Você está na página 1de 13

See

discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.net/publication/12891408

Suicidal Ideation among Adolescent School


Children, Involvement in BullyVictim
Problems, and Perceived Social...

Article in Suicide and Life-Threatening Behavior February 1999


DOI: 10.1111/j.1943-278X.1999.tb01050.x Source: PubMed

CITATIONS READS

225 495

2 authors:

Ken Rigby Phillip Thomas Slee


University of South Australia Flinders University
108 PUBLICATIONS 4,922 CITATIONS 155 PUBLICATIONS 4,218 CITATIONS

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

COPING WITH BULLING PROGRAM View project

kidsmatter View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Phillip Thomas Slee on 26 February 2015.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Suicidal Ideation among Adolescent School
Children, Involvement in Bully-Victim
Problems, and Perceived Social Support
Ken Rigby, PhD, and Phillip Slee, PhD

Relationships among suicidal ideation, involvement in bully-victim problems at school,


a n d perceived social support were investigated with samples of adolescent students ( N =
1103 a n d N = 845) attending secondary school in South Australia. Results obtained from
self-reports and peer nomination procedures to identify bullies and victims indicated t h a t
involvement in bully-victim problems at school, especially for students with relatively
little social support, was significantly related to degree of suicidal ideation.

For some time it has been recognized that ey & Gordon, 19501, which includes bully-
negative peer relations may significantly ing but also other factors that suggest in-
contribute to depression and even suicidal terpersonal difficulties. In this article, we
behavior in some children and adolescents focus specifically upon the concept of bul-
(Topol & Reznikoff, 1982). The situation lying, a form of interpersonal aggression
for young people appears to be particularly that can be defined, along lines proposed
acute when children are victimized by by Farrington ( 1993), a s repeated oppres-
their peers and suffer a serious loss of self- sion-physical or psychological-by a more
esteem. Pfeffer (1990) put the matter as powerful person or group of persons.
follows: Probably one of the most critical Thus defined, bullying can take many
factors in precipitating suicidal ideation- forms. It may be physical, as in hitting or
arising from interpersonal problems is hu- kicking; verbal, as in teasing and name
miliation-feelings of disgrace andpublic calling; and indirect, as in exclusion from
disparagement may shatter a youngsters groups and in rumor spreading. Often vic-
healthy sense of narcissism and sense of tims are subjected to all of these treat-
identity, and loss of a basic sense of ones ments. In the population of interest in this
worthwhileness is a powerful force to in- study, Australian adolescents attending
crease thoughts of self-annihilation (p. secondary school, it has been estimated on
81). Not surprisingly, it has been sug- the basis of surveys of over 17,000 stu-
gested that being repeatedly bullied by dents in this age group (see Rigby, 1996)
peers a t school constitutes one potential that approximately 1 male in 6 and 1 fe-
source of public humiliation that can have male in 8 is bullied by peers on a weekly
tragic consequences for the victim. basis. Many adolescents occasionally en-
The study by Topol and Reznikoff (1982) gage in bullying their peers in Australian
was concerned, among other things, with schools, either as individuals or in groups.
serious peer problems as indicated by Those who repeatedly engage in this be-
the Mooney Problem Check List (Moon- havior (on a weekly basis) are relatively

Ken Righy is Adjunct Associate Professor of Psychology, Faculty of Social Science, University of South
Australia, Underdale. Phillip Slee is Senior Lecturer in Education, School of Education, Flinders University,
Adelaide, South Australia.
Address correspondence to Dr. K. Rigby, c/o ARI,University of South Australia, Underdale Campus,
Holbrooks Road, Underdale 5032, South Australia, Australia.
The authors thank the Australian Rotary Health Association for the grant that cnahled them to under-
take this study, and Rohyn Cunningham who provided research assistance.
Suicide and Life-Threatening Behavior, Vol. 29(2), Summer 1999
0 1999 The American Association of Suicidology 119
120 SUICIDE AND LIFE-THREATENING BEHAVIOR

few, comprising about 8%of boys and 4%of tively, motivate children to bully others.
girls. We should recognize also that some They do, however, suggest that it may be
children may be categorized as bully/vic- useful to examine more directly whether
tims (Olweus, 1993):that is, they continu- adverse peer relations may be related to
ally bully others and are themselves vic- suicide ideation. For example, we might
timized. find that involvement in bully-victim
Much of the current research interest in problems puts a child at greater risk of
bullying or peer victimization in schools self-destructive cognitions or behavior.
was triggered by claims of cases of suicide What such associations could mean in
among children who had been victimized terms of causative influences remains to
by their peers at school. For example, the be examined in the Discussion section.
antibullying campaign started in Norway Although suicidal ideation is relatively
in 1983 was strongly motivated by news- common among adolescents-in some sam-
paper reports of three boys in northern ples it has been reported as including 63%
Norway who committed suicide allegedly of high school students in the United
as a result of severe bullying by peers (01- States (Smith & Crawford, 1 9 8 6 j i t is
weus, 1993). Subsequently, reported sui- clear that its occurrence considerably in-
cides of schoolchildren in England and Ja- creases the risk of a student engaging in
pan have similarly focused attention on life-threatening behavior. In South Aus-
the possible contribution of adverse peer tralia, where the present study was con-
relations to such actions. It is, however, ducted, the incidence of suicidal ideation
difficult to draw firm conclusions from among adolescent schoolchildren ages 13-
such reports. Cases of adolescent suicide 17 years, as indicated by agreement with
are relatively rare and generally unaccom- the statement I have thoughts about sui-
panied by reliable data on the nature of cide, was estimated at 24.5% (Martin,
the peer relations of the suicide victim. 1992). According to a recent study under-
Evidence from survey research that has taken with adolescent schoolchildren in
assessed the quality of childrens peer re- Western Australia, approximately 7.5% of
lations and related it to indicators of psy- all adolescents in that state between the
chological well-being suggests that chil- ages of 12 and 16 have deliberately tried
dren who become involved repeatedly in to hurt or kill themselves (Zubrick et al.,
interpersonal conflicts at school experi- 1995). The researchers estimate that
ence more severe mental health problems. these comprise about one third of adoles-
Research reports from Norway (Olweus, cents who reported having had suicidal
1992), Ireland (OMoore & Hillery, 1991), thoughts.
England (Boulton & Underwood, 19921, Although the likelihood of suicidal ide-
and Australia (Rigby, 1996; Rigby & Slee, ation can be expected to be greatest among
1993) have concurred in finding that vic- adolescents who are continually involved
timized children commonly experience low in conflict with their peers at school, one
self-esteem. There is also evidence that might also anticipate that suicidal ide-
children who bully others at school are ation would be less evident among chil-
frequently more unhappy than others dren who could claim to have substantial
(Rigby & Slee, 1993) and also more prone social support from others. Research has
to be depressed than average (Slee, 1995), provided support for this hypothesis; for
even in later years when they become example, reported illness and poor psycho-
adults (Dietz, 1994). social health has been found to be less com-
These reported associations do not, how- mon among people for whom either social
ever, amount to demonstrations that in- integration or support was greatest (Blax-
volvement in bully-victim problems causes ter, 1990).It has further been proposed, in
reduced psychological well-being: conceiv- the so-called buffering hypothesis, that
ably, low states of well-being may elicit social support is especially beneficial to
victimization from others, or, alterna- individuals who experience more extreme
RIGBY AND SLEE 121

stressful conditions (Cohen& McKay, 1984). sized that schoolchildren who are victim-
Sources of support for adolescent school- ized by peers or, alternatively, engage in
children appear likely to be found princi- bullying others would be more prone to
pally from their families or at school. Pre- suicidal ideation than others. Also, given
vious research has suggested that support that the availability or otherwise of social
from parents is most crucial in offsetting support has been shown to be related lev-
the effects of stress, including suicidal ide- els of stress, it was further hypothesized
ation (Hollis, 1996; Kienhorst, De Wilde, that the perception of such support as be-
Van Den Bout, Diekstra, & Wolsters, 1990; ing more readily available would be associ-
Martin, Rozanes, Pearce, & Allison, 1995; ated with reduced tendency toward sui-
Rubenstein, Heeren, Housman, Rubin, & cidal ideation.
Stechler, 1989). The potential effects of
support provided at school appear less cer-
tain, as reflected in a number of conflict- STUDY 1
ing reports addressing this issue. Thus,
Topol and Reznikoff (1982) reported that Instruments
the hospitalized suicidal adolescents in
their study were significantly less likely Suicidal Ideation Scale
t o have a close confidante than a control This scale assessed proneness of students
group of nonhospitalized coping young- to think about suicide and consisted of four
sters. Subsequently, failure to obtain peer items from the Goldberg Health question-
support was suggested as a major factor naire (Goldberg & Williams, 1988)For ex-
in the suicidal reactions of adolescents ample, students indicated whether over
(Harter & Marold, 1990).In addition, Lew- the last few weeks they had found the
insohn, Rohde, and Seeley (1993)have re- idea of taking your own life kept coming
ported that perceived support from friends into your mind. A 4-point scale is used to
was negatively correlated with past sui- record frequency for each item, and scores
cide attempts, although this relationship are summed for the Suicidal Ideation
was not significant after controlling for Scale.
depression. On the other hand, in a recent
study of native Hawaiian high school stu- The Bully Scale
dents, peer support was found to be uncor- This scale is a measure of the tendency of
related with suicide attempts (Yuen et al., students to engage in bullying others at
1996). There appears to be a lack of infor- school. This 6-item self-report scale has
mation as to whether perceived support been shown to have good reliability and
from teachers can have a significant ef- concurrent validity (Rigby & Slee, 1993).
fect. One object of this enquiry was to ex- An example is I am part of a group that
amine whether suicidality, as inferred goes around teasing others, to which stu-
from a measure of suicidal ideation, was dents may answer on a 4-point scale rang-
related to the perception of being sup- ing from never to very often.
ported generally, and/or by specific others
(parents, peers, and teachers) among ado- The Victim Scale
lescents attending secondary school in This 5-item scale (Rigby & Slee, 1993)reli-
Australia. ably assesses the extent to which students
are bullied by others. Students report how
HYPOTHESES frequently they experience being bullied
(e.g., I get picked on by others) on a 4-
Given that poor mental health, such as is point scale of frequency.
indicated by low self-esteem and by high
levels of depression, has been found to be Social Support Measures
associated with involvement in bully-vic- (i) Students were asked t o say how much
tim problems at school, it was hypothe- help they thought they would get from the
122 SUICIDE AND LIFE-THREATENINGBEHAVIOR

following people if they were having a seri- Results


ous problem at school: Your class teacher;
your best friend; students in your class; Scale Reliabilities
your mother (if alive); your father (if
alive). Response categories on a 5-point Coefficient alpha was computed for each
scale range from a lot (5) to none or scale to assess reliability. The obtained
values for 912 students for whom there
hardly any (1). To provide a Social Sup-
port Scale item scores are summed. were no missing values were as follows:
Suicidal Ideation Scale, .91; Bully Scale,
(ii) A single item measure of Overall .85; Victim Scale, .86; Social Support
Support was also included: In general, do Scale, .71. In each case the scales reliabil-
you feel you can count on most people to ity was supported.
help you when you are having a bad time?
The response categories are Yes, defi- Mean Scores and Gender Comparisons
nitely; Yes, usually; I cant say; Not The mean scores on each of the Likert
usually; Definitely not. Scales and the 5-point rating scales are
given in Table 1. The results for this first
Participants
study indicated that boys reported bully-
In 1993 schoolchildren from two coedu- ing others and being victimized by others
cational secondary schools in Adelaide, significantly more than girls ( p > .001).
South Australia, answered questionnaires No significant difference, however, was
in class. Questionnaires were adminis- found between boys and girls on the Sui-
tered to 542 boys and 561 girls, with ages cidal Ideation Scale ( p > .OW. With respect
ranging from 12 to 18 years. For boys the to support from others, girls generally re-
mean age was 15.00 years (SD= 1.451,for ported that they received more support
girls, M = 14.94 years (SD = 1.38).All stu- than boys, especially from best friends
dents answered some of the questions, but and students in class ( p < .001) and also
there was at least one omission for 21.8% on the general measures of Overall
of the sample. (In reporting the results, Support and the Social Support Scale
N s are given for particular analyses.) ( p < .001).

TABLE 1
Means Scores and Sex Differences on Measures of Peer Relations, Suicidal Ideation,
and Perceived Support from Others
First Study Second Study
Boys Girls t p < Boys Girls t p <

Peer Relations
Bully Scale 9.88 8.01 9.70 .001 9.01 8.03 5.45 .001
Victim Scale 8.79 7.62 6.77 .001 9.00 8.21 4.15 .001
Suicidal Ideation Scale 7.72 8.04 1.78 ns 5.89 6.57 2.79 .01
Support from others
Teachers 3.14 3.17 0.42 ns 3.08 3.08 .OO ns
Best friend 3.91 4.44 8.15 001 3.64 4.29 8.32 .001
Students in class 3.07 3.39 4.89 ,001 3.06 3.26 2.72 .01
Mother 3.91 4.08 2.20 .05 4.11 4.21 1.05 ns
Father 3.71 3.65 0.72 ns 3.88 3.73 1.62 ns
Social Support Scale 17.76 18.71 3.65 .001 17.89 18.69 2.80 .01
Overall Support 3.80 4.08 4.67 .001 3.62 3.99 5.47 ,001
~~

Note. There are slight variations in numbers completing each question. In Study 1: boys, N > 489; girls,
n > 500. In Study 2: boys, n > 396; girls, n > 356.
RIGBY AND SLEE 123

TABLE 2
Partial Correlations for Self-Report Measures with
Suicidal Ideation, Controlling for Age
Study 1 Study 2
Boys Girls Boys Girls
Peer Relations
Bully Scale .12** 1g***
Victim Scale 21*** 1 5 ~
Social Support
Teacher - 16*** -.15***
Best Friend -.27*** -.12*
Students in class - 1g*** -.07ns
Mother - 26*** -.25***
Father -.33*** -.20***
Social Support Scale -.33*** -.24***
Overall support -.23*** -.24***
Note. Due to missing values there were variations in numbers employed in
computing correlations between dependent variables and dependent variable
In Study 1: for boys, N > 458; for girls, N > 468. In Study 2: for boys, N > 386;
for girls, N > 348.
*p < .05;**p < .01;***p < .001; ns = not significant. All are two-tailed tests.

Correlations with Suicidal Ideation cidal Ideation were computed again, this
To assess relationships with Suicidal Ide- time partialing out for scores on the Social
ation, partial correlations were computed Support Scale and also (given the age
for each gender separately controlling for range of these students) age in years. The
age. The results are given in Table 2. The magnitude of the correlations was re-
results from Study 1indicated that scores duced, but remained significant at the .05
for both boys and girls on the Bully Scale level (two-tailed test) for the Victim Scale:
and the Victim Scale correlated positively, for boys, partial r = .lo; for girls, .ll ( p <
as predicted, with Suicidal Ideation. The .05). With the Bully Scale the result was
correlations were in each case small but significant for girls (partial r = .17, p <
significant at the .001 level (two-tailed .001) but not for boys (partial r = .06,
test). For indices of social support, again, p > .05, two-tailed test). These results
as predicted, correlations were negative suggested that the relationship between
(and all but one-students in classm- sig- peer victimization and suicidal ideation,
nificant at the .05 level), implying that though small, remains significant for both
suicidal ideation was less common among sexes after taking into account degree of
children who believed they had access to perceived social support.
support from others. On the Social Sup-
port Scale the r values were -.33 (for boys) Regression Analysis
and -.24 (for girls); in each case, p < .001.
Notably the highest correlations with To examine the relative contributions of
specified sources of support were with peer victimization, bullying behavior, so-
mother and with father. cial support, gender, and age to suicidal
To examine whether social support in ideation, a multiple regression analysis
general might compensate, as it were, for was conducted. Given the suggestion that
negative peer relations, correlations be- social support may have a buffering ef-
tween the peer relating variables and Sui- fect such that students who were involved
124 SUICIDE AND LIFE-THREATENING BEHAVIOR

more in bully-victim problems would ben- ness to engage in bullying behavior or to


efit more (as indicated by lower Suicidal be victimized by others was inferred from
Ideation Scale scores) by higher levels of the judgments of peers.
perceived social support, two additional
variables were created to assess relevant Instruments
interaction effects. These consisted of Vic-
tim Scale x Social Support Scale scores Peer Rating Measures
(Victim-Support interaction) and Bully To provide measures of engagement in bul-
Scale x Social Support Scale scores (Bully- lying behavior and being bullied by others,
Support interaction). The results after in- students were asked to indicate which stu-
dependent variables had been included, dents of the same gender as themselves
including the interaction variables, are fitted various descriptions. Those signify-
given in Table 3. ing bullying were enjoys upsetting oth-
It is evident that the Bully Scale, the ers, always teasing others, shows oth-
Victim Scale, and the Social Support ers hes (or shes) the boss, and likes to
Scale each made an independent and sig- scare others. Those signifying being a vic-
nificant contribution to Suicidal Ideation tim were gets picked on a lot, kids make
( p < .05). Sex (being female) also contrib- fun of him or her, gets hit and pushed
uted to Suicidal Ideation. However, nei- around, and gets left out by others.
ther of the interaction variables was sig- Eight filler items were included, such as
nificant ( p > .05), nor was the age of the always losing things. A list of names of
student. Overall, however, the amount of the students in each group was provided.
variance accounted for by the combination Students received scores on each of the 8
of variables, though significant, was rela- items according to the numbers of stu-
tively low-less than 10%. dents whom they had indicated fitted the
description divided by the number of stu-
dents involved in making the judgments.
STUDY 2 Adding together scores for bullying behav-
iors and peer victimization provided scale
A possible weakness of the first study was scores for Peer-Rated Bullying and Peer-
that it relied upon reports provided exclu- Rated Victimization, respectively.
sively by the students about themselves.
Other Measures
An alternative and complementary ap-
proach was used in the next study, con- In order to replicate results from Study 1
ducted in 1995,wherein estimates of prone- with a fresh set of subjects, the self-report

TABLE 3
Results for Regression Analyses for Two Samples of Data, with
Suicidal Ideation as the Dependent Variable
Sample 1 Sample 2
beta t p< beta t P<

Victim Scale (VS) .08 2.09 .05 .21 5.83 .001


Bully Scale (BS) .09 2.71 .01 .21 5.64 .001
Social Support Scale (SS) -.25 -7.38 .001 -.20 -5.53 .001
Sex (1 = male; 2 = female). .13 3.93 .001 .19 5.29 .001
VS x SS (interaction) -.04 -1.13 ns -.06 -1.60 ns
BS x SS (interaction) -.05 -1.56 ns .oo .OO ns
Age (years) .05 1.52 ns .oo .OO ns

Note. For Sample 1, multiple R = .33, df= 7,867, p < ,001; for Sample 2, multiple R = .41,
df= 7,699, p < .001.
RIGBY AND SLEE 125

measures of bullying others and being vic- Scores on the Bully Scale and the Victim
timized were included in the question- Scale again correlated significantly, as
naire together with the measures of social predicted, with the Suicidal Ideation Scale
support and the Suicidal Ideation Scale for boys and for girls. Scores on the Social
from the Goldberg Scale (Goldberg & Wil- Support Scale correlated negatively and
liams, 1988). significantly with Suicidal Ideation for
both sexes. Again, correlations with per-
ceived support from parents were higher
Procedure than those obtained in relation to other
sources of support; notably, for boys the
Questionnaires were administered in class- negative correlations with perceived sup-
rooms at three coeducational secondary port from best friend and students in
schools in Adelaide to students of both class, although in the predicted direction,
genders between the ages of 12 and 16. failed to reach an acceptable level of signif-
The nomination of students from the list icance. Thus, although the results from
of names as fitting or not fitting each de- the two studies indicated relatively low
scription was the first task. A set of names correlations between the independent
for male or females classmates of the sub- variables and Suicidal Ideation, they
jects own gender was provided. The num- were, apart from correlations with mea-
ber of names on the list varied between 6 sures of peer support, statistically signifi-
and 26. Subsequently respondents an- cant and provided a high level of consis-
swered questions about themselves. In to- tency across samples.
tal, questionnaires were answered fully or As in the analyses of results obtained
partially by 450 boys and 395 girls. The in Study 1, we examined whether the rela-
mean age for boys was 14.02 years, SD = tionships between the Suicidal Ideation
.77; for girls, M = 13.92 years, SD = .71. Scale and the Victim Scale and the Bully
Scale, respectively, remained significant
after partialing out for perceived social
Results support as indicated by the Social Support
Scale, and also age. With the Victim Scale,
Replications
the partial correlations remained signifi-
The reliabilities of the scales with the cant at the .05 level (two-tailed test) with
fresh sample of students who completed partial rs of .17 for boys and .30 for girls.
all items of the scales (N = 788) were again Results for the Bully Scale were signifi-
found to be satisfactory, with alpha values cant at this level for boys (partial r = .27)
of .92 for the Suicidal Ideation Scale, .80 but not for girls (partial r = .lo, p > .05).
for the Bully Scale, .87 for the Victim Thus, for both studies the results indi-
Scale, and .69 for the Social Support Scale. cated that the relationship between peer
Gender differences were in most cases rep- victimization (but not reported bullying of
licated (see Table 1).As before, boys scored others) and suicidal ideation was signifi-
significantly higher on the Bully Scale and cant for both sexes after taking into ac-
on the Victim Scale. However, in this sec- count perceived level of social support.
ond study the mean difference between We may note from Table 3 that the re-
scores on the Suicidal Ideation Scale gression analysis conducted using scale
reached statistical significance ( p < .Ol), scores and demographic variables from
with girls scoring higher. On perceived Study 2 provided results similar t o those
support from others, girls again scored sig- reported for the same analysis using data
nificantly higher on the Social Support from Study 1.The Victim Scale, the Bully
Scale and also on support from peers (i.e., Scale, the Social Support Scale, and Sex
best friend and students in class). were each independently related to Sui-
Correlations with Suicidal Ideation also cidal Ideation; neither of the two interac-
followed a similar pattern (see Table 2). tion effects was significant, nor was age.
126 SUICIDE AND LIFE-THREATENING BEHAVIOR

The main differences lay in the higher a score of 1.5 or more on the relevant scale.
beta values obtained in Study 2 with the The use of this criterion yielded approxi-
peer relations variables (the Victim and mately the same proportions of bullies
Bully Scales), and the greater amount of and victims as indicated in previous re-
variance accounted for. These differences search using self-report data according to
are considered further in the discussion which victims were operationally de-
section. fined as reporting being bullied at least
Given that the measures of peer victim- once a week at school by peers and bul-
ization, bullying others, and social support lies as reporting engaging in bullying oth-
made significant independent contribu- ers often(see Rigby, 1997).Among boys,
tions in accounting for suicidal ideation, 11.3%were classified as bullies; among
it was of interest to examine the amount girls, 7.8%.Those classified as victims
of variance explained by combinations of consisted of 16.4% of boys and 9.1% of
these variables for each sex. Drawing on girls. The results obtained for the same
data for the later study in which students constructs from self-reports and peer nom-
had been first sensitised to bullyhictim inations were significantly ( p < .01) but
behaviors by having to rate others on rele- not highly correlated. Point biserial corre-
vant behaviors, the joint contribution of lations between the self-report Victim
reporting being victimized and having low Scale and the categories of high and low
social support was 6.7% for boys and levels of peer victimization from peer nom-
17.1%for girls, and for bullying others and inations were .44 ( N = 445) for boys and
having low social support, 12.1%for boys .48 (N = 392) for girls. The corresponding
and 11.5%for girls. The combination of correlations based on self-report and peer
being victimized and bullying others ac- nomination results for bullying were
counted for 15.0%and 14.6%of the vari- somewhat lower, .17 for boys and .36 for
ance for boys and girls, respectively. When girls.
all three factors-peer victimization, bul-
Suicidal Ideation of Students
lying others, and low socal support were Classified According to Types
combined, the variance accounted for in
suicidal ideation was 15.5%for boys and Mean scores on the Suicidal Ideation
18.7%for girls. Scale were computed for students classi-
fied as bullies, victims, and othersf7
Comparisons of Findings Based on using categories derived from self-reports
Peer and Self-Reports and peer nominations. Analyses by two-
Given the highly skewed nature of results way ANOVA were used with type and gen-
obtained from peer reports, with a rela- der as factors. The results are given in
tively large number of students receiving Table 4.
low scores on the Victim measure and on As predicted, results obtained from both
the Bully measure, respectively, the re- peer and self-reports consistently sup-
sults were treated as nominal data so that ported the prediction that victims of peer
students were categorized as bullies or bullying tend to have relatively high levels
nonbullies and also as victimsor non- of suicidal ideation. However, for perpetra-
victims. Total scores were computed for tors of bullying, the evidence that they,
each student, these being the sum of all too, were more prone to suicidal ideation
nominations received indicating that the was weaker, being unequivocally support-
student behaved as (1) a bully and (2) a ed by data from self-reports only. Mean
victim. These total scores were then di- Suicidal Ideation for peer-nominated bul-
vided by the number of raters, to provide lies was, as predicted, higher than for
standardized bully and victim scores, peer-nominated n~nbullies~ but signifi-
ranging from 0 (no nominations) to 4 (nom- cant by t test ( t = 1.67) only by one-tailed
inated by all raters). Students were classi- test; for girls, mean differences were not
fied as bullies or as victims if they received significant ( t = .60, p > .05). The results
RIGBY AND SLEE 127

TABLE 4
Mean Scores on the Suicidal Ideation Scale for Students according to Gender
and Mode of Assessment of Bully-Victim Status, with Results f;;r ANOVA
Self-Reports Peer Nominations
Male Female Male Female
Victim status
Victim 7.07 8.78 6.37 7.77
( n = 81) ( n = 40) ( n = 68) ( n = 31)
Nonvictim 5.60 6.31 5.81 6.47
( n = 342) ( n = 340) (n = 359) ( n = 349)
ANOVA results F(Victim)= 39.26, p < .001 F(Victim)6.70, p < .01
F(Sex)= 11.86,p < .001 F(Sex)= 8.20, p < .001
F(Sex x Victim) = 1.94, ns F(Sex x Victim) = 1.06, ns
Bully status
Bully 7.38 6.95 6.56 6.94
( n = 45) ( n = 20) ( n = 48) ( n = 31)
Nonbully 5.70 6.55 5.81 6.54
(n= 376) ( n = 353) ( n = 379) ( n = 349)
ANOVA results F(Bul1y)= 5.61, p < .05 F(Bul1y) = 2.10, ns
F(Sex)= 0.22, ns F(Sex)= 2.09, ns
F(Bul1y x Sex)= 2.10, ns F(Bu1ly x Sex) = 0.22, ns

based on peer nominations thus are less in that study were asked first to rate each
conclusive than those based on self-re- other according to whether they fit de-
ports and suggest a trend only for male scriptions that emphasized behaviors
bullies to be more inclined toward suicidal typical of bullies or victims. It seems ex-
ideation than other males. tremely likely that this would have in-
creased the salience of peer relations for
subjects, leading to more considered self-
DISCUSSION ratings of how they behaved with their
peers. It was under these conditions that
The results support the view that adoles- peer relations emerged as more closely re-
cent students who are more frequently vic- lated to suicidal ideation and accounted
timized by peers at school and feel gener- for considerably more variance in suicidal
ally unsupported by others when they ideation (see Table 3).
have a problem are, in general, more likely Analyses based on results from peer
to experience suicidal ideation than oth- nominations as opposed to self-reports
ers. Although data from self-reports indi- also produced differences in outcomes
cate that students who engage in bullying with respect to hypothesized effects of bul-
others are also at some risk of more than lying others, most notably for comparisons
average suicidal ideation, results based on between girl bullies and girl nonbullies
peer nominations of bullies and nonbullies (see Table 4). One possible reason for this
suggest that this generalization may may lie in the greater difficulty in observ-
apply to boys only. ing and accurately identifying bullying
The main features of the results re- among girls. Bullying by girls is less com-
ported in Study 1 are consistent with those mon than by boys (Rigby, 1996) and gener-
reported in the Study 2; however, there ally more covert (Sharp & Cowie, 1994).
are some differences that should now be In accounting for the obtained associa-
considered. The second study contained an tions between involvement in adverse peer
important variation in method. Students relations at school and suicidal ideation,
128 SUICIDE AND LIFE-THREATENING BEHAVIOR

alternative explanations should be consid- may be entertained. It is possible that the


ered. Both being victimized and engaging relatively high levels of suicidal ideation of
in bullying others could conceivably result male bullies are related to generally high
from psychiatic illness and/or social mal- levels of aggressiveness; alternatively, the
adjustment, with either or both of these explanation may lie in psychological ef-
conditions giving rise to suicidal ideation. fects of having engaged repeatedly in un-
However, evidence has been accumulating justifiable acts of aggressiveness against
that suicide risk in young people is reliably less powerful individuals, that is, to hav-
associated with psychosocial factors, in- ing bullied their peers. The first possibil-
cluding school problems and the experi- ity is consistent with earlier claims that
ence of stressful life events (of which bully- aggressive children are especially prone
ing is an example) after controlling for to suicidal behavior (see Pfeffer, Plutch-
psychiatric illness (Gould,Fisher, Parides, nik, & Mizruchi, 19831, and the second
Flory, & Shaffer, 1996). In a recent study is consistent with research revealing that
school adjustment problems such as social acts of unjustified aggression such as bul-
isolation and avoidance of others were pre- lying typically induce feelings of shame
dicted from measures of peer victimiza- and depression (Rigby, 1997b),which can
tion, but indices of maladjustment prior lead to self-injury(James, 1995).These pos-
to victimization were not found to be sig- sibilities need to be evaluated further.
nificant predictors of being victimized at The study also suggests that low social
school by peers (Kochenderfer & Ladd, support may make a significant contribu-
1996). tion to suicidal ideation for both boys and
The results of this study are also consis- girls. The failure to obtain a significant
tent with earlier findings that low psycho- result in the regression analysis (Table 3)
logical well-being-of which suicidal ide- for the interaction of either peer victimiza-
ation is an extreme manifestation-is tion or bullying others with social support
commonly viewed, a t least by students, indicates that social support cannot be
as a consequence of peer victimization. said to have bufferingrole, in the sense
Among Australian adolescents who re- that students who are more frequently vic-
ported being bullied at least once a week at timized by peers or alternatively, who are
school, substantial proportions indicated more inclined to bully others benefit from
feeling worse about themselves after be- social support in the form of a reduction
ing bullied: 48.8% of boys and 62.5% of in proneness to suicidal ideation. How-
girls (Rigby, 1997a). The percentages in- ever, on the basis of self-report data from
creased markedly with reported frequency Study 2, the additive effects of social sup-
of being victimized, as did the proportions port on those of the bully-victim variables
of students reporting feeling sad and mis- increase the variance in Suicidal Ideation
erable after being bullied, and also stay- accounted for quite substantially, to ap-
ing away from school because of bul- proximately 15% for boys and 19% for
lying., girls.
Although self-reports of bullying others The question of whether involvement in
were repeatedly found to be associated bully-victim problems at school consti-
with relatively high levels of suicidal ide- tutes a risk factor for suicidal ideation de-
ation, the failure to obtain consistent re- pends upon the criteria set for making
sults using data obtained from peer nomi- such a decision. As far as peer victimiza-
nations of bullies, especially among girls, tion is concerned, the results showing as-
must limit the generalizability of the asso- sociations between measures of being bul-
ciation. The present pair of studies sug- lied a t school and suicidal ideation are
gest that bullying others may be an indi- statistically significant and replicated in
cator of a greater likelihood of suicidal two studies using different methods (peer
ideation among boys only. Again, alterna- reports and peer nominations) to assess
tive explanations for such an association peer victimization, for both boys and girls.
RIGBY AND SLEE 129

This finding thus appears to be robust. On Boulton, M. J., & Underwood, K. (1992) Bully/victim
problems among middle school children. British
the other hand, the amount of variance Journal of Educational Psychology, 62, 73-87.
in Suicidal Ideation accounted for by peer Cohen, S., & McKay, G. (1984). Social support, stress
victimization is quite low, especially for and the buffering hypothesis: A theoretical analy-
boys. However, when the contribution of sis. In A. Baum, J. E. Singer, & S. E. Taylor (Eds.),
Social psychological aspects of health (vol. 4, pp.
low social support is added, there is much 253-267). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
more substantial evidence of risk. Know- Dietz, B. (1994). Effects on subsequent heterosexual
ing that a student is frequently involved shyness and depression of peer victimization at
in bully-victim problems at school and has school. Paper presented at the International Con-
ference on Childrens Peer Relations, Institute of
low social support can more readily be ac- Social Research, University of South Australia,
cepted as a n indicator that the student is Adelaide.
at risk of having relatively high levels of Farrington, D. P. (1993). Understanding and pre-
suicidal ideation. venting bullying. In M.Tonny & N. Morris (Eds.),
Crime andjustice (Vol. 17, pp. 381-459). Chicago:
There is currently a need for longitudi- University of Chicago Press.
nal studies in this area to determine Goldberg, D., & Williams, P. (1988). A users guide
which, if any, of the pychosocial factors to the General Health Questionnaire. Slough, U K
associated with suicidal ideation have a NFER.
Gould, M. S., Fisher, P., Parides, M., Flory, M., &
causal influence. These include factors de- Shaffer, D. (1996). Psychsocial risk factors of child
rived from students experiences in fami- and adolescent completed suicide. Archives of Gen-
lies as well as school. Recent studies have eral Psychiatry, 53(12), 1155-1162.
shown that reports from students of family Harter, S., & Marold, D. B. (1990). Psychosocial risk
factors contributing to adolescent suicidal ide-
dysfunction or strained relations with par- ation. In G. G. Noam & s. Borst (Eds.), Children,
ents are associated with both involvement youth and suicide: Developmentalperspectiues. San
in bully-victim problems (Rigby, 1993, Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
1994) and also suicidal ideation (Martin Hollis, C. (1996). Depression, family environment,
adolescent suicidal behavior. Journal of the Ameri-
et al., 1995). Furthermore, the present re- can Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry,
search suggests the positive effects of so- 35(5), 622-630.
cial support by schools in reducing suicidal James, 0. (1995). Juvenile violence in a winner-loser
ideation; future studies could usefully in- culture. London: Free Association Books.
Kienhorst, C. W. M., De Wilde, E. J., Van Den Bout,
corporate measures of the perceived help- R. F., Diekstra, R. F. W., & Wolsters, W. H. G.
fulness of teacher and counselor support (1990). Characteristics of suicide attempters in a
services. population-based sample of Dutch adolescents.
In conclusion, as Lewinsohn et al. (1993) British Journal of Psychiatry, 156, 243-248.
Kochenderfer, B. J., & Ladd, G. W. (1996). Peer vic-
have argued, suicide should be viewed as timization: Cause or consequence of school malad-
a function of the number of risk factors justment. Child Development, 67, 1305-1317.
that are present in individual cases. Lewinsohn, P. M., Rohde, P., & Seeley, J. R. (1993).
Among these, adverse peer relations, espe- Psychosocial characteristics of adolescents with a
history of suicide attempt. Journal of the American
cially under conditions of low social sup- Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 32(1),
port, may play a significant part. Longitu- 60-68.
dinal research is still needed, however, to Martin, G. (1992). Postvention in adolescent suicide.
determine the nature of causal connec- In S. McKillop (Ed.),Preventing youth suicide:Pro-
ceedings of a conference of the Australian Institute
tions that may link high levels of peer vic- of Criminology (pp.111-120).
timization and low social support to a Martin, G., Rozanes, P., Pearce, C., & Allison, S.
proneness to high levels of suicidal ide- (1995). Adolescent suicide, depression and family
ation, which can, in some circumstances, dysfunction. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 92,
336-344.
lead to suicidal behavior. Mooney, R. L., & Gordon, L. V. (1950). The Mooney
problem check lists: Manual 1950 revisions. New
York Psychological Corporation.
Olweus, D. (1992).Victimisation by peers: Anteced-
REFERENCES ents and long term outcomes. In K. H. Rubin & J .
B. Asendorf (Eds.), Social withdrawal, inhibition
Blaxter, M. (1990). Health and lifestyles. London: and shyness in children (pp. 315-341). Hillsdale,
TavistockiRoutledge. NJ: Erlbaum.
130 SUICIDE AND LIFE-THREATENING BEHAVIOR

Olweus, D. (1993). Bullying at school. Cambridge: mal adolescents: Risk and protective factors.
Blackwell. American Orthopsychiatry, 59(1), 59-71.
OMoore, A. M., & Hillery, B. (1991). What teachers Sharp, S., & Cowie, H. (1994). Empowering pupils
need to know. In M. Elliott (Ed.),BullyingtApracti- to take positive action against bullying. In P. K.
cal guide to coping for schools (pp. 56-69). Harlow: Smith & Sonia Sharp (Eds.), School bullying: Zn-
Oxford University Press. sights and perspectives (pp. 108-132). London:
Pfeffer, C. R. (1990). Manifestation of risk factors. In Routledge.
G. MacLean (Ed.), Suicide in children and adoles- Slee, P. T. (1995). Peer victimization and its relation-
ship to depression among Australian primary
cents (pp. 65-88).Toronto: Hogrefe & Huber.
school students. Personality and Individual Differ-
Pfeffer, C. R., Plutchnik, R., & Mizruchi, M. S. (1983). ences, 18(1), 57-62.
Suicidal and assaultive behaviour in children: Nee, P. T.,& Rigby, K. (1993). The relationship of
Classification, measurement, and interrelations. Eysencks personality factors and self-esteem to
American Journal of Psychiatry, 140, 154-157. bully/victim behaviour in Australian school boys.
Rigby, K. (1993). School childrens perceptions of Personality and Individual Differences, 14, 371-
their families and parents a s a function of peer 373.
relations. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 154(4), Smith, K., & Crawford, S. (1986). Suicidal behavior
501-514. among normal high school students. Suicide and
Rigby, K. (1994). Psycho-social functioning in fami- Life-Threatening Behavior, 16, 313-325.
lies of Australian adolescent schoolchildren in- Topol, P., & Reznikoff, M. (1982). Perceived peer and
volved in bully/victim problems. Journal of Family family relationships, hopelessness, and locus of
Therapy, 16(2) 173-189. control as factors in adolescent suicide attempts.
Rigby, K. (1996).Bullying in Australian schools4nd Suicide and Life Threatening Behavior, 12, 141-
what to do about it. Melbourne: Australian Council 150.
Yuen, N., Andrade, N., Nahulu, L., Makini, G., Mc-
for Educational Research.
Dermott, J. F., Danko, G., Johnson, R., & Waldron,
Rigby, K. (1997a). What children tell us about bully- J. (1996). Suicide and Life-Threatening Behavior,
ing in schools. Children Australia, 22(2), 28-34. 26, 27-36.
Rigby, K. (1997133.Attitudes and beliefs of Australian Zubrick, S. R., Silburn, S. R., Girto, A., Burton, P.,
schoolchildren regarding bullying in schools. Irish Carlton, J., Shepherd, C., & Lawrence, D. (1995).
Journal of P.qychology, 18, 202-220. WesternAustralian child health survey: Developing
Rigby, K., & Nee, P. T.(1993). Dimensions of interper- health and well-being in the nineties. Perth: Aus-
sonal relating among Australian school children tralian Bureau of Statistics and Institute for Child
and their implications for psychological well-being. Health Research.
Journal of Social Psychology, 133(1), 33-42.
Rubenstein, J. L., Heeren, T., Housman, D., Rubin Received: April 11, 1997
C., & Stechler, G. (1989). Suicidal behavior in nor- Revision Accepted: March 15,1998

View publication stats

Você também pode gostar