Você está na página 1de 10

ARTICLE IN PRESS

Assessments of Voice Use and Voice Quality Among


College/University Singing Students Ages 1824
Through Ambulatory Monitoring With a Full
Accelerometer Signal
*Matthew J. Schloneger and Eric J. Hunter, *Hesston, Kansas, and East Lansing, Michigan

Summary: The multiple social and performance demands placed on college/university singers could put their still-
developing voices at risk. Previous ambulatory monitoring studies have analyzed the duration, intensity, and frequency
(in Hertz) of voice use among such students. Nevertheless, no studies to date have incorporated the simultaneous acous-
tic voice quality measures into the acquisition of these measures to allow for direct comparison during the same voicing
period. Such data could provide greater insight into how young singers use their voices, as well as identify potential
correlations between vocal dose and acoustic changes in voice quality.
The purpose of this study was to assess the voice use and the estimated voice quality of college/university singing
students (1824 years old, N = 19). Ambulatory monitoring was conducted over three full, consecutive weekdays mea-
suring voice from an unprocessed accelerometer signal measured at the neck. From this signal, traditional vocal dose
metrics such as phonation percentage, dose time, cycle dose, and distance dose were analyzed. Additional acoustic mea-
sures included perceived pitch, pitch strength, long-term average spectrum slope, alpha ratio, dB sound pressure level
13 kHz, and harmonic-to-noise ratio. Major findings from more than 800 hours of recording indicated that among
these students (a) higher vocal doses correlated significantly with greater voice intensity, more vocal clarity and less
perturbation; and (b) there were significant differences in some acoustic voice quality metrics between nonsinging, solo
singing, and choral singing.
Key Words: Voice useVocal doseAmbulatory voice monitoringVocal pedagogyVoice quality.

Few previous studies provide empirical data regarding the typical load through measurements of acoustic quality and sound pres-
vocal dose acquired by college/university students who partic- sure level (SPL). A majority of these studies show a positive
ipate in multiple singing activities. These students frequently correlation between potentially related factors (eg, increasing vocal
experience heavy vocal demands: voice lessons, choral and theatre doses, later hours of the day, reports of vocal fatigue, and/or the
activities, student-organized music groups, church activities, busy Lombard effect8) and changes in acoustic properties such as in-
social lives, sports, and sometimes jobs that involve heavy vocal creases in fundamental frequency (F0);4 loudness (dB SPL),9,10
demands (eg, waiting tables or phone centers).1 They may develop harmonic-to-noise ratio (HNR),11 and spectral energy as mea-
less-than-desirable sleep and vocal hygiene habits.2 Further, these sured by long-term average spectrum (LTAS),9,12 as well as
students may be unaware of the negative, cumulative effects of decreases in voice perturbation (shimmer and jitter).13,14
heavy vocal loads on their voices, lacking or ignoring training It is possible that these acoustic characteristics might relate to
in vocal hygiene.3 vocal fatigue. For example, increases in F0 and upper frequency
Both teachers and students would benefit from published, sci- LTAS energy have been linked to increases in dB SPL,15,16 and
entific standards of voice use for young singers with developing the above changes to F0, dB SPL, HNR, shimmer, jitter, and LTAS
and stabilizing vocal instruments, but formulation of such stan- have been connected to increased muscular activity and tension
dards remains an elusive task. To date, despite a considerable that occurred following a fatiguing loading activity.14 Neverthe-
body of literature reporting on the vibratory, acoustic, and per- less, Boucher and Ayad5 found that individual variations in F0 did
ceived effects of vocal loading among various populations, there not consistently reflect measured muscular fatigue in laryngeal
remains a paucity of data pinpointing when particular vocal in- structures. Further, there have been studies that either showed no
efficiencies may first develop.47 One potential explanation for change6,17 or increases in shimmer and jitter following vocal-loading
this gap may be a lack of studies that simultaneously analyze tasks.18 Acoustic perturbation measures have not yet received at-
vocal dose and voice quality in a real-time field setting. tention in ambulatory field studies of voice use, where they could
Numerous studies, nearly all of them in controlled laborato- be measured at the vocal source by an accelerometer transducer.
ry situations, have been completed analyzing the effect of vocal A growing body of studies has also analyzed vocal load in the
field, without acoustic voice quality analysis. Voice dosimeters,
Accepted for publication December 29, 2015.
first developed in the early 2000s, were created to measure vocal
Presented at the 44th Annual Symposium: Care of the Professional Voice, May 26 to dose, defined as vocal fold tissue exposure to vibration over time.19,20
May 31, 2015, Philadelphia Westin.
From the *Department of Fine Arts, Hesston College, Hesston, Kansas; and the Com-
Rather than relying on acoustic audio-recording methods, these
municative Sciences and Disorders, Michigan State University, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania. devices used an accelerometer transducer to record skin vibrations
Address correspondence and reprint requests to Matthew J. Schloneger, 2713 Goldenrod
Road, North Newton KS 67117. E-mail: mattschloneger@gmail.com
in the neck. In this way, phonation activities could be tracked in
Journal of Voice, Vol. , No. , pp. - isolation from ambient sounds. Various methods for accelerometer-
0892-1997
2015 The Voice Foundation. Published by Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
based voice dosimetry have been examined in the literature, and
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jvoice.2015.12.018 techniques for analyzing and calibrating accelerometer signals from
ARTICLE IN PRESS
2 Journal of Voice, Vol. , No. , 2016

voice have been discussed.19,2123 vec et al found that mean SPL All participants completed a short demographic questionnaire
from voiced speech could be predicted by a skin accelerometer during their first meeting. In addition to confirming their current
with an accuracy of better than 2.8 dB.21 Specifically, Mehta et participation in a college choir and in voice lessons, the ques-
al24 showed that although an average F0 error and estimated average tionnaire asked the students to provide details about number of
SPL error dropped to 1% after 12 hours and 20 hours of moni- semesters enrolled, estimated hours of singing per week during
toring, respectively, dose calculations past tense needed at least the current semester, years of voice lesson experience, years of
26 hours of monitoring for average errors to drop below 10%. The choral experience, and details about any previous vocal limitation/
study recommended that future voice dosimetry should involve injury that may have required a health-care professional. Because
the recording of raw, rather than sampled, accelerometer signals. the effect of musical style was not a focus of this study, the par-
Several published studies began to quantify a typical vocal ticipants were not asked to distinguish between their use of classical
dose among different populations, including teachers, 2529 and contemporary commercial singing styles in solo singing.
children,30,31 and various populations of singers (eg, high school
students,32 graduate student vocalists,33,34 and undergraduate Equipment
student vocalists1). Although these studies provided data that ad- Recordings were conducted using a Roland R-05 digital audio re-
dressed questions about the typical vocal doses among different corder (Roland Corporation U.S., Los Angeles, CA) with storage
populations, voice data collected included only processed in- to a 16 GB SD card and the collar of VoxLog portable voice ana-
formation about the duration, frequency, and amplitude of lyzer collar (Sonovox AB) (Figure 1), adjusted to comfortably fit
vibrations with no insight of the efficiency with which those vi- the circumference of the participants neck (Figure 2). Within each
brations were produced. Further, these studies did not examine collar were two transducers: (1) a Panasonic WM-61A omnidi-
voice quality alongside vocal dose because the dosimeters used rectional microphone (Panasonic Corporation of North America,
in these studies did not allow for simultaneous real-time anal- Secaucus, NJ) to sample the airborne acoustics, and (2) a Knowles
ysis of spectral and voice perturbation data. BU-1771 Model accelerometer (Knowles Acoustics, Itasca, IL).
To date, no ambulatory field study of healthy singers has si-
multaneously acquired a combination of the participants vocal
dose with additional measures obtained from postprocessing of
a full recording signal to examine how the quality of vocal pro-
duction might relate to the vocal dose and the vocal efficiency of
each individual. Such measurements in the study of young singers
could be important in understanding why some young singers dem-
onstrate declines in vocal efficiency more quickly than others.
Anecdotal experience suggests that some young singers may cul-
tivate strong, efficient singing techniques through voice lessons
or choral experience, yet develop vocal problems due to poor vocal
hygiene, unhealthy quality of speech, and heavy speech doses.35
The opposite could also be true if young vocalists with efficient
speech habits develop inefficient singing habits.
The purpose of this study was to assess the voice use, voice
quality, and perceived singing voice function of college/
university singing students to answer the following research
question: Are there statistically significant relationships between
FIGURE 1. VoxLog collar.
students vocal dose measures and common metrics related to
voice quality as calculated from a raw accelerometer signal?

METHODS
Participants
A convenience sample of 25 traditional-age college/university
singing students (1824 years old), enrolled in both voice lessons
and choir, was recruited. The study was approved by the Human
Subjects Committee at the primary authors university. None of
these students reported a history of any vocal pathology. The
students represented four different institutions of higher educa-
tion (a private 2-year college, a private 4-year college, and two
state universities) and five different private voice teachers. Al-
though a balance of men and women was sought in the sample,
voice type differences were not considered due to the large FIGURE 2. VoxLog collar worn around the neck and attached to a
number of variables already being considered. Roland R-05 Digital Recorder.
ARTICLE IN PRESS
Matthew J. Schloneger and Eric Hunter Voice Use and Quality of College/University Singing Students 3

Participants carried the Roland R-05 in a tune belt vertical micro- of 2530 dB SPL, the maximum level that could be reached
phone transmitter carrier belt worn around the participants without clipping voicing data.
midsection underneath the clothes. This setup was described in detail
in previous studies.36 Multiple day data processing
Parameter extraction was accomplished using custom MATLAB
Calibration to process the recordings. Files were processed in 1-minute in-
Prior to beginning the daylong recordings, each participant com- crements and then underwent an automatic presegmentation
pleted an SPL calibration; the specific procedure is described process similar to the technique described by Bckstrm et al.38
in detail by Schloneger.37 To summarize, the participant com- The 1-minute segment then underwent several stages of analy-
pleted a series of three spoken /a/ vowels at a comfortable pitch sis. First, in 10-millisecond intervals, MATLAB estimated dB SPL
in a quiet room while holding a standard sound level meter at (based on the calibration) and also estimated vocal pitch and vocal
a distance of 30 cm from the mouth. The VoxLog collar was worn F0 using the Audswipe algorithm39 as well as PRAAT software40
around the neck while recording the /a/ vowels with an iPad video (command line version). Then the script concatenated all voiced
recorder (Designed by Apple Inc, manufactured by Foxconn). segments from the 1-minute interval to calculate additional
The /a/ vowel files were then processed using GoldWave v5.70 metrics. In all, for each 1-minute window, the output included
(www.goldwave.com) digital audio editing software. The re- voicing percentage and the following metrics with summary sta-
searcher observed the video of each /a/ vowel frame by frame tistics (eg, mean, IRQ, standard deviation, skewness): F0, P0, dB
and logged each dB SPL level (from an economic sound level SPL, LTAS slope, alpha ratio, dB 1K3K, shimmer, jitter, pitch
meter, DT 85A) on a spreadsheet. A custom MATLAB strength, and HNR. The MATLAB script repeated this process
(www.mathworks.com) script was used to obtain calibration levels for each minute of the full-day files and saved an output file with
for each participant and each transducer. aggregated data for each minute. An Excel (Microsoft Corpo-
ration) spreadsheet template was used to aggregate the time
interval data and calculate the results of each measure for the
Data collection entire period analyzed, with appropriately weighted averages.
The participants voices were recorded during all waking hours The Excel spreadsheet also employed formulas that com-
over three weekdays while classes were in session. The partici- piled several vocal dose measures: (1) phonation time dose (Dt)
pants wore the monitors 10.518.0 hours each day, putting the refers to the cumulative duration of time (hh:mm:ss) or the per-
monitors on immediately after dressing in the morning and re- centage of time the vocal folds have actually touched in a given
moving the monitors just before retiring for the evening (AA period; (2) cycle dose (Dc) refers to the accumulated number
batteries, proven to be sufficient for 20+ hour a day, were used). of such repetitive cycles in a particular time period; and (3) dis-
The researcher remained available by phone throughout the study tance dose (Dd) is an estimate of how far vocal folds travel
periods in the event of any technical problems. On the second in a period of time incorporating total phonation time, F0, and
and third morning of monitoring, each participant replaced the amplitude into one dosage measure. Taken together, these three
batteries and inserted a blank 16 GB SD card (externally labeled measures provide a detailed picture of the volume and intensi-
Day 1, Day 2, and Day 3) in the recorder. Approximately, 2 GB ty of voice use.20,41
of data were recorded for each 3.33 hours of monitoring. The It should be noted that MATLAB output included two newly
captured unprocessed data were transferred to a separate exter- developed measures. First, perceived pitch (P0) is a term devel-
nal hard drive and then backed up on a cloud storage system at oped for this study to represent the frequency output of the
the end of each participant monitoring period. Audswipe algorithm.39 As the algorithm examines the entire har-
To determine what activities occurred during each recorded monic spectrum, its output can be considered similar to the
phonation period and when, the participants completed daily ac- perceived auditory pitch as opposed to a simple reading of F0.
tivity logs.23,25 Each participant documented vocal tasks and Second, pitch strength,42 a measure of voice clarity, is a quan-
significant activities throughout the day along with the time each titative interpretation of the strength of the pitch sensation created
activity commenced, noting the time displayed on the Roland by a complex tone, measured as a percentage; the higher the pitch
digital recorder. strength, the more tonal the sound is judged.

File processing and analysis Analysis procedures and assumptions


Audio processing As with any acoustic analysis, a range of assumptions and pro-
The Roland recorded in stereo WAV file format, with the left cedures were necessary. The primary assumptions and procedures,
channel recording accelerometer data and the right channel re- based on previous ambulatory monitoring studies referred in this
cording acoustic transducer data. Using GoldWave, accelerometer paper, are only briefly summarized here (the full detail of as-
and microphone channels were segmented into individual WAV sumptions can be found in Schloneger 201437). To minimize false
files for different activity periods for comparison. The sam- voicing readings, the MATLAB analysis discarded any voicing
pling rate for each file was reduced from 44,100 kHz to segments shorter than 40 milliseconds, any full minutes of anal-
14,700 kHz to make the file sizes manageable. The accelerom- ysis that contained less than 3% voicing, and any readings of
eter files recorded signals at a much lower intensity level than voicing for which the calibrated SPL level was below 47 dB SPL.
the audio files, so these files were normalized by amplification LTAS was calculated, per an analysis based on techniques
ARTICLE IN PRESS
4 Journal of Voice, Vol. , No. , 2016

described in previously,43 from the concatenated voiced segment. ships between each of the four measures of student vocal dose
From the LTAS, the spectrum between the median F0 and 5000 Hz (phonation percentage, Dt, Dc, and Dd) and each of 10 mea-
was used to obtain the LTAS slope in dB/Hz. The alpha ratio sures of voice quality (F0, P0, dB SPL, LTAS slope, alpha ratio,
for any designated period of time was determined by taking the dB SPL 13 kHz, pitch strength, shimmer, jitter, and HNR) ac-
LTAS for the period and dividing the summed intensity in dB quired with the VoxLog collars unfiltered accelerometer signal.
from 1001 Hz to 5000 Hz by the summed intensity from 50 Hz
to 1000 Hz. The summed intensity of the sound between 1 kHz
and 3 kHz (3125 Hz) normalized the total intensity of the entire Full days and activities totals
spectrum.44 Accelerometer data were collected for all the above variables
Data from each of the three full monitoring days were ana- over three full ambulatory monitoring days and disaggregated
lyzed as a whole and as disaggregated by activity. Disaggregations the full days into different activities: choir, solo singing, and
included choral singing, solo singing, instrumental playing, and nonsinging. Totals and measures of central tendency per day and
nonsinging time. Due to consistent problems with the MATLAB for each activity are displayed below for all participants, men,
script interpreting instruments in close proximity as voicing (eg, and women (Tables 2, 3, and 4, respectively).
loud saxophone playing), all instrumental playing minutes were
removed from the overall analysis. Vocal dose and voice quality
Correlations between vocal dose and voice quality
measurements for each segmented activity were compiled in
over full monitoring days
1-minute intervals (using the MATLAB output) and then aggre-
Pearson correlation coefficient tests were employed between each
gated those data to determine the overall activity measurements
of the 10 voice quality measures (F0, P0, dB SPL, LTAS slope,
for each individual and for the study population.
alpha ratio, dB SPL 13 kHz, pitch strength, shimmer, jitter,
and HNR) and each of the four vocal dose measures not derived
RESULTS
in part from a voice quality measure (voicing %, Dt, Dc, and
Of the initial 25 participants, 2 students withdrew from the study.
Dd) for two different disaggregations of full-day ambulatory
A total of 23 students completed 3 days of monitoring, but as
monitoring data: (1) 3-day totals by participant (N = 19), and
with many ambulatory studies,45,46 4 of them had large gaps in
(2) individual day totals (N = 57).
recording, necessitating their removal from the study. The 19
students (11 men, 8 women) who successfully completed 3 days
of at least 10.5 hours of monitoring per day were included in 3 day voice quality means vs. 3 day vocal dose totals
the current study, following the practice of previous ambulato- Table 5 displays Pearson correlation results for the 3-day am-
ry monitoring studies with uneven compliance. 47 The 19 bulatory monitoring totals for each participant. There were
participants recorded for an average of 14 hours and 36 minutes multiple significant correlations between dose measures and per-
each day. They had the monitor turned off for an average of 31 turbation measures, particularly pitch strength and shimmer, and
minutes of reported waking hours, with a range of 0 minutes moderately strong although not statistically significant correla-
to 4 hours and 23 minutes not recorded (eg, during contact sports, tions between dose measures and jitter and HNR. In each case,
avoiding contact with water [showering or swimming], and private a higher vocal dose correlated with less perturbation.
conversations).
Table 1 provides basic demographic data for the partici-
pants. A two-tailed independent samples t test, t(18), revealed Daily voice quality means vs. daily vocal dose totals
no significant differences between age, years of choral experi- Table 6 displays Pearson correlation results for individual am-
ence, or years of voice lessons between these men and women. bulatory monitoring day totals. Like the 3-day totals, there were
Results are presented according to the research questions posed multiple moderate, significant correlations between various dose
for this investigation. A predetermined alpha level of 0.01 served measures and perturbation measures among individual daily totals,
to indicate significance for all statistical tests. The researchers in this case with each of the four perturbation measures (pitch
chose this alpha level in lieu of an alpha level of 0.05 with applied strength, jitter, shimmer, and HNR) having at least one signif-
Bonferroni corrections, a method that was considered too con- icant correlation with a vocal dose measure. Among the individual
servative when considering the large number of within-family days, there were also significant correlations between dB SPL
tests. Data were examined for statistically significant relation- and both voicing % and Dt.

TABLE 1.
Demographic Data: Participants (N = 19) Completing Three Full Days of Monitoring
M t Test for Sex
Mean Mode Standard Deviation Range Men Women t P
Age 19.95 19 2.51 6 20.00 19.89 0.132 0.896
Years in choir 8.40 7 3.7 13 7.27 9.78 1.483 0.163
Years of voice lessons 2.78 0.5 2.55 7.5 2.86 2.67 0.168 0.864
ARTICLE IN PRESS
Matthew J. Schloneger and Eric Hunter Voice Use and Quality of College/University Singing Students 5

TABLE 2. TABLE 3.
Measures of Central Tendency, Full Monitoring Days: All Measures of Central Tendency, Full Monitoring Days: Men
Participants (N = 19) (N = 11)
Measure Total Nonsing Choral Solo Measure Total Nonsing Choral Solo
Recording 42.69 37.80 2.93 2.16 Recording 42.54 37.35 3.66 1.98
hourstotal htotal
Voicing % 11.92 8.63 38.10 34.50 Voicing % 13.07 9.28 38.30 36.89
Dttotal (min) 305.25 195.65 62.65 44.79 Dttotal 335.55 208.06 84.01 43.86
Dtper h (min) 7.15 5.18 22.86 20.70 (min)
Dctotal (kc) 4153.53 2483.88 919.24 761.66 Dtper h 7.84 5.57 22.98 22.13
Dcper h (kc) 97.32 65.71 334.40 351.96 (min)
Ddtotal (m) 15815.04 9248.14 3783.41 2879.60 Dctotal (kc) 3475.61 2004.77 1006.50 520.17
Ddper h (m) 370.57 244.65 1376.31 1330.66 Dcper h (kc) 81.25 53.67 275.33 262.47
F0 mean (Hz) 228.00 209.57 260.61 271.47 Ddtotal (m) 17914.26 10049.29 5068.46 3094.60
F0 median (Hz) 215.08 252.99 265.69 368.17 Ddper h (m) 418.76 269.05 1386.51 1561.49
F0 mode (Hz) 192.89 220.83 230.42 255.32 F0 mean (Hz) 171.56 157.36 191.18 188.47
F0 SD (Hz) 73.20 71.49 85.78 82.00 F0 median (Hz) 162.81 145.94 186.27 184.14
P0 mean (Hz) 225.34 251.73 351.75 370.77 F0 mode (Hz) 148.47 131.85 171.61 165.35
P0 median (Hz) 216.15 198.10 287.70 268.27 F0 SD (Hz) 53.42 53.03 54.81 55.29
P0 mode (Hz) 210.49 191.19 267.74 266.26 P0 mean (Hz) 174.19 156.39 196.04 194.25
P0 SD (Hz) 63.34 60.03 131.58 133.29 P0 median (Hz) 166.53 147.12 190.80 189.89
dB SPL mean 76.95 75.49 79.26 77.98 P0 mode (Hz) 164.37 145.31 189.47 179.35
dB SPL median 77.20 75.58 80.06 78.92 P0 SD (Hz) 48.90 46.13 50.71 54.99
dB SPL mode 77.47 75.50 80.66 79.39 dB SPL mean 78.14 75.93 77.58 80.03
dB SPL SD 5.47 5.25 5.61 6.22 dB SPL median 78.50 76.05 78.22 80.88
LTAS slope 0.58 0.54 0.63 0.66 dB SPL mode 78.86 76.10 79.05 81.87
(100) dB SPL SD 5.50 5.21 5.55 6.26
Alpha ratio 23.15 22.47 23.64 24.47 LTAS slope 0.61 0.58 0.68 0.72
dB SPL 13 kHz 56.87 55.97 57.15 56.29 (100)
Pitch strength 32.27 29.25 34.29 37.95 Alpha ratio 23.76 23.18 24.73 24.62
mean dB SPL 13 kHz 58.17 58.07 58.65 56.99
Pitch strength 10.81 10.62 10.36 12.09 Pitch 33.64 30.03 37.75 40.60
SD strength M
Jitter 0.032 0.037 0.026 0.025 Pitch 11.05 10.70 10.70 12.47
Shimmer 0.128 0.140 0.127 0.107 strength SD
HNR 11.86 9.79 11.81 13.92 Jitter 0.032 0.038 0.023 0.023
Note: Only 15 of the 19 participants participated in a choral rehearsal during Shimmer 0.123 0.137 0.112 0.099
the study period. The choral singing results represent the averages for HNR 12.11 9.83 13.73 15.08
only these 15 participants.
Note: Only 9 of the 11 male participants participated in a choral rehears-
Abbreviation: SD, standard deviation.
al during the study period. The choral singing results represent the averages
for only these 9 participants.
Abbreviation: SD, standard deviation.

Changes between activities


Accelerometer data were disaggregated for the three full am-
bulatory monitoring days by three different types of activities Post hoc pairwise comparisons of activities with least signif-
(nonsinging, choral singing, and solo singing). The analysis in- icant difference t tests revealed that the majority of the significant
cluded one-way repeated measures ANOVAs with independent differences between these measures occurred between the speak-
variables being the three different types of activities and depen- ing voice and the singing activities (Table 8). Dose measures
dent variables being each of the 4 vocal dose measures and 10 and F0/P0, dB SPL, jitter, and HNR all had significant differ-
voice quality measures discussed above (Table 7). Because 4 of ences between nonsinging and singing activities, but no significant
the 19 participants had no choral rehearsal during the 3 days of differences between the two types of singing. Alpha ratio and
monitoring (although enrolled in choir), the repeated measures pitch strength were significantly higher in nonsinging periods
ANOVAS were completed with N = 15 participants. Mauchlys than in solo singing, but not significantly different between choral
sphericity test was completed on each measure with no signif- singing and nonsinging. There were four significant differ-
icant results, indicating that sphericity could be assumed for each ences between choral singing and solo singing, including LTAS
measure. The ANOVA revealed that there were significant dif- slope (significantly steeper in solo singing than in choral singing),
ferences between activities for all measures, except the spectral pitch strength (significantly stronger in solo singing), and shimmer
measures of dB SPL 13 kHz and alpha ratio. and jitter (significantly less perturbation in solo singing).
ARTICLE IN PRESS
6 Journal of Voice, Vol. , No. , 2016

TABLE 4. TABLE 5.
Measures of Central Tendency, Full Monitoring Day: Pearson Correlation Coefficient Tests Between 3-Day Vocal
Women (N = 8) Dose Totals and Voice Quality Means
Measure Total Nonsing Choral Solo Measure Voicing % Dt Dc Dd
Recording 42.78 38.42 1.85 2.41 F0 r 0.373 0.344
htotal P 0.116 0.149
Voicing % 10.33 7.75 37.50 31.80 P0 r 0.341 0.302
Dttotal (min) 263.55 178.60 41.57 46.07 P 0.153 0.209
Dtper h (min) 6.20 4.65 22.50 19.08 dB SPL r 0.493 0.512 0.250
Dctotal (kc) 5085.72 3142.67 941.57 1093.72 P 0.032 0.025 0.302
Dcper h (kc) 119.55 81.80 509.72 452.96 LTAS slope r 0.068 0.041 0.393 0.020
Ddtotal (m) 12928.60 8146.57 2486.39 2583.98 P 0.783 0.868 0.096 0.934
Ddper h (m) 303.92 212.04 1346.02 1070.16 Alpha ratio r 0.148 0.237 0.112 0.359
F0 mean (Hz) 321.30 301.73 357.78 393.68 P 0.546 0.328 0.647 0.131
F0 median (Hz) 301.61 278.83 342.25 383.37 dB 13kHz r 0.233 0.262 0.177 0.224
F0 mode (Hz) 267.18 247.45 296.85 319.41 P 0.337 0.279 0.468 0.357
F0 SD (Hz) 105.09 101.67 123.44 119.81 Pitch r 0.665 0.636 0.296 0.675
P0 mean (Hz) 319.82 282.92 403.16 403.01 strength P 0.002 0.003 0.219 0.002
P0 median (Hz) 307.79 267.71 400.76 394.33 Jitter r 0.501 0.484 0.435 0.432
P0 mode (Hz) 295.67 255.15 359.31 390.25 P 0.029 0.036 0.063 0.065
P0 SD (Hz) 90.00 77.08 131.36 118.49 Shimmer r 0.689 0.672 0.402 0.656
dB SPL mean 74.73 74.45 77.96 75.88 P 0.001 0.002 0.088 0.002
dB SPL median 74.81 74.31 78.44 76.34 HNR r 0.558 0.543 0.463 0.577
dB SPL mode 74.91 74.06 79.23 76.98 P 0.013 0.016 0.046 0.010
dB SD 5.41 5.22 5.77 5.55 Note: P < .01, two tailed, is indicated in boldface.
LTAS slope 0.54 0.51 0.59 0.63
(100)
Alpha ratio 22.09 21.61 21.97 24.19
cant correlations at the 0.01 level for dB SPL, pitch strength,
dB SPL 54.54 54.15 55.39 55.78
13 kHz
shimmer, and jitter. Although nonsinging time had weak to mod-
Pitch 30.20 27.94 27.79 33.83 erate correlations that moved in the same direction, there were
strength M no significant correlations between nonsinging Dt or recording
Pitch 10.58 10.33 9.35 11.04
strength SD
Jitter 0.032 0.036 0.033 0.029 TABLE 6.
Shimmer 0.135 0.145 0.160 0.116 Pearson Correlation Coefficient Tests Between Daily Vocal
HNR 11.52 10.01 10.78 13.07 Dose Totals and Daily Voice Quality Means
Note: Only six of the eight female participants participated in a choral re- Measure Voicing % Dt Dc Dd
hearsal during the study period. The choral singing results represent the
averages for only these six participants. F0 r 0.251 0.237
Abbreviation: SD, standard deviation. P 0.060 0.076
P0 r 0.205 0.201
P 0.127 0.134
dB SPL r 0.401 0.406 0.213
Correlations between singing time and overall voice
P 0.002 0.002 0.112
quality readings
LTAS slope r 0.157 0.171 0.163 0.225
Because the overall 3-day measurements had significant corre- P 0.242 0.203 0.226 0.093
lations in terms of dB SPL, pitch strength, jitter, shimmer, and Alpha ratio r 0.124 0.138 0.065 0.249
HNR that moved in the same direction as the significant corre- P 0.359 0.306 0.629 0.062
lations between activities, significant full-day correlations could dB 13 kHz r 0.166 0.229 0.115 0.211
have been influenced by the total amount of singing time and P 0.217 0.087 0.393 0.116
singing dose. To see if this was the case, singing and nonsinging Pitch r 0.474 0.477 0.237 0.603
times were disaggregated, and Pearson Correlation tests strength P <0.001 <0.001 0.076 <0.001
were run among (1) the amount of total singing time recorded, Jitter r 0.508 0.447 0.414 0.498
(2) 3-day total singing doses (Dt, Dc, and Dd), (3) 3-day total P <0.001 <0.001 0.001 <0.001
Shimmer r 0.457 0.423 0.220 0.504
nonsinging doses (Dt, Dc, and Dd), and (4) the overall 3-day
P <0.001 0.001 0.101 <0.001
totals (singing and nonsinging together) for dB SPL, pitch
HNR r 0.422 0.383 0.382 0.502
strength, jitter, shimmer, and HNR (Table 9). Results revealed P 0.001 0.003 0.003 <0.001
moderate to strong correlations between the amount of singing
Note: P < .01, two tailed, is indicated in boldface.
time and each of the five voice quality measures, with signifi-
ARTICLE IN PRESS
Matthew J. Schloneger and Eric Hunter Voice Use and Quality of College/University Singing Students 7

TABLE 7.
Activity Repeated Measures ANOVA Results
Measure Nonsing M (SD) Choral M (SD) Solo M (SD) Df1 Df2 F P
Voicing % 9.35 (2.82) 39.37 (6.76) 44.34 (7.15) 2 28 156.83 <0.001
Dt (min/h) 5.28 (1.69) 23.18 (4.00) 25.63 (3.29) 2 28 213.18 <0.001
Dc (kc/h) 64.84 (24.21) 364.94 (132.89) 416.62 (166.86) 2 28 61.026 <0.001
Dd (m/h) 255.88 (131.21) 1320.38 (390.82) 1565.60 (494.77) 2 28 109.02 <0.001
F0 213.54(74.07) 257.82 (91.89) 272.02 (112.72) 2 28 15.04 <0.001
P0 206.16(63.92) 278.89 (111.47) 279.18 (114.44) 2 28 21.91 <0.001
dB SPL 75.09 (5.31) 77.48 (5.37) 78.49 (5.09) 2 28 12.24 <0.001
LTAS SLP (100)* 0.55 (0.86) 0.64 (0.14) 0.70 (0.14) 1.4 20.2 3.50 <0.001
Alpha ratio 22.15 (3.51) 23.63 (2.91) 24.42 (4.13) 2 28 4.60 0.019
dB SPL 13 kHz 56.07 (3.67) 57.34 (3.76) 56.48 (2.67) 2 28 1.69 0.202
Pitch strength 29.35 (4.36) 33.76 (8.45) 38.26 (7.03) 2 28 16.68 <0.001
Jitter % 3.87 (0.64) 2.67 (0.90) 2.47 (0.74) 2 28 17.03 <0.001
Shimmer % 13.99 (1.97) 13.10 (3.67) 10.51 (2.39) 2 28 11.57 <0.001
HNR 10.52 (0.47) 13.01 (0.98) 14.99 (0.74) 2 28 13.05 <0.001
Note: P < .01 is indicated in boldface. *For those variables that violated the assumption of sphericity based on Mauchlys test for sphericity (P < .05), a Greenhouse-
Geisser correction was applied.

duration and these four voice quality measures. It should be noted pitch strength and both nonsinging Dd and nonsinging dB SPL.
that because Dd is derived in part from dB SPL, the strength Once again, it should be noted that Dd was derived in part from
of these correlations was expected. dB SPL so a significant correlation was expected.

Correlations between nonsinging doses and DISCUSSION


nonsinging quality This investigation documented and explored relationships among
To examine the relationship between the nonsinging doses and voice use, voice quality, and perceived voice function of 19
voice quality measures during nonsinging periods more closely, college/university singing students over the course of three active
Pearson correlation tests were completed between 3-day days. The instrumentation and procedures of this investigation
nonsinging vocal doses and five voice quality measures ac- yielded numerous statistically significant relationships (P < 0.01).
quired during only nonsinging periods (Table 10). The correlations These relationships can be summarized according to two major,
were not as strong as the 3-day totals, but there were mostly mod- overarching findings. First, higher vocal doses, as a whole, appear
erate correlations that moved in the same direction as the full- to correspond with greater voice amplitude, more vocal clarity
day totals. There were significant correlations between nonsinging and less perturbation. Second, significant differences in voice
quality (F0, P0, dB SPL, and voice clarity/perturbation) also appear
to correlate with various vocal activities, with solo singing having
TABLE 8. the highest pitch, dB SPL and clarity and least perturbation.
Significant Differences Between Activity Means Further, vocal dose readings for these young singers (11.92%
P voicing, 7.15 minutes Dt per hour, 97,320 Dc per hour, and
Measure Nonsing/Choral Nonsing/Solo Choral /Solo
370.57 m Dd per hour) were similar to the dose readings ob-
tained in several earlier studies of student singers.1,33,34
Voicing % <0.001 <0.001 0.070
Dt (min/h) <0.001 <0.001 0.056
Dc (kc/h) <0.001 <0.001 0.101 Correlations between accelerometer-acquired vocal
Dd (m/h) <0.001 <0.001 0.022 dose and voice quality data
F0 <0.001 <0.001 0.286 There appears to be a significant correlation between vocal dose
P0 <0.001 <0.001 0.976 and voice quality measures acquired during ambulatory moni-
dB SPL 0.007 <0.001 0.144 toring (voice amplitude level or voice perturbation), both in terms
LTAS slope 0.002 <0.001 <0.001 of total 3-day monitoring means (N = 19) and individual moni-
Alpha ratio 0.071 0.002 0.383 toring day means (N = 57). There were also moderate significant
dB 13 kHz 0.109 <0.001 0.152 positive correlations between dB SPL and both voicing percent-
Pitch 0.019 <0.001 0.008 age (r = 0.401) and dose time (r = 0.406) during individual
strength
monitoring days.
Jitter 0.002 0.001 <0.001
Correlation tests between dose measures and mean dB SPL
Shimmer 0.299 <0.001 0.010
HNR 0.004 <0.001 0.093 were completed separately for singing and nonsinging periods.
Although singing is typically louder than conversational speech,
Note: P < .01 is indicated in boldface.
the strength of the correlations was similar between singing and
ARTICLE IN PRESS
8 Journal of Voice, Vol. , No. , 2016

TABLE 9.
Pearson Correlations Between Three Day Voice Quality Means (All Recorded Hours) and Total Singing / Non-singing Doses
Total Singing Total Total Total Total Total Total
Recording Singing Singing Singing Nonsinging Nonsinging Nonsinging
Measure Duration Dt Dc Dd Dt Dc Dd
dB SPL r 0.395 0.496 0.401 0.744 0.395 0.126 0.736
P 0.094 0.031 0.089 <0.001 0.094 0.607 <0.001
Pitch r 0.564 0.648 0.498 0.743 0.441 0.123 0.508
strength P 0.012 0.003 0.030 <0.001 0.059 0.615 0.026
HNR r 0.470 0.548 0.590 0.633 0.403 0.311 0.440
P 0.042 0.015 0.008 0.004 0.087 0.195 0.059
Jitter r 0.570 0.667 0.637 0.628 0.443 0.378 0.481
P 0.011 0.002 0.003 0.004 0.057 0.111 0.037
Shimmer r 0.618 0.683 0.509 0.701 0.477 0.174 0.456
P 0.005 0.001 0.026 0.001 0.039 0.477 0.050
Note: P < .01 is indicated in boldface.

nonsinging. This similarity may indicate that the percentage of strength of the correlations may in part be the result of the im-
singing time was insufficient to have much of an influence on proved voicing that one would expect with singing.
dB SPL readings. Rather, the overall vocal load correlated pos- A second explanation could be that pitch clarity and perturba-
itively with amplitude, a finding that was in agreement with tion measures improve after periods of vocal loading. Although
previous studies.13,4850 The rise in amplitude may suggest that the amount of singing likely contributed to the strength of the cor-
individuals engage in more effortful phonation after periods of relations among dose and pitch strength, HNR, shimmer, and jitter,
higher vocal loading. there were also moderate correlations between these measures and
There were also moderately significant positive correlations the vocal doses acquired during nonsinging periods alone. This
between vocal dose measures and voice clarity (pitch strength factor was especially true of Dd, with a significant positive cor-
and HNR) and strong, significant negative correlations between relation between Dd during nonsinging time and pitch strength
vocal dose and perturbation (shimmer and jitter). Although this during nonsinging time. These results accord with the findings of
result may seem counterintuitive (ie, high vocal doses should several previous studies13,14,51 in which the authors suggested that
result in less vocal clarity and more perturbation), there are at vocal loading resulted in vocal hyper-function, and thereby a more
least two plausible explanations for these significant correla- firm closing of the vocal folds. This circumstance resulted in a
tions. First, some individuals likely sang more than others. The higher amplitude and increased clarity in voicing.
3-day means of these four measures correlated significantly with Given these positive correlating changes in voice quality and
the amount of singing time and levels of vocal dose from singing. dose, it is unclear to what extent self-reported healthy singers
As compared with speaking, singing involves longer periods of vocal efficiency improves after a certain level of voice use (a
time singing vowels (voicing) and greater attention to breath warming up effect) and at what point vocal efficiency begins to
support and resonance. It follows that more singing time over decline. According to Boucher and Ayad,5 there may be a dis-
3 days would lead to greater mean readings of voice clarity and cernible point after which vocal stability temporarily decreases,
less perturbation. There were also significant differences in these followed by a compensatory increase in vocal tension that masks
measures between different activities (see below). Thus, the the problem. The analysis methods in this study do not confirm

TABLE 10.
Pearson Correlations Between Three Day Nonsinging Doses and Nonsinging Voice Quality
Measure: Nonsinging Periods Total Nonsinging Dt Total Nonsinging Dc Total Nonsinging Dd
dB SPL r 0.381 0.154 0.735
P 0.107 0.528 <0.001
Pitch strength r 0.505 0.263 0.594
P 0.027 0.277 0.007
HNR r 0.484 0.431 0.519
P 0.036 0.065 0.023
Jitter r 0.439 0.492 0.458
P 0.060 0.032 0.049
Shimmer r 0.513 0.272 0.481
P 0.025 0.260 0.037
Note: P < .01 is indicated in boldface.
ARTICLE IN PRESS
Matthew J. Schloneger and Eric Hunter Voice Use and Quality of College/University Singing Students 9

the existence of this point of instability or make clear at what recording, including the state where this study was conducted,
point it might have happened, nor do the results make clear what privacy, HIPPA, and recruitment can be issues that complicate
might constitute a warming-up effect on the voice and what might full measurement of all phonation. Behavioral studies have long
constitute increased compensatory tension due to fatigue. De- allowed the recording of study participants with consent, but if
tailed minute-to-minute analyses of acquired ambulatory full recordings of the VoxLogs microphone are used in a health-
monitoring data with this temporary change in mind would be care environment, patient privacy becomes a much more
a logical possibility for future research. significant concern and limitation.

Significant differences between activities


Ambulatory monitoring data indicated significant differences CONCLUSIONS
between the various periods of participant activity assessed in Students studying voice performance frequently experience heavy
this study. All four vocal dose measures and most voice quality vocal demands. Additionally, their lifestyles may not be con-
means, including vocal pitch and dB SPL, showed significant ducive to healthy vocal habits. Because they may be unaware
increases between nonsinging and both types of singing activi- of the negative, cumulative effects of heavy vocal loads on ef-
ties. An increase in pitch strength and HNR with a corresponding ficient phonation, concern for these students vocal well-being
decrease in shimmer and jitter accompanied this higher dB SPL, is warranted. Despite a considerable body of literature report-
likely indicating the increased vocal fold closure that typically ing on the vibratory, acoustic, and perceived effects of vocal
occurs with singing. loading among various populations, there remains an absence
Choral singing and solo singing require different vocal tech- of data aimed at understanding possible relationships between
niques, and the results of this study indicated differences between vocal dose and voice quality (observed and perceived) among
the two styles of singing among this group of singers. Pitch young, developing singers using data acquired in natural set-
strength was significantly higher in solo singing than in choral tings. This study addresses this lack of knowledge by presenting
singing, whereas jitter and shimmer were significantly lower. The a relatively low cost protocol for measuring both multiple voice
measurements of vocal pitch, dB SPL, and HNR were all higher and long-term samples.
in solo singing than in choral singing, although not to a signif- The current study identifies among its dependent variables a
icant degree. One would expect these results given the higher number of significant relationships that appear to agree with pre-
demands for projection in solo singing. The greater vocal fold vious studies conducted in laboratories. Major findings suggest
closure needed for projection would logically correspond with that higher vocal doses correlate significantly with greater voice
a clearer voice and less perturbation. intensity, more vocal clarity, and less perturbation. Further, there
However, the results of the three measures of vocal reso- were significant differences in some acoustic voice quality metrics
nance (LTAS slope, alpha ratio, and dB SPL 13 kHz) used to between nonsinging, solo singing, and choral singing. The fact
compare participants nonsinging, choral singing, and solo singing that these field-based results agree with the literature could in-
time periods in this study appear to contradict some previous dicate that the protocols and procedures of this study, although
findings.52 On the basis of previous investigations, one would imperfect, are sufficiently robust to accurately identify trends in
expect to observe increased resonance in singing as opposed to vocal dose and in changes in voice quality.
speaking, as well as increased resonance in the upper partials in
solo singing when compared with choral singing. However, LTAS Acknowledgements
slope, alpha ratio, and dB SPL 13 kHz each decreased from We acknowledge the efforts and contributions of James F. Daugh-
nonsinging to choral singing to solo singing in the current study. erty. This research was partially supported by the National Institute
LTAS slope also decreased from choral singing to solo singing. On Deafness and Other Communication Disorders of the Na-
It is possible that these results could have occurred in part as a tional Institutes of Health under Award Number R01DC012315.
result of the technical limitations of this study, including the fact The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does
that the accelerometer transducer largely measured vocal source not necessarily represent the official views of the National In-
vibrations rather than source/filter vibrations and was less sen- stitutes of Health.
sitive to higher frequencies. Subsequent studies should explore
this apparent discrepancy to see if it is unique to the participants
and to the procedures in this study, or if it occurs more widely. REFERENCES
The limitations for this study include the need to establish 1. Gaskill CS, Cowgill JG, OBrien SG. Comparing the vocal dose of university
students from vocal performance, music education and musical theater.
thresholds for the inclusion and exclusion of data, meaning that
J Sing. 2013;70:10.
some nonvoice data may have been included (false-positives), 2. Timmermans B, De Bodt MS, Wuyts FL, et al. Poor voice quality in future
whereas some voicing data might have been excluded. Further, elite vocal performers and professional voice users. J Voice. 2002;16:372
because the 19 participants did not record for a reported average 382.
of 31 waking minutes during each monitoring day, daily changes 3. Timmermans B, De Bodt MS, Wuyts FL, et al. Analysis and evaluation of
a voice-training program in future professional voice users. J Voice.
in voice quality were not compared with a full 100% of each
2005;19:202210.
participants daily vocal dose. Privacy is a potential challenge 4. Hunter EJ, Titze IR. Quantifying vocal fatigue recovery: dynamic vocal
for future studies that employ collars with an audio micro- recovery trajectories after a vocal loading exercise. Ann Otol Rhinol Laryngol.
phone. While most states allow for single consent (nontargeted) 2009;118:449460.
ARTICLE IN PRESS
10 Journal of Voice, Vol. , No. , 2016

5. Boucher VJ, Ayad T. Physiological attributes of vocal fatigue and their 29. Gaskill CS, OBrien SG, Tinter SR. The effect of voice amplification on
acoustic effects: a synthesis of findings for a criterion-based prevention of occupational vocal dose in elementary school teachers. J Voice. 2012;26:667,
acquired voice disorders. J Voice. 2010;24:324336. e19-e27.
6. Eustace C, Stemple J, Lee L. Objective measures of voice production in 30. Hunter EJ, Halpern AE, Spielman JL. Impact of four nonclinical speaking
patients complaining of laryngeal fatigue. J Voice. 1996;10:146154. environments on a childs fundamental frequency and voice level:
7. Lehto L, Laaksonen L, Vilkman E, et al. Occupational voice complaints a preliminary case study. Lang Speech Hear Serv Sch. 2012;43:25363A.
and objective acoustic measurementsdo they correlate? Logoped Phoniatr 31. Hunter E. A comparison of a childs fundamental frequencies in structured
Vocol. 2006;31:147152. elicited vocalizations versus unstructured natural vocalizations: a case study.
8. Bottalico P, Graetzer S, Hunter EJ. Effects of voice style, noise level, and Int J Pediatr Otorhinolaryngol. 2009;73:561571.
acoustic feedback on objective and subjective voice evaluations. J Acoust 32. Daugherty JF, Manternach JN, Price KK. Student voice use and vocal health
Soc Am. 2015;138:EL498EL503. during an all-state chorus event. J Res Music Educ. 2011;58:346367.
9. Artkoski M, Tommila J, Laukkanen A-M. Changes in voice during a day 33. Schloneger MJ. Graduate student voice use and vocal efficiency in an opera
in normal voices without vocal loading. Logoped Phoniatr Vocol. rehearsal week: a case study. J Voice. 2011;25:e265e273.
2002;27:118123. 34. Gaskill CS, Cowgill JG, Tinter SR. Vocal dosimetry: a graduate level voice
10. Remacle A, Finck C, Roche A, et al. Vocal impact of a prolonged reading pedagogy course experience. J Sing. 2013;69:543555.
task at two intensity levels: objective measurements and subjective self- 35. Carroll T, Nix J, Hunter E, et al. Objective measurement of vocal fatigue
ratings. J Voice. 2012;26:e177e186. in classical singers: a vocal dosimetry pilot study. Otolaryngol Head Neck
11. Sisakun S. Detecting vocal fatigue in student singers using acoustic measures Surg. 2006;135:595602.
of mean fundamental frequency, jitter, shimmer and harmonics-to-noise ratio 36. Hunter EJ. A budget voice dosimetry system for research: Benefits and
[doctoral dissertation] 2000. limitations. In: Deliyski DD, The 10th International Conference on Advances
12. Lfqvist A, Mandersson B. Long-time average spectrum of speech and voice in Quantitative Laryngology, Voice and Speech Research. Cincinnati, OH:
analysis. Folia Phoniatr Logop. 1987;39:8. AQL Press; 2013:2122.
13. Laukkanen A-M, Ilomki I, Leppnen K, et al. Acoustic measures and 37. Schloneger M. Assessments of voice use, voice quality, and perceived singing
self-reports of vocal fatigue by female teachers. J Voice. 2008;22:283 voice function among college/university singing students, ages 1824 through
289. simultaneous ambulatory monitoring with accelerometer and acoustic
14. Rantala L, Vilkman E. Relationship between subjective voice complaints transducers [doctoral dissertation]. Lawrence, KS: University of Kansas;
and acoustic parameters in female teachers voices. J Voice. 1999;13:484 2014.
495. 38. Bckstrm T, Lehto L, Alku P, et al. Automatic pre-segmentation of running
15. Gramming PSJ, Ternstrm S, Leanderson R, et al. Relationship between speech improves the robustness of several acoustic voice measures. Logoped
changes in voice pitch and loudness. J Voice. 1988;2:9. Phoniatr Vocol. 2003;28:101.
16. Sundberg J, Nordenberg M. Effects of vocal loudness variation on spectrum 39. Camacho A, Harris JG. A sawtooth waveform inspired pitch estimator for
balance as reflected by the alpha measure of long-term-average spectra of speech and music. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.
speech. J Acoust Soc Am. 2006;120:453457. 2008;124:16381652.
17. Cho S-W, Yin CS, Park Y-B, et al. Differences in self-rated, perceived, and 40. Boersma P, Weenink D. Praat: doing phonetics by computer [Computer
acoustic voice qualities between high- and low-fatigue groups. J Voice. software] (Version 5.3.53). Amsterdam, The Netherlands: Phonetic Sciences,
2011;25:544552. University of Amsterdam; 2014 Retrieved from: http://www.fon
18. Scherer R, Titze I, Rahpael B, et al. Vocal fatigue in a trained and an .hum.uva.nl/praat/.
untrained voice user. In: Baer T, Sasaki C, Harris K, eds. Laryngeal Function 41. Popolo PS, Svec JG, Titze IR. Adaptation of a pocket PC for use as a
in Phonation and Respiration. San Diego, CA: Singular; 1987. wearable voice dosimeter. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2005;48:780791.
19. vec JG, Popolo PS, Titze IR. Measurement of vocal doses in speech: 42. Shrivastav R, Eddins DA, Anand S. Pitch strength of normal and dysphonic
experimental procedure and signal processing. Logoped Phoniatr Vocol. voices. J Acoust Soc Am. 2012;131:22612269.
2003;28:181. 43. Monson BB, Hunter EJ, Story BH. Horizontal directivity of low- and
20. Titze I, Svec JG, Popolo PS. Vocal dose measures: quantifying accumulated high-frequency energy in speech and singing. Journal of the Acoustical
vibration exposure in vocal fold tissues. J Speech Lang Hear Res. Society of America. 2012;132:433441.
2003;46:919932. 44. Krause JC, Braida LD. Acoustic properties of naturally produced clear speech
21. Svec JG, Titze IR, Popolo PS. Estimation of sound pressure levels of voiced at normal speaking rates. J Acoust Soc Am. 2004;115:16.
speech from skin vibration of the neck. J Acoust Soc Am. 2005;117:1386 45. Nix J, vec JG, Laukkanen A-M, et al. Protocol challenges for on-the-job
1394. voice dosimetry of teachers in the United States and Finland. J Voice.
22. Szabo A, Hammarberg B, Granqvist S, et al. Methods to study pre-school 2007;21:385396.
teachers voice at work: simultaneous recordings with a voice accumulator 46. Hunter EJ. Teacher response to ambulatory monitoring of voice. Logoped
and a DAT recorder. Logoped Phoniatr Vocol. 2003;28:29. Phoniatr Vocol. 2012;37:3.
23. Hillman RE, Heaton JT, Masaki A, et al. Ambulatory monitoring of 47. Ghassemi M, Van Stan JH, Mehta DD, et al. Learning to detect vocal
disordered voices. Ann Otol Rhinol Laryngol. 2006;115:795801. hyperfunction from ambulatory neck-surface acceleration features: initial
24. Mehta DD, Woodbury RL, Cheyne HA, et al. Duration of ambulatory results for vocal fold nodules. Biomed Eng IEEE Trans. 2014;61:16681675.
monitoring needed to accurately estimate voice use. InterSpeech: Annual 48. Vilkman E, Lauri E-R, Alku P, et al. Effects of prolonged oral reading on
Conference of the International Speech Communication Association; F0, SPL, subglottal pressure and amplitude characteristics of glottal flow
Portland, OR, 2012. waveforms. J Voice. 1999;13:303312.
25. Hunter EJ, Titze IR. Variations in intensity, fundamental frequency, and 49. Laukkanen A-M, Jaervinen K, Artkoski M, et al. Changes in voice and
voicing for teachers in occupational versus nonoccupational settings. J Speech subjective sensations during a 45-min vocal loading test in female subjects
Lang Hear Res. 2010;53:862875. with vocal training. Folia Phoniatr Logop. 2004;56:335346.
26. Morrow SL, Connor NP. Comparison of voice-use profiles between 50. Jonsdottir V, Laukkanen A-M, Siikki I. Changes in teachers voice quality
elementary classroom and music teachers. J Voice. 2011;25:367 during a working day with and without electric sound amplification. Folia
372. Phoniatr Logop. 2003;55:267280.
27. Bottalico P, Astolfi A. Investigations into vocal doses and parameters 51. Boucher VJ. Acoustic correlates of fatigue in laryngeal muscles: findings
pertaining to primary school teachers in classrooms. J Acoust Soc Am. for a criterion-based prevention of acquired voice pathologies. J Speech Lang
2012;131:28172827. Hear Res. 2008;51:11611170.
28. Franca MC. A comparison of vocal demands with vocal performance among 52. Miller DG. Resonance in Singing: Voice Building Through Acoustic
classroom student teachers. J Commun Disord. 2013;46:111123. Feedback. Delaware, OH: Inside View Press; 2008.

Você também pode gostar