Você está na página 1de 7

Downloaded from http://jnnp.bmj.com/ on February 8, 2015 - Published by group.bmj.

com

Research paper

Poor attentional function predicts cognitive decline in


patients with non-demented Parkinsons disease
independent of motor phenotype
J-P Taylor, E N Rowan, D Lett, J T OBrien, I G McKeith, D J Burn

Institute for Ageing and Health, ABSTRACT systems, in particular neurodegeneration within
Newcastle upon Tyne, UK Background: Postural instability gait difficulty (PIGD) the cholinergic system.911
Correspondence to:
motor phenotype in Parkinsons disease (PD) is associated Even before the development of dementia,
Dr J-P Taylor, Institute for with a faster rate of cognitive decline than in tremor cognitive deficits occur in patients with non-
Ageing and Health, Wolfson dominant cases and may be a risk factor for incident demented PD and encompass a wide range of
Research Centre, Newcastle dementia. People with PD display attentional deficits; domains whose neural substrates may include
General Hospital, Westgate
Road, Newcastle upon Tyne NE4 however, it is not clear whether attentional deficits in fronto-subcortical and temporo-parietal circuits.8 12
6BE, UK; patients with non-demented PD are associated with (i) Diverse neurotransmitter systems have been impli-
john-paul.taylor@ncl.ac.uk PIGD phenotype and/or (ii) subsequent cognitive decline. cated, but cognitive sequelae in PD and incident
Aims: (i) To examine rates of cognitive decline (Mini- dementia are particularly dependent on non-
Received 22 February 2008 Mental State Examination (MMSE) and Cambridge dopaminergic systems, notably the cholinergic
Revised 29 April 2008
Accepted 2 June 2008 Cognitive Examination (CAMCOG)) over 3 years in system.5
Published Online First subjects with non-demented PD aged over 65 years, (ii) Attentional deficits are an important component
27 June 2008 using Cognitive Drug Research computerised assessment of cognitive impairment in PD.13 Furthermore,
test battery, determine the rate of change in power of attentional impairment in PDD is a strong
attention (PoA) scores over time (sum of mean choice predictor of the ability of an individual to perform
reaction time, simple reaction time and digit vigilance their activities of daily living.14 It is unclear
reaction time scores), (iii) determine whether the PIGD however whether or not attentional deficits in
phenotype is associated with changes in PoA and (iv) PD identify individuals who will develop dementia;
establish whether baseline PoA is associated with this postulate has biological plausibility given the
subsequent cognitive decline. fact that the same neural substrates (eg, fronto-
Results: 14 subjects (38%) were classified as PIGD subcortical and non-dopaminergic systems)
motor phenotype at baseline. Cognitive decline was involved in the mediation of attention are known
greater in PIGD compared with non-PIGD subjects to be compromised in PD and the associated
(p(0.02). PIGD phenotype was not associated with dementia.5 15
baseline PoA score although it was associated with Postural control and gait is dependent on the
subsequent worsening in PoA (mean PoA increase/year: integrity of attentional systems, and interplay
non-PIGD subjects 11.4 ms; PIGD subjects 244.0 ms; between these systems may be compromised in
p = 0.01). Higher baseline PoA scores were associated disease states such as PD.16 Gait function is
with greater cognitive decline (MMSE, p = 0.03; adversely affected in patients with PD tested using
CAMCOG, p = 0.05) independent of PIGD status. dual task paradigms which impose high attentional
Conclusion: PIGD phenotype and attention deficits are demands.17 It is unclear whether patients with PD
independently associated with a greater rate of cognitive with a PIGD motor phenotype are more predis-
decline in patients with non-demented PD. We propose posed to attentional deficits.
that subtle attentional deficits in patients with non- In this study, we followed-up a cohort of
demented PD predict subsequent cognitive impairment. patients with non-demented PD over 3 years.
The aims were: (i) to examine rates of cognitive
decline (Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE)
The occurrence of dementia in Parkinsons disease and Cambridge Cognitive Examination
(PDD) increases mortality and has a significant (CAMCOG)), (ii) using the Cognitive Drug
adverse impact on function and quality of life.13 Research (CDR) computerised assessment test
The cumulative incidence of PDD may be as high battery to determine the rate of change in power
as 80%.4 A multiplicity of risk factors have been of attention (PoA) scores over time (PoA is the sum
proposed for the emergence of PDD5; consistent of mean choice reaction time, simple reaction time
predictors include older age, severity of motor and digit vigilance reaction time scores), (iii) to
symptoms, executive dysfunction and poor verbal determine whether the PIGD phenotype is asso-
fluency.4 68 In addition, motor phenotype, speci- ciated with changes in PoA and (iv) to establish
fically the non-tremor dominant phenotype that whether baseline PoA is associated with subse-
includes postural gait instability difficulty quent cognitive decline. We hypothesised first, that
(PIGD), is associated with a faster rate of baseline motor phenotype would predict impair-
cognitive decline in PD and higher incident risk ment in attention (with PIGD type associated with
of dementia.9 10 It has been postulated that the greater deficits) and second, that greater impair-
PIGD motor phenotype arises as a result of ment in baseline measures of attention would
additional dysfunction in non-dopaminergic independently predict cognitive decline.

1318 J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2008;79:13181323. doi:10.1136/jnnp.2008.147629


Downloaded from http://jnnp.bmj.com/ on February 8, 2015 - Published by group.bmj.com

Research paper

METHODS basis of DSM-IV criteria and an MMSE score (24. The study
Recruitment of patients with PD received approval from the local research ethics committee.
Consecutive patients were recruited over 24 months from local
dementia and movement disorder clinics in Newcastle upon Statistical analysis
Tyne and Sunderland. Eighty-one subjects with a provisional SPSS (V.15.0) was used for all statistical analyses. For baseline
diagnosis of PD were screened for the study. Thirty-nine of variables, comparisons were made between subjects with and
these patients with PD agreed to participate, met Queen Square without the PIGD motor phenotype. Baseline variables con-
Brain Bank criteria18 and were aged 65 years or over. Subjects sidered for inclusion were determined a priori on the basis of
were only included if taking L-dopa monotherapy and no other previous evidence of their potential effect on cognitive function
medications for treatment of their PD. All subjects were in PD and included: age, sex, duration of parkinsonian
assessed clinically as not having any form of dementia (on the symptoms, UPDRS-III scores, MMSE, CAMCOG and GDS-15.
basis of DSM-IV criteria) and scored more than 24 on the In addition, baseline PoA score was included. Cognitive decline
MMSE.19 On entry into the study, no subjects were taking was determined by taking an average of the annual change in
antipsychotic medications or cholinesterase inhibitors. One MMSE and CAMCOG scores in individual subjects; this process
subject was receiving an anticholinergic agent at baseline. Of controlled for intersubject baseline variability in cognitive test
the 42 subjects with PD who were not recruited, 28 refused scores that might be accounted for by, for example, poor vision
participation, one was too frail, one initially agreed but then did or low premorbid IQ. In addition, to examine for specific effects
not complete the baseline assessment and 12 others did not of variables on different cognitive domains, the average annual
meet the study criteria. change in the different subscores of the CAMCOG (orientation;
All subjects were assessed at entry to the study (baseline) and attention; language comprehension and expression; memory
annually thereafter for 3 years. The 2 year follow-up visit and remote, recent and learning; praxis; calculation; abstract
3 year visit were performed a mean of 24 (SD 0.4) months and thinking; and perception) were included in the analyses.
36 (1.16) months after the baseline visit, respectively. Similarly, an average of the annual change in PoA score was
At baseline, subjects were assessed using the Unified also calculated for further analysis.
Parkinsons Disease Rating Scale (UPDRS, activities of daily Outcome measures were examined both graphically and
living, part II and motor impairments, part III).20 Items derived using the ShapiroWilk test to establish whether they were
from these two subscales permit determination of PD subtype, approximately normally distributed. For comparison of baseline
according to the method proposed by Jankovic and colleagues.21 characteristics, MannWhitney U tests, Students t tests and x2
In the present report, motor phenotypes were defined at tests were used where appropriate.
baseline as either PIGD or non-PIGD (including 18 tremor To examine the association between baseline variables and
dominant and six indeterminate subtypes). At baseline and rates of decline/change in cognitive and attentional functions, a
follow-up assessments, all subjects underwent cognitive assess- two stage statistical analysis was carried out: (i) baseline
ments, including the MMSE and CAMCOG.22 Depression was variables (age, sex, motor phenotype, UPDRS-III baseline score,
assessed using the Geriatric Depression Scale 15 (GDS-15).23 At CAMCOG baseline score, GDS baseline score, L-dopa daily dose
baseline and follow-up, subjects underwent CDR computer and baseline PoA score), if not already categorical, were
based attentional tasks that included assessment of simple and dichotomised into two groups around the median, and
choice reaction times and digit vigilance (all measured in comparisons of MMSE, CAMCOG, CAMCOG subscores and
milliseconds), as described by Wesnes and colleagues.24 PoA PoA annual rate of change were made using MannWhitney U
was calculated by summing the choice reaction time, simple tests; (ii) baseline variables associated with significant differ-
reaction time and digit vigilance reaction time scores; higher ences in outcome variables were then entered into backward
scores represent worse attentional performance and reflect how stepwise linear regression analyses. Categorical data were coded
well a person can maintain concentration to a particular 0 and 1 (eg, motor phenotype). The distribution of PoA baseline
task.24 25 Subjects were re-evaluated clinically at each time point scores was positively skewed and thus this variable was log
for the development of dementia; diagnosis was made on the transformed prior to entry into models. Final models were

Table 1 Baseline demographic, cognitive and motor characteristics


Non-PIGD group PIGD group p Value

No of cases at baseline 23 14 N/A


Age (years) 74.5 (5.8) 76.3 (5.4) 0.81 (t test)
Sex (men: women)* 17:6 10:4 Fisher exact test p = 1.00
Duration of PD symptoms at baseline (years) 3.8 (4.5) 5.3 (4.1) 0.14
Baseline UPDRS III 23.9 (8.8) 27.8 (12.4) 0.30
Baseline MMSE 26.7 (2.3) 26.6 (1.8) 0.62
Baseline CAMCOG 90.8 (6.5) 86.9 (6.1) 0.04*
Baseline PoA (ms) 1377 (247) 1443 (253) 0.27
Baseline GDS 4.1 (3.4) 3.4 (2.0) 0.89
On levodopa at baseline 15 13 Fisher exact test p = 0.11
Baseline levodopa dose (mg) 353.3 (235.6) 526.9 (374.0) 0.21
*Overall preponderance of men over women in the study (p = 0.008).
Means (SD) are displayed where appropriate. MannWhitney U tests were used for comparison unless stated otherwise.
CAMCOG, Cambridge Cognitive Examination; GDS, Geriatric Depression Scale; MMSE, Mini-Mental State Examination; NA, not
applicable; PD, Parkinsons disease; PIGD, postural instability gait difficulty; PoA, power of attention; UPDRS III, Unified Parkinsons
Disease Rating Scale III.

J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2008;79:13181323. doi:10.1136/jnnp.2008.147629 1319


Downloaded from http://jnnp.bmj.com/ on February 8, 2015 - Published by group.bmj.com

Research paper

Table 2 Bivariate analysis of the effect of baseline variables on MMSE decline, CAMCOG decline and annual change in PoA
MMSE CAMCOG PoA
Variable (decline/year) p Value (decline/year) p Value (increase/year) p Value

Age (years)
,74 0.60 (1.45) 0.17 1.16 (4.25) 0.92 127.79 (391.34) 0.66
>74 20.06 (1.35) 1.04 (2.92) 79.70 (199.28)
Sex
Men 0.41 (1.57) 0.11 1.76 (3.85) 0.06 140.43 (347.32) 0.06
Women 20.17 (0.82) 20.70 (1.83) 3.26 (82.19)
Motor phenotype
Non-PIGD 20.28 (1.20) 0.002* 20.15 (2.23) 0.02* 11.36 (93.44) 0.01*
PIGD 1.14 (1.34) 3.14 (4.44) 244.03 (443.40)
Duration of PD symptoms at baseline (years)
,3.6 20.39 (1.10) 0.01* 0.17 (2.24) 0.34 67.81 (223.47) 0.35
>3.6 0.81 (1.45) 1.88 (4.31) 136.15 (367.77)
Baseline UPDRS III
,24 0.03 (0.88) 0.29 0.07 (2.34) 0.23 28.37 (81.77) 0.001*
>24 0.43 (1.73) 1.87 (4.17) 185.09 (377.35)
Baseline CAMCOG
,91 0.68 (1.71) 0.01* 2.07 (4.30) 0.19 181.86 (392.23) 0.03*
>91 20.24 (0.76) 20.05 (2.05) 8.52 (80.49)
Baseline GDS-15
,3 0.08 (1.10) 0.19 0.48 (3.13) 0.37 146.07 (430.57) 0.96
>3 0.38 (1.62) 1.52 (3.86) 75.03 (190.18)
Levodopa dose (mg)
,300 20.38 (0.79) 0.03* 20.88 (1.71) 0.03* 6.08 (116.20) 0.04*
>300 0.49 (1.54) 1.83 (3.82) 145.16 (350.02)
Power of attention at baseline (ms)
,1344 20.27 (1.63) 0.008* 0.20 (3.92) 0.04* 25.72 (71.76) 0.28
>1344 0.71 (1.05) 1.86 (3.15) 185.15 (421.09)
Means (SD) are displayed where appropriate.
CAMCOG, Cambridge Cognitive Examination; GDS, Geriatric Depression Scale; MMSE, Mini-Mental State Examination; PD, Parkinsons disease; PIGD, postural instability gait
difficulty; PoA, power of attention; UPDRS III, Unified Parkinsons Disease Rating Scale III.

determined with the stepwise exclusion of variables if p.0.10. groups in terms of age, duration of parkinsonian symptoms,
Independent variable interaction was determined by the UPDRS-III scores, MMSE, GDS or PoA scores. Similar propor-
inclusion of centred cross product of variables to regression tions of patients in both groups were receiving L-dopa
models and additionally the presence of interactions was treatment at baseline and dosages were approximately similar.
examined using two way ANOVA to look for any significant There was a suggestion that the PIGD group had slightly lower
interactions between dichotomised independents variables. In CAMCOG scores at baseline (p = 0.04). In addition, there was
all regression models there was no evidence of multicollinearity an overall preponderance of males over females in the study
(variance inflation factor ,2) and no cases exerted undue (p = 0.008) although gender proportions were similar between
influence, as evidenced by Cooks distances (D,1) and the PIGD and non-PIGD groups.
Mahalanobis distances (p,0.001). Standardised residuals were Mean annual decline in MMSE and CAMCOG scores for the
all ,3 and Durbin Watson values ,2. whole group was 0.26 points and 1.09 points, respectively.
PoA scores increased per year by an average of 103 ms.
RESULTS Bivariate analysis of the effect of baseline variables on MMSE
Thirty-nine subjects with PD were entered into the study. Over decline, CAMCOG decline and change in PoA are shown in
the course of the 3 year period reported here, five patients with table 2. More rapid MMSE decline was associated with PIGD
non-demented PD at baseline developed dementia. Four of these phenotype, longer duration of parkinsonian symptoms prior to
patients had the PIGD phenotype at baseline compared with one baseline, lower baseline CAMCOG, higher L-dopa dose and
non-PIGD phenotype at baseline (Fishers exact test, p = 0.06). higher baseline PoA score (p,0.05). More rapid CAMCOG
Two subjects died during follow-up (one after the first year decline was associated with PIGD phenotype, higher L-dopa
assessment, the other after the second year assessment). One of dose and greater baseline PoA score (p,0.05). A higher annual
the patients who died had developed dementia; both had the PIGD increase in PoA score was associated with higher baseline
phenotype and thus the rate of change in terms of cognitive decline UPDRS III score, lower baseline CAMCOG score and higher
and attention in these subjects was derived from a reduced number L-dopa dose (p,0.05). These variables were therefore selected
of data points. Two subjects who were assessed at baseline were for inclusion into their respective multiple regression models.
lost to follow-up and were excluded from the analysis. During the Multivariate analysis shown in table 3 demonstrated that in
study period, one subject was initiated on donepezil, two subjects the final models, PIGD phenotype and higher baseline PoA
on quetiapine, four subjects on catechol-O-methyltransferase scores were significant predictors of subsequent MMSE and
inhibitors and two subjects on dopamine agonists. CAMCOG decline. Addition of the centred cross product of
Baseline characteristics of the PIGD and non-PIGD patients PIGD phenotype and baseline PoA to models did not
are shown in table 1. There were no differences between the demonstrate any significant interaction effect (MMSE decline,

1320 J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2008;79:13181323. doi:10.1136/jnnp.2008.147629


Downloaded from http://jnnp.bmj.com/ on February 8, 2015 - Published by group.bmj.com

Research paper

phenotype for cognitive decline (MMSE decline, F = 1.13,


p = 0.30; CAMCOG decline, F = 0.19, p = 0.67). Of the variables
entered into the analysis of annual change in PoA score, only
PIGD phenotype significantly predicted a higher annual increase
in PoA scores. The influence of motor phenotype on mean
MMSE, CAMCOG and PoA over time is demonstrated
graphically in fig 1. Of the 17 patients with baseline PoA scores
less than 1344 ms (median baseline score), none developed
dementia by the third year compared with five out of 20
patients (25%) who had PoA scores greater than 1344 ms
(Fishers exact test p = 0.05).
Considering CAMCOG subscore changes, PIGD phenotype
was a significant predictor for decline in orientation (b = 0.64,
p = 0.04) whereas baseline PoA score was a significant predictor
for decline in orientation (b = 1.76, p = 0.04), language expres-
sion (b = 2.03, p = 0.01) and remote memory (b = 0.69,
p,0.001).

DISCUSSION
Consistent with previous studies, this prospective study of
patients with non-demented PD demonstrated that PIGD
motor phenotype was associated with a more rapid cognitive
decline.8 9 Additional findings were that: (i) PIGD motor
phenotype, while not associated with obvious baseline atten-
tional impairment, as measured by PoA, led to a more rapid
decline in attentional function over 3 years (ii) poor baseline
attentional function was associated with a more rapid cognitive
decline.
The rate of decline on the MMSE was low at 0.25 points per
year over the 3 year study period in all 39 subjects; this is
consistent with previous findings by our group over a 2 year
longitudinal study and by others.9 26 In contrast, separation of
the cohort into PIGD and non-PIGD motor phenotypes
demonstrated that the PIGD subgroup had a more marked
cognitive decline of 1.15 points annually on the MMSE,
although this was approximately half the rate of cognitive
decline noted in individuals with PD with established demen-
tia.9 26 This higher rate of cognitive decline in the non-tremor
motor phenotype has previously been reported8 and has been
postulated to relate to non-dopaminergic lesions.5 9 26
We hypothesised that given the putative common neural
Figure 1 Influence of motor phenotype on mean (A) MMSE, (B)
CAMCOG and (C) power of attention over time in PIGD and non-PIGD substrate mediating attention and postural/gait difficulties, we
patients. CAMCOG, Cambridge Cognitive Examination; MMSE, Mini- would find evidence of poor baseline attentional function
Mental State Examination; PIGD, postural instability gait difficulty. associated with PIGD motor phenotype. Interestingly, while we
noted a slight increase in baseline PoA scores in PIGD patients
b = 0.41, p = 0.87; CAMCOG decline b = 2.37, p = 0.72). This compared with non-PIGD patients (mean PoA baseline score:
observation was confirmed by a lack of interaction in two way PIGD patients 1443 (SD 253) ms versus non-PIGD patients 1377
ANOVA of dichotomised baseline PoA score against PIGD (SD 247) ms), this was not significant. However, over the

Table 3 Multivariate regression models for MMSE decline, CAMCOG decline and annual change in PoA
Variable b coefficient p Value

MMSE decline/year
R2 = 0.584, F = 8.79, p = 0.001 Constant 219.66 0.03
PIGD motor phenotype 1.30 0.003
Ln (PoA at baseline) 2.69 0.03
CAMCOG decline/year
R2 = 0.290, F = 6.93, p = 0.003 Constant 245.52 0.05
PIGD motor phenotype 3.00 0.008
Ln (PoA at baseline) 6.29 0.05
Ln (PoA increase/year)
R2 = 0.130, F = 4.72, p = 0.04 Constant 4.66 ,0.001
PIGD motor phenotype 1.03 0.04
CAMCOG, Cambridge Cognitive Examination; MMSE, Mini-Mental State Examination; PIGD, postural instability gait difficulty; PoA,
power of attention.

J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2008;79:13181323. doi:10.1136/jnnp.2008.147629 1321


Downloaded from http://jnnp.bmj.com/ on February 8, 2015 - Published by group.bmj.com

Research paper

course of the study there was a divergence between PIGD and with the known gender ratio difference in incidence of PD,31 it
non-PIGD subjects in terms of PoA, particularly at years 2 and 3 could be argued that the current study findings are applicable to
(see fig 1). We observed that PIGD motor phenotype and poor only men. However, gender ratios were similar when patients
baseline attention function were associated with an increased were grouped either by motor phenotype (see table 1) or around
rate of cognitive decline although these predictors appeared to median baseline PoA scores (PoA score ,1344 ms: males 12,
be independent of each other. females 6; PoA score .1344 ms: males 15, females 4; p = 0.48)
PIGD motor phenotype in patients with non-demented PD is which suggests that the study findings are applicable to both
likely to be mediated, at least in part, by brainstem lesions sexes.
affecting non-dopaminergic systems.5 9 In contrast, attentional Although not within the dementia range, baseline CAMCOG
function, while dependent on brainstem activation and ascend- scores for the PIGD group were lower than for the non-PIGD
ing cholinergic systems, is likely to be heavily influenced by the group suggesting that, even at baseline, these subjects were
integrity of neocortical systems, including fronto-subcortical more likely to have accelerated cognitive decline.
circuits involving the thalamus. Striatal dopaminergic pathways Whether the relative contribution to subsequent cognitive
may also have a role in inhibitory and attentional function in decline is primarily caused by the PIGD motor phenotype or to
patients with PD.27 28 We suggest, therefore, that our observa- the degree of pre-existing cognitive impairment is not clear from
tions may indicate that, initially, there are dissociate processes the present findings. However, Alves and colleagues10 noted in
mediating attentional dysfunction and PIGD motor phenotype, their longitudinal study that individuals who developed
but given that they arise from common neurodegenerative incident PIGD motor phenotype had a high risk of subsequent
processes (eg, progressive loss of cholinergic function) that with cognitive decline and dementia. This therefore suggests that the
time there is a convergence of symptoms that is inclusive of PIGD motor phenotype is likely to be a predominant
attentional, cognitive and motor impairments. Importantly, the contributor to the observed longitudinal cognitive decline in
study findings suggest that early changes in PoA, even the the present study.
absence of clear cognitive impairment, may predict incipient In addition, a follow-up period of 3 years is comparatively
cognitive decline and dementia in PD. short and while the current study assumes a linear temporal
The association of low baseline PoA with decline in progression in cognitive decline, it is recognised that this is
CAMCOG subscores of language expression and remote unlikely in clinical practice.32 33
memory was consistent with known cognitive deficits in PD In conclusion, our results suggest that the PIGD motor
in terms of verbal fluency and recollection of remote phenotype and attentional deficits are associated with a faster
events.8 29 30 In contrast, the lack of any obvious relationship rate of cognitive decline in patients with non-demented PD. We
between either PIGD motor phenotype and baseline PoA score suggest that subtle deficits in attention and PIGD motor
with the CAMCOG attention subscore was unexpected. The phenotype in patients with non-demented PD are each
attention subscore in CAMCOG consists of counting from 20 to predictive of subsequent cognitive impairment and are poten-
1 and serial 7s; thus while this task will require attention tially additive. This conclusion requires validation in a larger
function, the neural substrate is likely to be widely distributed sample but if robust may help to inform prognosis and future
with a large element dependent on parietal lobe networks which trials of early interventions in preventing or delaying dementia
are less likely to be affected by the PIGD motor phenotype and in PD.
attentional function, as measured by PoA. Overall, however, we
would argue that the observations of a relationship between Funding: This work was supported by the Medical Research Council. The Alzheimer
Research Trust supported ENR.
PIGD status and PoA scores and various CAMCOG subscores
need to be viewed cautiously given the smaller annual rates of Competing interests: None.
change (and thus greater variability) compared with the global Ethics approval: The study received approval from the local research ethics
CAMCOG score. committee.
There did not appear to be any association between GDS-15
scores and cognitive or attentional function; however, subject REFERENCES
scores were low and the effect of any potential depressive 1. Schrag A, Jahanshahi M, Quinn N. What contributes to quality of life in patients with
symptoms on cognitive and attentional function is likely to Parkinsons disease? J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2000;69:30812.
2. Levy G, Tang MX, Louis ED, et al. The association of incident dementia with mortality
have been limited in the present study. in PD. Neurology 2002;59:170813.
3. Marras C, Rochon P, Lang AE. Predicting motor decline and disability in Parkinson
disease: a systematic review. Arch Neurol 2002;59:17248.
Strengths of the study 4. Aarsland D, Andersen K, Larsen JP, et al. Prevalence and characteristics of
Strengths of this study include its prospective design, low dementia in Parkinson disease: an 8-year prospective study. Arch Neurol
dropout rate, inclusion of patients diagnosed according to 2003;60:38792.
5. Emre M. Dementia associated with Parkinsons disease. Lancet Neurol 2003;2:22937.
formal diagnostic criteria and verification by consensus agree-
6. Hughes TA, Ross HF, Musa S, et al. A 10-year study of the incidence of and factors
ment. In addition, cognitive changes were assessed using both predicting dementia in Parkinsons disease. Neurology 2000;54:1596603.
the MMSE and CAMCOG. Few subjects were taking drugs at 7. Levy G, Schupf N, Tang M-X, et al. Combined effect of age and severity on the risk of
baseline, or were initiated on at follow-up which could have dementia in Parkinsons disease. Ann Neurol 2002;51:7229.
8. Williams-Gray CH, Foltynie T, Brayne CEG, et al. Evolution of cognitive dysfunction
affected cognitive and attentional scores. in an incident Parkinsons disease cohort. Brain 2007;130:178798.
9. Burn DJ, Rowan EN, Allan LM, et al. Motor subtype and cognitive decline in
Parkinsons disease, Parkinsons disease with dementia, and dementia with Lewy
Weaknesses of the study bodies. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2006;77:5859.
Potential weaknesses include the relatively small sample size 10. Alves G, Larsen JP, Emre M, et al. Changes in motor subtype and risk for incident
and potential recruitment bias (via outpatient clinics rather dementia in Parkinsons disease. Mov Disord 2006;21:112330.
11. Tiraboschi P, Hansen LA, Alford M, et al. Cholinergic dysfunction in diseases with
than a community based sample, with all patients aged over
Lewy bodies. Neurology 2000;54:40711.
65 years), which may limit the generalisability of the results. 12. Ehrt U, Aarsland D. Psychiatric aspects of Parkinsons disease. Curr Opin Psychiatry
Indeed, while the male preponderance in the study is in keeping 2005;18:33541.

1322 J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2008;79:13181323. doi:10.1136/jnnp.2008.147629


Downloaded from http://jnnp.bmj.com/ on February 8, 2015 - Published by group.bmj.com

Research paper

13. Litvan I, Mohr E, Williams J, et al. Differential memory and executive functions in 23. Meara J, Mitchelmore E, Hobson P. Use of the GDS-15 geriatric depression scale as
demented patients with Parkinsons and Alzheimers disease. J Neurol Neurosurg a screening instrument for depressive symptomatology in patients with Parkinsons
Psychiatry 1991;54:259. disease and their carers in the community. Age Ageing 1999;28:358.
14. Bronnick K, Ehrt U, Emre M, et al. Attentional deficits affect activities of daily living 24. Wesnes KA, McKeith IG, Ferrara R, et al. Effects of rivastigmine on cognitive
in dementia-associated with Parkinsons disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry function in dementia with Lewy bodies: a randomised placebo-controlled international
2006;77:113642. study using the cognitive drug research computerised assessment system. Dement
15. Dubois B, Pilon B, Lhermitte F, et al. Cholinergic deficiency and frontal dysfunction in Geriatr Cogn Disord 2002;13:18392.
Parkinsons disease. Ann Neurol 1990;28:11721. 25. Emre M, Aarsland D, Albanese A, et al. Rivastigmine for dementia associated with
16. Woollacott M, Shumway-Cook A. Attention and the control of posture and gait: a Parkinsons disease. N Engl J Med 2004;351:250918.
review of an emerging area of research. Gait Posture 2002;16:114. 26. Aarsland D, Andersen K, Larsen JP, et al. The rate of cognitive decline in Parkinson
17. OShea S, Morris ME, Iansek R. Dual task interference during gait in people with disease. Arch Neurol 2004;61:190611.
Parkinson disease: effects of motor versus cognitive secondary tasks. Phys Ther 27. Barnes J, Boubert L. Executive functions are impaired in patients with
2002;82:88897. Parkinsons disease with visual hallucinations. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry
18. Gibb WR, Lees AJ. The relevance of the Lewy body to the pathogenesis of idiopathic 2008;79:1902.
Parkinsons disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1988;51:74552. 28. Nieoullon A. Dopamine and the regulation of cognition and attention. Prog Neurobiol
19. Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR. Mini-mental state. A practical method for 2002;67:5383.
grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res 1975;12:18998. 29. Leplow B, Dierks C, Herrmann P, et al. Remote memory in Parkinsons disease and
20. Fahn S, Elton R, and members of the UPDRS development committee. Unified senile dementia. Neuropsychologia 1997;35:54757.
Parkinsons Disease Rating Scale. In: Fahn S, Mardson C, Goldstein M, et al, eds. 30. Sagar HJ, Cohen NJ, Sullivan EV, et al. Remote memory function in Alzheimers
Recent developments in Parkinsons disease. New York: Macmillan, 1987:15363. disease and Parkinsons disease. Brain 1988;111:185206.
21. Jankovic J, McDermott M, Carter J, et al. Variable expression of Parkinsons 31. Taylor KSM, Cook JA, Counsell CE. Heterogeneity in male to female risk for
disease: a base-line analysis of the DATATOP cohort. The Parkinson Study Group. Parkinsons disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2007;78:9056.
Neurology 1990;40:152934. 32. Ballard C, OBrien J, Swann A, et al. One year follow-up of parkinsonism in dementia
22. Roth M, Tym E, Mountjoy CQ, et al. CAMDEX. A standardised instrument for the with Lewy bodies. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 2000;11:21922.
diagnosis of mental disorder in the elderly with special reference to the early 33. Helmes E, Bowler JV, Merskey H, et al. Rates of cognitive decline in Alzheimers
detection of dementia. Br J Psychiatry 1986;149:698709. disease and dementia with Lewy bodies. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 2003;15:6771.

Quality & Safety in Health Care


Quality & Safety in Health Care is a leading international peer-review journal in the growing area of
quality and safety improvement. It provides essential information for those wanting to reduce harm and
improve patient safety and the quality of care. The journal reports and reflects research, improvement
initiatives and viewpoints and other discursive papers relevant to these crucial aims with contributions
from researchers, clinical professionals and managers and experts in organisational development and
behaviour.

qshc.bmj.com

J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2008;79:13181323. doi:10.1136/jnnp.2008.147629 1323


Downloaded from http://jnnp.bmj.com/ on February 8, 2015 - Published by group.bmj.com

Poor attentional function predicts cognitive


decline in patients with non-demented
Parkinson's disease independent of motor
phenotype
J-P Taylor, E N Rowan, D Lett, J T O'Brien, I G McKeith and D J Burn

J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2008 79: 1318-1323 originally published


online June 27, 2008
doi: 10.1136/jnnp.2008.147629

Updated information and services can be found at:


http://jnnp.bmj.com/content/79/12/1318

These include:

References This article cites 32 articles, 16 of which you can access for free at:
http://jnnp.bmj.com/content/79/12/1318#BIBL

Email alerting Receive free email alerts when new articles cite this article. Sign up in the
service box at the top right corner of the online article.

Topic Articles on similar topics can be found in the following collections


Collections Memory disorders (psychiatry) (1254)
Dementia (911)

Notes

To request permissions go to:


http://group.bmj.com/group/rights-licensing/permissions

To order reprints go to:


http://journals.bmj.com/cgi/reprintform

To subscribe to BMJ go to:


http://group.bmj.com/subscribe/

Você também pode gostar