these warm relationships play in the person's
life. We do not know why relationships are valued or Proposition 2. Materialistic people are self-centered.
the benefits that are expected to accrue from these re- Many have noted that an overemphasis on material
lationships. Ranking methods also make comparison possessions results in selfishness, and Belk (1983) has
across individuals impossible. Of two individuals who reviewed religious and other writings that espouse this
have ranked "warm relationships" fourth, we cannot view. An overriding concern with possessions and ac-
tell which one considers warm relationships more im- quisition for oneself is inherently incompatible with
portant, nor can we tell whether someone ranking warm sharing and giving to others. Wachtel (1983) has ob-
relationships second actually considers them more im- served that self-interest and the pursuit of individual
portant in an absolute sense than someone ranking them rather than community goals predominate where afflu-
third or fourth. ence and acquisition are emphasized.
There are practical problems with ranking methods Proposition 3. Materialists will pursue a life of ma-
as well. Ranked data are ipsative, and the analysis of terial complexity rather than material simplicity. An
ipsative data is fraught with problems (Hicks 1970). In emphasis on material possessions is often linked with
addition, respondents find it difficult to rank large positive attitudes toward growth (e.g., Heilbroner 1956;
numbers of items such as those found on the Rokeach Inglehart 1981; Looft 1971), a reliance on technology
value batteries. Decreasing the number of items, as with to solve problems (e.g., Mukerji 1983), and an uncon-
Kahle et al.'s (1986) approach, can result in the over- cern for the things of nature or the environment (e.g.,
simplification of complex psychological phenomena. Lasch 1978; Linden 1979). Voluntary simplicity is the
Rating methods are sometimes used to avoid the prob- opposite perspective. It is a life-style of moral respon-
lems of ranked-values data (Alwin and Krosnick 1985), sibility, spiritual growth, and self-actualization that is
but rating has its own problems, including a large num- manifested in the economic behaviors of low con-
ber of ties among valued end states and behaviors. Rat- sumption, ecological responsibility, and self-sufficiency
ing data suffers equally with ranking in terms of super- (Elgin 1981; Shama and Wisenblit 1984). These char-
ficiality of measurement. acteristics are so contrary to the spirit of materialism
To avoid the problems inherent in ranking and rating that Rudmin and Kilbourne (1992) have described vol-
procedures, we took a different approach to measuring untary simplicity as "deliberately denied materialism."
materialism. We maintained the conceptualization of Thus, there should be a negative relationship between
materialism as a value but attempted to obtain greater materialism and voluntary-simplicity behaviors.
depth by measuring beliefs relevant to the value. Thus,
we considered materialism to be a set of centrally held Proposition 4. Materialists tend to be less satisfied
beliefs about the importance of possessions in one's life than others with their lot in life. Although materialists
(cf. Rokeach's definition of value) and measured the expect acquisition to make them happy, many writers
three belief domains described above: acquisition cen- have observed that the lust for goods can be insatiable;
trality, the role of acquisition in happiness, and the role the pleasures of a new acquisition are quickly forgotten
of possessions in defining success. Our final measure is and replaced with a desire for more. This cycle leads
not intended to assess all beliefs relevant to material inevitably to dissatisfaction and discontent (Brickman
values but rather those that emerged consistently from and Campbell 1971; Scitovszky 1976). Empirical tests
analysis of lay and theoretical notions as described using earlier measures of materialism support this hy-
above. pothesis (Belk 1984; Dawson 1988; Richins 1987).

Propositions Concerning Materialism SCALE DEVELOPMENT


The remainder of this article describes development Item Generation
of a materialism measure that meets the goals described
above. To partially assess the validity of the resulting Item generation for the materialism scale relied on
scale, it was used to test several propositions concerning both popular and theoretical notions of materialism.
materialism that have been discussed widely in the lit- In exploratory research, a convenience sample of 11
erature. adult consumers was asked to describe in an open-ended
format the attitudes and values of materialistic people
Proposition 1. Materialistic people value acquisi- they knew and of materialistic people in general. The
tion and the means to acquire possessions more highly sample included nearly equal numbers of males and
than those low in materialism. They also value posses- females and was spread across age and income cate-
sions and their acquisition more than other life goals gories. Frequently mentioned attitude descriptions were
and more than their relationships with other people converted into items. In addition, the researchers con-
(Fromm 1976; Schudson 1984). For most writers, this structed items to represent the three domains of ma-
MATERIALISM AND ITS MEASUREMENT 309

TABLE 2
DESCRIPTION OF CONSUMER DATA COLLECTIONS AND ASSOCIATED MEASURES

Response rate
Survey and location Final n Measures

1. Medium-Sized northeastern
city 36.0 144 48 materialism items; 10 social desirability items (Crowne
and Marlowe 1960)

2. Large western city 33.3 250 30 materialism items; trait scales (Belk 1984); spending items
(see text)

3. Large western city 31.3 235 30 materialism items; Rosenberg (1965) self-esteem scale;
desired income

4. Northeastern college town and


northeastern rural area 43.0 and 39.7 86 and 119 30 materialism items; 13 voluntary simplicity items (Leonard-
Barton 1981); list of values (Kahle et al. 1986); five life-
satisfaction items (Andrews and Withey 1976)

terialism described above. Another source of items was the first survey {n = 144). Thirty materialism items were
characterizations of materialistic people in the literature retained as a result of these analyses. Factor analysis
and those mentioned by social critics. Finally, a few and additional reliability assessments were performed
items were adapted from earlier studies in which ma- for these 30 items using data from the later data col-
terialism and related constructs were measured (Belk lections (all with sample sizes greater than 200; see Table
1984; Heslin et al. 1989; Richins 1987; Wackman, 2). These analyses resulted in a scale containing 18 items
Reale, and Ward 1972; Yamauchi and Templer 1982). that behaved consistently across the samples and possess
Items were cast to reflect values and attitudes about adequate reliability. The final set of items is shown in
possessions rather than specific behavior or personality Table 3.
traits. A Likert scale format was used for all items with
response categories of strongly agree, agree, neutral,
disagree, and strongly disagree. Structure of the Measure
During initial data collection efforts, more than 120 Exploratory factor analysis from the first survey sug-
items were generated. Redundant, ambiguous, leading, gested a scale with three moderately correlated factors.
and other faulty items were eliminated in initial screen- To show the relationships among scale items. Table 3
ing. Subsequent screening was based on empirical tests provides the pattern matrix from the principal com-
of reliability, validity, and social desirability bias. ponents analysis (with oblique rotation) with data from
survey 2. The matrix is typical of those obtained from
Item RefinementStudent Samples all data collections, and the three factors correspond to
the elements of materialism noted in construct defini-
Early data collections for item refinement were un- tion. The first factor (labeled "success") represents the
dertaken at three major universities in different parts use of possessions as an indicator of success in life,
of the country (the South, the Northeast, and the West). which corresponds to the third domain of materialism
Reliability, social desirability, and validity assessments described in the literature review. The second factor
based on student samples have been described elsewhere ("centrality") concerns the importance of acquisition
(Richins and Dawson 1990) and resulted in a pool of and possession generally, and the third ("happiness")
48 items that were retained for further analysis. concerns the perception that possessions are needed for
happiness. Confirmatory factor analysis was performed
Consumer Samples with the data from surveys 2, 3, and 4. Although the
chi-square statistics were significant in the three anal-
Additional item refinement and validation tests were yses, other indicators suggested an acceptable fit of the
carried out with more heterogeneous samples obtained model. Adjusted goodness-of-fit indices ranged from .86
through four consumer mail surveys. All involved ran- to .88, and in every analysis the i-values for maximum
domly chosen samples of households in which initial likelihood estimates all exceeded 5.0.
mailings were followed by a reminder letter and second
copy of the questionnaire mailed two weeks later. Fur- Because the latent constructs were moderately or
ther details of the data collections and associated ques- highly correlated in all analyses (phi coefficients
tionnaires are shown in Table 2. ranged from .39 to .79), the chi-square for a single
Exploratory factor analysis, reliability assessment, factor model was compared with chi-square for the
and social desirability tests were performed on data from three factor model. The difference in chi-square test
310 JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH

TABLE 3
Reliability
EXPLORATORY FACTOR ANAYLSIS OF MATERIALISM ITEMS
Coefficient alpha was calculated separately for the
Factor
items comprising the three factors and for the 18 items
as a single scale. The seven centrality items produced
Item 1 alpha coefficients between .71 and .75 in the latter three
surveys. For the six-item success subscale alpha ranged
Success: from .74 to .78, and for the five happiness items, alpha
I admire people who own expensive homes,
was between .73 and .83. When combined into a single
cars, and clothes. .70
Some of the most important achievements
scale, alpha for the 18 items varied between .80 to .88.
in life include acquiring material Test-retest reliability (three-week interval) was cal-
possessions. .69 culated on data from a sample of 58 students at an urban
I don't place much emphasis on the amount university. The reliability correlations were .82, .86, and
of material objects people own as a sign
of success.* .68
.82 for the centrality, happiness, and success subscales,
The things I own say a lot about how well respectively, and .87 for the combined scale.
I'm doing in life. .58
I like to own things that impress people. .56 Social Desirability
I don't pay much attention to the material
objects other people own* -.43 While materialism may be more socially acceptable
Centrality: today than in some past eras, because of recent media
I usually buy only the things I need.* -.78 attention to the negative aspects of materialism we con-
I try to keep my life simple, as far as
possessions are concerned.* -.62 sidered it important to test the measure for susceptibility
The things I own aren't all that important to to social desirability bias. Social desirability was mea-
me.* -.60 sured in the first consumer data collection with 10 items
I enjoy spending money on things that from the Marlowe-Crowne scale (Crowne and Marlowe
aren't practical. .60 1960). These items were chosen from the larger scale
Buying things gives me a lot of pleasure. .54
I like a lot of luxury in my life. .52
because they have been shown to possess greater sen-
I put less emphasis on material things than sitivity than other items and are appropriately keyed
most people I know.* -.49 for current standards of desirable behavior (Ballard,
Happiness: Crino, and Rubenfeld 1988). Correlations with the so-
I have all the things I really need to enjoy
life.* -.80
cial desirability measure were .12, .03, and .06 for
My life would be better if I owned certain the centrality, happiness, and success subscales, re-
things I don't have. .65 spectively, and -.09 for the combined scale. The low
I wouldn't be any happier if I owned nicer correlations suggest that social desirability bias was not
things.* -.58 a problem for these measures.
I'd be happier if I could afford to buy more
things. .58
It sometimes bothers me quite a bit that I Descriptive Statistics
can't afford to buy all the things I'd like. .37 .55
The distributions for the overall materialism measure
NOTE.Only loadings greater than .35 are shown. An asterisk indicates reverse and its three components were approximately normal
scored items. A five-point Likert scale response format was used.

construct and notes that, in doing so, researchers have assumed either
was significant in tests of all three data sets, indicating that the underlying construct is assessed indirectly by measures of its
that the three-factor model is superior in fitting the various manifestations (the latent variable approach) or that the con-
struct is something more than the sum of its component parts (the
data. synergistic approach). For purposes of the materialism measure, we
While confirmatory factor analysis served to explicate make the former assumptionthat the three subscales are manifes-
the three hypothesized manifestations of the underlying tations of materialism and the latent variable approach is thus ap-
construct, the three factors were summed for purposes propriate. Carver describes the advantages of summing the compo-
nents in such a case and discusses the patterns of results that justify
of validation. This approach was followed because summed vs. separate component analysis. In the research reported
analyses showed that the three factors normally act in here, all hypotheses were investigated using both the summed scale
concert with respect to external variables. Carver (1989) and the component scales. On average, the summed multidimensional
has noted that, in these situations, using the summed index relates to the diverse constructs in the hypothesis tests better
than does any one component dimension. In such cases, "the higher
index instead of subscales is appropriate and advanta- level information (i.e., the consistent relation of the multifaceted
geous in terms of parsimony and clarity of communi- [summed] construct to many outcome variables) is more important
cation.^ than the lower level [individual subscale] information," and the use
of the summed construct measure instead of individual subscales is
preferred (Carver 1989, p. 580). For this reason, summed scale results
^Carver (1989) has discussed at length the appropriateness of com- are presented here. Results of" hypothesis tests at the subscale level
bining component scores into a summed measure representing a latent are available from the authors.
MATERIALISM AND ITS MEASUREMENT 311

in all surveys (largest value for skew = .67; largest value TABLE 4
for kurtosis = 1.01). Means, standard deviations, and DESCRIPTIVE STATISTICS FOR THE MATERIALISM SCALE AND
ranges for surveys 2, 3, and 4 are presented in Table 4. ITS COMPONENTS
The relationships between demographics and the
materialism measures were assessed. With the exception Scale Mean SD Range Skew Kurtosis
of age, all correlations were quite low and no consistent
patterns emerged. For the four materialism measures Centrality component:
Survey 2 19.8 4.2 9-33 .20 .44
(three components plus the overall scale) in the three Survey 3 19.3 4.0 9-32 .12 -.13
later data collections, the median point biserial corre- Survey 4 19.3 4.0 9-31 -.07 .14
lations with sex and with marital status were .05 and Happiness component:
Survey 2 13.3 4.2 5-25 .14 -.60
.01, respectively. Median correlations for household
Survey 3 13.1 3.5 5-22 .23 -.24
size, education, and income were .02, -.06, and .04. Survey 4 12.8 4.1 5-25 .37 -.26
For age, however, all correlations except one were neg- Success component:
ative; the median correlation was -.19. This result is Survey 2 14.7 3.9 6-24 .06 -.38
consistent with the notion that materialism declines af- Survey 3 14.3 3.7 6-26 .52 .49
ter middle age (see Belk 1985). Survey 4 13.8 4.1 6-30 .67 1.01
Overall scale:
Survey 2 47.9 10.2 23-80 .16 .00
Survey 3 46.7 8.3 20-71 .08 .39
SCALE VALIDATION Survey 4 45.9 9.8 23-84 .45 .83

Earlier in the article, four propositions concerning


materialism were described. In validity assessment,
multiple tests were performed for each proposition. centages for respondents in the top ( = 71) and bottom
( = 68) terciles."*
As expected, respondents higher in materialism were
Materialists and the Value of Acquisition more likely to value "financial security" and less likely
to value "warm relationships with others" than respon-
According to theorists, materialistic people value ac- dents low in materialism {p < .01). They were also less
quisition and the means to acquire possessions more likely to choose "a sense of accomplishment" as an im-
highly than those low in materialism. In addition, they portant goal {p < .01).
value acquisition more than other life goals. Three Finally, the importance materialists place on financial
analyses were carried out to examine these ideas. matters relative to other goals was examined. Table 5
Respondents in survey 3 were asked in an open-ended shows the median ranking for values in the LOV scale.
format to indicate the level of annual household income For respondents low in materialism, four values were
that "would satisfy your needs." People who desire a rated as more important than "financial security": self-
lot of possessions will need more money to acquire those respect, warm relationships, family security, and a sense
possessions and thus are expected to report a higher of accomplishment. For those high in materialism, only
desired level of income. For purposes of analysis, re- self-respect and family security were rated as more im-
spondents were divided into terciles based on their ma- portant; warm relationships were approximately tied in
terialism scores; the desired income level of respondents importance with financial security. This analysis sup-
in the top {n = 76) and bottom ( = 71) terciles were ports the belief that materialists tend to value the means
compared. Respondents high in materialism felt they to acquire (financial security) more highly than some
needed significantly more income {X = $65,974) than of the life goals valued by those low in materialism.
those low in materialism [X = $44,761; t = 3.65, df However, the contentions of Fromm (1976) and others
= 120.1, p<.001).5 that materialists sacrifice personal relationships in their
A second test of the importance of the means to ac- pursuit of wealth and possessions were not supported.
quire was carried out using Kahle's List of Values (LOV) Respondents low in materialism do appear to place
scale (Kahle et al. 1986). Respondents in survey 4 were considerably more importance on interpersonal rela-
asked to read the nine values in a revised version of the tionships than on financial security, but high-materi-
scale and then to rank the four values that were most alism respondents gave interpersonal relationships
important to them. Respondents were divided into ter- equal footing with financial concerns.
ciles based on their materialism scores and the per- Two separate tests, one concerning values and the
centage of respondents including each value in their other concerning desired income, indicate that the ma-
top three choices was examined. Table 5 shows per- terialism scale effectively identifies consumers who

'The homogeneity of variance assumption was not met for this "Results are similar when comparing those who did and did not
test, necessitating the use of separate variance estimates and resulting include a value in their top/our choices except that the significance
in a noninteger value for degrees of freedom. level declines to .05 for significant comparisons.
312 JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH

TABLE 5
PERCENT INCLUDING A VALUE IN THEIR TOP THREE CHOICES AND MEDIAN RANKING
FOR RESPONDENTS HIGH AND LOW IN MATERIALISM

High materialism Low materialism


(n = 71) (n = 68)

Percent Percent
Goal choosing Median" choosing Median' z-tes
Financial security 46.5 4 22.1 5 3.02
Warm relationships with others 45.1 4 64.7 3 -2.32
Sense of accomplishment 22.5 5 45.6 4 -2.88
Self-respect 54.9 3 63.2 2 -.99
Famiiy security 56.3 3 50.0 3 .74
Seif-fulfiliment 35.2 5 27.9 5 .93
Fun and enjoyment In life 16.9 13.2 .61
Being weil-respected 12.7 7.4 1.04
Sense of belonging 9.9 5.9 .87

'Respondents ranked their four most important goals; unranked goals were assigned a rank of five; the median is not reported for qoals chosen bv less than 30
percent of respondents (indicated by ellipses). KM y OU
"p< .01.

highly value the means to acquire. However, because ity is more fully examined below.) The scale contains
the analysis of values with ranked data suffers from an item asking how often respondents contribute to
limitations mentioned earlier, future investigations into ecological or conservation organizations. The correla-
the links between materialism and values would benefit tion between this item and the materialism scale was
from the use of more sensitive values measures. -.21 (p<.01).
The third assessment of selfishness was the measure
Materialism and Self-Centeredness of nongenerosity developed by Belk (1984) administered
in survey 2. This scale measures nongenerosity with
One of the common statements about materialists is possessions and other nonmonetary resources and con-
that they are self-centered and unconcerned about oth- tains items such as "I don't like to lend things, even to
ers. Three tests of this proposition were carried out. In good friends" and "I enjoy having guests stay in my
survey 2, respondents were asked to assume they had home" (reverse scored). The correlation between the
been unexpectedly given $20,000. They were then given materialism scale and the nongenerosity scale (alpha
a list of six ways in which the money could be spent, = .63) was .25 {p < .001).
plus an "other" category. Three versions of the ques- These analyses support the hypothesis that materi-
tionnaire were administered in a split-ballot procedure, alists prefer to retain their resources for their own use
the only difference among the questionnaires being the and are less willing than others to share what they have,
order of listing of the spending categories. Spending both in terms of their money (as measured by the
categories were developed from pretests in which adult spending analysis) and their possessions (as evidenced
respondents were asked in an open-ended format how by the correlation with the nongenerosity scale). This
they would spend an unexpected gift of $20,000. reluctance to share extends to those with whom they
For purposes of analyses, respondents were again di- have close social ties (friends and family) and more so-
vided into terciles based on their materialism scores; cially distant entities such as charitable and ecological
Table 6 shows spending intentions for respondents in organizations.
the top and bottom terciles. On average, respondents
high in materialism said they would spend three times
as much on things for themselves as would low-mate- Materialism and Voluntary Simplicity
rialism respondents {p < .001), would contribute less Although Rudmin and Kilbourne (1992) have noted
than half of what low materialists would to charitable that "the essential feature of voluntary simplicity is not
or church organizations {p < .001), and would give less the manifest behavior, but the underlying values and
than half as much to friends and family {p < .01). They beliefs which . . . motivate that behavior," Leonard-
also would spend less on travel {p < .05). Barton (1981) posits that the values of voluntary sim-
A second test of the relationship between materialism plicity are likely to result in certain kinds of behaviors,
and selfishness comes from an item in the voluntary which she includes in the voluntary-simplicity life-style
simplicity life-style measure used in survey 4. (The re- scale. To assess the relationship between materialism
lationship between materialism and voluntary simplic- and voluntary simplicity, a shorter (13-item) version of
MATERIALISM AND ITS MEASUREMENT 313

TABLE 6 between voluntary-simplicity life-style and materialism


AMOUNT INTENDED TO BE SPENT IN DIFFERENT CATEGORIES did emerge, supporting the contentions of Rudmin and
BY RESPONDENTS HIGH AND LOW IN MATERIALISM Kilbourne (1992) and others.

High
nnaterialism
Low
materiaiism
Materialism and Satisfaction
($) ($) Philosophers have frequently warned that the continual
Spending category {n = 91) {n = 85) f pursuit of goods can lead only to dissatisfaction. The re-
3,445 1,106 5.38***
lationship between materialism and satisfaction was mea-
Buy things 1 want or need
Give to church organization sured in different ways in the three validation surveys. In
or charity 733 1,782 -3.79*** survey 4, respondents completed measures of satisfaction
Give or lend to friends or with life as a whole, amount of fun, family life, income
relatives 1,089 2,631 -3.08** or standard of living, and relationships with friends using
Travel 2,090 3,015 -2.08* the delighted-terdble response scale described by Andrews
Pay off debts 4,281 3,271 1.41
Savings or investments 7,413 7,471 -.07
and Withey (1976). Materialism was negatively related to
Other 948 724 .27 satisfaction in all the aspects of life measured. The rela-
tionship was strongest for satisfaction with income or
< .05. standard of living {r = -.39) and weakest for satisfaction
p< .01.
with family life (r = .17). Correlations for satisfaction
with life as a whole, fun, and friends were -.32, -.34,
and -.31, respectively (all p < .01).
the scale described by Leonard-Barton, with some re- Envy involves a coveting of what another has and,
vision to item wording and response categories, was frequently, a resentment of the person who possesses
administered in survey 4. In analysis, the three-con- the desired objects (see Belk 1984). As such, it implies
struct structure for the scale proposed by Cowles and a dissatisfaction with one's own possessions and lot in
Crosby (1986) was used because this structure resulted life. Survey 2 contained a measure of envy (Belk 1984;
in better prediction than the six-factor structure pro- alpha = .52); the correlation between materialism and
posed by Leonard-Barton. Cowles and Crosby labeled envy was .47 (p < .001).
their constructs "material simplicity" (which involves As a final measure of satisfaction, the Rosenberg
buying used goods and relying on bicycles instead of (1965) self-esteem scale was included in survey 3 (alpha
automobiles for transportation), "self-determination" = .81). Self-esteem has often been described as an eval-
(making rather than buying items and attempting to uation of the self, an assessment of how good or bad
do home repairs oneself), and "ecological awareness" one is (e.g., Epstein 1980). The correlation between
(recycling and contributing to ecological organi- materialism and self-esteem was -.12 (p < .05).
zations). According to critics, the quest for happiness through
Rudmin and Kilbourne have distinguished between possessions is destined to result in dissatisfaction (e.g.,
voluntary simplicity, reflecting underlying values, and Leiss 1976). A number of authors have suggested that
involuntary simplicity that results from inadequate re- materialism and the pursuit of possessions is not the
sources (income) to engage in a more complex life-style. cause but the result of dissatisfaction, that the desire to
To avoid confounding these two constructs, partial cor- possess and consume stems from insecurities or deeper
relations controlling for income were used when as- dissatisfactions with one's self and one's life (e.g., Braun
sessing the relationship between materialism and the and Wicklund 1989; Cushman 1990; Fromm 1976;
voluntary simplicity indices for material simplicity, self- Wachtel 1983). While a thorough research program
determination, and ecological awareness; resulting par- would be necessary to determine the validity of these
tial correlations were - . 18, - . 15, and -.24 for the three propositions, the data here suggest that materialists are
respective indices. The partial correlation when all 13 more likely to be dissatisfied with their circumstances
items are summed was -.28 (p < .01 for all partial than with themselves. However, more sensitive mea-
correlations). sures of self-concept are necessary to adequately assess
The relationship between materialism and the volun- the relationship between self variables and materialism.
tary simplicity life-style scale, while significant, is not es-
pecially strong. This may be due to weaknesses of the DISCUSSION
voluntary simplicity scale itself. Like many other behav- The research program described here was undertaken
iors, some of those included in the scale may be deter- to develop a measure of material values that represents
mined as much by one's situation as by one's values or
attitudes (e.g., bike to work; grow vegetables). A second
limitation of the scale is its low reliability (Cronbach's
alpha for the scale and subscales were between .48 and 'Prior research has not reported coefficients alpha for the voluntary
.62).' Despite these limitations, a significant relationship simplicity scale.
314 JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH

the construct of materialism as described in the social that represented here. For instance, measures that assess
sciences literature and in common usage. The resulting the extent to which individuals use material possessions
scale measures three correlated aspects of materialism: to assist in defining the self or as an expression of group
acquisition centrality, the role of acquisition in the pur- membership and belonging will allow researchers to ex-
suit of happiness, and the role of possessions in defining amine more broadly consumers' relationships with ma-
success. The scale possesses acceptable reliability, and terial objects.
preliminary assessments of scale validity were suc-
cessful.
The search for a measure of materialism led us to the [Received August 1991. Revised February 1992.]
study of values, and one contribution of this research
is the recognition that values can and sometimes should REFERENCES
be measured in ways other than with traditional ranking
procedures. An examination of published consumer re- Alwin, Duane F. and Jon A. Krosnick (1985), "The Mea-
search suggests that the study of values has been some- surement of Values in Surveys: A Comparison of Ratings
what neglected. Helgeson et al. (1984) examined con- and Rankings," Public Opinion Quarterly, 49 (Winter),
sumer-behavior studies from 1950 through 1981 and 535-552.
found that only 0.8 percent of the studies concerned Andrews, Frank M. and Stephen B. Withey (1976), Social
Indicators of Well-Being: Americans' Perceptions of Life
values or beliefs. Our own informal review of the more Quality, New York: Plenum.
recent literature (1984-1991) also revealed little re- Ballard, Rebecca, Michael D. Crino, and Stephen Rubenfeld
search on values. Excluding studies of materialism, only (1988), "Social Desirability Response Bias and the Mar-
seven articles in the Journal of Consumer Research and lowe-Crowne Social Desirability Scale," Psychological
the Association for Consumer Research proceedings Reports, 63 (August), 227-237.
dealt with consumer values, and the main focus of four Belk, Russell W. (1983), "Worldly Possessions: Issues and
of these was to examine revised lists of values from the Criticisms," in Advances in Consumer Research, Vol. 10,
ranking procedures used by Rokeach (e.g., Kahle et al. ed. Richard P. Bagozzi and Alice M. Tybout, Ann Arbor,
1986; Munson and McQuarrie 1988) rather than to ad- MI: Association for Consumer Research, 514-519.
dress substantive issues concerning consumer values. (1984), "Three Scales to Measure Constructs Related
Instead of revising the Rokeach scales, it might be ap- to Materialism: Reliability, Validity, and Relationships
propriate at this time to develop entirely different value to Measures of Happiness," in Advances in Consumer
measures more suited to the usual application of indi- Research, Vol. 11, ed. Thomas Kinnear, Provo, UT: As-
vidual difference variables in consumer behavior. sociation for Consumer Research, 291-297.
(1985), "Materialism: Trait Aspects of Living in the
In consumer-behavior research, we usually wish to Material World," Journal of Consumer Research, 12
measure the intensity of a variable using a metric scale (December), 265-280.
and multiple items so that we can establish reliability, (1987), "Material Values in the Comics: A Content
compare individuals or groups, and assess associations Analysis of Comic Books Featuring Themes of Wealth,"
Journal of Consumer Research, 14 (June), 26-42.
with other variables. This is difficult to accomplish with (1988), "Third World Consumer Culture," in Re-
rank data. Furthermore, values are complex phenom- search in Marketing, Suppl. 4, ed. Erdogan Kumcu and
ena, and the use of complex measures is required. It is A. Fuat Firat, Greenwich, CT: JAI, 113-127.
no more reasonable to measure the value of "warm re- Bengston, Vern L. and Mary Christine Lovejoy (1973), "Val-
lationships with others" with a single item than it is to ues, Personality, and Social Structure," American Be-
measure attitudes toward religion with a single question havioral Scientist, 16 (July/August), 880-912.
on a survey. Single-item measures are also unsatisfac- Braun, Ottmar L. and Robert A. Wicklund (1989), "Psycho-
tory from a reliability perspective (Nunnally 1978). It logical Antecedents of Conspicuous Consumption,"
is hoped that this research will encourage others to ex- Journal of Economic Psychology, 10 (June), 161-187.
tend the multiple-item measurement philosophy to ad- Bredemeier, Harry C. and Jackson Toby (1960), Social Prob-
ditional values important in the study of consumer be- lems in America: Costs and Casualties in an Acquisitive
Society, New York: Wiley.
havior.
Brickman, Philip and Donald T. Campbell (1971), "Hedonic
We also hope that our work will spawn more research Relativism and Planning the Good Society," in Adap-
on materialism, including investigations into its ante- tation-Level Theory, ed. Mortimer H. Appley, New York:
cedents and consequences. Such research should in- Academic Press, 287-302.
vestigate the potential positive effects of materialism Burdsal, Charles, Jr. (1975), "An Examination of Second Or-
(both personal and economic) as well as the negative der Motivational Factors as Found in Adults," Journal
ones more frequently mentioned in the literature. Other of Genetic Psychology, 127 (September), 83-89.
research can investigate the relationship between ma- Campbell, Colin (1987), The Romantic Ethic and the Spirit
terial values and marketing stimuli such as advertising of Modern Consumerism, New York: Basil Blackwell.
Campbell, Donald T. (1969), "Various Social Attitude
(e.g., Pollay 1984), store displays, and product char- Scales," in Measures of Political Attitudes, ed. John P.
acteristics. Finally, additional research may seek to Robinson and Phillip R. Shaver, Ann Arbor, MI: Survey
broaden the conceptualization of materialism beyond Research Center, University of Michigan, 648-653.
MATERIALISM AND ITS MEASUREMENT 315

Carver, Charles S. (1989), "How Should Multifaceted Per- (1976), To Have or To Be? New York: Harper & Row.
sonality Cotistructs Be Tested? Issues Illustrated by Self- Gaines, Judith (1990), "New Hampshire's New Homeless
Monitoring, Attributional Style, and Hardiness," Journal Belie the Stereotypes," Boston Globe, (December 2), 85-
of Personality and Social Psychology, 56 (April), 577- 86.
585. Hawkins, Del I., Roger J. Best, and Kenneth A. Coney (1989),
Churchill, Gilbert A., Jr. (1979), "A Paradigm for Developing Consumer Behavior, Homewood, IL: Irwin.
Better Measures of Marketing Constructs," Journal of Helgeson, James G., E. Alan Kluge, John Mager, and Cheri
Marketing Research, 16 (February), 64-73. Taylor (1984), "Trends in Consumer Behavior Literature:
Cowles, Deborah and Lawrence A. Crosby (1986), "Measure A Content Analysis," Journal of Consumer Research, 10
Validation in Consumer Research: A Confirmatory Fac- (March), 449-454.
tor Analysis ofthe Voluntary Simplicity Lifestyle Scale," Heilbroner, Robert L. (1956), The Quest for Wealth: A Study
\n Advances in Consumer Research, Vol. 13, ed. Richard of Acquisitive Man, New York: Simon & Schuster.
J. Lutz, Provo, UT: Association for Consumer Research, Henkoff, Ronald (1989), "Is Greed Dead?" Fortune (August
392-397. 14), 40-46.
Crowne, Douglas P. and David Marlowe (1960), "A New Scale Heslin, Richard, Blair T. Johnson, and Brian F. Blake (1989),
of Social Desirability Independent of Psychopathology," "Saver-Spender Scales," in Proceedings ofthe Society for
Journal of Consulting Psychology, 24 (August), 349-354. Consumer Psychology 1988 Annual Convention, ed. Da-
Csikszentmihaiyi, Mihaly and Eugene Rochberg-Halton vid W. Schumann, Washington, DC: Society for Con-
(1978), "Reflections on Materialism," University of Chi- sumer Psychology, 179-185.
cago Magazine, 70 (3), 6-15. Hicks, Lou E. (1970), "Some Properties of Ipsative, Nor-
and Eugene Rochberg-Halton (1981), The Meaning mative, and Forced-Choice Normative Measures," Psy-
of Things: Domestic Symbols and the Self, Cambridge: chological Bulletin, 74 (September), 167-184.
Cambridge University Press. Hirschman, Elizabeth C. (1990), "Consumption Styles ofthe
Cushman, Philip (1990), "Why the Self Is Empty," American Rich and Famous: The Semiology of Saul Steinberg and
Psychologist, 45 (May), 599-611. Malcolm Forbes," in Advances in Consumer Research,
Daun, Ake (1983), "The Materialistic Life-style: Some Socio- Vol. 17, ed. Marvin E. Goldberg, Gerald Gorn, and
psychological Aspects," in Consumer Behavior and En- Richard W. Pollay, Provo, UT: Association for Consumer
vironmental Quality, ed. Liisa Uusitalo, New York: St. Research, 850-855.
Martin's, 6-16. lnglehart, Ronald (1981), "Post-materialism in a Environ-
Dawson, Scott (1988), "Trait Materialism: Improved Mea- ment of Insecurity," American Political Science Review,
sures and an Extension to Multiple Domains of Life Sat- 75 (December), 880-900.
isfaction," in AM A Winter Educators' Conference, ed. Jackson, Douglas N., Sadrudin A. Ahmed, and Nelson A.
Stanley Shapiro and A. H. Walle, Chicago: American Heapy (1976), "Is Achievement a Unitary Construct?"
Marketing Association, 478-481. Journal of Research in Personality, 10 (March), 1-21.
de Tocqueville, Alexis ([1835] 1954), Democracy in America, Justice, Blair and Roger Birkman (1972), "An Effort to Dis-
New York: Vintage. tinguish the Violent from the Nonviolent," Southern
De Young, Raymond (1985-1986), "Encouraging Environ- Medical Journal, 65 (6), 703-706.
mentally Appropriate Behavior: The Role of Intrinsic Kahle, Lynn R., Sharon E. Beatty, and Pamela Homer (1986),
Motivation," Journal of Environmental Systems, 15 (4), "Alternative Measurement Approaches to Consumer
281-292. Values: The List of Values (LOV) and Values and Life
Dickins, Dorothy and Virginia Ferguson (1957), Practices and Styles (VALS)," Journal of Consumer Research, 13 (De-
Attitudes ofRural White Children and Parents concerning cember), 405-409.
Money, Mississippi Agricultural Experiment Station Lange, Frederick Albert ([1865] 1925), The History of Ma-
Technical Bulletin 43. terialism, London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.
Du Bois, Cora (1955), "The Dominant Value Profile of Lasch, Christopher (1978), The Culture of Narcissism, New
American Culture," American Anthropologist, 57 (De- York: Norton.
cember), 1232-1239. Leiss, William (1976), The Limits to Satisfaction: On Needs
Elgin, Duane (1981), Voluntary Simplicity, New York: Mor- and Commodities, Toronto: University of Toronto Press.
row. Leonard-Barton, Dorothy (1981), "Voluntary Simplicity
Engel, James F., Roger D. Blackwell, and Paul W. Miniard Lifestyles and Energy Conservation," Journal of Con-
(1990), Consumer Behavior, Chicago: Dryden. sumer Research, 8 (December), 243-252.
Epstein, Seymour (1980), "The Self-Concept: A Review and Linden, Eugene (1979), Affluence and Discontent: The Anat-
the Proposal of an Integrated Theory of Personality," in omy of Consumer Societies, New York: Viking.
Personality: Basic Aspects and Current Research, ed. Er- Looft, William R. (1971), "The Psychology of More," ^wer-
vin Staub, Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall, 81-132. ican Psychologist, 26 (June), 561-565.
Fournier, Susan and Marsha L. Richins (1991), "Some Theo- Moschis, George P. and Gilbert A. Churchill, Jr. (1978),
retical and Popular Notions concerning Materialism," "Consumer Socialization: A Theoretical and Empirical
Journal of Social Behavior and Personality, 6, 403-414. Analysis," Journal of Consumer Research, 15 (Novem-
Fox, Richard Wightman and T. J. Jackson Lears (1983), The ber), 599-609.
Culture of Consumption: Critical Essays in American Mowen, John C. (1990), Consumer Behavior, New York:
History, 1880-1980, New York: Pantheon. McGraw-Hill.
Fromm, Erich (1967), "The Psychological Aspects ofthe Mukerji, Chandra (1983), From Graven Images: Patterns of
Guaranteed Income," in The Guaranteed Income, ed. Modern Materialism, New York: Columbia University
Robert Theobald, Garden City, NY: Anchor, 183-193. Press.
316 JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH

Munson, J. Michael and Edward F. McQuarrie (1988), Rudmin, Floyd W., and William E. Kilbourne (1992), "The
"Shortening the Rokeach Value Survey for Use in Con- Meaning and Morality of Voluntary Simplicity: History
sumer Research," in Advances in Consumer Research, and Hypotheses on Deliberately Denied Materialism,"
Vol. 15, ed. Michael J. Houston, Provo, UT: Association in Consumption and Marketing: Macro Dimensions, ed.
for Consumer Research, 381-386. Nikhilesh Dholakia and Russell W. Belk, Northridge, CA:
Nunnally, Jum C. (1978), 'Psychometric Theory, New York: PWS-Kent, in press.
McGraw-Hill. Sahlins, Marshall (1976), Culture and Practical Reason, Chi-
Oxford English Dictionary (19S9), Oxford: Clarendon. cago: University of Chicago Press.
Peter, J. Paul (1981), "Construct Validity: A Review of Basic Schudson, Michael {\9S4), Advertising, The Uneasy Persua-
Issues and Marketing Practices," Journal of Marketing sion, New York: Basic.
Research, 18 (May), 133-145. Scitovszky, Tibor (1976), The Joyless Economy, New York:
Polanyi, Karl (1944), The Great Transformation, New York: Oxford University Press.
Rinehart. Shama, Avraham and Joseph Wisenblit (1984), "Values of
Voluntary Simplicity: Lifestyle and Motivation," Psy-
Pollay, Richard W. (1984), "The Identification and Distri- chological Reports, 55 (August), 231-240.
bution of Values Manifest in Print Advertising 1900-
Spiggle, Susan (1986), "Measuring Social Values: A Content
1980," in Personal Values and Consumer Psychology, Analysis of Sunday Comics and Underground Comix,"
ed. Robert E. Pitts, Jr., and Arch G. Woodside, Lexing- Journal of Consumer Research, 13 (June), 100-113.
ton, MA: Lexington, 111-135. Veblen, Thorstein ([1899] 1953), The Theory of the Leisure
Rassuli, Kathleen M. and Stanley C. Hollander (1986), "De- Class: An Economic Study in the Evolution ofInstitutions,
sireInduced, Innate, Insatiable?" Journal of Macro- New York: American Library.
marketing, 6 (Fall), 4-24. Wachtel, Paul L. (1983), The Poverty of Affluence: A Psycho-
Richins, Marsha L. (1987), "Media, Materialism, and Human logical Portrait of the American Way of Life, New York:
Happiness," in Advances in Consumer Research, Vol. Free Press.
14, ed. Melanie Wallendorf and Paul Anderson, Provo, Wackman, Daniel B., Greg Reale, and Scott Ward (1972),
UT: Association for Consumer Research, 352-356. "Racial Differences in Responses to Advertising among
and Scott Dawson (1990), "Measuring Material Val- Adolescents," in Television in Day-to-Day Life, ed. Eli
ues: A Preliminary Report of Scale Development," in A. Rubenstein, George A. Comstock, and John P. Mur-
Advances in Consumer Research, Vol. 17, ed. Marvin E. ray, Rockville, MD: U.S. Department of Health, Edu-
Goldberg, Gerald Gorn, and Richard W. Pollay, Provo, cation, and Welfare, 543-551.
UT: Association for Consumer Research, 169-175. Ward, Scott and Daniel Wackman (1971), "Family and Media
Rochberg-Halton, Eugene (1986), Meaning and Modernity: Influences on Adolescent Learning," American Behav-
Social Theory in the Pragmatic Attitude, Chicago: Uni- ioral Scientist, 14 (January-February), 415-427.
versity of Chicago Press. Weber, Max ([ 1930] 1958), The Protestant Ethic and the Spirit
of Capitalism, trans. Talcott Parsons, New York: Scrib-
Rokeach, Milton (1973), The Nature of Human Values, New ner's.
York: Free Press. Yamauchi, Kent T. and Donald I. Templer (1982), "The De-
Rosenberg, Morris (1965), Society and the Adolescent Self- velopment of a Money Attitude Scale," Journal of Per-
image, Princeton: Princeton University Press. sonality Assessment, 46 (October), 522-528.
View publication stats

Você também pode gostar