Você está na página 1de 18

479384

rnal of Health and Social BehaviorTurner et al.


2013
HSBXXX10.1177/0022146513479384Jou

Mental Health
Journal of Health and Social Behavior

Community Disorder, 54(2) 258275


American Sociological Association 2013
DOI: 10.1177/0022146513479384
Victimization Exposure, and jhsb.sagepub.com

Mental Health in a National


Sample of Y
outh

Heather A.Turner1, Anne Shattuck1, Sherry


Hamby2, and David Finkelhor1

Abstract
This study considers whether elevated distress among youth living in more disordered neighborhoods
can be explained by personal exposure to violence and victimization, level of non-victimization adversity,
and family support. Analyses were based on a sample of 2,039 youth ages 10 to 17 who participated in
the National Survey of Childrens Exposure to Violence, a national telephone survey conducted in 2008.
Using structural equation modeling, we find no direct effects of community disorder on distress, once the
significant mediating effects of victimization, family support, and adversity are taken into account. Using
a comprehensive measure of victimization covering several domains of experiences, we show that past-
year exposure to child maltreatment, sexual victimization, peer assault and bullying, and property crime
each significantly mediate the community disorderdistress association. A measure of the total number
of victimization types to which youth were exposed (i.e., level of poly-victimization) had the strongest
mediating effect.

Keywords
bullying, child maltreatment, community disorder, sexual victimization, witnessing violence

BACKGROUND Factors Linking Community Disorder to


Adolescent Distress
Research has demonstrated associations between
neighborhood/community disadvantage and disor- Although living in disordered neighborhoods may
der and the mental health of both adults and youth be damaging to the well-being of residents, including
(Aneshensel and Sucoff 1996; Gary, Stark, and youth, the mechanisms that explain this associa-
LaVeist 2007; Hill, Ross, and Angel 2005; Latkin
and Curry 2003; Ross and Mirowsky 2009). Com-
munity disorder can be defined as a neighborhood
1
environment that presents residents with observ- University of New Hampshire, Durham, NH, USA
2
able signs that social control is weak and that there University of the South, Sewanee, TN, USA
is little concern or ability to maintain a safe and
Corresponding Author:
orderly physical environment. Such neighbor- Heather A. Turner, Department of Sociology and Crimes
hoods are often characterized by rundown build- against Children Research Center, University of New
ings, graffiti, litter, public drinking and drug use, Hampshire, Horton SS Center, 4th Floor, Durham, NH,
vandalism, and cues that crime is common (Geis 03824, USA.
and Ross 1998; Kim and Conley 2011). E-mail: heather.turner@unh.edu
Turner et al. 259

tion have yet to be fully understood. A more com- associated with poor parenting behaviors and
prehensive consideration of stress exposure reduced warmth toward children (Cutrona et al.
associated with disordered neighborhoods may 2006; Wickrama and Bryant 2003). To the extent
provide some insights into the sources of elevated that disordered communities are associated with
risk. As Turner, Wheaton, and Lloyd (1995) argued reduced levels of family support, we might expect
over a decade ago, the importance of social stress youth in these communities to experience signifi-
in explaining variations in mental health is often cantly greater distress.
not sufficiently assessed because most studies fail A potent source of stress for youth that is often
to adequately measure stress exposure. As we out- not adequately assessed (Turner, Finkelhor, and
line below, it is our contention that stress exposure, Ormrod 2006) is exposure to violence and victimi-
especially exposure to multiple forms of violence zation. These types of stressors are likely to be
and victimization, represents an especially power- substantially higher in neighborhoods character-
ful predictor of youth mental health and one that ized by high community disorder and may repre-
may largely explain the significance of community sent a primary mechanism by which community
disorder for well-being. We also consider whether disorder affects well-being. However, surprisingly,
deficits in an important resource for youth, social few studies that address the effects of neighbor-
support from family, may further explain the nega- hood disorder focus on actual exposure to violence
tive effects of community disorder. and victimization as a main component of the
It has been suggested that individuals living in research. One study (Curry, Latkin, and Davey-
disordered communities are more likely to expe- Rothwell 2008) found that the effect of objective
rience negative life events, imposed by lack of neighborhood crime (based on police crime data)
community resources and an unhealthy physical was fully mediated by perceived neighborhood
environment (Cutrona, Wallace, and Wesner disorder and experienced violence, each of which
2006; Sampson, Morenoff, and Gannon-Rowley showed independent effects on depression. Many
2002). Although poverty may both contribute to studies, however, assume that fear of crime,
adverse events and select families into disordered rather than experienced crime, is the primary com-
communities, stressors such as accidents, ill- ponent of community disorder (Hill et al. 2005;
nesses, interpersonal losses, and unemployment Perkins, Meeks, and Taylor 1992; Perkins and
may also be more common for individuals living Taylor 1996; Ross and Mirowsky 2001).
in disordered communities, independent of eco- Other studies focus on psychosocial processes
nomic status. In other words, disordered neigh- such as low perceived control, powerlessness,
borhood contexts can contribute to a alienation, and mistrust as core mediators between
proliferation of stressors (Pearlin 1999) by, for neighborhood disorder and psychological distress
example, undermining physical health and safety, (Geis and Ross 1998; Kim and Conley 2011; Ross
limiting economic opportunities, and inhibiting and Mirowsky 2009; Ross, Mirowsky, and Pribesh
effective coping. Greater exposure to stressful 2001; Schieman and Meersman 2004). Ross and
life events, in turn, may contribute to elevated Mirowsky (2009), for example, in a study of adults
distress among young residents (Cutrona et al. found that mistrust and powerlessness were impor-
2006; Hill et al. 2005; Ross and Mirowsky 2009; tant mediators linking neighborhood disorder and
Sampson et al. 2002). symptoms of anxiety, anger, and depression. In
Community disorder may also reduce the social contrast, the researchers found that personal crimi-
support available to residents. Research suggests nal victimization accounted for only a small frac-
that people in disordered communities are less tion (10 percent) of the mediating influence
likely to form ties with others in the neighborhood between neighborhood disorder and distress. The
because residential turnover is high and because authors write, Individuals living amid neighbor-
trust in others is low (Cutrona et al. 2006; Hill hood disorder are robbed, burglarized, and
et al. 2005; Ross and Mirowsky 2009; Sampson assaulted more often than those in orderly neigh-
et al. 2002). Not only are social ties limited, but the borhoods. Nonetheless, the likelihood of personal
perceived supportiveness of relationships with criminal victimization is low. On the other hand,
both friends and family is also lower in disordered residents live every day with the threat of victimi-
neighborhoods (Aneshensel and Sucoff 1996). zation (p. 50).
This may be a particularly salient issue for youth, Although factors such as powerlessness and
since disordered neighborhoods provide fewer role mistrust may indeed be crucial consequences of
models for supportive parenting and have been residing in disordered neighborhoods, we argue
260 Journal of Health and Social Behavior 54(2)

that a more comprehensive assessment of personal Given the evidence outlined above, past studies
exposure to violence and victimization may show focusing on community disorder, especially those
it to have a more central role than is often assumed. addressing its effects among youth, have not ade-
In fact, we argue that the chances of victimization quately assessed the role of actual exposure to
are not low but are quite high, especially among crime, violence, and victimization. A more com-
children and youth. That is, direct exposure to prehensive measurement of multiple forms of vic-
violence and victimization stressors may be the timization is needed.
primary basis for why community disorder has
deleterious effects on young residents.
Youth suffer higher rates of exposure to vio- Assessing Community Disorder and
lence than do adults (Finkelhor 2008). A recent Victimization
nationally representative survey (Finkelhor et al.
2009) found that a clear majority of the U.S. popu- Community/neighborhood disorder has been mea-
lation of children ages 0 to 17 had experienced at sured in a variety of ways. Some researchers have
least one direct or witnessed victimization in the employed objective aggregate measures such as
past year. Almost half had experienced a physical neighborhood-level socioeconomic status (SES),
assault in the study year, 1 in 4 a property offense, crime rates, and availability of neighborhood
and 1 in 10 a form of child maltreatment, and resources, such as stores and recreational facilities.
almost 10 percent had witnessed an intra-family Other researchers have used subjective individual-
assault. Importantly, almost 40 percent of the sam- level measures, asking respondents themselves
ple had been exposed to two or more victimiza- about the social and physical conditions that char-
tions, and 11 percent of the sample had experienced acterize their neighborhoods. Although it has been
five or more different forms of victimization dur- argued that objective measures of community dis-
ing the past year. Exposure to most individual order are less susceptible to reporting bias, several
forms, in addition to the likelihood of experiencing studies have shown substantial overlap between
multiple types of victimization, is highest among objective measures and residents perceptions of
adolescents (Finkelhor et al. 2009; Turner, Finkelhor, disorder (Quillian and Pager 2001; Sampson and
and Ormrod 2010) and is very likely to be further Raudenbush 2004). Indeed, Elo et al. (2009) found
elevated in disordered communities. It would that objective indicators of neighborhood disorder
appear that, especially for youth in these commu- explained over 70 percent of between-neighborhood
nities, direct exposure to violence and victimiza- variance in respondents perceptions of disorder.
tion is far from rare. Other studies have found the effects of objective
There is a large literature linking childrens expo- measures of disorder to be fully or largely medi-
sure to many individual forms of victimization and ated by subjective measures of disorder (Curry et al.
psychological distress and disorder. For example, 2008; Stiffman et al. 1999; Wen, Hawkley, and
investigators have documented linkages between a Cacioppo 2006). Thus, it appears that subjective
variety of emotional and behavioral problems and indices may be more directly linked to the well-
physical abuse (Dodge, Pettit, and Bates 1997; Toth, being of residents than aggregate-level objective
Manly, and Cicchetti 1992), sexual abuse (Browne measures. The current study uses a subjective
and Finkelhor 1986; Feiring, Taska, and Lewis 2002; measure of community disorder that incorporates
Green 1993), neighborhood violence (Osofsky et al. aspects of both physical and social disorder.
1993; Richters and Martinez 1993), peer bullying Many past studies use measures of community
(Bond et al. 2001; Espelage and Holt 2001), and disorder that are confounded with witnessing vio-
witnessing parental violence (Edleson et al. 2003; lence. In our opinion, perceptions of neighborhood
Kitzmann et al. 2003). Importantly, exposure to mul- decay and low social control, indexed by residents
tiple forms of victimization, or what has been termed observations of broken windows, graffiti, public
poly-victimization, has been found to have espe- drinking, and crime tape or arrests, should be dis-
cially powerful effects on youth mental health tinguished from direct witnessing of violence.
(Finkelhor, Ormrod, and Turner 2007a; Turner et al. Especially for children, actually witnessing vio-
2010). Specifically, exposure to multiple forms of lence may involve different cognitive processes,
victimization was more strongly related to distress attributions, or defense mechanisms and, as a
than exposure to any individual form of victimiza- result, may have different implications and out-
tion, even chronic levels of serious victimizations comes than does seeing evidence of neighborhood
such as physical abuse (Turner et al. 2010). decay, loitering, or that a crime has taken place.
Turner et al. 261

Thus, the current research employs a measure of aggregate categories of violence and victimization,
community disorder that is not confounded with the total number of victimization types to which
exposure to either witnessed or personal violence. youth were exposed, as well as levels of past-year
Witnessing violence in the neighborhood, what stressful life events and family support, among
is often referred to in the literature as community youth living in neighborhoods with lower versus
violence, should also be distinguished from other higher levels of disorder; and (3) using structural
types of victimization. There is considerable co- equation modeling, examine direct and indirect
occurrence of community violence (witnessing effects of community disorder on distress symptoms
violence in the community), domestic violence by considering the extent to which the above factors
(witnessing violence between parents or other fam- mediate or otherwise explain greater distress among
ily members), and personal victimization (crime youth living in more disordered communities.
and violence directed toward the child) (Aisenberg
and Herrenkhohl 2008; Finkelhor et al. 2007a;
Finkelhor et al. 2009; Hamby et al. 2010). Only by
DATA AND METHODS
assessing multiple forms of victimization sepa- Participants
rately and comprehensively can researchers deter-
mine the extent to which community disorder is The National Survey of Childrens Exposure to
linked to particular forms of victimization (includ- Violence was designed to obtain incidence and
ing but not limited to community violence) and prevalence estimates of a wide range of childhood
which of those types of victimization are most victimizations. The National Survey of Childrens
influential in creating negative mental health out- Exposure to Violence is the largest and most com-
comes in youth. prehensive survey ever devoted to childhood vic-
timization (Finkelhor et al. 2009; Finkelhor et al.
2010; Turner et al. 2010).
Objectives
Conducted between January 2008 and May
Using a nationally representative sample of youth 2008, the survey addressed the experiences of a
ages 10 to 17, the present study seeks to examine the nationally representative sample of 4,549 children
effects of community disorder on symptoms of dis- ages 0 to 17 years living in the contiguous United
tress and to identify mediating factors that may help States. The current study focuses on the subsample
to explain this association. Of particular interest is of 2,039 children aged 10 to 17 years. Demo-
the extent to which different forms of personal and graphic characteristics of this sample are shown in
witnessed violence and victimization help to account the last column of Table 1.
for the association. To this end we employ a more Interviews were conducted over the phone by
comprehensive assessment of victimization than has the employees of an experienced survey research
been used in past studies on community disorder, firm. Telephone interviewing is a cost-effective
including past-year exposure to child maltreatment, methodology (McAuliffe et al. 1998; Weeks et al.
sexual victimization, property crime, peer assault 1983) that has been demonstrated to be comparable
and bullying, witnessing community violence, and to in-person interviews in data quality, even for
witnessing family violence, as well as considering reports of victimization, psychopathology, and other
exposure to multiple forms of victimization. We sensitive topics (Acierno et al. 2003; Bajos et al.
also assess level of exposure to other stressful life 1992; Bermack 1989; Czaja 1987; Marin and Marin
events (such as family accidents, illnesses, and job 1989; Pruchno and Hayden 2000). In fact, some
loss) and levels of family support as potential evidence suggests that telephone interviews are
mediators of the association between community perceived by respondents as more anonymous and
disorder and levels of youth distress. Specific aims private than in-person modes (Acierno et al. 2003;
are to (1) describe the demographic distribution of Taylor 2002) and may encourage greater disclosure
respondents living in neighborhoods characterized of victimization events (Acierno et al. 2003).
by higher versus lower levels of community disor- The primary foundation of the design was a
der; (2) compare past-year exposure to 6 different nationwide sampling frame of residential telephone
262 Journal of Health and Social Behavior 54(2)

Table 1. Sample Characteristics and Victimization Exposure by Community Disorder Level.


Community Disorder Group

Low High Total Sample


(n = 1,441) (n = 598) (n = 2,039)
Age (mean)*** 13.1 14.4 13.5
Sex (percentage)
Male 50.3 54.0 51.4
Female 49.7 46.0 48.6
Race-ethnicity (percentage)***
White, non-Hispanic 65.8 50.4 61.3
Black, non-Hispanic 12.3 22.4 15.3
Other, non-Hispanic 5.1 7.5 5.8
Hispanic, any race 16.8 19.6 17.6
Family structure (percentage)***
Two parents, biological or adopted 63.6 47.3 58.8
Parent and stepparent/partner 12.9 17.4 14.2
Single parent 19.6 27.6 21.9
Other adult caregiver 4.0 7.7 5.0
Socioeconomic statusmean (standard deviation)*** .11 .16 .03
(.9) (.9) (.9)
Distress scoremean (standard deviation)*** 41.0 48.2 43.2
(10.1) (12.7) (11.4)
Past-year adversity scoremean (standard .6 1.2 .8
deviation)*** (.9) (1.3) (1.1)
Family social support scoremean (standard 14.5 13.4 14.2
deviation)*** (1.9) (2.6) (2.2)
Percentage of sample experiencing
Any property victimization*** 19.9 41.0 26.1
Any maltreatment*** 9.9 24.7 14.2
Any peer victimization*** 32.6 54.9 39.2
Any sexual victimization*** 6.9 23.8 11.9
Any witnessing family violence*** 7.6 22.9 12.1
Any witnessing community violence*** 27.5 58.0 36.4
Number of past-year victimizations by typemean (standard deviation)
Property victimization*** .25 .56 .34
(.6) (.8) (.6)
Maltreatment*** .12 .35 .19
(.4) (.7) (.5)
Peer victimization*** .57 1.19 .75
(1.1) (1.5) (1.2)
Sexual victimization*** .08 .31 .15
(.3) (.6) (.5)
Witnessing family violence*** .08 .28 .14
(.3) (.6) (.4)
Witnessing community violence*** .34 .95 .52
(.6) (1.0) (.8)
Number of all past-year victimizationsmean (stan- 1.45 3.63 2.09
dard deviation)*** (2.0) (3.2) (2.7)
Note: Comparison between low and high community disorder groups were made using chi-square analysis for
percentages and t-tests for means.
*p < .05; **p < .01; ***p < .001.
Turner et al. 263

numbers from which a sample of telephone house- 2005; Keeter et al. 2006; Singer 2006). Although
holds was drawn by random-digit dialing. This the potential for response bias remains an impor-
nationally representative cross section represented tant consideration, several recent studies have
67 percent of the completed interviews. To ensure shown no meaningful association between
that the study included a sizeable proportion of response rates and response bias (Curtin, Presser,
minorities and low-income respondents for more and Singer 2000; Groves 2006; Keeter et al. 2000;
accurate subgroup analyses, there was also an Merkle and Edelman 2002). We also conducted
over-sampling of U.S. telephone exchanges that our own non-response analysis with the current
had a population of 70 percent or more of African data and found that respondents who refused to
American, Hispanic, or low-income households. participate (or could not be reached), but for whom
This over-sample yielded 33 percent of the com- parent screener information was obtained, were
pleted interviews. Sample weights were applied to not systematically different from respondents on
adjust for differential probability of selection due factors related to victimization risk (details of the
to: (1) study design, (2) demographic variations non-response analyses can be obtained from the
in non-response, and (3) variations in within- authors). Nevertheless, because participation
household eligibility. required consent of both the parent and child, par-
ent-perpetrated violence may be underestimated
since abusive parents may be less likely to provide
Procedure
consent.
A short interview was conducted with an adult
caregiver (usually a parent) in each household to
Measures
obtain family demographic information. One child
was randomly selected from all eligible children Community disorder. A measure of community
living in a household by selecting the child with disorder, covering both physical and social aspects
the most recent birthday. The selected 10- to of disorder, was developed for the current survey
17-year-old respondent completed the main inter- by sampling domains common to a variety of exist-
view him- or herself, following consent from both ing measures. In doing so, we were careful not to
parent and child. Respondents were promised include items that would be confounded with per-
complete confidentiality and were paid $20 for sonal victimization or witnessed violence (Hamby
their participation. The interviews, averaging et al. 2007). Specifically, respondents were asked
45 minutes in length, were conducted in either nine questions regarding conditions in their neigh-
English or Spanish. borhoods and schools in the past year. These ques-
tions asked if the adolescent had (1) witnessed
street drug sales (16 percent), (2) witnessed an
Response Rates
arrest (21 percent), (3) lived in a neighborhood
The cooperation rate (percentage of contacted with gangs (13 percent), (4) seen gang graffiti in
respondents who completed the survey) for the the neighborhood (18 percent), (5) witnessed
random-digit dialing portion of this survey was 71 police raid a building or block off a crime scene
percent, and the response rate (the percentage of all (12 percent), (6) been prohibited by parents from
eligible respondents who completed the survey) playing outside because of crime (3 percent), (7)
was 54 percent. The cooperation and response lived in a neighborhood characterized by physical
rates associated with the smaller over-sample were deterioration (3 percent), (8) attended a school
somewhat lower, at 63 percent and 43 percent, where there were gangs (22 percent), and (9)
respectively. These are good rates by current sur- attended a school where someone brought a knife
vey research standards (Babbie 2007; Keeter et al. or gun to school (20 percent). Item wording is
2006), given the steady declines in response rates shown in Appendix A in the online supplement
that have occurred over the past three decades (available at http://hsb.sagepub.com/content/by/
(Atrostic et al. 2001) and the particularly marked supplemental-data). Because items 3 and 4 were
drop in recent years (Curtin, Presser, and Singer highly correlated, they were combined into a single
264 Journal of Health and Social Behavior 54(2)

item indicating whether an adolescent had lived in trum of developmental stages (Finkelhor, Hamby,
a neighborhood in the past year where there were et al. 2005).
gangs or gang graffiti (23 percent). For bivarate The JVQ obtains reports on youth victimization
analysis, yes responses to these items were covering five general areas of interest: conven-
summed to form a community disorder score with tional crime, maltreatment, victimization by peers
possible values of 0 to 8. In this sample, 49 percent and siblings, sexual victimization, and witnessing
of adolescents had a community disorder score of and indirect victimization (Finkelhor, Ormrod, et
0; 22 percent had a score of 1, and 29 percent al. 2005b). Follow-up questions for each item
scored 2 or higher. In the structural equation model gathered additional information, including whether
portion of the analysis, community disorder was the event occurred in the past year and perpetrator
modeled as a latent variable with the eight items as characteristics.
observed indicators. Some kinds of victimizations can be considered
Distress. Mental health status was measured with to be variations of a general class, such as property
the anger/aggression, depression, anxiety, dissocia- crime, physical assault, and sexual victimization.
tion, and posttraumatic stress scales of the Trauma For this analysis, scores were calculated to meas-
Symptoms Checklist for Children and thus includes ure adolescents past year experience of six classes
both internalizing and externalizing symptoms. For of victimization by summing the total number of
the purpose of this study, the instruments were short- victimization types that the adolescent experienced
ened for a total of 28 items. Respondents are asked to in each of the six areas. The six classes of victimi-
indicate how often they have experienced each symp- zation and the number of specific types measuring
tom within the past month. Response options are on a each one are as follows: (1) property crime
four-point scale from 1 (not at all) to 4 (very often). (3 specific types), (2) peer-perpetrated victimiza-
For the bivariate analysis shown in Table 1, all item tion (12 specific types), (3) maltreatment (4 spe-
responses for the five scales together were summed to cific types), (4) sexual victimization (6 specific
create an aggregate distress symptom score. The types), (5) witnessing family violence (4 specific
Trauma Symptoms Checklist for Children has shown types), and (6) witnessing community violence
very good reliability and validity in both population- (5 specific types). The items used to identify these
based and clinical samples (Briere 1996; Briere et al. types of victimization are shown in Appendix A.
2001). In this study, the alpha coefficient for this scale Consistent with earlier research on poly-victimiza-
was .93. In the structural equation modeling portion tion (Finkelhor, Ormrod, and Turner 2007a, 2007b;
of our analysis, distress is modeled as a latent variable Turner et al. 2006; Turner et al. 2010), a summary
using respondents mean scores on each of the five measure was also constructed representing the
subscales as the observed indicators. total number of individual victimization types
Victimization. The survey utilized an enhanced from all classes of victimization experienced by
version of the Juvenile Victimization Questionnaire each child in the past year. The mean number of
(JVQ), the most comprehensive inventory of child- victimization types to which children were exposed
hood victimization available for use in a survey within each of the seven aggregate measures is
format (Finkelhor, Hamby, et al. 2005; Finkelhor, displayed in the last column of Table 1. In addition,
Ormrod, et al. 2005a; Hamby et al. 2004). The use of we report the percentage of youth who experienced
simple language and behaviorally specific questions any victimization in each of the six specific cate-
clearly defines the types of incidents that children gories in the past year.
should report. The JVQ was extensively reviewed Family support. Four questions about the adoles-
and tested with victimization specialists, focus cents relationship with his or her family were used
groups of parents and children, and cognitive inter- to construct an indicator of family support. These
views with children to determine the suitability of its represent modified items from the family support
language and content. It has been used in numerous dimension of the Multidimensional Scale of Per-
surveys and has shown evidence of good test-retest ceived Social Support (Zimet et al. 1988). The four
reliability and construct validity across a wide spec- items were, My family really tries to help me, My
family lets me know that they care about me, I
Turner et al. 265

can talk about my problems with my family, and community disorder (see Table 1). For this analy-
My family is willing to help me make decisions. sis, community disorder scores were used to divide
Response choices ranged from 1 = never to 4 = the sample into two groupsthose residing in
always. For bivariate analysis, these four indicators neighborhoods with higher or lower levels of dis-
were summed to create a total family support score order. The mean community disorder score of 1.45
with possible scores ranging from 4 to 16 and was used as the dividing point, with scores of 0 or
higher values indicating higher levels of support 1 classified as lower disorder and scores of 2 or
(Cronbachs alpha: .70). For the structural equation higher classified as higher disorder.
model analyses, family support was modeled as a In the second phase of the analysis, we used
latent variable with scores on the four questions structural equation modeling to model the hypoth-
serving as observed indicators of the latent esized relationships between variables. We began
construct. with confirmatory factor analysis for the latent
Adverse life events. Respondents were asked to variables of community disorder, family support,
report whether they had experienced any of several and distress. We then fit the structural models,
negative non-victimization life events in the past using maximum likelihood estimation in AMOS
year (Turner and Butler 2003; Turner et al. 2006). 19. Our first model assessed the relationship
A sum score was created that indicates how many between community disorder and distress alone.
of nine possible events the adolescent had experi- The subsequent two models examined the relation-
enced. Events included the following: experienced ship between community disorder and distress
a disaster such as a fire, tornado, or hurricane; once adverse events, family support, and victimi-
involved in a very bad accident or had a very zation were added as possible mediators. Victimi-
bad illness; someone close to the respondent was zation was addressed in the structural models in
involved in a bad accident or suffered a bad illness; two ways. In one model, the 6 aggregate types of
repeated a grade in school; a parent lost a job or victimization were considered as separate varia-
was unable to find work; and someone close to the bles to assess their relative associations with com-
respondent attempted suicide or died because of an munity disorder and distress. In the next model, the
accident or illness. 6 aggregate types were removed and replaced with
Demographics. Demographic information was a single summary measure of the total number of
obtained in the initial parent interview, including individual victimization types (out of 34 possible)
the childs gender, age (in years), race-ethnicity experienced in the past year to address the signifi-
(coded into four groups: white non-Hispanic, cance of poly-victimization. All models include
black non-Hispanic, other race non-Hispanic, and sex, age, race, family structure, and SES as exog-
Hispanic any race), SES, and perceptions of neigh- enous predictors. Errors of measurement were
borhood dangerousness. SES is a composite based allowed to correlate between the variables for race,
on the sum of the standardized household income family structure, and SES as these variables were
and standardized parental education (for the parent reported by the adolescents parent in the parent
with the highest education) scores, which was then interview portion of the survey.
restandardized. Family structure, defined by the We report the overall 2 goodness-of-fit statistic
composition of the household, was categorized for each model, but in all models, this statistic was
into four groups: children living with (1) two bio- significant at p < .001. Non-significant 2 statistics
logical or adoptive parents, (2) one biological indicate better model fit, however, in large sam-
parent plus partner (spouse or non-spouse), (3) a ples, it is possible that rather small model-data
single biological parent, and (4) another caregiver. discrepancies can result in a statistically significant
value of the model 2 statistic (Kline 2011,
p. 201). Given our sample size of 2,039, we chose
Data Analysis
not to reject our models based solely on the 2
Data analysis took place in two phases. In the first statistic. We further assessed our model fit using
phase, we conducted bivariate analysis of sample the Bentler Comparative Fit Index (CFI) (Bentler
demographics and predictor variables by level of 1990), the Tucker-Lewis Index (TLI) (Tucker and
266 Journal of Health and Social Behavior 54(2)

Lewis 1973), the Incremental Fit Index (IFI) (Bol- disorder groups. For example, 7 percent of
len 1989), and the Steiger-Lind root mean square respondents in the lower community disorder
error of approximation (RMSEA) (Steiger 1990). group experienced some form of sexual victimiza-
tion in the past year, while nearly 24 percent of
those in the higher disorder group were exposed to
Results this form of victimization. About 10 percent of
Bivariate Analyses youth in lower disorder communities reported mal-
treatment by a caregiver, yet over 24 percent in
Table 1 displays demographic characteristics, vic- higher disorder communities experienced mal-
timization rates, and mean scores for adversity, treatment in the past year. The total number of
family support, and distress for the sample as a different victimization types to which youth were
whole (last column), and across community disor- exposed was also significantly greater in the higher
der group (columns 1 and 2). Substantial differ- community disorder group. The average number of
ences across the two community disorder groups victimization types for the lower disorder group
were evident for almost all factors considered. was 1.5, while it was 3.6 for those living in higher
With respect to race-ethnicity, white non-Hispanics disorder neighborhoods.
were over-represented in lower disorder communi- The table in Appendix B in the online supplement
ties, while all other racial-ethnic groups were over- (available at http://hsb.sagepub.com/content/by/sup-
represented in communities with higher disorder. plemental-data) displays correlations among com-
The percentage of black youth living in higher munity disorder, distress, all demographic factors,
disorder neighborhoods, for example, was twice and all hypothesized predictors. Significant correla-
that of those living in lower disorder neighbor- tions between the demographic factors, distress, and
hoods. Similarly, youth living with two biological many of the hypothesized predictors confirms the
or adoptive parents were more likely to live in need to control for these in analyses assessing direct
lower disorder communities, while youth in all and indirect effects of community disorder.
other family structures were over-represented in
higher disorder communities. Those living with
Structural Equation Models
single parents were particularly over-represented
in neighborhoods with higher levels of disorder. As Our first model (not shown) was a structural equa-
expected, there were also significant differences in tion model estimating the direct effect of commu-
the mean SES level of the higher and lower disor- nity disorder on distress symptoms controlling for
der neighborhoods. Finally, older youth were more demographic characteristics (2 = 991; df = 180;
likely to report living in a disordered community p < .001; CFI = .92; TLI = .90; IFI = .92; RMSEA
than were younger youth. There was no significant = .05), including SES, race-ethnicity, age, gender,
gender difference in the lower and higher disorder and family structure. The unstandardized coeffi-
groups. cient of the path from community disorder to dis-
As expected, mean levels of distress for youth tress was .98 (p < .001), and the standardized
living in higher disorder neighborhoods were sig- estimate was .43, indicating a relatively strong
nificantly greater than for those in the lower com- relationship between community disorder and ado-
munity disorder group. The same is true of all the lescent distress, with higher levels of community
hypothesized mediators. Mean adversity scores disorder associated with higher levels of distress,
were significantly higher, and mean levels of fam- independent of the demographic correlates shown
ily support were lower in the higher community in Table 1. The squared multiple correlation (R2smc)
disorder group. Moreover, the percentage of youth for distress in this model was .19.
exposed to each of the six types of victimization Next, we estimated a model of the relationships
was significantly greater in the higher disorder between community disorder, distress, and the
group. All victimization differences are substan- other variables of interest: past-year life events,
tial, ranging from over 1.5 to almost 4 times the victimization, and family social support (see
rate of exposure in higher versus lower community Figure 1). In this model, the six aggregate types
Turner et al. 267

Family Cares about Talk about Helps


helps me me problems w/decisions
Female Drug sales .52
.59 .56 .66 .61
Age
Arrests .55 Adverse Life Family Social
Events - PY .07 Support
SES
Neighborhood
.40 -.35 Anxiety
gangs or gang .64
Black -.39
graffi .74
.06 Anger
Hispanic Community .74
.44 Disorder Distress
Police raids Depression
Other .83
.34 .36 Property .13
race
Stay inside .75 Dissociaon
.30 .11
.32 Maltreatment
Single
.33 .07 .75
parent Decay .52 Sexual Post-traumac
stress
Parent & .43 .19
Peer
partner Gangs in
school .45
.28 .03
Family violence
Other adult
Guns/knives in Community
.51
school .03
violence

Figure 1. Structural Equation Model of Community Disorder and Distress: Mediating Effects of Adverse Life Events,
Family Support, and Six Aggregate Victimization Types.
Note: Standardized coefficients are shown. Dotted lines represent non-significant paths for unstandardized estimates.
Model 2 = 1,622; df = 418; p < .001; CFI = .92; TLI = .90; IFI = .92; RMSEA = .04; R2smc for distress = .42.

of victimization were entered as separate varia- Community disorder was also associated with
bles. Paths, coefficients, and correlated errors of all six types of victimization exposure. Although,
measurement for demographic controls are omitted as might be expected, community disorder is
from the diagram for simplicity, as are the distur- most strongly associated with witnessing com-
bance terms and correlations between them. In the munity violence, it also has independent associa-
first iteration of this model, a path was estimated tions with property crime, child maltreatment,
between past-year adversity and family social sup- sexual victimization, peer-perpetrated assault and
port; however, this path was not significant and was bullying, and witnessing family violence. The
trimmed from the model shown in Figure 1. four types of personal victimizationproperty
This model fits the data reasonably well given crime, maltreatment, sexual victimization, and
the number of parameters estimated. (2 = 1,622; peer assault/bullyingwere, in turn, associated
df = 418; p < .001; CFI = .92; TLI = .90; IFI = .92; with increased distress. Witnessing community
RMSEA = .04). As Figure 1 shows, there are sev- violence and witnessing family violence were not
eral factors that significantly mediate the commu- significantly related to distress with the other
nity disorderdistress association. Community forms of victimization, life events, and family
disorder has a strong positive association with support controlled.
exposure to recent negative life events (Beta = .40) Of particular relevance to the goals of the study
and a strong negative association with level of is the finding that there were no direct effects of
family social support (Beta = .39). Both these community disorder on symptomatology once vic-
factors, in turn, were related to distress symptoms. timization exposure, family support, and adverse
Family support had a particularly strong associa- (non-victimization) life events were controlled. It
tion, with higher levels of support related to lower appears that the strong association between com-
levels of distress (Beta = .35), independent of all munity disorder and distress in this national sam-
other variables in the model. ple of youth is fully mediated by these other
268 Journal of Health and Social Behavior 54(2)

Adverse Life independent and relative effects of community dis-


Family Social
Events Past Year Support order on specific aggregate categories of victimiza-
.08
tion and assessed the specific types as independent
.40 -.36 predictors of distress, the analyses presented in
-.38
Figure 2 focus on the mediating effects of poly-
Community .05 Distress victimization. The model in Figure 2 shows an
Disorder
acceptable fit (2 = 1,332; df = 308; p < .001; CFI =
.92, TLI = .91; IFI = .92; RMSEA = .04). The over-
.64 .35 all fit and variance explained are very similar in
Number of Past
Year Vicmizaon both models, as are the coefficients for adverse life
Types events and family support. However, the strength of
Figure 2. Structural Equation Model of Community the associations between community disorder and
Disorder and Distress: Mediating Effects of Adverse Life poly-victimization and between poly-victimization
Events, Family Support, and Total Number of and distress are particularly strong. Indeed, cumula-
Victimization Types (Poly-Victimization). tive exposure to multiple forms of victimization is
Note: Standardized coefficients are shown. Dotted lines
represent non-significant paths for unstandardized the most powerful mediator of the community dis-
estimates. Model 2 = 1,332; df = 308; p < .001; CFI = orderdistress relationship evident in either model.
.92; TLI = .91; IFI = .92; RMSEA = .04; R2smc for distress The indirect effect of community disorder on dis-
= .42.
tress through the multiple victimization measure is
.22 (.64 .35; p < .001), which accounts for 57
percent of the total indirect effect of .39 in this
factors. The model explains 42 percent of the vari- model. Again, there is no direct effect of community
ance in distress symptoms. disorder on psychological distress once multiple
The total indirect effect of community disorder on victimization, adverse life events, and family sup-
distress through the mediators of adversity, family port are taken into account.
support, and victimization types is .37 in this model. We also tested two alternative models that were
The statistical significance of indirect paths in the nested in our final model of Figure 2 (not shown). In
model was tested using the unstandardized coeffi- one of these, the paths between community disorder
cients and the Sobel test (Kline 2011). All indirect and past-year adversity and between community
paths through a single mediator were significant, disorder and family social support were trimmed.
except those from community disorder to distress This models fit was inferior to that of the model in
through witnessing family violence and from com- Figure 2 (2 = 1,710; df = 310; p < .001; CFI = .90;
munity disorder to distress through witnessing com- TLI = .87; IFI = .90; RMSEA = .04; R2smc for dis-
munity violence. Comparing indirect effects using tress = .38). A second nested model, in which the
standardized coefficients, more than one third of the path from community disorder to number of past-
indirect effect of community disorder on distress year victimization types was trimmed, was also
(36.6 percent) is through family support (.39 .35 significantly poorer in fit relative to the model
= .14; p < .001), which has the strongest indirect depicted in Figure 2 (2 = 1,854; df = 309; p < .001;
effect in the model. The next strongest indirect effects CFI = .89; TLI = .86; IFI = .89; RMSEA = .04; R2smc
of community disorder on distress are through peer for distress = .39). Although not central to our
assault and bullying (.43 .19 = .08; p < .001; 22 objectives, we also tested whether total victimiza-
percent), property victimization (.36 .13 = .05; p < tion exposure interacted with community disorder in
.001; 13 percent), and adverse life events (.40 .07 = its effects on adolescent distress, but this interaction
.03; p < .001; 8 percent). was not significant (not shown).
Figure 2 displays the same structural equation
model substituting the specific victimization catego-
ries with a measure of poly-victimization (i.e., sum
Discussion
of the total number of individual victimization types Residence in disordered neighborhoods is unevenly
to which youth were exposed in the past year). Thus, distributed by social status. Findings confirm that
while analyses shown in Figure 1 examined the black youth, those living with single parents, and
Turner et al. 269

those whose families have lower SES are espe- with significantly lower levels of family support
cially likely to live in disordered communities. and higher levels of adverse life events.
Older adolescents also report higher levels of com- Structural equation modeling was used to
munity disorder than younger youth, likely because examine the direct and indirect effects of commu-
their greater independence and activities outside of nity disorder on level of distress through victimiza-
the household make them more aware of commu- tion exposure, family support, and adverse life
nity conditions and occurrences. events. Community disorder had a relatively strong
Findings also clearly confirm that youth living association with distress symptoms, independent
in neighborhoods with higher levels of disorder of all demographic factors, when none of the
experience significantly higher levels of psycho- hypothesized mediators were included. However,
logical distress, independent of the demographic the full model indicated that the effect of commu-
correlates listed above. The current study has nity disorder on distress was entirely indirect, fully
sought to better specify this association; what are accounted for by the mediating effects of the other
the mechanisms that explain increased distress factors.
among youth in more disordered communities? Although this study did not address intergen-
Earlier research with this same objective has erational processes directly, findings do suggest
tended to emphasize chronic adversity, lack of that community disorder affects youth mental
positive social attachments, and fear of crime and health, at least in part, through family processes.
victimization as important mediators. Although Family support showed a strong mediating effect;
past studies acknowledge that community disorder community disorder was associated with lower
increases risk of actual victimization exposure, family support, which in turn was related to greater
most studies have not adequately assessed experi- adolescent symptomatology. Although research on
enced victimization. Consistent with sociological adults has suggested that lower social support in
inquiries showing the importance of a more thor- disordered communities occurs because residents
ough assessment of stress exposure (Turner et al. are less likely to engage in neighborhood social
1995), current research incorporated a more com- activities (Ross 2000), experience less trust and
prehensive assessment of victimization to better reciprocity from their neighbors (Ross and
determine whether it represents an important medi- Mirowsky 2009), and are less likely to feel cared
ator in the association between community disor- for, esteemed, or able to count on neighbors (Kim
der and distress in youth. We also considered 2010), the current research highlights how family
whether other forms of stress exposure (adverse relationships may be affected by community disor-
life events) and an important resource in the stress der. Less perceived support from family may, at
process, family support, operated as additional least in part, be a function of the damaging effects
mediators. of community disorder on parent mental health,
Consistent with recent epidemiological data on parenting competence, and the quality of interac-
childhood exposure to violence, this nationally tions between the adolescent and parents and sib-
representative sample of 10- to 17-year-olds lings. Past research has found correlates of
reported substantial levels of victimization. The community disorder, such as neighborhood pov-
proportion of the population experiencing each erty, instability, and crime, to be associated with
category of victimization, within a one-year period, less parental warmth, more harsh and inconsistent
was certainly not trivial, and in each case, the like- discipline practices, poorer parental efficacy,
lihood of exposure was significantly greater for and greater maternal distress (Christie-Mizell and
youth living in more disordered communities. Erickson 2007; Christie-Mizell, Steelman, and
Importantly, cumulative exposure across different Stewart 2003; Gutman, McLoyd, and Tokoyawa
types of victimization was substantially greater in 2005; Pinderhughes et al. 2001). These conditions
more disordered neighborhoods, with the higher and behaviors, in turn, are likely to reduce adoles-
disorder group exposed to an average of almost cents sense of support from parents. Numerous
four different forms of victimization in the past studies have shown the importance of perceived
year. Community disorder was also associated parental support in predicting both internalizing
270 Journal of Health and Social Behavior 54(2)

and externalizing symptoms in adolescents (Auer- poly-victimization. Youth living in more disor-
bach et al. 2011; Bogard 2005; Needham 2008; dered neighborhoods were exposed to significantly
Stice, Ragan, and Randall 2004). The current find- more different types of victimization within a one-
ings on the effects of family support on distress are year period. Consistent with several recent studies,
certainly consistent with this previous research. poly-victimization, in turn, has a strong negative
Another mechanism that may also implicate effect on the mental health of youth (Finkelhor
family processes is the mediating influence of et al. 2007a; Turner et al. 2010). Although the rea-
adverse life events. The total number of negative sons for the powerful effects of poly-victimization
life events (non-victimization) occurring in the have yet to be fully specified, exposure to many
past year was significantly greater for youth living different forms of victimizations likely means
in neighborhoods with higher levels of community widespread adversity and threats to safety across
disorder, and this was in turn associated with multiple contexts of adolescents lives. Thus, those
greater distress levels. Many of these events repre- residing in disordered communities are more likely
sent occurrences, such as illnesses, accidents, and to experience personal victimization as a chronic
job loss, that involve a parent or close family mem- life condition than are other youth. Cross-context
ber. Thus, they may represent stress not only for victimization may also damage the adolescents
the adolescent but also for the family as a whole. potential for resiliency by creating deficits in
The positive associations between community dis- social and personal resources (such as social sup-
order and two primary forms of child victimiza- port, self-esteem, and mastery) that would nor-
tionchild maltreatment and witnessing family mally help to moderate the negative effects of
violencealso point to family processes. That is, victimization and suggests no safe refuge in an
disordered neighborhoods are associated with alternative domain.
greater exposure to caregiver-perpetrated violence, Our results do not necessarily challenge past
not just violence perpetrated by peers, neighbors, findings on the importance of psychosocial medi-
and other community members, suggesting its ators, such as mastery, powerlessness, and mis-
influence on adolescents through parent function- trust, since it is possible that these psychosocial
ing. Future research would benefit from a direct processes also operate among youth who have
examination of the intergenerational family pro- experienced substantial victimization. However,
cesses linking community disorder to the mental our findings do challenge the notion emphasized
health of adolescents. in some past studies on adults (Ross 2011; Ross
A primary goal of this study was to assess the and Mirowsky 2009) that community disorder
role of actual exposure to violence and victimiza- triggers distress-producing processes independent
tion in explaining elevated distress among youth in of actual victimization exposure. Indeed, our find-
higher disorder communities. We found that vic- ings suggest that personal exposure to violence
timization does importantly mediate the associa- and victimizationfound in hundreds of studies
tion. Several different types of victimization had to have devastating effects on both the short- and
independent effects, while cumulative exposure to the long-term well-being of youthrepresented a
multiple forms of victimization was an especially core factor explaining the distressing effect of
powerful mediator. Specifically, significant indi- community disorder. It is unclear whether these
rect effects of community disorder on distress were contrasting findings are due to a greater preva-
found through increased exposure to property vic- lence and/or potency of victimization among
timization, maltreatment by caregivers, sexual vic- young people relative to adults or whether the
timization, and peer-perpetrated assault and more comprehensive victimization assessment in
bullying. Thus, several forms of victimization not the current study allowed for a more accurate
typically assessed in community disorder research determination of its importance. Future research
were both important outcomes of community dis- on community disorder would benefit from direct
order and predictors of distress. The widespread comparative analyses of child and adult popula-
risk of victimization in disordered neighborhoods tions that incorporate adequate victimization
is also highlighted by its strong association with measurement.
Turner et al. 271

Limitations indicators. Nevertheless, incorporating commu-


nity-level indicators, which were not available in
A number of limitations should be acknowledged. our data, such as crime rates, institutional resources
First, this study is cross-sectional, and as a result, (e.g., availability of medical services, parks and
the causal order of some factors cannot be fully recreation, libraries) and housing quality (e.g.,
established. Although adolescent victimization is housing value, home ownership, vacancy rates),
very likely an outcome of residing in particular may help to better specify the mechanisms that
neighborhoods rather than the reverse, certain link disordered communities to youth victimiza-
negative life events like parental job loss can tion, family adversity, and low social support.
sometimes require relocation to more disadvan-
taged neighborhoods. Other selection processes
may also occur whereby adults who perpetrate
Conclusion
violence, including violence directed at family Community disorder has substantial negative
members, may be more likely to move into disor- effects on the mental health of youth. The detri-
dered neighborhoods. Additionally, despite the mental effects of residing in neighborhoods char-
substantial research establishing the effects of acterized by rundown housing, graffiti, public
social support on well-being, it is possible that loitering, alcohol and drug use, and gang activity
adolescent distress contributes to lower percep- exist because young people in their neighborhoods
tions of family support. It is also possible that experience considerable personal victimization
families who are most supportive and concerned and often witness violence. Community disorder is
with childrens safety, stability, and nurturance are also associated with greater exposure to other
more likely to avoid or leave disordered neighbor- forms of adversity and lower family support.
hoods. Although our cross-sectional data leave Efforts to address problems of community disorder
open the possibility of alternative interpretations, for children must acknowledge the important role
we believe the advantages of our comprehensive of actual exposure to violence and victimization as
measurement of actual youth victimization expo- well as its implication for parenting behaviors,
sure and the findings demonstrating their potent including abusive interactions and the inability to
effects over and above community disorder offer provide adequate support to children.
an important and unique contribution to the litera-
ture. Future research should attempt to examine
these mediating associations prospectively with
References
longitudinal data. Acierno, Ron, Heidi Resnick, Dean G. Kilpatrick, and
As is a potential problem in many studies of Wendy Stark-Riemer. 2003. Assessing Elder Victim-
stress exposure, individuals may sometimes tele- izationDemonstration of a Methodology. Social
scope more distal events into the study frame Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology 38:64453.
period. Although analyses on a longitudinal survey Aisenberg, Eugene and Todd Herrenkhohl. 2008. Com-
using the same victimization measures found little munity Violence in Context: Risk and Resilience in
evidence of telescoping bias (Finkelhor, Ormrod, Children and Families. Journal of Interpersonal Vio-
and Turner 2007c), telescoping can inflate rates of lence 23:296315.
past-year victimization. To the extent that dis- Aneshensel, Carol S. and Clea A. Sucoff. 1996. The
tressed youth are particularly likely to telescope Neighborhood Context of Adolescent Mental Health.
victimization events into the past-year time frame, Journal of Health and Social Behavior 37:293310.
associations between distress and victimization Atrostic, Barbara K., Nancy Bates, Geraldine Burt, and
would also be inflated. Adriana Silberstein. 2001. Nonresponse in U.S.
Finally, as cited previously, there is ample evi- Government Household Surveys: Consistent Mea-
dence that subjective indicators of community sures, Recent Trends, and New Insights. Journal of
disorder represent valid indices of objective neigh- Official Statistics 17:20926.
borhood characteristics and are more often strongly Auerbach, Randy Patrick, Joseph S. Bigda-Pey-
linked to distress than objective aggregate-level ton, Nicole K. Eberhart, Christian A. Webb, and
272 Journal of Health and Social Behavior 54(2)

Moon-Ho Ringo Ho. 2011. Conceptualizing the Pro- Curry, Aaron D., Carl A. Latkin, and Melissa Davey-
spective Relationship between Social Support, Stress, Rothwell. 2008. Pathways to Depression: The
and Depressive Symptoms among Adolescents. Impact of Neighborhood Violent Crime on Inner-city
Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology 39:47587. Residents in Baltimore, Maryland, USA. Social Sci-
Babbie, Earl. 2007. The Practice of Social Research. Bel- ence & Medicine 67:2330.
mont, CA: Wadsworth. Curtin, Richard, Stanley Presser, and Eleanor Singer.
Bajos, Nathalie, Alfred Spira, Beatrice Ducot, and 2000. The Effects of Response Rate Changes on
Antoine Messiah. 1992. Analysis of Sexual Behav- the Index of Consumer Sentiment. Public Opinion
ior in France (ACSF): A Comparison between Two Quarterly 64:41328.
Modes of Investigation: Telephone Survey and Face- Curtin, Richard, Stanley Presser, and Eleanor Singer.
to-face Survey. AIDS 6:31523. 2005. Changes in Telephone Survey Nonresponse
Bentler, Peter M. 1990. Comparative Fit Indexes over the Past Quarter Century. Public Opinion Quar-
in Structural Analysis. Psychological Bulletin terly 69:8798.
102:23846. Cutrona, Carolyn E., Gail Wallace, and Kristin A.
Bermack, Elaine. 1989. Effects of Telephone and Face- Wesner. 2006. Neighborhood Characteristics and
to-face Communication on Rated Extent of Self-Dis- Depression: An Examination of Stress Process. Cur-
closure by Female College Students. Psychological rent Directions in Psychological Science 15:18892.
Reports 65:25967. Czaja, Ronald. 1987. Asking Sensitive Behavioral
Bogard, Kimber L. 2005. Affluent Adolescents, Depres- Questions in Telephone Interviews. International
sion, and Drug Use: The Role of Adults in their Quarterly of Community Health Education 8:2332.
Lives. Adolescence 40:281306. Dodge, Kenneth A., Gregory S. Pettit, and John E. Bates.
Bollen, Kenneth A. 1989. A New Incremental Fit Index 1997. How the Experience of Early Physical Abuse
for General Structural Equation Models. Sociologi- Leads Children to Become Chronically Aggressive.
cal Methods and Research 17:30316. Pp. 26368 in Rochester Symposium on Developmental
Bond, Lyndal, John B. Carlin, Lyndal Thomas, Kerryn Psychopathology. Vol. 8, The Effects of Trauma on the
Rubin, and George Patton. 2001. Does Bullying Developmental Process, edited by D. Cicchetti and S.
Cause Emotional Problems? A Prospective Study L. Toth. Rochester, NY: University of Rochester Press.
of Young Teenagers. British Medical Journal 323: Edleson, Jeffrey L., Lyungai F. Mbilinyi, Sandra K.
48084. Beeman, and Annelies K. Hagemeister. 2003. How
Briere, John. 1996. Trauma Symptoms Checklist for Chil- Children Are Involved in Adult Domestic Violence:
dren (TSCC): Professional Manual. Odessa, FL: Psy- Results from a Four-city Telephone Survey. Journal
chological Assessment Resources. of Interpersonal Violence 18:1832.
Briere, John, Kerri Johnson, Angela Bissada, Linda Elo, Irma T., Laryssa Mykyta, Rachel Margolis, and Jennifer
Damon, Julie Crouch, Eliana Gil, Rochelle Hanson, F. Culhane. 2009. Perceptions of Neighborhood Disor-
and Vickie Ernst. 2001. The Trauma Symptom der: The Roles of Individual and Neighborhood Charac-
Checklist for Young Children (TSCYC): Reliability teristics. Social Science Quarterly 90:12981320.
and Association with Abuse Exposure in a Multi-site Espelage, Dorothy L. and Melissa K. Holt. 2001. Bully-
Study. Child Abuse & Neglect 25:100114. ing and Victimization during Early Adolescence: Peer
Browne, Angela and David Finkelhor. 1986. The Impact Influences and Psychosocial Correlates. Pp. 13242
of Child Sexual Abuse: A Review of the Research. in Bullying Behavior: Current Issues, Research, and
Psychological Bulletin 99:6677. Interventions, edited by R. A. Geffner and M. Loring.
Christie-Mizell, C. Andre and Rebecca J. Erickson. 2007. Binghamton, NY: Haworth.
Mothers and Mastery: The Consequences of Per- Feiring, Candice, Lynn Taska, and Michael Lewis. 2002.
ceived Neighborhood Disorder. Social Psychology Adjustment following Sexual Abuse Discovery: The
Quarterly 70:34065. Role of Shame and Attributional Style. Developmen-
Christie-Mizell, C. Andre, Lala Carr Steelman, and Jen- tal Psychology 38:7992.
nifer Stewart. 2003. Seeing Their Surroundings: The Finkelhor, David. 2008. Childhood Victimization: Vio-
Effects of Neighborhood Setting and Race on Mater- lence, Crime, and Abuse in the Lives of Young People.
nal Distress. Social Science Research 32:40729. New York: Oxford University Press.
Turner et al. 273

Finkelhor, David, Sherry L. Hamby, Richard K Ormrod, Gutman, Leslie M., Vonnie C. McLoyd, and Teru Tokoy-
and Heather A. Turner. 2005. The JVQ: Reliabil- awa. 2005. Financial Strain, Neighborhood Stress,
ity, Validity, and National Norms. Child Abuse & Parenting Behaviors, and Adolescent Adjustment
Neglect 29:383412. in Urban African American Families. Journal of
Finkelhor, David, Richard K Ormrod, and Heather A. Research on Adolescence 15:229425.
Turner. 2007a. Poly-victimization: A Neglected Hamby, Sherry L., David Finkelhor, Richard K. Ormrod,
Component in Child Victimization Trauma. Child and Heather A. Turner. 2004. The Juvenile Victimiza-
Abuse & Neglect 31:726. tion Questionnaire (JVQ): Administration and Scor-
Finkelhor, David, Richard K Ormrod, and Heather A. ing Manual. Durham, NH: Crimes against Children
Turner. 2007b. Poly-victimization and Trauma in Research Center.
a National Longitudinal Cohort. Development and Hamby, Sherry L., David Finkelhor, Heather A. Turner,
Psychopathology 19:14966. and Melissa K. Holt. 2007. The Community Disorder
Finkelhor, David, Richard K Ormrod, and Heather A. Questionnaire. Durham, NH: Crimes against Chil-
Turner. 2007c. Revictimization Patterns in a National dren Research Center.
Longitudinal Sample of Children and Youth. Child Hamby, Sherry L., David Finkelhor, Heather A. Turner,
Abuse & Neglect 31:479502. and Richard Ormrod. 2010. The Overlap of Witness-
Finkelhor, David, Richard K Ormrod, Heather A. Turner, ing Partner Violence with Child Maltreatment and
and Sherry L. Hamby. 2005a. Measuring Poly- Other Victimizations in a Nationally Representative
victimization Using the JVQ. Child Abuse & Neglect Survey of Youth. Child Abuse & Neglect 34:73441.
29:12971312. Hill, Terrence D., Catherine E. Ross, and Ronald J. Angel.
Finkelhor, David, Richard K Ormrod, Heather A. Turner, 2005. Neighborhood Disorder, Psychophysiological
and Sherry L. Hamby. 2005b. The Victimization of Distress, and Health. Journal of Health and Social
Children and Youth: A Comprehensive, National Sur- Behavior 46:17086.
vey. Child Maltreatment 10:525. Keeter, Scott, Courtney Kennedy, Michael Dimock, Jona-
Finkelhor, David, Heather A. Turner, Richard Ormrod, than Best, and Peyton Craighill. 2006. Gauging the
and Sherry L. Hamby. 2009. Violence, Abuse, and Impact of Growing Nonresponse on Estimates from
Crime Exposure in a National Sample of Children a National RDD Telephone Survey. Public Opinion
and Youth. Pediatrics 124:113. Quarterly 70:75979.
Finkelhor, David, Heather A. Turner, Richard Ormrod, Keeter, Scott, Carolyn Miller, Andrew Kohut, Robert M.
and Sherry L. Hamby. 2010. Trends in Childhood Groves, and Stanley Presser. 2000. Consequences of
Violence and Abuse Exposure: Evidence from Two Reducing Nonresponse in a National Telephone Sur-
National Surveys. Archives of Pediatrics & Adoles- vey. Public Opinion Quarterly 64:12548.
cent Medicine 164:23842. Kim, Joongbaeck. 2010. Neighborhood Disadvan-
Gary, Tiffany L., Sarah A. Stark, and Thomas A. LaVeist. tage and Mental Health: The Role of Neighborhood
2007. Neighborhood Characteristics and Mental Disorder and Social Relationships. Social Science
Health among African Americans and Whites Living Research 39:26071.
in a Racially Integrated Urban Community. Health Kim, Joongbaeck and Meghan E. Conley. 2011. Neigh-
and Place 13:56975. borhood Disorder and the Sense of Personal Control:
Geis, Karlyn J. and Catherine E. Ross. 1998. A New Which Factors Moderate the Association? Journal
Look at Urban Alienation: The Effect of Neighbor- of Community Psychology 39:894907.
hood Disorder on Perceived Powerlessness. Social Kitzmann, Katherine M., Noni K. Gaylord, Aimee R. Holt,
Psychology Quarterly 61:23246. and Erin D. Kenny. 2003. Child Witnesses to Domestic
Green, Arthur H. 1993. Child Sexual Abuse: Immediate Violence: A Meta-analytic Review. Journal of Consult-
and Long-term Effects and Intervention. Journal of ing and Clinical Psychology 71:33952.
the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psy- Kline, Rex B. 2011. Principles and Practice of Structural
chiatry 32:890902. Equation Modeling. 3rd. ed. New York: Guilford.
Groves, Robert M. 2006. Nonresponse Rates and Nonre- Latkin, Carl A. and Aaron D. Curry. 2003. Stressful
sponse Bias in Household Surveys. Public Opinion Neighborhoods and Depression: A Prospective Study
Quarterly 70:64675. of the Impact of Neighborhood Disorder. Journal of
274 Journal of Health and Social Behavior 54(2)

Health and Social Behavior 44:3444. Quillian, Lincoln and Devah Pager. 2001. Black Neigh-
Marin, Gerardo and Barbara VanOss Marin. 1989. A bors, Higher Crime? The Role of Racial Stereotypes
Comparison of Three Interviewing Approaches for in Evaluations of Neighborhood Crime. American
Studying Sensitive Topics with Hispanics. Hispanic Journal of Sociology 107:71767.
Journal of Behavioral Sciences 11:33040. Richters, John E. and Pedro Martinez. 1993. The NIMH
McAuliffe, William E., Stephanie Geller, Richard LaBrie, Community Violence Project: I. Children as Victims
Susannah Paletz, and Elizabeth Fournier. 1998. Are of and Witnesses to Violence. Psychiatry 56:721.
Telephone Surveys Suitable for Studying Substance Ross, Catherine E. 2000. Neighborhood Disadvantage
Abuse? Cost, Administration, Coverage and Response and Adult Depression. Journal of Health and Social
Rate Issues. Journal of Drug Issues 28:45581. Behavior 41:17787.
Merkle, Daniel and Murray Edelman. 2002. Nonre- Ross, Catherine E. 2011. Collective Threat, Trust, and
sponse in Exit Polls: A Comprehensive Analysis. the Sense of Personal Control. Journal of Health and
Pp. 34358 in Survey Nonresponse, edited by R. M. Social Behavior 52:28796.
Groves, D. A. Dillman, J. L. Eltinge, and R. J. A. Ross, Catherine E. and John Mirowsky. 2001. Neighbor-
Little. New York: John Wiley. hood Disadvantage, Disorder, and Health. Journal of
Needham, Belinda L. 2008. Reciprocal Relationships Health and Social Behavior 42:25876.
between Symptoms of Depression and Parental Ross, Catherine E. and John Mirowsky. 2009. Neighbor-
Support during the Transition from Adolescence to hood Disorder, Subjective Alienation, and Distress.
Young Adulthood. Journal of Youth & Adolescence Journal of Health and Social Behavior 50:4964.
37:893905. Ross, Catherine E., John Mirowsky, and Shana Pribesh.
Osofsky, Joy D., Sarah Wewers, Della M. Hann, and 2001. Powerlessness and the Amplification of
Ana C. Fick. 1993. Chronic Community Violence: Threat: Neighborhood Disadvantage, Disorder,
What Is Happening to Our Children? Psychiatry and Mistrust. American Sociological Review 66:
56:3645. 56891.
Pearlin, Leonard I. 1999. The Stress Process Revisited: Sampson, Robert J., Jeffrey D. Morenoff, and Thomas
Reflections on Concepts and Their Interrelation- Gannon-Rowley. 2002. Assessing Neighborhood
ships. Pp. 395415 in Handbook of the Sociology of Effects: Social Processes and New Directions in
Mental Health, edited by C. S. Aneshensel and J. C. Research. Annual Review of Sociology 28:44378.
Phelan. Boston: Kluwer Academic. Sampson, Robert J. and Stephen W. Raudenbush. 2004.
Perkins, Douglas D., John W. Meeks, and Ralph B. Seeing Disorder: Neighborhood Stigma and the
Taylor. 1992. The Physical Environment of Street Social Construction of Broken Windows. Social
Blocks and Resident Perceptions of Crime and Dis- Psychology Quarterly 67:31942.
order: Implications for Theory and Measurement. Schieman, Scott and Stephen C. Meersman. 2004.
Journal of Environmental Psychology 12:2134. Neighborhood Problems and Health among Older
Perkins, Douglas D. and Ralph B. Taylor. 1996. Eco- Adults: Received and Donated Social Support and the
logical Assessments of Community Disorder: Their Sense of Mastery as Effect Modifiers. The Journals
Relationship to Fear of Crime and Theoretical Impli- of Gerontology, Series B: Psychological and Social
cations. American Journal of Community Psychol- Sciences 59B:S8997.
ogy 24:63107. Singer, Eleanor. 2006. Introduction: Nonresponse Bias
Pinderhughes, Ellen E., Robert Nix, E. Michael Fos- in Household Surveys. Public Opinion Quarterly
ter, and Damon Jones. 2001. Parenting in Context: 70:63745.
Impact of Neighborhood Poverty, Residential Stabil- Steiger, James H. 1990. Structural Model Evaluation
ity, Public Services, Social Networks, and Danger on and Modification: An Interval Estimation Approach.
Parental Behaviors. Journal of Marriage and the Multivariate Behavioral Research 25:17380.
Family 63:94153. Stice, Eric, Jennifer Ragan, and Patrick Randall. 2004.
Pruchno, Rachel A. and Jason M. Hayden. 2000. Inter- Prospective Relations between Social Support and
view Modality: Effects on Costs and Data Quality in Depression: Differential Direction of Effects for Par-
a Sample of Older Women. Journal of Aging and ent and Peer Support? Journal of Abnormal Psychol-
Health 12:324. ogy 113:15559.
Turner et al. 275

Stiffman, Arlene Rubin, Eric Hadley-Ives, Diane Elze, Author Biographies


Sharon Johnson, and Peter Dore. 1999. Impact
of Environment on Adolescent Mental Health and Heather A. Turner is a professor of sociology and a
Behavior: Structural Equation Modeling. American senior research associate at the Crimes against Children
Journal of Orthopsychiatry 69:7386. Research Center at the University of New Hampshire.
Taylor, Anthea. 2002. Ill Call You Back on My Mobile: Dr. Turners research program has concentrated on social
A Critique of the Telephone Interview with Adoles- stress processes and mental health. She is especially
cent Boys. Westminster Studies in Education 25: interested in the effects of violence, victimization, and
1934. other forms of adversity on the health and development of
Toth, Sheree L., Jody T. Manly, and Dante Cicchetti. 1992. children and adolescents. Dr. Turner is also the director of
Child Maltreatment and Vulnerability to Depression. the International Conference on Social Stress Research
Development & Psychopathology 4:97112. and past chair of the Sociology of Mental Health Section
Tucker, Ledyard R. and Charles Lewis. 1973. A Reli- of the American Sociological Association.
ability Coefficient for Maximum Likelihood Factor
Analysis. Psychometrika 38:110. Anne Shattuck is a research associate at the University
Turner, Heather A. and Melissa J. Butler. 2003. Direct of New Hampshires Crimes against Children Research
and Indirect Effects of Childhood Adversity on Center. Her work at the Crimes against Children Research
Depressive Symptoms in Young Adults. Journal of Center focuses on analyzing patterns of crime and vio-
Youth and Adolescence 32:89103. lence against children and youth and associations between
Turner, Heather A., David Finkelhor, and Richard Orm- childhood victimization and outcomes such as mental
rod. 2006. The Effect of Lifetime Victimization on health and juvenile delinquency. She is a doctoral candi-
the Mental Health of Children and Adolescents. date in sociology at the University of New Hampshire.
Social Science & Medicine 62:1327.
Turner, Heather A., David Finkelhor, and Richard Orm- Sherry Hamby is a research professor of psychology at
rod. 2010. Poly-victimization in a National Sample the University of the South and the founding editor of the
of Children and Youth. American Journal of Preven- American Psychological Association journal Psychology
tive Medicine 38:32330. of Violence. A licensed psychologist, she conducted the
Turner, R. Jay, Blair Wheaton, and Donald A. Lloyd. first reservation-based study of domestic violence among
1995. The Epidemiology of Social Stress. Ameri- American Indians. She is the author or co-author of more
can Sociological Review 60:10425. than 75 works including The Conflict Tactics Scales
Weeks, Michael F., Richard A. Kulka, Judith T. Lessler, Handbook. Her most recent book, authored with John
and Roy W. Whitmore. 1983. Personal versus Tele- Grych, is the recently released Web of Violence.
phone Surveys from Collecting Household Health
Data at the Local Level. American Journal of Public David Finkelhor is the director of the Crimes against
Health 73:138994. Children Research Center, a co-director of the Family
Wen, Ming, Louise C. Hawkley, and John T. Cacioppo. 2006. Research Laboratory, a professor of sociology, and Uni-
Objective and Perceived Neighborhood Environment, versity Professor at the University of New Hampshire. He
Individual SES and Psychosocial Factors, and Self-rated has been studying the problems of child victimization,
Health: An Analysis of Older Adults in Cook County, child maltreatment, and family violence since 1977. He is
Illinois. Social Science & Medicine 63:257590. well known for his conceptual and empirical work on the
Wickrama, K. A. S. and Chalandra M. Bryant. 2003. problem of child sexual abuse and has also written about
Community Context of Social Resources and Ado- child homicide, missing and abducted children, and chil-
lescent Mental Health. Journal of Marriage and the dren exposed to domestic and peer violence and other
Family 65:85066. forms of family violence. He is the editor and author of
Zimet, Gregory D., Nancy W. Dahlem, Sara G. Zimet, 12 books and over 200 journal articles and book chapters.
and Gordon K. Farley. 1988. The Multidimensional He has received grants from the National Institute of
Scale of Perceived Social Support. Journal of Per- Mental Health, the U.S. Department of Justice, and a
sonality Assessment 52:3041. variety of other sources.

Você também pode gostar