Você está na página 1de 8

The Effect of Price, Brand Name, and Store Name on Buyers' Perceptions of Product Quality:

An Integrative Review
Author(s): Akshay R. Rao and Kent B. Monroe
Source: Journal of Marketing Research, Vol. 26, No. 3 (Aug., 1989), pp. 351-357
Published by: American Marketing Association
Stable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/3172907 .
Accessed: 16/06/2014 18:26

Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at .
http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp

.
JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of
content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms
of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact support@jstor.org.

American Marketing Association is collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to
Journal of Marketing Research.

http://www.jstor.org

This content downloaded from 85.139.73.51 on Mon, 16 Jun 2014 18:26:23 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
AKSHAYR. RAOand KENTB. MONROE*

The authors integrate previous research that has investigated experimentally the
influenceof price, brandname, and/or store nameon buyers'evaluationsof prod-
uct quality. The meta-analysissuggests that, for consumerproducts,the relation-
shipsbetweenpriceand perceivedqualityand betweenbrandnameand perceived
qualityare positiveand statisticallysignificant.However,the positiveeffect of store
name on perceivedqualityis smalland not statisticallysignificant.Further,the type
of experimentaldesign and the strengthof the price manipulationare shownto
significantlyinfluencethe observedeffect of price on perceivedquality.

The Effect of Price, Brand Name, and Store


Name on Buyers' Perceptions of Product
Quality: An Integrative Review

Since Leavitt (1954) examined buyers' tendencies to research that has examined the effects of price, brand
use price as an indicator of quality, numerous studies name, and store name on product quality perceptions. In
have examined the price-quality relationship with little addition, we examine certain methodological differences
consensus as to its magnitude, generalizability, or sta- among price-perceived quality research studies to de-
tistical significance. Various qualitative reviews of the termine whether those differences are associated with
evidence conclude that there seems to be a positive re- variations in outcomes among the studies. An exami-
lationship between price and perceptions of quality for nation of the criticisms in previous commentaries on the
certain products and within certain price ranges; how- research stream generated hypotheses about four meth-
ever, these effects are moderated by methodological de- odological variables to explain outcome variation across
ficiencies, a variety of contextual and situational factors, studies. We test these hypotheses using meta-analytic
and a weak underlying theoretical explanation (Monroe procedures on the results of previous research studies.
1973, 1977; Monroe and Dodds 1988; Monroe and
Krishnan 1985; Olson 1977; Peterson and Wilson 1985; OVERVIEWOF THE RESEARCHDOMAIN
Rao and Monroe 1988; Zeithaml 1988).
When suggesting that people may judge quality by
The tradition in this research stream has evolved from
price, Scitovsky (1945) pointed out that such behavior
considering the effect of only price on buyers' percep- is not irrational;it simply reflects a belief that the forces
tions of product quality to including other intrinsic and
of supply and demand would lead to a "natural"ordering
extrinsic cues. To begin to unravel some issues and ad-
of products on a price scale, leading to a strong positive
dress some questions in the area, we integrate previous
relationship between price and product quality. Empiri-
cal attempts to verify this strong actual correlation have
concluded that, generally, there is a positive correlation;
*Akshay R. Rao is Assistant Professor of Marketing, The Curtis however, though statistically significant, this correlation
L. Carlson School of Management, University of Minnesota. Kent B. varies considerably across product-markets, producing
Monroe is the Robert 0. Goodykoontz Professor of Marketing, The
R. B. Pamplin College of Business, Virginia Polytechnic Institute and an average correlation of r = .27 (Tellis and Wernerfelt
State University. 1987).
The authors gratefully acknowledge helpful comments on previous We examine another research stream that has at-
drafts of the article by William Dodds, Edward Fern, George John,
R. Krishnan, Diane Schmalensee, Raghu Tadepalli, Orville Walker,
tempted to determine whether buyers perceive a positive
John Wheatley, and Valarie Zeithaml. price-quality relationship. The statistical results across
previous studies have been inconsistent and the com-

351

Journal of Marketing Research


Vol. XXVI (August 1989), 351-7

This content downloaded from 85.139.73.51 on Mon, 16 Jun 2014 18:26:23 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
352 JOURNAL
OF MARKETING AUGUST1989
RESEARCH,

mentaries on past research have been rathercritical. Pos- solute differences in prices lead to differential responses
sible methodological explanations for the inconsistent (Monroe 1973; Monroe and Petroshius 1981). There-
statistical results include the type of product, experi- fore, we expect that the greater the relative difference
mental procedures used, and uncontrolled or unmea- between the prices used as levels of an independent vari-
sured individual response variations (Monroe 1977; Ol- able in a given study, the larger will be the observed
son 1977). Focusing on the experimentalproceduresused effect on subjects' perceptions of quality, and, ceteris
produces four methodologicalissues that may explain why paribus, the better the chance of detecting a statistically
previous studies have generated such inconsistent re- significant effect.
sults: number of cues, experimental design, price ma-
Price Level
nipulation, and price level.
Number of Cues Finally, it has been suggested that people are more
likely to use price as an indicator of quality for relatively
Olson (1973) argued that consumers use a variety of expensive products (Olson 1977). As the price level in-
cues to infer product quality. These cues include extrin- creases, the risk of an incorrect assessment increases and
sic cues that are not related directly to product perfor- buyers often are less familiar with the product because
mance and intrinsic cues that are derived directly from of the infrequency of purchases. In such situations, sim-
the physical product and, if changed, would change the ple learned heuristics based on folk wisdom such as "you
product itself. For example, price, brand name, and store get what you pay for" are likely to be used.
name (extrinsic cues) as well as nutrition content (in- In sum, the conventional wisdom in the literaturesug-
trinsic cue) would be used by consumers in their as- gests that the magnitude of the price-perceived quality
sessment of the quality of a breakfast cereal. Therefore, effect is influenced by at least four methodological fac-
because numerous cues affect quality perceptions, the tors: number of cues used, type of design, size of the
use of multiple independent variables in addition to the price manipulation, and relative price level of the stim-
price cue is necessary for valid empirical tests. ulus. Several other substantive factors have been pro-
Further, single-cue price-quality studies have been posed to influence the magnitude of the price-perceived
considered somewhat artifactual; in the absence of any quality effect (Monroe and Dodds 1988; Peterson and
other information, subjects should naturally exhibit a Wilson 1985; Zeithaml 1988), but our review focuses on
positive price-quality effect (Enis and Stafford 1969). the four methodological criticisms of previous price-
Therefore, the implicit inference is that single-cue price- perceived quality studies.
perceived quality studies should manifest larger effects
than multicue studies. Hypotheses
The preceding commentary suggests four hypotheses
Experimental Design about the impact of methodological variables on the ob-
Conceptually, it has been argued that the hypothesis served price-perceived quality effect.
of people using price as an indicator of quality is studied The price-perceived quality effect size will:
best by an idiothetic approach-that is, repeated mea-
sures over several prices-as the experimental situation H,: vary positivelywith the price level of the test prod-
ucts,
created is analogous to a real-world situation of exam- H2: varypositivelywith the strengthof the pricemanip-
ining several different choices at different prices (Mon- ulation,
roe and Dodds 1988). However, the use of a repeated- H3: be smallerfor multicuestudies than for single-cue
measures design (within subjects) has been criticized as studies, and
being potentially artifactual as subjects responding to H4: be smaller for between-subjectsdesigns than for
several prices sequentially may guess the true intent of within-subjectsdesigns.
the researcher and respond accordingly (Sawyer 1975).
Further, because variability due to individual difference REVIEWPROCEDURES
is controlled in a within-subjects design, thus reducing To find relevant research studies, we examined the
error variance, within-subjects designs are likely to pro- references of previous review articles and conducted a
duce larger effects than between-subjects designs. For computer bibliographic search. Because the hypotheses
both of these reasons, price-perceived quality studies pertain to methodological variations across laboratory
having within-subjects designs should show larger ef- experiments,only laboratorystudies involving one or more
fects than studies having between-subjects designs. of price, brand, and store cues were used in the review.
Further, when the results of a study were reported in
Price Manipulation
multiple outlets, the study was included only once. A
In experimental research, generally the strength of the final set of 36 studies that collectively report 85 effects
independent variable manipulation has an important of price, brand name, or store name on perceptions of
bearing on whether statistically significant differences in quality were used in the analysis and tests.
the dependent variable occur. Price perception research We computed the effect size index i12 for all main ef-
suggests that people's perceptions of relative and not ab- fects by using

This content downloaded from 85.139.73.51 on Mon, 16 Jun 2014 18:26:23 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
BUYERS' OF PRODUCT
PERCEPTIONS QUALITY 353

(1) v12 = F(dfb)/[F(dfb) + dfer] to capital equipment. However, for a preponderance of


where F is the ratio reported from relevant ANOVA consumer goods, prices ranged from $.11 (table salt) to
$400 (stereo receivers). Only four consumer products
summarytables, dfbis degrees of freedom associated with exceeded $100 in price and 60% cost less than $30. The
the treatment effect, and dfer, is degrees of freedom as-
sociated with the error term. Further, r2 is a special case strength of the price manipulations ranged from .18 to
of _q2 when the F-ratio has only one degree of freedom .86. Twelve of the 54 price-perceived quality results
associated with the numerator and is an effect size in- stemmed from a single-cue presentation and 19 results
dicator for studies comparing only two groups. Cohen's were obtained with a within-subjects design. Table 1
(1977) d effect size indicator was used for one study and provides a stem and leaf plot, which is a data display
converted to r2 by using technique analogous to a histogram (Rosenthal 1984),
and a statistical summary of all price-perceived quality
(2) r2 = d2/(d2 + 1/p.q) effect sizes. Table 2 provides a stem and leaf plot and
a statistical summary of all brand- and store-perceived
where p and q are the proportions of the sample that each
mean represents (Rosenthal 1984). For studies using quality effect sizes. (A complete list of studies coded and
their characteristics is available from the authors.)
MANOVA the effect size used was (Bray and Maxwell
1984) Comparing Results Across Studies
(3) 1i2(mult)= Pillai'strace/s, Studies investigatingthe effect of price, brand, or store
where Pillai's trace is I Xi/(1 + ki), X, is the eigenvalue on buyers' product evaluations have tested whether there
of the ith variable, and s is min(number of variables, hy- is a degree of association between these variables and
pothesis degrees of freedom). buyers' evaluations. As these relationships have been
The design of the study (within or between) and num- studied several times, three questions should be ad-
ber of cues used (single or multiple) were coded as 0,1 dressed (Fleiss 1981):
dummy variables. Price level was determined by cal- 1. Is there evidence that the degree of associationis con-
culating the mean of the highest and lowest price treat- sistentacrossstudies?
ments in each study [(high - low)/2]. The strength of 2. If consistent,is the commondegree of associationsta-
the price manipulation was determined by computing a tisticallysignificant?
proportionvariation between the highest and lowest price 3. Whatis the best estimateof the association,its standard
treatments for each product studied [(high - low)/high] error,and confidenceinterval?
and ranged from 0 to 1.
The dependent variable measured in a study was de- Essentially, the first question seeks to determinewhether
termined by examining the indicators used. Perceived the studies to be integrated have produced effect sizes
that are from the same underlying population of effect
quality as the dependent variable was indicated by such sizes. A homogeneity of effect size test using an ap-
terms as "durability," "reliability," "workmanship,"
"quality," "goodness," and "excellence."' proximate chi square with (K results - 1) degrees of
freedom addresses this issue. If the test fails to accept
RESULTSAND ANALYSIS the null hypothesis of no significant differences, the con-
clusion is that the variation in results across the studies
Description of the Data is not explained simply by sampling error and that sys-
The spectrum of products used as stimuli ranged from tematic differences are present across the studies. Ac-
consumer nondurable products (e.g., butter, margarine) ceptance of the null hypothesis is evidence of homoge-

Table 1
AND STATISTICAL
STEMAND LEAFPLOTSa SUMMARY:
PRICE-PERCEIVED EFFECTS
QUALITY

S12
Stem Leaf Summary statistics (all results)
.8 7 Weighted mean, -l2 .12
.7 0 Weighted standard deviation, s .148
.6 3 Standard error .017
.5" 0 05 95% confidence limit +.033
.4 2 79.22
-- (53)
.3 0 8
.2 0135568
.1 0001 1 1 1 1 44479
.0 00000001111111223456666778
(K = 54)
"To read the plot, note that three effects are > .5 but < .6: .50, .50, .55.

This content downloaded from 85.139.73.51 on Mon, 16 Jun 2014 18:26:23 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
354 JOURNAL
OF MARKETING AUGUST1989
RESEARCH,

Table 2
STEMAND LEAFPLOTSAND STATISTICAL OF PERCEIVED
SUMMARY EFFECTS:
QUALITY BRANDAND STORE

Stem Leaf Summary statistics


Brand
.6 0 Weighted mean, il2 .142
.5 7 Weighted standard deviation, s .122
.4 - Standard error .027
.3 4 95% confidence limit +.053
.2 5 5 X2(14) 19.9
.1 01124
.0 33557
(K = 15)

Store
.6 - Weighted mean, .052
;2
.5 - Weighted standard deviation, s .171
.4 3 4 Standard error .030
.3 - 95% confidence limit +.058
.2 - X2(16) 6.13
.1 2
.0 00011223346788
(K = 17)

neity or consistency of the degree of association across The homogeneity test for the price-perceived quality
studies and the results then can be integrated and ana- results using all 54 datapoints shows that the data are
lyzed. The statistic used to test the homogeneity of effect not homogeneous (X2(53) = 79.22; Table 1), implying
sizes was adapted from Fleiss (1981, p. 163): some systematic variation across these studies. The ef-
K fect sizes for both the brand (X2(14) = 19.9) and store
(4) (X2(16) = 6.13) results are homogeneous; therefore these
X = wi(ri - j12)2 results can be integrated. As shown in Table 2, the as-
sociation between brand and perceived quality is signif-
where: icant at the 95% confidence level and the best estimate
is the individual effect size for each result,
2 of the association is the weighted mean of fq2 = .142.
is the weighted estimator of effect size,
;f2 However, though the association between store and per-
wi is the weight of each result, and ceived quality is consistent, it is not significant at the
K is the number of results being compared. 95% confidence level because the confidence interval for
the weighted mean includes zero.
When results are compared across studies, substantial
variation in average effect size can occur depending on Residual Analysis
whether individual effect sizes are weighted by sample The price-perceived quality dataset was examined for
size. Further, if sample sizes differ, the estimates of the outliers and influential datapoints and a no-intercept
larger sample studies will be more precise. Therefore, a regression model based on the hypotheses stated before
weighting procedure that gives more weight to the more was assessed for collinearity. Forty of the 54 results were
precise estimates is adapted from Fleiss (1981, p. 161): used in this analysis as the other 14 results did not pro-
K vide sufficient detail for coding of all four independent
i = 1
variables.
(5) Wi = (1/vI), K,
(1/vi) ..... An examination of R-Student (ti) values (an externally
standardizedresidual) revealed four datapoints that were
where: more than the conventional 1.5 standarddeviations away
= + (l92)2/2Ni, from the centroid of the data cluster (Myers 1986). These
vi NlBi four datapointstherefore were classified as outliers. Out-
Ni = ni,E + i,c, liers are errors in the y direction caused by a large ran-
Bi = (ni,E)(ni,c), and dom error component in the response, producing a large
ni,E and ni,c are the sample sizes of the experimental residual. Datapoints also may be extreme in the x direc-
and control groups, respectively. tion. Such datapoints tend to have a large influence on
This procedure minimizes the variances of the weighted the regression, producing large hat diagonal (hi,) values.
estimates and produces an asymptotically efficient Using the traditional cutoff of 2p/n (=.20) for hat di-
weighted estimator. agonal values (Myers 1986), we identified three data-

This content downloaded from 85.139.73.51 on Mon, 16 Jun 2014 18:26:23 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
BUYERS' OF PRODUCT
PERCEPTIONS QUALITY 355

points as influential. These three datapoints represent the absence of any significant multicollinearity. Thus,
relatively high priced industrial products and therefore we have little concern about multicollinearity being a
have a large influence on the slope of the regression line. problem in this model.
Thus, seven results were segregated for the meta-anal-
Regression Analysis and Findings
ysis, leaving 33 datapoints for the tests of the hy-
potheses. To test the four hypotheses, we performed a multiple
For the reduced set of 33 price-perceived quality da- regression analysis on the dataset using the weighted
tapoints used in the regression analysis, a homogeneity price-perceived quality effect sizes as the dependent
analysis indicated that the data were homogeneous (X2(32) variable. The regression model tested the relationship
= 37.64). This dataset had weighted mean between price-perceived quality effects and price level,
i12 = .117, size of price manipulation, number of cues, and research
weighted s = .147, standard error = .025, and 95% con-
fidence interval ? .049. Thus, for this reduced set of 33 design. As is evident from Table 3, the model performed
results, 3632 additional studies with a zero average ef- well. The preceding theoretical argument suggests a pos-
fect size would be needed to reduce the statistical sig- itive coefficient for price level and price manipulation
nificance below the .05 level. Next, the 14 datapoints and a negative coefficient for number of cues. Also,
that had been eliminated for the regression analysis were within-subjects studies were expected to produce larger
retrieved and added to the 33 datapoints used in the effects than between-subjects studies. The results of the
regression analysis, producing a set of 47 homogeneous regression run are reported in Table 3.
results (x2(46) = 49.73) with weighted mean =12= .116, Support for the hypotheses was determined by ex-
weighted s = .125, standarderror = .018, and 95% con- amining the sign and significance of the coefficients for
fidence interval ?+.035. These summary results are very the reduced dataset comprising the 33 homogeneous re-
robust; an additional 8897 studies with an average zero sults. As shown in Table 3, price level is not signifi-
effect size would be needed to reduce the statistical sig- cantly related to size of effect (H,). However, the rela-
nificance of the price-perceived quality relationship be- tive strength of price manipulation is associated
low the .05 level. This result lends confidence to the significantly and positively with effect size (H2). Though
claim that the overall results are not due to sampling bias there is no statistically significant difference, multicue
in the studies selected for review (Rosenthal 1984). studies produced marginally larger effect sizes (;92 =
.124) than single-cue studies = .115) (H3). Finally,
Multicollinearity Diagnostics (il2 measures) generated
within-subjects designs (repeated
The possibility of linear dependencies in the regressor larger effects (;-2 = .20) than between-subjects designs
variables was investigated and a summary of the mul- (i2 = .10) (H4).
ticollinearity diagnostics is included in Table 3. The rel- DISCUSSION
atively low variance inflation factors are indicative of
low multiple correlations among the independent vari- Single Versus Multiple Cues
ables. Further, the relatively high minimum eigenvalue Using criteria suggested by Cohen (1977), the price
(hmin)
and low condition number ( = k max/hmin) confirm effect on perceived quality for consumer products (.12)
is moderately large and statistically significant. The ef-
fect of brand name on perceived quality is slightly larger
Table 3 (. 14) and statistically significant, but the effect of store
SUMMARY OF REGRESSION ANALYSIS: PRICE-PERCEIVED name on perceived quality is small (.05) and not statis-
EFFECTS
QUALITY tically significant. Moreover, though statistically not sig-
(33 homogeneousresults) nificant, multicue studies generated slightly larger ef-
fects (.124) than single-cue studies (.115). Taken together,
these results suggest that a price-perceived quality main
Collinearity
VIFmax 7.51 effect generated from a multicue study is not necessarily
Xmin 0.09 smaller than that generated from a single-cue study. This
4) 32.88 conclusion supportsan earlier observationby Monroe and
Model
R2 .85
Krishnan (1985) that price-perceived quality effects ac-
Rj .83 tually increased in the presence of brand information.
PRESS 1870.32 Thus, rather than brand name or other cues suppressing
Parameters price as an indicator of product quality, a reinforcing
Price level -.01 (p < .11) effect is likely if the multiple cues are consistent in their
Standardized estimate -.30
Manipulation 14.51 (p < .01) signaling of quality. This finding is also consistent with
Standardized estimate 1.19 the covariation literature (Nisbett and Ross 1980).
Design 4.69 (p < .08)
Standardized estimate .77 Price Manipulations
Cues 1.17 (p < .66)
Standardized estimate .19
The regression analysis indicates that the relative
strength of the price manipulations had a significant ef-

This content downloaded from 85.139.73.51 on Mon, 16 Jun 2014 18:26:23 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
356 JOURNAL
OF MARKETING AUGUST1989
RESEARCH,

fect on the observed price-perceived quality relation- priced and less frequently purchased. Indeed, for four
ship. Methodologically, this finding is an important ex- results for consumer products priced at more than $100,
planation for some of the inconsistency in statistical the weighted mean effect is ;-2 = .39. Two of the results
findings across previous price-perceived quality re- classified as outliers are for products priced above $100
search and therefore is of significance in designing price with an average effect size of .57. (The other two out-
perception research in the future; strong manipulations liers have effect sizes of .00.) Clearly, more research
are more likely to produce large effects, thereby en- using moderately priced and relatively expensive prod-
hancing the chance of a statistically significant effect. ucts is necessary to establish whether the price-per-
When buyers do infer a positive relationship between ceived quality relationship differs by relative price level
price and product quality, they are likely to compare the and whether it differs by the general nature of the prod-
price of the product against another price (price in mem- uct.
ory or price of an alternative option). If the actual price
is perceived as significantly different from this reference CONCLUSIONS
price, the higher priced option is likely to be perceived Meta-analyses often have been criticized for two rea-
as being of higher quality. The key point is that judg- sons. First, the "file drawer" problem prevents publi-
ments of quality based on price information are neces- cation of many studies that do not show significant ef-
sarily comparative and perceived differences in prices fects. Second, the notion of statistical significance is
lead to relative judgments that product quality varies sig- considered moot when a census of results is being con-
nificantly. In the research reviewed here, the greater the sidered. In our review, we attempt to address the first
ratio between the highest and lowest experimentalprices, issue by estimating the number of nonsignificant (and
the greater is the price-perceived quality association. thus "filed away") studies required to negate the values
Regardless of whether a between-subjects or within-sub- computed from published results. The evidence suggests
jects design is used, the greaterthe difference in the price that the findings are relatively robust. Second, the notion
treatments, the more likely individuals are to perceive of statistical significance is an importantone, as the find-
differences in prices and begin to make quality infer- ings are being generalized to a population of effects. In
ences. Thus, weak price manipulations are a possible ex- other words, the studies considered represent a sample
planation for previous price-perceived quality studies' of effects of the phenomenon under consideration. The
failure to find a statistically significant price main effect. interested reader will find considerable information on
this issue in books by Fleiss (1981), Hedges and Olkin
Research Design
(1985), Hunter, Schmidt, and Jackson (1982), and Ro-
The finding that within-subjects designs generated sig- senthal (1984).
nificantly larger effects than between-subjects designs Our review, in addition to providing estimates of the
(.20 vs. .10) does not necessarily mean that use of such size of the effect of price, brand name, and store name
designs in price researchis incorrector artifactual.It does on quality perceptions, investigates four possible expla-
mean that researchers must be aware that within-subjects nations for the lack of consistent results across studies.
designs generally are more powerful and should consider Two methodological variables-type of research design
this point when designing their studies and developing and strength of price manipulations-provide some ex-
their conclusions. The choice of the appropriate design planationfor the inconsistentfindings from previous price-
and price operationalizationshould be consistent with the perceived quality research. The review also indicates the
particularresearch question being studied. For instance, relative narrowness of previous price-perceived quality
Monroe and Dodds (1988) make the point that between- research. At this point in time the phenomenon has been
subjects designs are more appropriatefor price present/ shown to be present at a moderate level of association
absent studies that examine the marginal impact of price for relatively lower priced, frequently purchased goods,
information. but whether the strength of the association increases for
Further, a managerial implication consistent with the higher priced, less frequently purchased goods has not
referenceprice argumentmentionedbefore is evident here. been documented adequately. The quality perception re-
Multiple responses to differentprices (as in walking down search domain should be extended also to services and
an aisle in a store) are a more powerful means of gen- purchases made by business and institutional buyers.
erating price-quality inferences than exposures to single Though our integrative review answers some ques-
prices (as in television commercials). For the latter case, tions about the effects of price, brand name, and store
the cognitive effort involved in retrievinga referenceprice name on buyers' perceptions of product quality, it also
and using it for comparativepurposesmay result in weaker isolates important issues that warrant further investiga-
effects. tion. Still needed is research on how quality perceptions
are formed and how these quality perceptions influence
Price Level
perceptions of value, product or service benefits, and
One obvious implication of the lack of a significant eventual choice. Further, the interactive effect of brand
association between price level and size of effect is the name, store name, and price on quality perceptionsshould
dearth of studies using products that are relatively higher be investigated. Finally, the effect of consumers' prod-

This content downloaded from 85.139.73.51 on Mon, 16 Jun 2014 18:26:23 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
PERCEPTIONS
BUYERS' OF PRODUCT
QUALITY 357

uct knowledgemay moderatethe price-perceivedqual- Consumer Behavior, 3rd ed., Harold H. Kassarjian and
ity effect. Thomas S. Robertson, eds. Glenview, IL: Scott, Foresman
and Company, 43-55.
REFERENCES
Myers, Raymond H. (1986), Classical and Modern Regres-
Bray, James H. and Scott E. Maxwell (1985), Multivariate sion With Applications. Boston, MA: Duxbury Press.
Analysis of Variance. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage Publications, Nisbett, Richard and Lee Ross (1980), Human Inference:
Inc. Strategies and Shortcomings of Social Judgment. Engle-
Cohen, Jacob C. (1977), Statistical Analysis for the Behav- wood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall, Inc.
ioral Sciences. New York: Academic Press, Inc. Olson, Jerry C. (1973), "Cue Properties of Price: Literature
Enis, Ben and James E. Stafford (1969), "Influence of Price Review and Theoretical Considerations," paper presented at
and Store Information Upon Product Quality Perception," 83rd Annual Convention of the American Psychological As-
Southern Journal of Business, 4 (April), 90-9. sociation, Montreal, Canada (August).
Fleiss, J. L. (1981), Statistical Methods for Ratios and Pro- (1977), "Price as an Informational Cue: Effects on
portions, 2nd ed. New York: John Wiley & Sons, Inc. Product Evaluations," in Consumer and Industrial Buying
Hedges, Larry V. and Ingram Olkin (1985), Statistical Meth- Behavior, Arch G. Woodside, Jagdish N. Sheth, and Peter
ods for Meta-Analysis. Orlando, FL:-Academic Press, Inc. D. Bennett, eds. New York: North Holland Publishing
Hunter, John E., Frank L. Schmidt, and Gregg B. Jackson Company, 267-86.
(1982), Meta-Analysis:CumulatingResearch Findings Across Peterson, Robert A. and William R. Wilson (1985), "Per-
Studies. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage Publications, Inc. ceived Risk and Price-Reliance Schema as Price-Perceived
Leavitt, Harold J. (1954), "A Note on Some Experimental Quality Mediators," in Perceived Quality: How Consumers
Findings About the Meaning of Price," Journal of Business, View Stores and Merchandise, Jacob Jacoby and Jerry C.
27 (July), 205-10. Olson, eds. Lexington, MA: Lexington Books, Inc., 247-
Monroe, Kent B. (1973), "Buyers' Subjective Perceptions of 67.
Price," Journal of Marketing Research, 10 (February), 70- Rao, Akshay R. and Kent B. Monroe (1988), "The Moder-
80. ating Effect of Prior Knowledge on Cue Utilization in Prod-
(1977), "Objective and Subjective Contextual Influ- uct Evaluations," Journal of Consumer Research, 15 (Sep-
ences on Price Perceptions," in Consumer and Industrial tember), 253-64.
Buying Behavior, Arch G. Woodside, Jagdish N. Sheth, and Rosenthal, Robert (1984), Meta-Analytic Procedures for So-
Peter D. Bennett, eds. New York: North Holland Publishing cial Research. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage Publications, Inc.
Company, 267-86. Sawyer, Alan G. (1975), "Demand Artifacts in Laboratory
S and William B. Dodds (1988), "A Research Program Experimentation,"Journal of ConsumerResearch, 1 (March),
for Establishing the Validity of the Price-Quality Relation- 20-30.
ship," Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, 16 Scitovsky, Tibor (1945), "Some Consequences of the Habit of
(Spring), 151-68. Judging Quality by Price," Review of Economic Studies, 12
S and R. Krishnan (1985), "The Effect of Price on Sub- (32), 100-5.
jective Product Evaluations," in Perceived Quality: How Tellis, Gerard J. and Birger Wernerfelt (1987), "Competitive
Consumers View Stores and Merchandise, Jacob Jacoby and Price and Quality Under Asymmetric Information," Mar-
Jerry Olson, eds. Lexington, MA: Lexington Books, Inc., keting Science, 6 (Summer), 240-53.
209-32. Zeithaml, Valarie A. (1988), "Consumer Perceptions of Price,
S and Susan M. Petroshius (1981), "Buyers' Perceptions Quality, and Value: A Means-End Model and Synthesis of
of Price: An Update of the Evidence," in Perspectives in Evidence," Journal of Marketing, 52 (July), 2-22.

ReprintNo. JMR263108

This content downloaded from 85.139.73.51 on Mon, 16 Jun 2014 18:26:23 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

Você também pode gostar