Você está na página 1de 7

Intensive and Critical Care Nursing (2014) 30, 204210

Available online at www.sciencedirect.com

ScienceDirect

journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/iccn

ORIGINAL ARTICLE

Alarm setting for the critically ill patient: A


descriptive pilot survey of nurses
perceptions of current practice in an
Australian Regional Critical Care Unit
Martin Christensen a,, Andrew Dodds b, Josh Sauer b,
Nigel Watts b

a
School of Health & Human Science, Southern Cross University, Rie Range Road, Lismore, NSW 2480,
Australia
b
Intensive Care Unit, Lismore Base Hospital, Lismore, NSW 2480, Australia

Accepted 27 February 2014

KEYWORDS Summary
Aim: The aim of this survey was to assess registered nurses perceptions of alarm setting and
Alarm fatigue;
management in an Australian Regional Critical Care Unit.
Nuisance alarms;
Background: The setting and management of alarms within the critical care environment is
Alarm
one of the key responsibilities of the nurse in this area. However, with up to 99% of alarms
desensitisation;
potentially being false-positives it is easy for the nurse to become desensitised or fatigued by
False alarms
incessant alarms; in some cases up to 400 per patient per day. Inadvertently ignoring, silencing
or disabling alarms can have deleterious implications for the patient and nurse.
Method: A total population sample of 48 nursing staff from a 13 bedded ICU/HDU/CCU within
regional Australia were asked to participate. A 10 item open-ended and multiple choice ques-
tionnaire was distributed to determine their perceptions and attitudes of alarm setting and
management within this clinical area.
Results: Two key themes were identied from the open-ended questions: attitudes towards
inappropriate alarm settings and annoyance at delayed responses to alarms. A signicant num-
ber of respondents (93%) agreed that alarm fatigue can result in alarm desensitisation and the
disabling of alarms, whilst 81% suggested the key factors are those associated with false-positive
alarms and inappropriately set alarms.

Corresponding author. Tel.: +61 7 5316 7508.


E-mail addresses: martin.christensen@scu.edu.au, mchristensen64@me.com (M. Christensen).

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.iccn.2014.02.003
0964-3397/ 2014 Published by Elsevier Ltd.
Alarm setting for the critically ill patient 205

Conclusion: This study contributes to what is known about alarm fatigue, setting and management
within a critical care environment. In addition it gives an insight as to what nurses within a
regional context consider the key factors which contribute to alarm fatigue. Clearly nursing
burnout and potential patient harm are important considerations for practice especially when
confronted with alarm fatigue and desensitisation. Therefore, promoting and maintaining an
environment of ongoing intra-professional communication and alarm surveillance are crucial in
alleviating these potential problems.
2014 Published by Elsevier Ltd.

Implications for Clinical Practice

The implications for practice are such that any further education around alarm setting and management whether
formally or informally are probably not warranted. However, identifying key factors that lead to alarm desensitisa-
tion/fatigue should be a priority because of the inherent risk to patient safety. Instead, practice could:
As part of its surveillance of adverse patient incidences, include a forum where adverse events have occurred that
were attributable to clinical alarms.
Provide ongoing education that focuses on identifying those behaviours that would be suggestive of alarm fatigue.
Creating an environment in which colleagues presenting and/or exhibiting behaviours conducive to alarm fatigue or
burnout feel supported.
In addition, nursing education teams reinforce the importance of alarm management as a part of safe patient
monitoring and meeting the requirements of competent professional practice as stipulated in the nursing code of
conduct.

Introduction
time (Sendelebach, 2012). Referred to as alarm fatigue, the
The intensive care unit (ICU) is notorious for exposing the ever increasing number and frequency of alarms can become
critically ill patient to unnecessary noise, and this noise is overwhelming, which can result in nurses delaying or even
strongly associated with signicant patient stress, the devel- failing to respond to or in some case disabling alarms (Solet
opment of ICU delirium and other problems. Noise levels and Barauch, 2012). It has been suggested that responding
in ICU have been recorded in excess of 80 decibels (dBs), to alarms can take up to 35% of the nurses time, which
mainly from mechanical alarms, which are similar to expo- would otherwise be more productive providing patient care
sure to a pneumatic drill in operation (Christensen, 2007). (Bitan et al., 2004), or putting this into a different context:
However, exposure to this level of noise is relatively spo- responding to 150400 alarms per day per patient (Keller,
radic. Despite this, there have been a number of studies 2012).
which have identied the average noise level in the ICU The causes of alarm fatigue can be multi-factorial;
to be between 55 dB and 60 dB (Christensen, 2007; Cmiel however, the most signicant cause is false alarms.
et al., 2004; Kahn et al., 1998; Kam et al., 1994) with False/positive alarms sometimes referred to as nuisance
the lowest level being 48 dB produced by low frequency alarms account for 7599% of all alarms encountered in
background noise such as air-conditioning and a working critical care environments (Atzema et al., 2006; Lawless,
ventilator (Christensen, 1997). Apart from the noise that is 1994; OCarroll, 1986). The most commonly reported false
created by human interaction, which accounts for nearly alarm is the pulse oximeter alarm due to a poor trace
80% of the noise produced in the ICU, it is mechanical (Bitan et al., 2004; Graham and Cvach, 2010). Yet, despite
alarms which are a cause for concern because of the inten- the sense of urgency in attending to alarms, a high per-
sity and startling physiological effect that is often produced centage of patient injuries have occurred as a result of
(Christensen, 2007). alarm desensitisation, which often meant serious debilitat-
There appears to be no uniformity as to a standard alarm ing patient outcomes (Atzema et al., 2006; Wyckoff, 2009).
sound and as such the critical care nurse must be able to It can be readily agreed that faced with a signicant number
distinguish differing alarms and which ones to respond to of alarms many of which are false, can lead nursing staff
as a matter of urgency. However, there is a hierarchical to distrust alarms. Korniewicz et al.s (2008) national sur-
order to alarms and these alert the nurse to either life- vey (n = 1327) on the effectiveness of clinical alarms found
threatening conditions (a crisis alarm) or are advisory as in that 78% of health care professionals distrusted the sounding
the case of a syringe driver for example (Cvach et al., 2009; alarm and purposely disabled the monitoring device. More-
Edworthy and Hellier, 2006). With a proliferation in the num- over, they found that when alarm management issues were
ber of medical devices currently seen within a typical ICU ranked in terms of importance, the most signicant were
the threshold for responding appropriately can diminish over the frequency and number of false alarms and inadequate
staff numbers responding to alarms as they occurred. This is
206 M. Christensen et al.

Table 1 Frequency of alarm setting and management content specic categories.

N = 48 %

Denition of a nuisance alarm


Alarm accuracy 15 32
Inappropriate alarm limits 13 27
Not attended to by Principal Nurse 12 25
Patient interference 5 10
Alarm setting practice
Individualised to Patient 30 64
Prescribed by Doctors 12 25
As per clinical practice guideline 5 11
The practice of silencing other nurses alarms
Principal nurse not present 14 30
Attending to other patient care 13 28
Patient not attended to by the Principal Nurse 9 19
Given permission 7 14
Inappropriate alarm limits 4 9
The practice of altering another nurses alarm limits
Inappropriate alarm limits 34 71
Patient not attended to by Principal Nurse 11 22
Patient condition changed 3 7
Informing colleagues of alarm limit change
Because of inappropriate alarm limits 13 27
Dont because of negative response 9 19
Because they are not responding to their alarms 7 15
Its part of being a team 7 15
Its the responsibility of RN in charge to inform the nurse responsible 6 12
Its not my responsibility its the primary nurses 6 12

worrying given the importance of monitoring patient phys- A recent audit of patient experiences at this study site
iological parameters to adjust and gauge responsiveness to identied that noise was a major disturbing factor for them
therapy. However, there is a consensus that the number of during their stay. Whilst the majority of individuals audited
alarms including false alarms can be reduced through simple did not describe specics noise sources, a small proportion
measures such as adjusting alarms to meet patient parame- of patients had commented that staff conversations and
ters or on-going education (Bell, 2008; Graham and Cvach, alarms played a signicant part in the noise levels that they
2010; Korniewicz et al., 2008). Although this appears to experienced. In addition, a signicant number of nurses
be straightforward, in practice this is not always the case commented that prolonged alarm noise caused them consid-
as competing demands placed on the nurse often force erable irritation and disturbed patients, especially so in the
them to prioritise care and forsake the alarm (Tanner, coronary care unit. Furthermore, New South Wales Health
2013). have invested heavily on the Essentials of Care (Ministry of
Alarm setting for the critically ill patient 207

Health, 2013), an evaluative care framework similar to the to ensure content validity prior to it being piloted in the unit.
United Kingdoms Essence of Care (Department of Health, The questions focused on issues about alarm fatigue and cur-
2010). This Ministry of Health directive together with the rent practices for managing alarms and their settings. The
aforementioned anecdotal observations then served as a participants were informed that they had the right to with-
justication for this study. draw from the study at any time and their questionnaire
would be removed from the nal analysis. The completed
Method questionnaires were kept in a locked ling cabinet. All elec-
tronic data were stored on a password protected computer
accessible only by the study team until the study ndings
Aim
are in print. Thereafter all material will be safely deleted.
The aim of this survey was assess registered nurses per-
ceptions of alarm setting and management in an Australian Data analysis
Regional Critical Care Unit.
The SPSS software for Windows, version 20 (SPSS, 2010)
Design was used to perform the data analysis. Descriptive statis-
tics were used to describe the frequency and percentages
of participant responses. In addition the open-ended ques-
A descriptive, survey design was used for this study.
tions were coded and analysed based upon the principles of
inductive content analysis to examine participant responses
Sample and setting in relation to those practices associated with alarm set-
ting and alarm management. These were open-coded based
A total population sample of 48 critical care nurses from a 13 on participant descriptions and formed into content spe-
bedded CCU/ICU/HDU from an Australian Regional Critical cic categories. It was these categories that were analysed
Care Unit were selected. A regional hospital in the Australian quantitatively for frequency.
context is akin to the United Kingdom model of district gen-
eral hospitals. This unit is able to provide care to level 2
HDU/CCU and 3 ICU patients, but patients requiring more
Results
complex care such as those with acute neurological trauma
or requiring cardiac surgery are transferred to the closest Descriptive categories were derived from the participant
major metropolitan hospitals typically based in the state responses. Five descriptive themes were identied based on
capitals. Typical length of stay within the unit is 23 days the questionnaire and frequency of those responses: den-
depending on the nature of the presenting condition. Nursing ing a nuisance alarm, alarm setting practice, the practice
staff in this unit are all trained to graduate certicate level of silencing other nurses alarms, the practice of altering
in intensive care nursing; most have Bachelors degrees and another nurses alarm limits and informing colleagues of
a very small number have Masters degrees; none have doc- alarm limit change (Table 1). Overall, the results provide
toral qualications. Substantive demographic data was not a mixed view of nurses perceptions of alarm management
collated due to the small cohort size and the ease with which in a regional ICU. Whilst most are able to identify the char-
participant statements could identify the respondent. The acteristics of a nuisance alarm, many felt that it was the
study was conducted over a two-month period from January responsibility of the primary nurse to set appropriate lim-
2013 to March 2013. its and responded to alarms in a timely manner. However,
descriptively it can be seen that inappropriate alarm set-
ting gured high in the participants responses across all ve
Ethical considerations themes (2771%) (Table 1). When asked what a frequent
nuisance alarm would be, 81% of respondents identied a
Ethics approval for this study was granted by the local area false positive or clinically irrelevant alarm as being the
health district ethics committee. Approval was also sought key factor. When compared with dening a nuisance alarm,
and granted from the Nurse Unit Manager. Consent to the interestingly, 57% of individuals suggested alarm accuracy
study was waived and based on return of the completed and inappropriate alarm setting were major contributing
questionnaire as an indication of the participants interest factors. Within those themes the practice of silencing and
in the study. Participants were informed that their answers altering alarm limits, >50% of alarms were as a result of the
would be anonymous and no individual identiers would be nurse being absent from the bed-space. This clearly sug-
used. gests that being present would, in all likelihood, reduce
the number of inappropriate alarms and provide more effec-
Data collection tive patient-centred alarm management. However, at times
when the primary nurse was not present, 48% of respondents
A self-administered 10-item questionnaire was developed did not change the alarm limits because of actual and/or
from the literature. The instrument consisted of multiple perceived negative responses from the primary nurse, or
choice questions (n = 2) (Sendelebach, 2012) together with they felt it was not their responsibility to interfere. Com-
open-ended questions (n = 8) (Tanner, 2013; Wyckoff, 2009). paring this with what effect alarm desensitisation can have
Question construction was based on observations made by on an individuals practice, 93% agreed that desensitisation
these authors where specic problems or issues were iden- can lead to decreased alarm reaction time and silencing or
tied. The nal questionnaire was vetted by the study team disabling alarms inappropriately.
208 M. Christensen et al.

Two major themes emerged from this study which char- in time or as a result of their intervention. However, when
acterised the registered nurses perceptions of alarm setting addressing the problem of distracting and annoying levels
and management in an ICU/CCU: of noise within the clinical environment, some nurses felt
vulnerable from a certain nursing element who were con-
Experiences of inappropriate alarm setting. sidered too rude (R36) or scary (R27) or aggressive
Experiences of delayed response times to alarms. verbally (R09) within the unit. In some cases individ-
uals felt very uncomfortable approaching these colleagues
Experiences of inappropriate alarm setting because it could be received as [a] critical judgement of
[a] colleagues practice (R15) or some simply did not bother
because:
The nurses in this study certainly had strong feelings about
the setting of inappropriate alarm limits. Many consid-
. . .some people are arrogant and think they know it all
ered the amount of false alarms rectiable given the nurse
and dont take to well constructive advice [so there is]
patient ratio within the unit. However, for some there was
no point really. (R21)
the perception that a small number of nurses failed to see
the importance of appropriate alarm setting inasmuch that
some respondents felt that: Discussion
. . .having alarms can promote laziness and reduce vig-
ilance and make nursing staff feel that they can stay at The aim of this survey was to determine the perceptions of
the nurses station and look after the patient remotely. nursing staff working in a regional Australian ICU/CCU/HDU
(R23) around alarm setting and management. Much of the work
around alarm fatigue appears to be anecdotal (Sendelebach,
This may be the case for those nurses who may have
2012; Solet and Barauch, 2012; Tanner, 2013). What few
become desensitised or attuned to the many different
studies that have been undertaken reiterate what has been
alarms present. In which case they either failed to respond
established here that alarm setting and alarm man-
or they felt did not need to respond depending on the type of
agement contributes signicantly to alarm fatigue in the
alarm. Others, however recognised that some nurses simply
critical care environment (Korniewicz et al., 2008). This
might not know or fully comprehend the importance of alarm
study clearly identied that inappropriate alarm setting was
setting, for example junior nursing staff new to the unit. Yet
one of the major factors that caused individual annoyance
there also appeared to be a sense of professional responsibil-
especially when relative inaction was witnessed in respon-
ity amongst some of those nurses who often felt compelled
ding to the initial alarm. Clearly false-positive alarms are
to intervene when alarms were not being silenced. Once
inevitable in this clinical environment because of the ever
having readjusted the alarms to what they thought was a
changing condition of the patient (Korniewicz et al., 2008),
more realistic limit, some individuals then informed the pri-
for example a ventilated patient coughing will trigger a high
mary nurse this is what they had done. In a small percentage
pressure alarm and the delusional patient removing moni-
of cases there appeared to be little negotiating or discus-
toring devices. However, these are manageable in the right
sion as to what was safe or appropriate for the patient from
context. It does become problematic when the intricacies of
both parties. Instead the intervening nurses often took it
monitoring prove to be challenging such as ST wave depres-
upon themselves to make a judgement call based on pre-
sion/elevation or cardiac ectopy monitoring (Atzema et al.,
vious experiences or they felt they were in the right. The
2006). This therefore comes down to the skill of the nurse
primary nurse either accepted these changes or changed the
in interpreting this information and setting alarms appropri-
alarms parameters themselves in private.
ately.
Yet, from this study there appeared to be no clear consen-
Experiences of delayed response times to alarms sus as to what appropriate alarm limits were despite there
being a clinical practice guideline available on the unit.
What appeared to be an insurmountable delay in responding Whilst many of the respondents set alarm limits individuali-
to alarms angered most nurses. The absence of the pri- sed to their patient, there appeared to be no consistency as
mary nurse from the bedside was a considerable annoyance. to what these meant. For example when setting mean arte-
However, this is explained especially in the HDU where the rial alarm limits, some individuals set these 20 mmHg above
primary nurse was attending to another patient or in the case and below the current reading or in the case a heart rate lim-
that someone was on a meal break. Yet, like the setting of its these were often set 10 beats/minute above and below
inappropriate alarms the sense of professional responsibility the current rate. For others, it was either those set by the
was very evident in the participants responses who were medical team or what they themselves (the nurse) deemed
clearly disappointed by colleagues behaviour: behaviour was within acceptable parameters; presumably based on
that some felt was not necessarily conducive to the profes- their level experience. Of course this did create problems
sional image of a critical care nurse for example, chatting for other nurses responding to patient alarms especially
at the desk (R02) or surng the internet checking sports where they were not familiar with the patient history or
scores (R12). the current treatment plan. Clearly, inadvertently changing
It was evident that alarm desensitisation was a common alarms can expose the patient to harm particularly if the
occurrence within this unit. Individual nurses at the patient primary nurse has not been made aware this has occurred
bed-space were either completely unaware of the alarm or or indeed places little signicance on the information given
assumed that whatever the cause was would rectify itself by the relieving nurse.
Alarm setting for the critically ill patient 209

Two key ndings from this study are worthy of discus- respondents in this study were the reasons why nurses did
sion; the time it took to respond to alarms within the unit not respond appropriately. There was universal agreement
and nursing team dynamics. It is evident from the language that nurses attending to another patient (an HDU patient
used in some of the participant responses that there are spe- where the nurse/patient ratio is 1:2) or on a meal break
cic issues around perceived professional behaviour when it was seen as acceptable and part of the culture within the
came to alarm setting and management in this clinical set- unit. However there were a minority of nurses who, it would
ting. The nature of the comments would rst suggest that appear, preferred to monitor their patient from the connes
there is perhaps an element of incivility perpetuated by of the nurses station or had become so desensitised to the
a select few nurses within the unit to the point that oth- alarms around them that they failed to respond altogether.
ers felt concerned by their responses. There is however,
the perception that bullying and aggressive behaviour are
commonly misinterpreted as being the same thing. Casual
observations of nursing communication suggest that aggres- Limitations
sion is sometimes the over-riding behaviour that is witnessed
and experienced on this unit. Three possible reasons may This study would be difcult to generalise to like sized
explain why some nurses felt distressed at the supposed regional ICU/HDU/CCUs because of patient and staff demo-
aggressive responses they encountered. First, some indi- graphics as well as differing levels of acuity within this
viduals whose alarms were not being attended to in an clinical environment compared with others. The survey
appropriate time frame angered others who therefore felt questionnaire itself was gleaned from the literature and
they had to intervene to ensure the patient was safe, or whilst not statistically validated to support construct valid-
at the very least reduce the level of noise the alarms cre- ity it did support content validity and as such it provided
ated. Second, the apparent self-protectionist behaviours in information that was seen as important to address this issue.
which certain individuals viewed constructive feedback as It may be appropriate in the future to consider more con-
a personal attack, would often responded inappropriately or cise methods to ensure construct validity such as exploratory
aggressively to fellow colleagues. This suggests that to some factor analysis to identify decits and rene the question-
degree these individuals may have felt embarrassment at naire or indeed use mixed method approaches to incorporate
having their nursing care come under close scrutiny, which focus groups or individual interviews to explore the issues
clearly was not the case. Last, the intensive care environ- identied here.
ment can be a source of stress for vulnerable nurses, so We also accept that the emotiveness that this topic raised
much so that burnout and compassion fatigue may be a com- is a little concerning given the language and words individual
mon occurrence for these individuals. Therefore, instead of respondents used to describe current alarm management on
responding in a more emotionally intelligent fashion simply this unit. Whilst we did not provide a more detailed debrief
lashed out aggressively. As a result this may have alien- for some individuals to work through specic issues, we are
ated these individuals to the point that some nurses may now conscious of the need to allow staff an outlet to express
not intervene on their behalf. What the root cause of this concerns when conducting future studies of this type.
phenomenon is likely to be however is difcult to pinpoint
and would be purely conjecture and perhaps grounds for
future work.
Whilst this study did not aim to directly measure response Conclusion
times, Bitan et al. (2004) demonstrated this succinctly when
they measured nurse response times to specic alarms. On Desensitisation to alarms through inappropriate settings
average it took a nurse more than 30 seconds to respond is clearly worrying given the nature of alarm observa-
to a heart rate alarm compared with 15 seconds for a sat- tions. Whilst many would agree that a more comprehensive
uration alarm. They concluded that the number of alarms approach be made to alarm monitoring such as a buddy-
relates to the acuity of the patient and length of time ing system or clinical rounds by the nurse in charge, the
the nurse responds and initiates treatment. For example overall responsibility for patient care and therefore patient
the more critically ill the patient the closer attention is monitoring surely rests with the allocated nurse. In some
paid by the nurse to the alarm. Whilst this unit does admit instances it may be technology-based solutions that may
level 3 patients the majority of those admitted tend to be well provide the answers, for example smart alarms that are
HDU or CCU patients whose length of stay in the unit is on based on algorithms which cover more than one condition.
average 23 days. Given the nature of this clinical environ- However, it would also seem that alarm management
ment it would seem reasonable to expect nurses to respond is undertaken on an ad-hoc basis with individuals setting
quickly and appropriately to all alarms, but when >75% of alarms limits to what they considered appropriate based
alarms are false-positives (Atzema et al., 2006; Lawless, on their knowledge of the patients condition and treat-
1994; OCarroll, 1986) it is no wonder a sense of impor- ment plan. Unfortunately, this is not always conveyed to
tance is not always forthcoming. Yet, judging by participant colleagues who might be relieving for meal breaks or res-
responses there appeared to be a sense of complacency ponding to the alarm when the primary nurse is otherwise
amongst some nurses as to what alarm was important and engaged. Whilst education seems to be an important aspect
therefore responding to an alarm took longer than would of understanding the monitoring system it is probably bene-
be normally tolerated. This of course encouraged an envi- cial to include such forums as incident or adverse reporting
ronment of annoyance, the danger of which was to silence as a surveillance measure to identify patient events that
an alarm without necessarily intervening. Of concern to the were attributable to clinical alarms.
210 M. Christensen et al.

References Graham KC, Cvach M. Monitoring alarm fatigue: standardising use


of physiological monitors and decreasing nuisance alarms. Am J
Crit Care 2010;19(1):2837.
Atzema C, Schull MJ, Borgundvaag B, Slaughter GRD, Lee CK.
Kahn DM, Cook TE, Carlisle CC, Nelson DL, Kramer NR, Millman RP.
ALARMED: adverse events in low-risk patients with chest
Identication and modication of environmental noise in an ICU
pain receiving continuous electrocardiographic monitoring in
setting. Chest 1998;114(2):53540.
the emergency department; a pilot study. Am J Emerg Med
Kam PCA, Kam AC, Thompson JF. Noise pollution in the anaesthetic
2006;24(1):627.
and intensive care environments. Anaesthesia 1994;49:9826.
Bell L. Monitoring clinical alarms. Am J Crit Care 2008;17(1):44.
Keller JP. Clinical alarm hazards: a top ten health technology safety
Bitan Y, Meyer J, Shinar D, Zmora D. Nurses reaction to alarms
concern. J Electrocardiol 2012;45(6):58891.
in the neonatal intensive care unit. Cogn Technol Work
Korniewicz DM, Clark T, David Y. A national online survey
2004;6(4):23946.
on the effectiveness of clinical alarms. Am J Crit Care
Cmiel C, Karr DM, Gasser DM, Oliphant LM, Neveau AJ. Noise con-
2008;17(1):3641.
trol: a nursing teams approach to sleep promotion. Am J Nurs
Lawless ST. Crying wolf: false alarms in a paediatric intensive care
2004;104(2):408.
unit. Crit Care Med 1994;22(6):9815.
Christensen M. (unpublished dissertation) A quantitative study of
Ministry of Health. Cultures that care. Sydney, Australia: NSW Min-
noise levels in a four bedded intensive care unit (unpublished
istry of Health; 2013.
dissertation). Bournemouth: Bournemouth University; 1997.
OCarroll TM. Survey of alarms in an intensive therapy unit. Anaes-
Christensen M. Noise levels in a general intensive care unit: a
thesia 1986;41(7):7424.
descriptive study. Nurs Crit Care 2007;12(4):18897.
Sendelebach S. Alarm fatigue. Nurs Clin North Am
Cvach M, Dang D, Foster J, Irechukwu J. Clinical alarms and
2012;47(3):37582.
the impact on patient safety; 2009, http://www.initiatives-
Solet LM, Barauch PR. Managing alarm fatigue in cardiac care. Prog
patientsafety.org/past.html [accessed June 2013].
Paediatr Cardiol 2012;33(1):8590.
Department of Health. Essence of Care 2010. London: Department
Tanner T. The problem of alarm fatigue. Nurs Womens Health
of Health; 2010.
2013;17(2):1537.
Edworthy J, Hellier E. Alarms and human behaviour: implications
Wyckoff M. Improving how we use and respond to clinical alarms.
for medical alarms. Br J Anaesth 2006;97(1):127.
Am Nurse Today 2009;4(9):379.

Você também pode gostar