Você está na página 1de 13

The Journal of Social Psychology, 1964, 64, 65-76.

COMMUNICATOR CREDIBILITY AND COMMUNICATION


DISCREPANCY AS DETERMINANTS OF OPINION CHANGE* ^
Boston University

T O N G - H E CHOO

A. INTRODUCTION

In recent years there has been an increasing number of experimental stud-


ies where the variables involved in opinion change are successfully isolated
and independently manipulated in order to assess the net effect of each of the
variables. However, more attempts should be made to examine the inter-
acting effects of known influences on opinion in order to assess their com-
bined as well as their separate effects. The main interest of the present study
is to vary experimentally the degree of communicator credibility and com-
munication discrepancy, thereby investigating the main and the interaction
effects of these two variables on opinion change.
The studies on communicator credibility in opinion change indicate, in gen-
eral, that the more credible the communication source, the greater is the
opinion change toward the communication advocated (8, 12).
Opinion change is also a function of the discrepancy between the confi-
municator's opinion position and the recipient's initial opinion position on a
given issue. Early studies by Ewing (1) and Sims (16) demonstrated that
there is a positive relationship between such discrepancy and opinion change
of the recipient. These studies were criticized because they made no effort to
control the possible artifacts, "ceiling" and "regression" effects. However,
the generalizations are still valid even after these artifacts are controlled (3,
7, 17).
On the basis of the works reviewed above it is hypothesized that:
1. Greater extent of opinion change toward the communication advocated
is expected when the communicator is high-credible rather than low-credible.
2. Greater extent of opinion change toward the communication advocated
is expected when the communication discrepancy is large rather than small.
3. When the communicator is high-credible, greater extent of opinion

Received in the Editoriai Office on December 6, 1962. Copyright, 1964, by The


Journal Press.
1 This is part of a doctoral dissertation submitted to the Graduate School of Bos-
ton University. The author wishes to thank Dr. Walter Weiss, whose support and
advice throughout the study were extremely valuable.
65
66 JOURNAL OF SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY

change toward the communication advocated is expected for large-discrepancy


groups rather than for small-discrepancy groups. When the communicator
credibility is low, the expectation is that (a) there will be no significant differ-
ence between large- and small-discrepancy groups in opinion change; or (b)
the opinion change of the large-discrepancy group will be significantly greater
than that of the small-discrepancy group, but both in the same direction; or
(c) the effect on the small-discrepancy group will be in the opposite direction
to that of the large-discrepancy group. If the latter should occur, then this
reversal interaction might well eliminate the main effect of discrepancy.
B. METHOD
In order to test the above hypotheses, a before-after design involving four
experimental and four control groups was employed. The overall design of
the study was to present communications which were identical except for the
two experimental variables: the High- and Low-Credibility of the source of
the communication, and the Large- and Small-Discrepancy manipulated by
the anchor statements.
The issue referred to the .causal relationship between cigarette smoking and
lung cancer.
The 5s, who were exposed to both sessions, were 216 undergraduates from
Boston University.
1. Before Session
The questionnaire booklet was presented to the 5s in a group as an opinion
survey on current social issues. The booklet consisted of (a) opinion state-
ments on current social issues to be rated in terms of 5's own opinion on the
issues (11-point scale ranging from zerocomplete disagreement, through
5uncertain, to 10complete agreement) ; (b) topic headings to be rated
in terms of the importance of the topic to the 5 (11-point scale ranging from
zeronot important, through 5moderately important, to 10extremely
important) ; and (c) names of persons and organizations to be rated in terms
of "expertness" and "trustworthiness" of the source as a communicator on
each topic. The scale for "expertness," which was defined as "the amount of
knowledge the communication source has on the topic concerned," had 5
points on a descriptive scale: "the source knowsalmost nothing about the
topic, only a few facts, some of the facts, most of the facts, all the facts."
The "trustworthiness" of the source referred to its being a fair and unbiased
communicator of the facts, and the scale ranged from "not trustworthy"
zero, through "moderately trustworthy"5, to "extremely trustwor-
thy"10.
TONG-HE CHOO 67

The measure of S's initial opinion position was determined from the ratings
on the statement: "Cigarette smoking is one of the causes of lung cancer"
(mean = 7.09). The importance of the topic of "cigarette smoking and lung
cancer" was also determined on the basis of the S's ratings in this session and
was found to be important (mean ^ 6 . 2 3 ) . Among the variety of sources
that were rated, "Head of the National Cancer Institute" and "Public
Health Service" were rated as very high in trustworthiness (means: 8.64 and
8.34 respectively). On the other hand, "American Tobacco Company" and
"Director of Tobacco Industry Public Relations Committee" were rated
considerably lower in trustworthiness (means: 2.98 and 3.92 respectively).
In the rating of the communicator's expertness, these four sources were all in
the category of "source likely to know most of the facts." Therefore, it was
decided to use "Dr. W. C. Hueper, Head of the Environmental Cancer Sec-
tion of the National Cancer Institute, Public Health Service," as the High-
Credible communicator, and "Mr. J. P. Richards, Director of Tobacco In-
dustry Public Relations Committee," as the Low-Credible source.

2. After Session
In this session, eight different booklets were randomly distributed to the
5s. The distribution of booklets resulted in the assignment of Ss to either
experimental or control groups (in an approximate two-to-one ratio) and to
the following treatments: High-Credibility with Large-Discrepancy (HC-
L D ) , High-Credibility with Small-Discrepancy (HC-SD), Low-Credibility
with Large-Discrepancy (LC-LD), and Low-Credibility with Small-Dis-
crepancy (LC-SD).
The after session was held two to four weeks after the before session, and
was administered by different personnel in order to prevent the Ss from as-
sociating this session with the before session.
The booklet consisted of six pages of mimeographed communication ad-
vocating essentially no causal relationship between cigarette smoking and
lung cancer, and the scales to measure: (a) 5s' judgments of the communi-
cator's position (11-point scale ranging from zerocomplete acceptance,
through 5uncertain or in doubt, to 10complete rejection of the state-
ment "cigarette smoking is one of the causes of lung cancer"); (b) trust-
worthiness of the communicator (same 11-point scale used in the before
measure); (c) fairness of the article (11-point scale ranging from zero
completely one-sided, through 5moderately fair, to 10completely fair);
(d) 5s' own opinion position on the statement used in judging the communi-
cator's position (same 11-point scale as used in the before measure); (e)
68 JOURNAL OF SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY

importance of the topic (same 11-point scale as in the before-measure); (/)


recall of the content (multiple-choice fact-quiz items based on the content of
the communication) ; and (ff) smoking habit.
The effect of communication was assessed immediately after the experi-
mental 5s were exposed to the communication. The control group, which cor-
responded to the experimental group in every respect other than the com-
munication effect, indicated opinion positions before being exposed to the
communication.
The two main experimental variables were manipulated in the following
manner.
a. Communicator credibility. At the beginning of the communication it
was indicated that the article was delivered as a speech by the author speci-
fied : that is. Dr. Hueper of the Public Health Service for the High-Credible
group, or Mr. Richards of the Tobacco Industry Public Relations Commit-
tee for the Low-Credible group.
b. Communication discrepancy. Taking the suggestion of the anchoring
effect on judgmental phenfcmena (13, 17), half of the group received the
communication with anchor statement and the other half without this anchor.
The anchor statement, to the effect that some people hold the extreme opin-
ions that cigarette smoking is unquestionably a cause of lung cancer or that
smoking bears no relationship to lung cancer, was inserted at the be-
ginning of the article in the expectation that it would cause the 5s to judge
the communication as less extreme than they would without this anchor. Ac-
cordingly, it was expected that the group with this anchor would be, on the
whole, less discrepant from the communicator's position than the group with-
out this statement.
C. RESULTS
The results are presented in three sections: 1) the effectiveness of the main
experimental variables, 2) the effect of the main variables on opinion change,
and 3) the effect of other related variables on opinion change.
1. The Effectiveness of the Main Experimental Variables
a. Communicator credibility. The mean after-measure on trustworthi-
ness of the communicator showed that the communicator credibility was suc-
cessfully differentiated. That is. Dr. Hueper of the National Cancer Insti-
tute, who was chosen as the High-Credible communicator, was rated by the
5s as more trustworthy than the Low-Credible communicator, Mr. Richards
of Tobacco Industry Public Relations Committee (means: 7.94 and 5.10 re-
spectively; the difference is significant at the 5 per cent level).
TONG-HE CHOO 69

b. Communication discrepancy. Variation in communication discrepancy


was to have been induced by the introduction or omission of the anchor state-
ment, as mentioned before. Contrary to expectation, however, the ratings of
the communicator's position made by the group with anchor statement was not
less extreme than the ratings given by the group without this anchor (means:
3.06 and 3.45 respectively). Accordingly, no difference between these two
groups in their subsequent opinion change would be expected. Therefore,
this discrepancy variable related to the anchor statement was not used.
Instead, the communication discrepancy of the present analyses was derived
from the 5s' ratings on the communicator's position. The 5s were divided
into two groups according to the median of all their judgments on the com-
municator's position (3.18) regardless of their own initial opinion positions.
All the experimental 5s' ratings of their own initial opinion position yielded
a mean of 7.13. The below-median groups' judgments of the communicator's
position yielded a mean of 1.54, whereas the above-median group yielded a
mean of 5.22. The below-median group was more discrepant (7.10-1.54,
or 5.56 points on the scale) than was the above-median group (7.16-5.22,
or L94 points), from their own mean initial position. In the following
analyses, the Large-Discrepancy group refers to this below-median group,
and the Small-Discrepancy group designates the above-median group. The
difference between these two mean discrepancies is significant at the 1 per
cent level.
To check on possible artifacts (ceiling, regression, and confounding with
5s' initial opinion position) in deriving this discrepancy variable, the correla-
tion between 5s' initial opinion position and the communicator's position
judged by the 5s was computed and found to be negligible. Accordingly, the
use of this derived discrepancy variable based upon the judgment of the com-
municator's position by the 5s in place of the original variable witb the anchor
statement is justified.
2. Communication Effect as a Function of Communicator
Credibility and Communication Discrepancy
Communication effect was assessed by the change score. The change score
is the difference between the before measures and the after measures in the
opinion scores obtained by the rating scale. Positive change score indicates
movement toward the communicator's position; i.e., toward the position that
there is no causal relationship between cigarette smoking and lung cancer.
Negative change signifies movement away from the position advocated by the
communicator.
70 JOURNAL OF SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY

In the assessment of opinion change, all control groups were combined, and
this combined control group served as the base line for comparison with the
experimental groups. This procedure is based on the assumption that the
control groups are homogeneous among themselves, for no significant differ-
ences among the control subgroups were found in the various measures ob-
tained in this study.
The mean opinion changes for the experimental groups and for the control
group are presented in Table 1; and the analysis of variance applied to the
2 X 2 table of the two main variables. Communicator Credibility and Com-
munication Discrepancy, is presented in Table 2. There was no significant
difference between the experimental and the control groups in their initial
opinion positions (means: 7.13 and 7.04 respectively).
TABLE 1
MEAN OPINION CHANCE FOR EACH EXPERIMENTAL SUBGROUP AND THE CONTROL GROUP

High-credibility group Low-credibility group


Control Large- Small- Large- Small-
group discrepancy discrepancy discrepancy discrepancy
N 67 38 40 42 29
Mean 0.28 2.16 1.13 1.21 0.45
SD 2.73 2.31 1.95 3.09 2.39
DifJ. 1.03 0.76
* Significant at the .01 level.

The group which was exposed to the communication (experimental group


as a whole) shows significantly greater extent of opinion change toward the
communication advocated than does the group which was not exposed to it
(control group).
The difference between the High- and the Low-Credibility groups within
the experimental group is significant by / test (/) < .05).^ The group that re-
ceived the communication attributed to the High-Credible source changed
more toward the communication advocated than did the group that read the
communication from the Low-Credible source. Also, the difference between
the Large- and the Small-Discrepancy groups is significant by t test
(p < .025). Thus, those who judged the communicator's position as more
discrepant from their own stand changed more toward the communication
than did those who judged it as less discrepant. The interaction effect be-
tween these two main variables. Communicator Credibility and Communica-
tion Discrepancy, is not significant. That is, within High- and Low-Credible

2 In the following analyses, all p values are derived from t ratios and one-tailed
tests, unless otherwise specified.
TONG-HE CHOO 71

groups, both Large-Discrepancy groups changed more toward the communi-


cation advocated than did the Small-Discrepancy groups.
When the experimental groups are compared with the control group, it is
noted that the overall High-Credible group shows significantly greater extent
of opinion change toward the communication than does the control group
{p < .0005). Similarly, only the overall Large-Discrepancy group has sig-
nificantly higher mean change score toward the communication than does the
control group {p < .005). Both overall Low-Credible and overall Small-
Discrepancy groups have higher mean change scores than does the control
group, but these differences are not significant (.10 > /i > .05 for both
groups).
TABLE 2
ANALYSIS OF VARIANCE OF CHANGE SCORES FOR EXPERIMENTAL GROUPS WITH
CREDIBILITY AND DISCREPANCY AS THE VARIABLES

Source s.s. df m.s. F p


Credibility (C) 19.63 1 19.63 3.14 .10 > ^ > .05
Discrepancy (D) 25.02 1 25.02 4.00 < .05
Interaction: C X D 5.85 1 5.85
Within 907.66 145 6.25
Total 958.16 148

Second-order comparisons show no significant interaction. Although all


four subgroups show greater mean opinion changes than the control group
shows, only the HC-LD group is significantly changed more toward the com-
munication.
The data were also analyzed by the net proportion of change^ and the same
conclusions, on the whole, were derived as from the change score presented
above, although some changes in the significant level of differences were ob-
served.
3. Communication Effect Related to Other Variables
In addition to the main variables, another important variable influencing
opinion change in this study is the "importance of the topic."
The experimental group was dichotomized according to the median of the
after measure on the importance of the topic (7.05). It was found that the
Low-Importance (below-median) group changed significantly more toward
the communication advocated than did the High-Importance (above-median)
group (mean change scores: 1.76 and 0.80 respectively, /> < .01). The initial

3 The net proportion of. change is defined by Hovland, Lumsdaine, and Sheffield
(11) as the difference between proportion changing positively and proportion chang-
ing negatively.
72 JOURNAL OF SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY

opinion positions of these two groups were not significantly different from
each other.
D. DISCUSSION

a. Communicator credibility. The main attribute of Communicator


Credibility in the present study was trustworthiness rather than expertness
[these two attributes were differentiated originally by Hovland, Janis, and
Kelley (10)]. Thus, the conclusion we could derive from the present data
is that when both communicators have high expertness, or when expertness is
held constant, the trustworthiness attribute alone can differentially affect the
S's opinion change.
This result seems to differ from that of Hovland and Mandell (6), who
found a marked effect on judgments of fairness of the article, but little effect
on amount of opinion change, where the "suspect" source differed from the
"nonsuspect" one primarily in characteristics of trustworthiness rather than
of expertness.
Hovland, Janis, and Kelley (10) indicated that the small effect they noted
in opinion change may be attributable to a special combination of factors
such that the extents of the speech and the qualification of the speaker (ex-
pertness) were more important than his personal motive (trustworthiness).
This explanation does not apply for the present study. A systematic study of
the attributes of communicator credibility, expertness and trustworthiness, is
one of the areas of research which needs further clarification.
b. Communication discrepancy. The generalization that opinion change
is a positive function of communication discrepancy is fairly well documented
in the sense of its comparative invariability under diverse experimental condi-
tions and measuring devices. A theory of cognitive dissonance proposed by
Festinger (2) presents a somewhat general conceptualization of these phe-
nomena. The central assertion of this theory is that a person who holds cogni-
tions about himself or the environment that are inconsistent with each other
experiences dissonance; i.e., psychological tension having drive characteristics.
Thus, when the communication advocated is discrepant from the position a
person held, "dissonance" is created and "tension" to resolve this dissonance
is mobilized to produce opinion change toward the communication. This the-
ory is not the only one which deals with "cognitive inconsistency" and its rela-
tion to opinion change. It is similar to Heider's cognitive balance (4), to
Osgood and Tannenbaums' principle of congruity (15), and Newcomb's
strain toward preferred states of equilibrium (14).
Hovland (5), however, stated in his recent paper that when the communi-
TONG-HE CHOO 73

cation source is perceived as credible, opinion change increases with increasing


discrepancy; but that when the source is viewed as neutral or ambiguous or
negative, then increasing discrepancy may lead to decreasing opinion change.
It appears at first that Hovland made a quite different prediction from the
predictions of the above cognitive theorists. If, however, the conditions which
Hovland, Harvey, and Sherif (9) dealt with are examined, their results are
not contradictory to other research findings. That is, when the communicator
is ambiguous or neutral and the communicator's position is divergent in an
extreme degree, it seems that mechanisms other than opinion changenamely,
misinterpretation or dissociation, etc.are readily available. Their data on
ratings of fairness of the communication can explain this phenomenon: for
example, where a communication was pro-Prohibition, nearly all of the 5s
who favored Prohibition considered it fair and impartial, but only a small
per cent of the 5s who opposed prohibition considered the identical commu-
nication fair.
The present results seem to be in accordance with the cognitive inconsist-
ency theories mentioned above. On the other hand, the present study failed
to indicate the resistance to change or contrast effect with Low-Credible
communicator and large Communication Discrepancy, contrary to the expec-
tation of Hovland et al. These results, however, could be mainly attributed to
the high expertness and also comparatively high trustworthiness of the Low-
Credible communicator (mean 5.10 on the scale is slightly above the midpoint,
moderately trustworthy) used in this study. Moreover, the communication
used for the present study was written in a logical form with supporting
argument, so that it could not be easily demerited. Therefore, resistance
against the communication or the "boomerang effect" described by Hovland
et al., what the cognitive theorists call "dissociation" or "devaluation" of
the communication, is less likely to have occurred in the present experi-
mental situation. All of the opinion changes with the Low-Credible com-
municator in the present study showed positive direction, although they were
not significantly different from the changes in the control group.
c. Importance of the topic. Present results showed that the more im-
portant the topic, the less the opinion change. As the communication advo-
cated that there is no causal relationship between cigarette smoking and lung
cancer, the 5s who felt the topic to be more important should have changed
more toward the communication than did the 5s who rated the topic less im-
portant, because this change would release or lessen their anxiety.
In the discussion on the resistance to change on the issue of Prohibition,
Hovland, Harvey, and Sherif (9) explained that the highly involved issue
74 JOURNAL OF SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY

and the established attitude which would serve as an anchor stimulus for the
5s might be responsible factors. If the 5s in the present study who rated the
topic more important are assumed to have formed a stronger initial attitude
than the 5s who rated the topic less important, a comparable explanation to
that given above could be made for the results.
Zimbardo (18), on the other hand, observed increasing opinion change as
involvement increased. He explained this difference between his result and
that of the above authors by differentiating "issue-involvement," where the
issue itself is intrinsically involving, and "response-involvement," where the
instrumental meaning of the 5's opinion is attached to the consequence of his
responses. When the process of acceptance of communication in terms of rein-
forcement is taken into consideration, Zimbardo's differentiation becomes
ambiguous. The concept of the established attitude again seems to be a per-
tinent one for the explanation of the difference in the above studies. That is,
in Zimbardo's 5s the communicator was highly credible, but there was little
established prior attitude on the issue strong enough to offset the credibility of
the communicator.
Hovland, Harvey, and Sherif (9) went a step further to predict that under
conditions of high involvement, increasing discrepancy would lead to decreas-
ing attitude change. When the effects of high and low importance are ex-
amined separately for Large- and Small-Discrepancy groups in the present
study, there was found to be no interaction. The degree of importance or of
involvement between the two studies might be different, and might be re-
sponsible for the different results. Without making independent and com-
parable measures of establishment of attitude, importance, discrepancy, etc.,
no final explanations of the different results are possible.

E. SUMMARY

The purpose of the present study was to vary experimentally the degree of
communicator credibility and communication discrepancy, and to investigate
the main and interaction effects of these two variables on opinion change.
A review of previous research led to the following hypotheses: Greater
extent of opinion change toward the communication is expected when the com-
municator is High-Credible rather than Low-Credible, and the Communica-
tion-Discrepancy is large rather than small. When the communicator is High-
Credible, greater extent of opinion change is expected for Large-Discrepancy
than for Small-Discrepancy groups.
In order to test the above hypotheses, a before-after design was used.
Among 216 college students, 149 5s, who served as the experimental group.
TONG-HE CHOO 75
were exposed to a communication which advocated no causal relationship be-
tween cigarette smoking and lung cancer. One half of the experimental Ss
read the communication in which the source was attributed to a High-Cred-
ible communicator and the other half read it from a Low-Credible communi-
cator. Also, for one half of each group, a few additional anchor statements,
that some people hold at the opinion-extremes on the issue, were inserted at
the beginning of the communication in the expectation that these anchor
statements would affect the 5s to judge the communication as less extreme
than they would without these anchor statements.
The checks on the experimental manipulations of the above two variables
indicated that only High vs. Low Communicator Credibility was success-
fully differentiated. Due to the failure of the manipulation of the discrep-
ancy variable by anchor statements, the Communication Discrepancy of the
present study was derived from the 5's judgments on the communicator's
position.
Opinion change was assessed by the change score from before communica-
tion to after communication. The experimental group showed significantly
greater extent of opinion change toward the communication than did the
control group. The results also confirmed the hypotheses advanced, thus:
1. The group that received the communication attributed to the High-
Credible source changed significantly more toward the communication than
did the group which read the communication from the Low-Credible source.
2. The 5s who judged the communicator's position as more discrepant
from their own stand changed significantly more toward the communication
than did those who judged it less discrepant.
3. The interaction effect between these two main variables. Communicator
Credibility and Communication Discrepancy, was not significant. That is,
within High- and Low-Credible groups, both Large-Discrepancy groups
changed more toward the communication than did the Small-Discrepancy
groups.
REFERENCES

1. EWING, T . H . A study of certain factors involved in changes of opinion. J. Soc.


Psychol., 1942, 16, 63-88.
2. FESTINGER, L. A Theory of Cognitive Dissonance. Nevf York: Row, Peterson,
1957.
3. GOLDBERG, B. C. Three situational determinants of conformity to social norms.
J. Abn. & Soc. Psychol., 1954, 49, 325-329.
4. HEIDER, F . Attitudes and cognitive organization. J. of Psychol., 1946, 21, 107-112.
5. HovLAND, C. I. Reconciling conflicting results derived from experimental and
survey studies of attitude change. Amer. Psychologist, 1959, 14, 8-17.
76 JOURNAL OF SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY

6. HovLAND, C. I., & MANDELL, W . An experimental comparison of conclusion-


drawing by the communicator and the audience. J. Abn. & Soc. Psychol., 1952,
47, 581-588.
7. HovLAND, C. I., & PRITZKER, H . A . Extent of opinion change as a function of
amount of change advocated. J. Abn. & Soc. Psychol., 1957, 54, 257-261.
8. HoVLAND, C. I., & WEISS, W . The influence of source credibility on communica-
tion effectiveness. Pub. Opin. Quart., 1951, 15, 633-650.
9. HovLAND, C. I., HARVEY, O . J., & SHERIF, M . Assimilation and contrast effects in
reaction to communication and attitude change. J. Abn. Sf Soc. Psychol., 1957,
55, 244-252.
10. HovLAND, C. I., JANIS, I. L., & KELLEY, H . H . Communication and Persuasion.
New Haven, Conn.: Yale Univ. Press, 1953.
11. HoVLAND, C. I., LUMSDAINE, A. A., & SHEFFIELD, F. D. Experiments on Mass Com-
munication. Princeton, N. J.: Princeton Univ. Press, 1949.
12. KELMAN, H . C , & HovLAND, C. I. "Reinstatement" of the communicator in de-
layed measurement of opinion change. / . Abn. & Soc. Psychol., 1953, 48, 327-
335.
13. MCGARVEY, H . R . Anchoring effects in the absolute judgment of verbal materi-
als. Arch. Psychol. (N. Y.), 1943, 39, No. 281.
14. NEWCOMB, T . M . An approach to the study of communicative acts. Psychol. Rev.,
1953, 60, 393-404.
15. OSGOOD, C . E., & TANNENBAUM, P. The principle of congruity and the prediction
of attitude change. Psychol. Rev., 1955, 62, 42-55.
16. SIMS, V. M. Factors influencing attitude toward TVA. J. Abn. & Soc. Psychol.,
1938, 33, 34-56.
17. WEISS, W . The relationship between judgments of a communicator's position and
extent of opinion change. J. Abn. & Soc. Psychol., 1958, 56, 382-384.
18. ZIMBARDO, P. G. Involvement and communication discrepancy as determinants of
opinion conformity. J. Abn. & Soc. Psychol., 1960, 60, 86-94.
State School and Hospital
Faribault, Minnesota

Você também pode gostar