Você está na página 1de 7

Learning and Individual Differences 39 (2015) 8995

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Learning and Individual Differences

journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/lindif

Dialectical thinking moderates the effect of extrinsic motivation on


intrinsic motivation
Yaoran Li a,, Kennon M. Sheldon b, Rude Liu c
a
Department of Educational, School, and Counseling Psychology, University of Missouri, United States
b
Department of Psychological Sciences, University of Missouri, United States
c
School of Psychology, Beijing Normal University, China

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: The present research tested the individual difference variable of dialectical thinking style (DT) as a moderator of
Received 16 February 2014 the relationship between extrinsic motivation and intrinsic motivation, in situations where rewards have been
Received in revised form 3 February 2015 introduced for an initially enjoyable activity. In Study 1, participants were asked to imagine gaining rewards
Accepted 29 March 2015
for doing an enjoyable activity and in Study 2, participants were given real rewards for doing an enjoyable activ-
Available online xxxx
ity, after which subsequent extrinsic motivation and intrinsic motivation were rated. Both studies revealed a sig-
Keywords:
nicant interaction effect of DT and self-reported extrinsic motivation in relation to subsequent self-reported
Dialectical thinking intrinsic motivation. The results of Study 3 replicated the general pattern again, using cultural membership
Extrinsic motivation (China vs. the U.S.) as a stand-in for DT. Findings indicate that DT mitigates the detrimental effects of extrinsic
Intrinsic motivation motivation on intrinsic motivation, identifying an important new individual difference moderator of the
Undermining effect undermining effect of extrinsic motivation on intrinsic motivation.
2015 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction construct, however, we rst provide more background information on


SDT.
Research has shown that peoples' intrinsic motivation to do an activ- Although early SDT research relied on the simple distinction be-
ity often decreases when they perceive external incentives for doing tween intrinsic and extrinsic motivation, later versions of the theory
that activity or for doing it well (Deci, 1971; Kruglanski, Friedman, & identied a broader spectrum of types of motivation. Today, the theory
Zeevi, 1971; Lepper, Greene, & Nisbett, 1973; Smith & Pittman, 1978; species a continuum of motivational internalization, anchored by ex-
Tang & Hall, 1995; Warneken & Tomasello, 2008). Cognitive Evaluation trinsic motivation (my behavior is caused by the incentives or pres-
Theory (CET), a facet of Self-determination Theory (SDT; Deci & Ryan, sures in the situation) on the non-internalized end, and intrinsic
1985), explained the undermining effect of external incentives by motivation (my behavior is caused by my interest in and enjoyment
considering people's perceived locus of causality for their own behavior of the activity) on the internalized end, in between lie introjected mo-
(Deci & Ryan, 1985; Ryan & Deci, 2000). If people perceive that their tivation (in which behavior is felt to be caused by non-integrated parts
personal interests and desires are the cause of their actions, then they of the self) and identied motivation (in which behavior is felt to be
tend to enjoy the activity; however, if they perceive that their actions caused by the self, but is not necessarily enjoyable; introjected and iden-
are caused by external contingencies or incentives, then they may feel tied motivation are not considered in the current article). Ryan and
controlled and thus the intrinsic motivation may be spoiled. Notably, ac- Connell (1989) supported the motivational internalization concept by
cording to CET, external rewards or incentives do not necessarily under- showing that the types of motivation conform to a simplex correlational
mine intrinsic motivation; what matters are people's perceptions of pattern, in which any two adjacent dimensions on the continuum corre-
those incentives, or the induced extrinsic motivation. Although situa- late positively with one another, and the two extreme dimensions cor-
tions containing external incentives are more likely, on average, to be relate negatively with each other. The typically observed negative
perceived as controlling and coercive, people can differ in their propen- correlation between intrinsic and extrinsic motivation is consistent
sity to perceive situations as controlling (Deci & Ryan, 1985), differences with the undermining effect discussed above; as perceived extrinsic
which can affect intrinsic motivation. This article will examine one such motivation increases, perceived intrinsic motivation tends to decrease.
individual difference, namely, dialectical thinking. Before turning to this Again, however, this is an on average phenomenon, and in some
cases perceived extrinsic and intrinsic motivation could be uncorrelated
Corresponding author at: 16 Hill Hall, University of Missouri, Columbia, MO 65211,
or even positively correlated (Cameron, Pierce, Banko, & Gear, 2005).
United States. Tel.: +1 602 568 3041. However, few studies have examined the moderators of the association.
E-mail address: yl6df@mail.missouri.edu (Y. Li). In this research we examined one potential individual difference

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2015.03.019
1041-6080/ 2015 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
90 Y. Li et al. / Learning and Individual Differences 39 (2015) 8995

moderator of the relationship between extrinsic incentives and intrinsic may be able to avoid or sidestep such dissonance, thereby maintaining
motivation, namely, dialectical thinking (DT). their intrinsic motivation. Notably, our research does not attempt to dis-
tinguish between these three explanations of undermining, as all three
1.1. Dialectical thinking and its role in the undermining effect make similar predictions for our purposes.
In sum, a person low in DT, engaging in one or the other thinking,
Cognitive developmental psychologists have found that there are re- may conclude that they are primarily motivated by the salient external
liable individual differences in how adults engage in various types of incentive(s), rather than by their own intrinsic interest. This is because
complex reasoning. One such difference is relevant in situations involv- their lay theories (Levy, Ramrez, Rosenthal, & Karafantis, 2013; Plaks,
ing potentially contradictory information. In such situations some peo- Levy, & Dweck, 2009) concerning the relationship between intrinsic and
ple only use formal thinking, that involves either fully rejecting or external motivation better ts the typical simplex pattern, in which
fully accepting one side of a contradiction by polarizing contradictory external motivation and intrinsic motivation are negatively correlated
propositions, and then choosing only one proposition to believe. In con- (Ryan & Connell, 1989). Thus the introduction of external incentives
trast, other people have developed dialectical thinking that involves may affect their anticipated desire to do the activity. In contrast, a per-
accepting seeming contradictions by viewing things in a multiple- son high in DT may be able to accommodate or compartmentalize the
dimensional way (Bassett, 2006; Peng & Nisbett, 1999; Yang, 2008). contradictory information and thus perceive that his/her behavior is
Dialectical thinkers believe that the world is composed of many con- motivated by both intrinsic interest and external rewards. In other
tradictions and is in constant change, and they are attentive to the cir- words, intrinsic and external motivation may be uncorrelated in their
cumstances of the object and the relationships that involve the object lay theories. Hence, their anticipated desire to do an activity may be un-
when making attributions, and rarely categorize objects into a right or affected by proffered extrinsic motivation. Furthermore, since dialecti-
wrong group (Nisbett, Peng, Choi, & Norenzayan, 2001, also see cal thinkers also adapt their cognitions and behaviors to new contexts
Spencer-Rodgers, Williams, & Peng, 2010, for a review). Dialectical more exibly than non-dialectical thinkers (Ji, Nisbett, & Su, 2001), it
thinkers not only perceive other objects and people in a multi- may be possible for them to reevaluate the situation and reinstate an in-
dimensional way, they also perceive their own personality as containing ternal locus of causality for later behavior after the external motivators
sometimes-contradictory factors (e.g., I am sometimes extraverted and I are taken away, such that any suppressed intrinsic interest will rebound.
am sometimes introverted) and their beliefs and behaviors can thus be For example, people high on DT are able to think, I am doing this for re-
more exible and amenable to change when the context has changed wards, but I also can do this because of my interest later or I was doing
(Boucher, 2011; Spencer-Rodgers, Williams, et al., 2010). With regard this before for rewards, but now I can do it because of my interest.
to emotional experience, dialectical thinkers are more comfortable
with mixed emotions than non-dialectical thinkers; though they tend 1.2. Current studies
to perceive more emotional complexity, especially in ambiguous situa-
tions (Leu et al., 2010). Research also showed that DT helps people for- Our three studies tested the hypothesized role of DT in moderating
mulate more exible coping strategies under changing or complex the association between external motivation and reported intrinsic mo-
circumstances (Cheng, 2009). Furthermore, in cultures where dialecti- tivation thereafter. We tested this hypothesis using a survey with imag-
cal thinking is prevalent, reports of unpleasant emotions have been inary reward scenarios (Study 1), an experiment which introduced real
found to be only weakly negatively associated, and sometimes non- rewards (Study 2), and a scenario study with cross-cultural samples
associated, with reports of pleasant emotions (e.g., Kitayama, Markus, (Study 3). Imaginary scenarios were chosen in Study 1 to measure
& Kurokawa, 2000; Spencer-Rodgers, Peng, & Wang, 2010). people's lay theories about the association between intrinsic motivation
Regarding the undermining effect, the introduction of external in- and external motivation. The use of real rewards in Study 2 allowed us
centives into a situation would normally be expected to increase extrin- to test whether the lay theory effect extended to doing a real activity.
sic motivation and thus undermine intrinsic motivation. However, we Study 3 was undertaken because previous research has found cultural
hypothesized that DT helps people resist this tendency for several pos- differences in the prevalent thinking style (Peng & Nisbett, 1999;
sible reasons. First, Cognitive Evaluation Theory (Deci & Ryan, 1985) Spencer-Rodgers & Peng, 2004; also see Spencer-Rodgers, Williams,
states that undermined intrinsic motivation results from a change in et al., 2010, for a review). Easterners were found to use DT to a greater
the person's perceived locus of causality from internal to external, and extent, while Westerners were found to use non-dialectical or formal
the psychological feeling of pressure that accompanies this change. Peo- logical thinking to a greater extent. Thus in Study 3 we extended the
ple high in DT may be able to experience increased external motivation, analysis to cultural differences, focusing on two cultural groups
but not perceive this as sources of pressure; in their minds, external mo- known to differ in DT (North Americans and East Asians). We hypothe-
tivations and intrinsic motivations can co-exist. Or, even if they do feel sized that the Asian group, presumably higher in DT, would evidence
pressure, this may not necessarily impact their positive emotions and less undermining by external motivation in imagined scenarios.
motivations, as suggested by Kitayama et al.'s (2000) research.
Another reason involves the over-justication theory account of the
undermining effect. Over-justication theory (Lepper et al., 1973) pro- 2. Study 1
poses that when external motivators are present along with intrinsic
motivations, people tend to adopt the more salient factor (proffered re- In Study 1 participants were presented with four initially enjoyable
wards) as the explanation for their behavior, discounting the impor- activities and then asked to imagine that rewards had been introduced
tance of the original intrinsic motivation. The behavior is over- for doing the activities. Afterwards they rated their extrinsic motivation
justied, and the attribution of intrinsic motivation is not needed. Peo- and estimated how their intrinsic interest in the activity would change
ple high in DT may be able to avoid such discounting, because they can after the reward was offered.
accept multiple reasons for doing things. Finally, cognitive dissonance
theory (Heider, 1958) provides a third reason for expecting DT to 2.1. Method
have impact. From this theoretical perspective, people feel dissonance
when their actions (I will get money for doing this) are inconsistent 2.1.1. Participants and procedure
with their attitudes (I am doing this because I enjoy it). The disso- Participants were 55 American undergraduate students who identi-
nance creates a non-conscious desire to change one of the contradictory ed themselves as European American (24 males and 31 females, mean
propositions, which causes them to revise the initial attitude. Because age = 20.8, SD = 1.4) attending the University of Missouri. They were
people high in DT are comfortable with contradictions, however, they all from an upper division psychology class, and received extra credit
Y. Li et al. / Learning and Individual Differences 39 (2015) 8995 91

in the class in exchange for their participation. The survey was present- regression model was employed with intrinsic motivation as the depen-
ed on-line. dent variable, and DT, extrinsic motivation, and DT extrinsic motiva-
After logging on to the survey participants rst completed demo- tion entered simultaneously as predictors. All variables were
graphic information and the DT measure (described below). Afterwards standardized before use. Neither main effect was signicant, however
they were asked to type the name of a particular web-site that they the hypothesized interaction effect was signicant, = .36, t(51) =
enjoyed browsing. They were then asked to imagine that a market re- 2.60, p = 0.01. Post-hoc probing of the interaction (Aiken & West,
search rm would pay them $10 a day to browse that site and answer 1991) indicated that for people whose DT score was one standard devi-
various questions about it. Then were then asked two questions: extrin- ation above the mean, extrinsic motivation was not signicantly associ-
sic motivation (During the market study, I would go to the website be- ated with intrinsic motivation, = .44, t(51) = 1.92, p = .06. In
cause I want the money, rated on a 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly contrast, for people whose DT rating was one standard deviation
agree) scale), and subsequent intrinsic motivation (After the study was below the mean, extrinsic motivation negatively predicted intrinsic mo-
over, my interest in browsing the website would probably where tivation, = .51, t(51) = 2.29, p = .03. Fig. 1 illustrates the plotted
the response options were decrease a lot, decrease a little, not interaction with the means of intrinsic motivation at different dialectical
change, increase a little, and increase a lot). We chose to use thinking levels (1SD and 1SD) and different extrinsic motivation level
these single items in Study 1 because they are prototypical exemplars (1SD and 1SD).
of external and intrinsic motivation, respectively, allowing us to exam-
ine the lay theory effects of DT. Again, our hypothesis was that DT 2.3. Brief discussion
would moderate the association between these two responses such
that those high in DT would evidence a less negative correlation be- Our central prediction was that DT would moderate the association
tween extrinsic and intrinsic motivation, compared to those low in DT. between rated extrinsic motivation and rated intrinsic motivation, in a
Participants then repeated this procedure for three further cued en- hypothetical context of how one would feel about doing formerly enjoy-
joyable activities: playing a sport they enjoy, playing a game they like, able activities after external incentives had been introduced. The results
and watching a movie they enjoy; in each case, after describing the ac- supported this prediction. The extrinsic motivation of people with rela-
tivity they were asked to imagine that an incentive had been intro- tively high DT scores was nearly positively associated with their intrin-
duced, after which they again rated the two items above. The four sic motivation, while the association was signicantly negative for
extrinsic motivation and the four intrinsic motivation items were each people with relatively low DT scores. This result suggests that DT can
averaged to create overall extrinsic and intrinsic motivation scores. buffer individuals from having their intrinsic motivation undermined
by the extrinsic motivation induced by external incentives.
2.1.2. Dialectical thinking One limitation of Study 1 is its usage of imaginary scenarios instead
Before participants started the imaginary tasks, they completed the of actual situations involving extrinsic incentives. Participants' real re-
32 item Dialectical Self Scale (DSS, Spencer-Rodgers et al., 2010), using sponse or motivation might not be in line with their lay theories. We
a 5-point Likert scale with higher scores indicating higher DT. The thus conducted a second study that introduced actual monetary re-
scale measures three components of DT: contradiction (e.g., When I wards for doing a particular concrete activity.
hear two sides of an argument, I often agree with both), cognitive
change (e.g., I often nd that my beliefs and attitudes will change 3. Study 2
under different contexts) and behavior change (e.g., I often change
the way I am, depending on who I am with). We averaged the three In Study 2, we wanted participants to experience doing an activity
subscales together based on a principal components analysis suggesting known to be intrinsically enjoyable. We returned to the Soma puzzles
that they formed a single factor. The Cronbach's alpha of the aggregate of the early intrinsic motivation research (Deci, 1971, 1972). The Soma
scale was 0.88. puzzle is a 3-dimensional puzzle that most people enjoy (Deci & Ryan,

2.2. Results

2.2.1. Preliminary analyses


There was one participant who did not complete the imaginary
tasks, and another participant who did not complete the DT scale.
Those two participants' data were excluded, leaving 53 participants
for the analyses reported below. Age and gender are ignored below, be-
cause they had no main effects on DT, extrinsic motivation or intrinsic
motivation, and they did not interact with DT or extrinsic motivation
when predicting intrinsic motivation. Descriptive statistics for the
Study 1 variables are shown in Table 1.

2.2.2. Tests of primary hypothesis


To examine the main hypothesis that DT moderates the association
between extrinsic motivation and intrinsic motivation, a multiple

Table 1
Descriptive statistics and variable inter-correlations from Studies 1 and 2.

Study 1 (n = 53) Study 2 (n = 27)

Variables M SD 1 2 M SD 1 2

1 Dialectical thinking 2.25 .44 3.39 .59


2 Extrinsic motivation 3.85 .85 .06 6.63 2.75 .37
3 Intrinsic motivation 3.14 .46 .14 .05 4.89 1.41 .00 .52**

Note: ** means p b .01. Fig. 1. Study 1: Plotted interaction in predicting intrinsic motivation.
92 Y. Li et al. / Learning and Individual Differences 39 (2015) 8995

1985). In Study 2, we also expanded our assessment of extrinsic and in- and correlations are shown in Table 1. All measures were standardized
trinsic motivation, using additional items, and we measured intrinsic in the analyses below.
motivation directly rather than asking about the change in interest.
Once again, we hypothesized that the intrinsic motivation of people 3.2.2. Tests of primary hypotheses
high in DT would be less affected by the induced extrinsic motivation.
3.2.2.1. Intrinsic motivation. A multiple regression with DT, extrinsic mo-
3.1. Method tivation, and their interaction as the predictors revealed a signicant
main effect of extrinsic motivation, = .45, t(23) = 2.46,
3.1.1. Participants p b 0.05, and a signicant interaction effect, = .38, t(23) = 2.22,
Participants were 27 undergraduates (13 males and 14 females, p b 0.05. Simple slope tests (Aiken & West, 1991) were conducted to
mean age = 18.7 SD = 1.1) attending the University of Missouri. They probe the signicant interaction effect. Results showed that the associ-
were introductory psychology students, and received required research ation between extrinsic motivation and the self-reported interest was
credit in exchange for their participation. signicantly negative when people's DT was one standard deviation
below the mean, = .78, t(23) = 4.16, p b 0.001, while the associ-
3.1.2. Procedure ation was not signicant when people's DT was one standard deviation
Participants were run one at a time. First, all participants completed above the mean, = .11, t(23) = .41, ns. Fig. 2 shows the plotted
a survey containing the DT measure. Then the experimenter showed the interaction with the means of intrinsic motivation at different dialectical
participant the Soma puzzle, which consists of several wooden pieces thinking levels ( 1SD and 1SD) and different extrinsic motivation
which can be arranged into various 3-dimensional shapes. Some 2- levels (1SD and 1SD).
dimensional pictures of shapes were shown. To make the task easier,
the rst important step by which the Soma pieces could be manipulated 3.3. Brief discussion
to arrive at those shapes was marked on the pictures. Participants were
then asked to try to construct 3 different shapes using the Soma puzzle Study 2 employed a real remunerative activity and tested the mod-
pieces. The type of rewards used has the same set-up as Deci's classic erating role of DT in the associations between extrinsic motivation
studies (e.g., Deci, 1971), which was classied as completion- and intrinsic motivation. The results of Study 2 replicated the results
contingent reward (Deci, Koestner, & Ryan, 1999). All participants of Study 1, that DT moderated the association between extrinsic motiva-
were told that they would have 10 min for each shape, and that they tion and intrinsic motivation. These results suggested that DT buffers
would earn $1 for each shape that they successfully constructed in people from the undermining effect of induced extrinsic motivation by
time. If a participant was unable to construct a shape in the 10 min, he remunerative activity, with regard to both their feelings of enjoyment.
or she was instructed to move on to the next shape. After the comple-
tion of the puzzle tasks, participants completed a post-task question- 4. Study 3
naire that included the measures of extrinsic motivation and intrinsic
motivation. Finally, participants were de-briefed. Although Studies 1 and 2 both demonstrated a moderating role of DT
in the association between extrinsic motivation and intrinsic interest, all
participants were from the same cultural context. Previous research
3.1.3. Measures
showed that East Asians are more likely to be dialectical thinkers than
Westerners (see Spencer-Rodgers, Williams, et al., 2010, for a review).
3.1.3.1. Dialectical thinking. These items were the same as those used
Specically, Chinese participants were found to have higher DT scores
in Study 1, measured with a 7 point Likert scale. The Cronbach's
than European Americans (Spencer-Rodgers & Peng, 2004). In Study 3,
alpha of the scale was 0.82 in Study 2.
we recruited both European Americans and Chinese as participants to

3.1.3.2. Extrinsic motivation. Participants' extrinsic motivation for playing


with the Soma puzzle was measured by two items. Specically, partici-
pants were asked to rate the extent to which they agree with the state-
ments I solved the Soma puzzles only because I wanted to get the
monetary reward and I solved the Soma puzzles only because the ex-
periment required me to, on a 7-point Likert scale, with 1 indicating
strong disagreement and 7 indicating strong agreement. The Cronbach's
alpha of this measure was .71.

3.1.3.3. Intrinsic motivation. The interest/enjoyment subscale of the In-


trinsic Motivation Inventory (IMI, e.g., Ryan, 1982; Ryan, Koestner, &
Deci, 1991) was used to assess participants' intrinsic motivation. This
measure has been used in previous laboratory studies on the
undermining effect of reward on intrinsic interest (e.g., Deci, 1971;
Wiechman & Gurland, 2009). It includes 7 items, one example being I
found the puzzle-solving activity very interesting. The Cronbach's
alpha of this measure was .97.

3.2. Results

3.2.1. Preliminary analyses


Age and gender had no main effects on intrinsic motivation, and did
not interact with DT or extrinsic motivation when predicting intrinsic
motivation. Thus they were ignored below. Every participant completed
at least 1 shape, and thus received at least 1 dollar. Descriptive statistics Fig. 2. Study 2: Plotted interaction in predicting intrinsic motivation.
Y. Li et al. / Learning and Individual Differences 39 (2015) 8995 93

test the moderating role of DT by investigating cultural differences in ns. Fig. 3 shows the mean difference of intrinsic motivation between
the association between extrinsic motivation and intrinsic motivation. scenarios and culture.
Instead of assessing self-reported extrinsic motivation, we created
paired scenarios which included a scenario with rewards and a scenario
4.3. Brief discussion
without rewards. We reasoned that participants should have more ex-
trinsic motivation when imagining the scenario with rewards than
Study 3 used a very different approach to test the moderating role of
when imagining the scenario without rewards. We conducted mixed
DT in the association between extrinsic motivation and intrinsic motiva-
2 (Culture: Chinese vs. European American) 2 (Scenario: reward vs.
tion. Results were in line with what we found in Studies 1 and 2, in that
no reward) analyses, with repeated measures on the second factor, to
only the European-American sample, showed a negative association be-
test our interaction hypothesis, namely, that the intrinsic motivation
tween intrinsic motivation and extrinsic motivation, not the Chinese
of the Chinese participants would be less affected by the reward/no re-
sample. Specically, European-Americans reported greater intrinsic
ward factor.
motivation in the no reward scenarios while Chinese reported greater
intrinsic motivation in the reward scenarios. Given the known differ-
4.1. Method
ences in DT between these two cultural groups, this supports our hy-
pothesis in a very different way.
4.1.1. Participants and procedure
Participants were 72 Chinese undergraduate students (15 male and
57 female, mean age = 20.4, SD = 0.7) from the Beijing Normal Univer- 5. General discussion
sity and 182 American undergraduates who identied themselves as
European American (62 male and 120 female, mean age = 19.9, The current study contributes new information concerning the neg-
SD = 1.4) from the University of Missouri; they were all taking an intro- ative association between extrinsic motivation and intrinsic motivation,
ductory psychology class. The questionnaire was translated into Chinese showing that DT moderates this association. The intrinsic motivation of
by a bilingual person, and then back-translated by a different bilingual people higher in DT was reduced less by extrinsic motivation than the
person; discrepancies in meaning were discussed and resolved to intrinsic motivation of people lower in DT. This occurred when partici-
yield an equivalent questionnaire in both languages. The brief question- pants imagined gaining rewards for doing enjoyable activities (Study
naire contained four imaginary scenarios, which were similar to the sce- 1) and when they were given real rewards for doing interesting activi-
narios used in Study 1. In two scenarios, participants were asked to ties (Study 2). The results of Study 3 replicated the general pattern,
imagine participating in an interesting activity (e.g., ice-skating) for using cultural membership as a stand-in for DT. DT stems from the belief
which a reward was provided. In the other two paired scenarios, partic- that the world is complex and is changing over time, such that particular
ipants were asked to imagine participating in the same activity with no situations can be interpreted in multiple ways (Spencer-Rodgers, Wil-
rewards. After they had read the description of a scenario and had imag- liams & Peng). In a rewarding situation, people who are low on DT
ined the situation, they rated the extent to which their intrinsic motiva- may believe that since they are doing the task in the present for re-
tion would change after they had done the activity compared with their wards, this should continue to be the case in the future. In contrast, peo-
initial motivation (After you have participated in this activity, your in- ple who are high on DT may believe that their intrinsic motivation and
terest in this activity that you were recently really interested in would extrinsic motivation are not competing, and their motivations can be
). Participants answered on a 5-point Likert scale (1 = decrease a quickly adjusted according to a new situation. In the present studies,
lot, 2 = decrease a little, 3 = not change, 4 = increase a little, 5 = in- participants with high DT did not report less extrinsic motivation in
crease a lot). The two ratings were averaged for each type of scenario the rewarding situations than those who are low on DT. This indicates
(reward, no reward). The two ratings were transformed for the results that people who are high on DT are indeed affected by rewards; howev-
below by subtracting 3, so that the 0 point indicates no change. er, what makes them different is their ability to maintain their intrinsic
motivation.
4.2. Results

The two samples did not signicantly differ in gender composition.


They differed in age, F(1, 249) = 44.50, p b .01, with the Chinese sample
being slightly younger (Chinese M = 19.56, SD = .77; American M =
20.14, SD = 1.01). Gender had neither main nor interactive effects on
intrinsic motivation, but age was signicantly associated with intrinsic
motivation in the reward condition, so age was entered as a covariate
in the following analysis.
A mixed MANOVA revealed a signicant effect of scenario, F(1,
242) = 7.39, p b 0.01: across the two cultures, people reported more
positive changes in intrinsic motivation in the reward scenario (M =
.43, SD = .05) than in the no reward scenario (M = .29, SD = .04).
The between-group effect for culture was not signicant, F(1, 242) =
2.24, ns. Most importantly, a signicant culture (European American
vs. Chinese) scenario (reward vs. no reward) interaction emerged,
Wilks' Lambda value = .95, Pillai's Trace value = .05, F(1, 242) =
11.99, p = .001. Post-hoc t-tests showed that the two samples did not
differ in their intrinsic motivation in the no reward scenarios,
F(1,247) = 1.27, ns, while there was a signicant group difference in
the reward scenarios, F(1,252) = 19.591, p b 0.01. In the reward
scenarios, the Chinese participants reported more intrinsic motivation
than in the no reward scenarios, t(71) = 5.33, p b 0.001; however, the
European American participants reported less intrinsic motivation in Fig. 3. Study 3: Intrinsic motivation change for the American and Chinese participants in
the reward scenarios than in the no reward scenarios, t(176) = 1.63, the reward versus no reward conditions.
94 Y. Li et al. / Learning and Individual Differences 39 (2015) 8995

One important implication of the results of our research is that Additional study limitations include the fact that we only employed
people's perceptions of reward stimuli may be more important than imaginary scenarios in Study 1, although Study 2 rectied this with a
the reward stimuli themselves. CET (Deci & Ryan, 1985) states that peo- real situation. Study 1 and Study 2 both only focused on rewarding sit-
ple interpret rewards in varying ways, sometimes as conveying uations, with no comparison to no reward control situations; this was
competence information, and sometimes as conveying information because we were only interested in individual differences in responses
about their ability to choose, and that the nature of these perceptions in- to the association between extrinsic motivation and intrinsic motiva-
uences people's intrinsic motivation (Reeve, Nix, & Hamm, 2003; tion, and a rewarding situation just to induce participants' extrinsic mo-
Vansteenkiste & Deci, 2003). One recent study addressed a relevant in- tivation. Another limitation was that participants' DT was not measured
dividual difference, showing that individual causality orientations mod- in Study 3, since we relied on prior ndings of Chinese/US differences.
erated the undermining effect of rewards (Hagger & Chatzisarantis, Future studies can examine whether participants' dispositional DT me-
2010). These investigators found that people who were dispositionally diates cultural differences in reward effects on intrinsic motivation. Fi-
inclined to perceive situations as autonomy supportive and informa- nally, N for these studies was fairly small, especially in Studies 1 and
tional showed less of an undermining effect by rewards than people 2; thus these effects in particular require further replication.
who are inclined to perceive situations as controlling. The current stud- Despite these limitations, the present studies discovered a new
ies explored a new type of individual difference moderator, DT. Instead moderator in the association between external incentives and intrinsic
of helping people feel less externally motivated in rewarding situations, motivation. DT may help people tolerate the seeming contradiction be-
DT may reduce the undermining effect by helping people segregate ex- tween salient external incentives and subsequent intrinsic motivation,
ternal and intrinsic motivation. In our studies, participants who were and help to restore their intrinsic motivation in the new situation
high on DT still reported high extrinsic motivation, but this motivation when rewards are removed. The ndings suggest that individual cogni-
apparently did not negatively impact their intrinsic motivation. tive thinking styles inuence people's perception of external incentives,
The present research provided the rst evidence that peoples' dia- and thus environments which are highly detrimental to some people
lectical thinking style moderates the effect of external motivation on in- are not necessarily harmful for other people.
trinsic motivation, extending previous studies of person by situation
interaction effects upon people's motivation. Our ndings have impor-
Acknowledgments
tant implications for applied areas such as education, work, and athletic
competition, where tangible rewards and strong competition are prev-
We thank Todd Schachtman for the comments on the rst version of
alent. Dialectical thinking apparently buffers people from the potential-
the manuscript, and David Bergin, Xi Bi, and Marc Halusic for their help
ly undermining impact of external incentives, by giving them the belief
on various aspects of the project.
that intrinsic and extrinsic motivation can co-exist. People who are high
on DT are thus enabled to maintain their original interest in an activity,
in circumstances where external motivators become salient. Presum- References
ably, DT can be primed or trained (Paletz and Peng, 2009; Cheng,
Aiken, L.S., & West, S.G. (1991). Multiple regression: Testing and interpreting interactions.
2009) and such a training may help students, workers and athletes Thousand Oaks, CA, US: Sage Publications, Inc.
maintain their internal passion when external motivators (e.g., rewards Bassett, C. (2006). Laughing at gilded butteries: Integrating wisdom, development and
or deadlines) are unavoidable. learning. In C. Hoare (Ed.), Oxford handbook of adult development and learning
(pp. 281306). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Boucher, H.C. (2011). The dialectical self-concept II: Cross-role and within-role consisten-
5.1. Limitations and future research cy, well-being, self-certainty, and authenticity. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology,
42(7), 12511271. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0022022110383316.
Cameron, J., Pierce, W.D., Banko, K.M., & Gear, A. (2005). Achievement-based rewards and
Compared to research on the effects of external incentives or per- intrinsic motivation: A test of cognitive mediators. Journal of Educational Psychology,
ceived extrinsic motivation on intrinsic motivation, studies exploring 97, 641655. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.97.4.641.
the moderating roles of individual differences have been limited. Thus, Cheng, C. (2009). Dialectical thinking and coping exibility: A multimethod approach.
Journal of Personality, 77, 471494. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-6494.2008.
there are many possible directions for future studies. One avenue 00555.x.
would be to look for mediators of the present moderation effect. Poten- Deci, E.L. (1971). Effects of externally mediated rewards on intrinsic motivation. Journal of
tial mediators could include some situational perception or feeling mea- Personality and Social Psychology, 18, 105115. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0030644.
Deci, E.L. (1972). Intrinsic motivation, extrinsic motivation, and inequity. Journal of
sures, for example, feelings of being controlled or coerced. According to
Personality and Social Psychology, 22, 113120. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0032355.
SDT, when people feel that the situation is controlling, their intrinsic in- Deci, E.L., Koestner, R., & Ryan, R.M. (1999). A meta-analytic review of experiments exam-
terest will be undermined (Deci & Ryan, 1985). However, people who ining the effects of extrinsic rewards on intrinsic motivation. Psychological Bulletin,
are high on DT may feel less controlled in a rewarding situation or 125, 627668. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.125.6.627.
Deci, E.L., & Ryan, R.M. (1985). Intrinsic motivation and self-determination in human behav-
may not persist in their feeling of being controlled after the rewards ior. New York: Plenum.
have been removed. Cognitive dissonance in the rewarding situation Glasford, D.E., Pratto, F., & Dovidio, J.F. (2008). Intragroup dissonance: Responses to
might also be a potential mediator. It can perhaps be assessed via phys- ingroup violation of personal values. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44,
10571064. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2007.10.004.
iological arousal indicators, self-reported discomfort or brain activity Hagger, M.S., & Chatzisarantis, N.L.D. (2010). Causality orientations moderate the
changes (Glasford, Pratto, & Dovidio, 2008; Jarcho, Berkman, & undermining effect of rewards on intrinsic motivation. Journal of Experimental Social
Lieberman, 2011; McMillen & Geiselman, 1974; Rodriguez & Morales, Psychology, 47, 485489. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2010.10.010.
Heider, F. (1958). The psychology of interpersonal relations. New York: John Wiley.
1972; Suinn, 1965). People who are high on DT may feel little arousal Jarcho, J.M., Berkman, E.T., & Lieberman, M.D. (2011). The neural basis of rationalization:
or cognitive dissonance in doing an initially interesting task under a re- Cognitive dissonance reduction during decision-making. Social Cognitive and Affective
warding situation, which might explain why their intrinsic motivation Neuroscience, 6, 460467. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsq054.
Ji, L. -J., Nisbett, R.E., & Su, Y. (2001). Culture, change, and prediction. Psychological Science,
was less likely to be undermined by the extrinsic motivation. 12, 450456. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1467-9280.00384.
Another study limitation is that we only tested the role of DT in the Kitayama, S., Markus, H.R., & Kurokawa, M. (2000). Culture, emotion, and well-being:
association between extrinsic motivation and intrinsic motivation, Good feelings in Japan and the United States. Cognition & Emotion, 14, 93124.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/026999300379003.
using situations where tangible rewards (i.e., money) were introduced.
Kruglanski, A.W., Friedman, I., & Zeevi, G. (1971). The effects of extrinsic incentive on
However, there are many other sources of extrinsic motivation, for ex- some qualitative aspects of task performance. Journal of Personality, 39, 606617 (Re-
ample, desires for promotions in the work place or desires to earn trieved from http://login.ezproxy1.lib.asu.edu/login?url=http://search.proquest.
teachers' praise in class. Future research can consider testing the role com/docview/615806207?accountid=4485).
Lepper, M.R., Greene, D., & Nisbett, R.E. (1973). Undermining children's intrinsic interest
of DT with different sources of the extrinsic motivation or in other po- with extrinsic reward: A test of the overjustication hypothesis. Journal of
tentially detrimental environments. Personality and Social Psychology, 28, 129137. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0035519.
Y. Li et al. / Learning and Individual Differences 39 (2015) 8995 95

Leu, J., Mesquita, B., Ellsworth, P. C., ZhiYong, Z., Huijuan, Y., Buchtel, E., & Masuda, T. Ryan, R.M., Koestner, R., & Deci, E.L. (1991). Varied forms of persistence: When free-
(2010). Situational differences in dialectical emotions: Boundary conditions in a cul- choice behavior is not intrinsically motivated. Motivation and Emotion, 15, 185205.
tural comparison of north americans and east asians. Cognition and Emotion, 24(3), http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF00995170.
419435. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02699930802650911. Smith, T.W., & Pittman, T.S. (1978). Reward, distraction, and the overjustication effect.
Levy, Sheri R., Ramrez, Luisa, Rosenthal, Lisa, & Karafantis, Dina M. (2013). In navigating Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 36(5), 565572. http://dx.doi.org/10.
the social world: What infants, children, and other species can teach us. In Mahzarin 1037/0022-3514.36.5.565.
R. Banaji, & Susan A. Gelman (Eds.), New York, NY, US: Oxford University Press. Spencer-Rodgers, J., Peng, K., & Wang, L. (2010a). Dialecticism and the co-occurrence of
McMillen, D.L., & Geiselman, J.H. (1974). Effect of cognitive dissonance on alpha frequen- positive and negative emotions. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 41, 109115.
cy activity: The search for dissonance. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 1, http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0022022109349508.
150151. Spencer-Rodgers, J., Williams, M.J., & Peng, K. (2004). The dialectical self: Contradiction,
Nisbett, R.E., Peng, K., Choi, I., & Norenzayan, A. (2001). Culture and systems of thought: change, and holism in the East Asian self-concept. In R. M. Sorrentino, D. Cohen, J.
Holistic versus analytic cognition. Psychological Review, 108, 291310. http://dx.doi. M. Olsen, & M. P. Zanna (Eds.), Culture and social behavior: The Ontario Symposium,
org/10.1037//0033-295X.108.2.291. Vol. 10. (pp. 227250). NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum: Mahwah.
Paletz, S. B. F., & Peng, K. (2009). Problem nding and contradiction: Examining the rela- Spencer-Rodgers, J., Srivastava, S., Boucher, H. C., English, T., Paletz, S. B., & Peng, K. (2010).
tionship between nave dialectical thinking, ethnicity, and creativity. Creativity The dialectical self scale. Unpublished data, as cited in Spencer-Rodgers, J., Peng, K.,
Research Journal, 21, 139151. Wang, L., & Hou, Y. (2004). Dialectical self-esteem and EastWest differences in psy-
Peng, K., & Nisbett, R.E. (1999). Culture, dialectics, and reasoning about contradiction. chological well-being. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30, 1416-1432. doi:
American Psychologist, 54, 741754. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037//0003-066X.54.9.741. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0146167204264243.
Plaks, J.E., Levy, S.R., & Dweck, C.S. (2009). Lay theories of personality: Cornerstones of Spencer-Rodgers, J., Williams, M.J., & Peng, K. (2010c). Cultural differences in expec-
meaning in social cognition. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 3(6), tations of change and tolerance for contradiction: A decade of empirical re-
10691081. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1751-9004.2009.00222.x. search. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 14, 296312. http://dx.doi.org/
Reeve, J., Nix, G., & Hamm, D. (2003). Testing models of the experience of self- 10.1177/108886831036298.
determination in intrinsic motivation and the conundrum of choice. Journal of Suinn, R.M. (1965). Anxiety and cognitive dissonance. Journal of General Psychology, 73,
Educational Psychology, 95(2), 375392. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.95.2. 113116. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00221309.1965.9711258.
375. Tang, S., & Hall, V.C. (1995). The overjustication effect: A meta-analysis. Applied Cognitive
Rodriguez, Kant A., & Morales, d. B. (1972). The potential of cognitive dissonance to in- Psychology, 9(5), 365404. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/acp.2350090502.
duce anxiety. Revista Latinoamericana De Psicologa, 4, 103115 (Retrieved from Vansteenkiste, M., & Deci, E.L. (2003). Competitively contingent rewards and intrinsic
http://login.ezproxy1.lib.asu.edu/login?url=http://search.proquest.com/docview/ motivation: Can losers remain motivated? Motivation and Emotion, 27, 273299.
615838848?accountid=4485). http://dx.doi.org/10.1023/A:1026259005264.
Ryan, R.M. (1982). Control and information in the intrapersonal sphere: An extension of Warneken, F., & Tomasello, M. (2008). Extrinsic rewards undermine altruistic tendencies
cognitive evaluation theory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 43, 450461. in 20-month-olds. Developmental Psychology, 44(6), 17851788. http://dx.doi.org/10.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037//0022-3514.43.3.450. 1037/a0013860.
Ryan, R. M., & Connell, J. P. (1989). Perceived locus of causality and internalization: Exam- Wiechman, B.M., & Gurland, S.T. (2009). What happens during the free-choice period?
ining reasons for acting in two domains. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Evidence of a polarizing effect of extrinsic rewards on intrinsic motivation. Journal
57, 749761. of Research in Personality, 43, 716719. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2009.03.008.
Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2000). Intrinsic and extrinsic motivations: Classic denitions and Yang, S. -Y. (2008). A process view of wisdom. Journal of Adult Development, 15, 6275.
new directions. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 25(1), 5467. http://dx.doi.org/ http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10804-008-9037-8.
10.1006/ceps.1999.1020.

Você também pode gostar