Você está na página 1de 9

504303

research-article2013
PSSXXX10.1177/0956797613504303Neuberg et al.Religion and Intergroup Conflict

Research Article

Psychological Science

Religion and Intergroup Conflict: Findings 2014, Vol. 25(1) 198206


The Author(s) 2013
Reprints and permissions:
From the Global Group Relations Project sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav
DOI: 10.1177/0956797613504303
pss.sagepub.com

Steven L. Neuberg1,2,3, Carolyn M. Warner2,4, Stephen A. Mistler1,


Anna Berlin1,5, Eric D. Hill6, Jordan D. Johnson2,7,
Gabrielle Filip-Crawford1, Roger E. Millsap1, George Thomas2,4,
Michael Winkelman8, Benjamin J. Broome9, Thomas J. Taylor10,
and Juliane Schober7
1
Department of Psychology, Arizona State University; 2Center for the Study of Religion and Conflict, Arizona State
University; 3Center for Social Dynamics and Complexity, Arizona State University; 4School of Politics and Global
Studies, Arizona State University; 5Global Institute of Sustainability, Arizona State University; 6Department of
Psychological Sciences, Albion College; 7School of Historical, Philosophical, and Religious Studies, Arizona State
University; 8School of Human Evolution and Social Change, Arizona State University; 9Hugh Downs School of Human
Communication, Arizona State University; and 10School of Mathematical and Statistical Sciences, Arizona State University

Abstract
How might religion shape intergroup conflict? We tested whether religious infusionthe extent to which religious
rituals and discourse permeate the everyday activities of groups and their membersmoderated the effects of two
factors known to increase intergroup conflict: competition for limited resources and incompatibility of values held
by potentially conflicting groups. We used data from the Global Group Relations Project to investigate 194 groups
(e.g., ethnic, religious, national) at 97 sites around the world. When religion was infused in group life, groups were
especially prejudiced against those groups that held incompatible values, and they were likely to discriminate against
such groups. Moreover, whereas disadvantaged groups with low levels of religious infusion typically avoided directing
aggression against their resource-rich and powerful counterparts, disadvantaged groups with high levels of religious
infusion directed significant aggression against themdespite the significant tangible costs to the disadvantaged
groups potentially posed by enacting such aggression. This research suggests mechanisms through which religion may
increase intergroup conflict and introduces an innovative method for performing nuanced, cross-societal research.

Keywords
religion, religious beliefs, intergroup conflict, intergroup dynamics, prejudice, values, resources, competition, cross-
cultural, global, methods, cross-cultural differences, violence

Received 7/3/13; Revision accepted 8/9/13

Consider Catholics and Protestants in Northern Ireland, instance, are the tensions between Israelis and Palestinians
Jews and Muslims in the Middle East, Muslims and Hindus due to clashing religious values or to competition for
in South Asia, the religious right and secularists in the limited resources between two groups that are also dis-
United States: Religion certainly seems to contribute sub- tinguishable by religion? Further complicating the issue,
stantially to intergroup conflict. whereas research focusing on individual differences and
Yet empirical knowledge about how religion might prejudice shows that religious people tend to be more
shape such conflict, and even whether it does, is elusive. prejudiced than nonreligious people against out-group
Although some scholars view religion as a core cause of
conflict (e.g., Huntington, 1993; Kaplan, 2007), other
Corresponding Author:
scholars view it as a mask for (or a post hoc justification Steven L. Neuberg, Department of Psychology, Arizona State
of) conflict that would otherwise occur for pragmatic rea- University, Tempe, AZ 85287-1104
sons (e.g., Berman, 2009; Fearon & Laitin, 2003). For E-mail: steven.neuberg@asu.edu
Religion and Intergroup Conflict 199

members (Hall, Matz, & Wood, 2010), research exploring tightly woven into everyday activityas is the case for
violent conflict between large groups suggests little evi- groups with high levels of religious infusionare thus
dence of direct, main effects of religion (e.g., Fox, 2004; likely to feel particularly threatened by groups holding
Russett, Oneal, & Cox, 2000). incompatible values, given the relatively substantial
In the present research, we conducted a global study changes to group life that could result if those contradic-
that begins to clarify the relationships between religion tory values were to supersede their own. This sense of
and intergroup conflict. We started with the knowledge threat will be especially strong if the targeted values are
that religion is more than just a set of beliefs; it also seen as sacred and thus not readily compromised (Atran
encompasses community practices, socialization func- & Ginges, 2012)and groups with higher levels of reli-
tions, organizational structures, and a range of other fea- gious infusion may be more likely to view their values as
tures (e.g., Lincoln, 2003). The multifaceted nature of sacred. Thus, the extent to which incompatible values
religion means that there is little reason to assume that all lead to intergroup prejudices and hostility is likely to be
of its features relate to conflict in similar ways or even greater for groups with high levels of religious infusion
relate to conflict at all. than for groups with low levels of religious infusion.
We focused on religious infusionthe extent to which Second, intergroup prejudices and behavioral conflict
religion permeates a groups private and public life. can result from competition over resources and power
Religious infusion is not tied to specific religions or sets (e.g., Campbell, 1965; Collier, Hoeffler, & Rohner, 2009;
of beliefs; any religion can be highly infused throughout Fearon & Laitin, 2003). This can occur when two poten-
a society, but infusion is not a necessary feature of any tially competing groups possess relatively equal power
particular religion. For example, our data reveal that and resources, as Sherif, Harvey, White, Hood, and Sherif
although religion is a highly dominant feature of life for (1961/1988) demonstrated in their classic Robbers Cave
Catholics in Nicaragua, it is not so for Catholics in Austria. experiment. However, power and resources are often dis-
Religious infusion is of special interest because of its tributed unequally between potentially competing groups,
potential to engage powerful group processes. For and inclinations toward intergroup conflict may manifest
instance, frequent public practice of religion and engage- differently for high- and low-power groups. Specifically,
ment in religion-tinged events and discourse may make even when weak and powerful groups are similarly preju-
group norms (both descriptive and injunctive) especially diced against one another, powerful groups may more
salient, elicit strong commitments to the group, and readily engage less powerful groups in behavioral conflict
enable especially effective intragroup communication simply because they canbecause the costs of doing so
and coordination. Such group processes may increase the are low relative to the potential gain of additional
likelihood that people will adopt and internalize predom- resources (e.g., Hegre, 2008). Although low-power groups
inant group prejudices (e.g., toward other groups). Such may also be highly motivated to engage in conflict to
processes may also enhance collective motivation and a improve their lotbecause of perceptions of unjust rela-
groups capacity for collective action. Thus, groups with tive deprivation or actual need (e.g., Crosby, 1976; Walker
high levels of religious infusion may be particularly & Smith, 2002)the calculus of deterrence suggests that
poised for and capable of effective intergroup conflict if they will hesitate to do so because the benefits of poten-
circumstances seem to call for it. tially gaining those resources are likely to be outweighed
What might such circumstances be? Two pathways to by the costs imposed by the high-power group in response
conflict have received much attention in psychology and to the low-power groups acts of aggression.
the social sciencesincompatibility of values and com- However, this calculus can be altered (Allen &
petition over tangible resources. We propose that reli- Fordham, 2011; Arreguin-Toft, 2001). For example,
gious infusion modulates each pathway in ways that groups with especially strong organizational structures
increase the likelihood of conflict. may be capable of overcoming significant resource and
First, groups tend to be more prejudiced against and power disadvantages (e.g., Iannaccone & Berman, 2006).
hostile toward groups that have incompatible values One source of organizational strength may be religious
(e.g., Barker, Hurwitz, & Nelson, 2008; Biernat, Vescio, infusion, which might strengthen intragroup trust and
Theno, & Crandall, 1996; Garcia-Retamero, Mller, & solidarity, enhance group coordination toward common
Rousseau, 2012; Schwartz, Struch, & Bilsky, 1990). Values goals, increase willingness to sacrifice for the group, and
have tangible consequences; they shape norms, policies, reduce member defection rates (e.g., Atran & Henrich,
laws, and all manner of individual and group decisions in 2010; Ginges, Atran, Sachdeva, & Medin, 2011; Ginges,
ways that disadvantage people whose values are not in Hansen, & Norenzayan, 2009). Thus, disadvantaged
accord with those of the group, as when antiabortion groups that also have high levels of religious infusion
values lead to restricted access to abortion even for those may be less deterred than disadvantaged groups with
who value prochoice positions. Groups whose values are low levels of religious infusion by the costs of conflict
200 Neuberg et al.

with high-power groups (Ginges & Atran, 2011; Toft, forms of intergroup relationsethnic-ethnic, religious-
2007) and hence more likely to engage in conflict. religious, secular-religious, state-state, state-ethnic minor-
We present findings from the Global Group Relations ity, or state-religious minority. Relevant groups were
Project, a data-gathering system we designed to test theo- identified within each site on the basis of population
ries about different forms of conflict between existing sizes. Two groups within each site were randomly
large groups. By taking advantage of research on accu- selected for study; for the state-state cases, multiple bor-
racy in personality judgment (e.g., Funder & West, 1993) dering countries were identified, and one was randomly
and on expertise (e.g., Ericsson, Charness, Feltovich, & selected for inclusion. Examples of the six forms include
Hoffman, 2006; Sternberg & Horvath, 1999), we asked ethnic-ethnic (e.g., the Gogo and Sukuma ethnic groups
scholarly experts on societies in many locations around in Tanzania), religious-religious (e.g., Christians and
the world to use their explicit and tacit knowledge to Sunni Muslims in Pakistan), secular-religious (e.g., secu-
provide parameter values for our studied variables. The larists and Protestants in Argentina), state-state (e.g., Laos
use of experts for such purposes has been an accepted and Thailand), state-ethnic minority (e.g., the state of
practice within political science (e.g., Cederman, Wimmer, Moldova and Ukrainians), or state-religious minority
& Min, 2010) and often yields better predictions of behav- (e.g., the state of Malaysia and Buddhists); see Table S1
ioral outcomes than does objective measurement (e.g., in the Supplemental Material for a list of the 100 sites and
Bueno de Mesquita & Feng, 1997; Glasgow, Lewis, & groups.
Neiman, 2012; Hooghe et al., 2010). Our survey covered
many sites and groups, including many groups not expe- Identification and recruitment of
riencing conflict (to provide sufficient variability and ade- expert respondents
quate comparisons), and quantitatively operationalized
variables consistently across all groups and sites in ways We identified and recruited an international network of
designed a priori to capture our focal constructs. English-speaking researchers with expertise in the
selected sites, as determined by scholarly visibility (e.g.,
published peer-reviewed articles about the site), and
Method each informant provided data for only one site and the
Site and group selection two groups therein. Expert informants responded by
completing an Internet survey, and 471 of them provided
One hundred study sites were selected at the country data for at least one of the measured variables presented
level (see the Supplemental Material available online for here. Together, informants provided data for 97 of the
additional information on site and group selection and 100 sites; the remaining 3 sites were dropped. Expert
on recruitment of expert respondents). Because severe informants reported having spent an average of 20.75
intergroup conflict is rare relative to the potential for it years studying their sites (range = 260 years), and 75%
(Fearon & Laitin, 1996; Themnr & Wallensteen, 2012), self-identified as political scientists, anthropologists, his-
we oversampled conflict by selecting 25 sites a priori torians, sociologists, or economists.
on the basis of either recent past or current conflict
between particular groups at those sites, which included,
for example, Algeria (Islamists and secularists), Cyprus Measures
(Turks and Greeks), India (Muslims and Hindus), From midsummer through late fall 2009, each expert
Northern Ireland (Catholics and Protestants), Rwanda informant provided information about the two groups
(Hutu and Tutsi), and Sri Lanka (the state of Sri Lanka within his or her assigned site on a wide range of social,
and the Tamil ethnic minority). political, religious, and psychological variables, all of
The remaining 75 cases were sampled randomly, with- which were presented in standardized form across sites
out replacement, from a list of United Nations member and groups. The survey instructions were intended to
states. Countries were eligible for inclusion if their popu- focus respondents on providing information that would
lation exceeded 0.01% of the world population (thereby be coherent within the site (i.e., would capture any exist-
increasing our chances of finding relevant experts). In all, ing differences between the two groups) and make sense
the 100 sites spanned five continents, and the countries across sites (i.e., would take into consideration the
included accounted for approximately 79% of the worlds comparative global standings of given sites and groups
population. on each given variable). Informants were not asked for
Because we were interested in whether certain factors their beliefs about how these variables were associated
would emerge as predictors of conflict across different with one another (i.e., this was not a survey of the
kinds of groups, we randomly assigned each of the 75 experts own hypotheses or theories), and they were not
randomly selected sites to represent one of six common informed of our hypotheses during recruitment or survey
Religion and Intergroup Conflict 201

instruction. Rather, for each site and for each of the assessed with the question To what extent do [members
groups within it, informants made estimates on 9-point of one group] perpetrate violent acts of physical aggres-
scales for our variables of interest. sion against [members of the other group], such as
Religious infusion was operationalized at the group assaults, murders, or rapes? Finally, to assess collective
levelthat is, informants provided scores for each group violence, we asked To what extent do [members of one
within a siteand was calculated as the mean of three group] perpetrate violent acts of physical aggression
survey items: To what extent, on average, is religious against [members of the other group], such as riots or
ritual infused into the social/public life of [members of police/military actions? We aggregated responses on all
the group]? To what extent do [members of the group] variables across informants within each site; the median
use religious values, narratives, and reasoning in public number of informants per site was three or four, depend-
discourse? and To what extent, overall, does religion ing on the measure.
play a dominant role in the everyday lives of [members of
the group]? (Group A: = .86; Group B: = .93).
Results
Value incompatibility was operationalized at the site
level and assessed by a single item: To what extent are We used structural equation modeling to create an inter-
the set of values held by [members of one group] and changeable dyadic model that controlled for the depen-
[members of the other group] actually incompatible with dency between groups within each site (Kenny, 1996;
one another? Olsen & Kenny, 2006).1 First, our three predictors
Resource-power differential was created at the site resource-power differential, value incompatibility, and
level from responses to group-level items. To compute religious infusionwere not significantly associated with
this composite, we collected data from informants on the one another (ps > .20), which is consistent with the view-
extent to which each group actually confronted scarce point that these are indeed conceptually independent
resources and political power by averaging responses to variables.
two items: To what extent do [members of the group], Second, there were independent linear effects of both
on average, actually lack access to sufficient food, water, resource-power differential and value incompatibility on
and/or land? and To what extent do [members of the many of the conflict measures, which replicates results
group], on average, actually lack access to political power from past research in which different methods were used
and educational and economic opportunities? Next, (e.g., Biernat et al., 1996; Katz & Hass, 1988; Russett et al.,
within each site, a difference score was created such that 2000; Schwartz et al., 1990) and thereby validates our
the group with the greater scarcity (i.e., relatively fewer methodology (i.e., the use of expert informants): Groups
resources and less power) had a negative resource- with power and resource advantages engaged in signifi-
power-differential score; the group with less scarcity (i.e., cantly more conflict with their counterparts than did less
relatively greater resources and power) received a posi- advantaged groups (prejudice: = 0.169, p < .001; inter-
tive resource-power-differential score. Thus, within each personal discrimination: = 0.126, p < .01; collective vio-
site, the two groups had resource-power-differential lence: = 0.080, p < .05), and groups whose values were
scores equal in magnitude but with opposite signs, more incompatible with one another exhibited more
potentially ranging from 8 to +8. A resource-power-dif- prejudice ( = 0.394, p < . 01) and interpersonal discrimi-
ferential score of zero meant that the two groups had nation ( = 0.367, p < .01).2
equal access to resources and held equal power. Religious infusion was also an independent predictor
The conflict measures were each operationalized by a of all forms of conflict; it significantly predicted increased
single item for each group on a scale ranging from 1 prejudice ( = 0.170, p < .05), interpersonal discrimina-
(very little/not at all) to 9 (to a great extent). Prejudice tion ( = 0.243, p < .01), individual violence ( = 0.143,
was assessed by asking To what extent do [members of p < .05), and collective violence ( = 0.122, p < .05), and
one group], on average, hold negative prejudices against it marginally predicted increased symbolic aggression
[members of the other group]? Interpersonal discrimina- ( = 0.097, p < .10). Qualifying all these main effects,
tion was measured with the question To what extent do however, and supporting our hypotheses, results showed
[members of one group], on average, interpersonally dis- that religious infusion interacted with both resource-
criminate against [members of the other group] (e.g., by power differential and value-incompatibility to predict
avoiding cross-group friendships, romantic relationships, several forms of conflict.
work relationships)? We assessed symbolic aggression As depicted in Figure 1, the extent to which value
by asking To what extent do [members of one group] incompatibility predicted prejudice and interpersonal dis-
perpetrate symbolic acts of aggression against [members crimination was moderated by level of religious infusion
of the other group] (e.g., by desecrating graves, bombing (prejudice: = 0.086, p = .049; interpersonal discrimina-
symbolically important sites)? Individual violence was tion: = 0.095, p = .030). When religious infusion was
202 Neuberg et al.

a b
Low Religious Infusion
Medium Religious Infusion
High Religious Infusion

9 9

8 8

7 7

Interpersonal Discrimination
6 6
Prejudice

5 5

4 4

3 3

2 2

1 1

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9
Value Incompatibility Value Incompatibility

Fig. 1. Scatter plots showing mean ratings of (a) prejudice and (b) interpersonal discrimination as a function of the level of incompatibility of
each groups values with the counterpart groups values and the degree to which religion is infused throughout each group. For prejudice and
interpersonal discrimination, a rating of 1 indicated the lowest level, and 9 indicated the highest level. Lines for religious infusion were generated
from estimates from the structural equation models and are plotted at low (1 SD below the mean), medium (mean), and high (1 SD above the
mean), and the religious-infusion level of the counterpart group was held constant at the grand mean. Data points represent 194 groups nested
within 97 sites.

high, value incompatibility strongly predicted prejudice Using a novel methodology to facilitate cross-societal
and interpersonal discrimination; when religious infusion investigation, we found that religious infusion enhanced
was low, it did not. the extent to which incompatible values predict inter-
Religious infusion also moderated the way in which group conflict: Among groups with high levels of reli-
resource-power differential predicted symbolic aggres- gious infusion, those with value incompatibilities were
sion ( = 0.067, p = .022), individual violence ( = more likely to be prejudiced and to interpersonally dis-
0.081, p = .005), and collective violence ( = 0.105, p = criminate against one another. Religious infusion also
.001). Specifically, whereas disadvantaged groups with altered the manner in which relative group disadvantage
low levels of religious infusion tended to avoid extreme predicts conflict: Disadvantaged groups with low levels
acts of aggression, disadvantaged groups for whom reli- of religious infusion tended to avoid acting aggressively
gion was a dominant aspect of everyday life exhibited toward their advantaged counterparts, a strategy consis-
much less reticence (Fig. 2). That is, even if there was a tent with a rational analysis of the deterrent capacity of
significant disadvantage in terms of resources and politi- powerful groups to impose great costs on less powerful
cal power, groups with high levels of religious infusion groups. In contrast, disadvantaged groups with high lev-
were likely to be aggressive toward other groups. This els of religious infusion showed less evidence of being
finding suggests that the actions of groups with high lev- deterred, tending instead to engage powerful groups
els of religious infusion may be relatively insensitive to with symbolic, individual, and collective aggression.
the tangible costs potentially imposed by more powerful Religious infusion appears to increase the willingness of
groups. otherwise weak groups to endure costly confrontation.
These findings agree with those of research focusing
on individual-level mechanisms through which religion
Discussion may shape intergroup conflict (e.g., Ginges & Atran,
The findings of the present study demonstrate several 2011; Ginges et al., 2009; Sheikh, Ginges, Coman, &
ways in which religion may shape intergroup conflict. Atran, 2012). For instance, to the extent that people view
Religion and Intergroup Conflict 203

a b
Low Religious Infusion
Medium Religious Infusion
High Religious Infusion
9 9

8 8

7 7
Symbolic Aggression

Individual Violence
6 6

5 5

4 4

3 3

2 2

1 1

7 6 5 4 3 2 1 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 7 6 5 4 3 2 1 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Resource-Power Differential Resource-Power Differential

c
9

7
Collective Violence

7 6 5 4 3 2 1 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Resource-Power Differential

Fig. 2. Scatter plots showing mean ratings of (a) symbolic aggression, (b) individual violence, and (c) collective violence as a function of the
extent to which each group faced resource and power advantage or disadvantage and the degree to which religion is infused throughout each
group. For symbolic aggression, individual violence, and collective violence, a rating of 1 indicated the lowest level, and 9 indicated the highest
level. Resource-power differential ranged from highly disadvantaged (6) to highly advantaged (+6). Lines for religious infusion were generated
from the structural equation models and are plotted at low (1 SD below the mean), medium (mean), and high (1 SD above the mean), and the
religious-infusion level of the partner group was held constant at the grand mean. Data points represent 190 groups nested within 95 sites.
204 Neuberg et al.

certain resources or territories as sacred (e.g., Israelis significantly from our models predictions. For instance,
and Palestinians views of Jerusalem), they are more not all groups with high levels of religious infusion and
likely to be insulted by tangible offers for them (Ginges low power engaged in the more aggressive forms of con-
et al., 2011). Such mechanisms likely complement (and flict. Identifying factors that distinguish these cases will
are complemented by) and reinforce (and are reinforced have significant implications for theory and may also
by) the mechanisms giving rise to the large-scale reli- have important practical implications related to reducing
gious-conflict phenomena assessed in our data. violent intergroup conflict.
Are other forms of infusion (e.g., infusions of political This study also makes an important methodological
ideologies or ethnic identities) associated with similar contribution to cross-societal research. If one is interested
outcomes? Such forms of infusion may also engender the in the behaviors of individuals, one can acquire partici-
types of psychological and social processes discussed pant samples from many societies and run them through
earlier (e.g., norm salience, effective intragroup commu- identical procedures. This approach can be quite expen-
nication, trust and solidarity, group coordination toward sive and introduces a host of conceptual and practical
common goals, willingness to sacrifice for the group), challenges associated with cross-cultural research. If one
thereby facilitating a groups movement toward inter- is interested in comparing, contrasting, and explaining
group conflict when faced with value incompatibilities more complex social and community-level processes in
and resource disadvantages. Moreover, even nonreligious which many people interact, these challenges multiply
groups can define places, objects, or values as sacred and and the costs become prohibitive. This explains why
thus worth fighting for (Atran & Ginges, 2012; Hassner, scholars interested in empirically exploring such phe-
2009). That said, religions that have monitoring and nomena so often cobble together existing data sets and
rewarding/punishing deities or that offer eternally make conceptual and operationalization compromises in
rewarding afterlives may be especially effective at garner- the process. Our expert-informant method addresses
ing commitment from and shaping the behaviors of many of these challenges: It combines the strengths of
adherents ( Juergensmeyer, 2003; Toft, 2007). It may be, case-study investigations (providing a richness of expert
then, that religious infusion is particularly able to reduce knowledge from which to glean data) and large-sample
the apparent costs of intergroup aggression for its group quantitative analysis (providing many sites for study with
members. great variability on the constructs of interest). It provides
Our findings point to the value of assessing conflict consistent operationalizations of theoretically driven con-
at multiple scales and intensity. Specifically, religious structs across many sites. Because the survey is intended
infusion predicted multiple forms of conflict, but to be used by expert scholars, the constructs can
each form of conflict was predicted by a different mecha- be operationalized with some degree of conceptual
nism. Whereas religious infusion interacted with value nuance. In addition, the method is (relatively) financially
incompatibility to predict levels of prejudice and inter- inexpensive.
personal discriminationbut not symbolic aggression, That said, the method is not without its challenges. It
individual violence, or collective violenceit interacted is possible that some of our experts lacked specific
with resource-power differential to predict levels of knowledge regarding the variables of focus, which could
symbolic aggression, individual violence, and collective lead to missing data or decreased reliabilityboth of
violencebut not prejudice or interpersonal discrimina- which work against the ability to detect true associations
tion. Different forms of conflict were predicted by differ- among variables. It is also possible that the expert
ent patterns of variables, and this seems worthy of further responders were biased by theory-based preconceptions,
conceptual and empirical work. although it is hard to conceive of how any such biases
Our sample included a diversity of group types (e.g., could generate the interactive findings presented here.
ethnic, religious, national), and the 194 groups differed Each expert provided responses for only 1 of 97 sites,
from one another in many other ways. One should not and they were not queried on their beliefs about how
be surprised, then, that our findings are of modest mag- variables relate to one another. Moreover, respondents
nitude. That we were able to identify clear patterns in the generally represented disciplines in which theory about
face of such global diversity and variability suggests that religion and conflict lacks consensus and for which our
the processes underlying these patterns may be particu- interactive hypotheses (and findings) were novel. In all,
larly robust. Our findings also raise the possibility that we believe the strengths of the expert-informant method
such processes may be part of a common, near-universal outweigh its weaknesses, especially when one is investi-
suite of forces that form a foundation for much inter- gating social-level hypotheses of the sort explored here.
group conflict. However, we are not suggesting that Studying the relationship between religious infusion
potentially unique, society-specific forces are unimport- and intergroup conflict in existing groups requires
ant. Indeed, much may be learned from cases that depart correlational designs. Inferences of causality are thus
Religion and Intergroup Conflict 205

unwarranted, and we have taken care to avoid causal J. Schober, and D. Schaefer), the Arizona State University Center
language.3 Other designs might usefully complement our for the Study of Religion and Conflict, and the Arizona State
cross-sectional design. Additional longitudinal data on University Institute for Social Science Research.
these sites would enable the exploration of the causal
direction of effects with additional confidence. Laboratory Supplemental Material
simulations in which religious infusion, resource-power Additional supporting information may be found at http://pss
differentials, and value incompatibility are experimen- .sagepub.com/content/by/supplemental-data
tally primed and manipulated in groups of religious peo-
ple could also serve as a useful complement to the Notes
current approach. 1. Because of missing data, the number of sites varied slightly
across analyses, with ns ranging from 94 to 97.
Conclusion 2. There were also quadratic effects of resource-power dif-
ferential: Greater prejudice (p < .001) and interpersonal dis-
We used an innovative methodology in which 194 groups crimination (p < .01) characterized groups that were especially
nested within 97 countries were sampled, and we learned advantaged and groups that were especially disadvantaged.
that religious infusion, interacting with value incompati- These more nuanced findings corroborate work by Cederman,
bility and resource-power differential, predicted multiple Weidmann, and Gleditsch (2011), who used a different meth-
forms of large-scale intergroup conflict. Such findings odology and data set to study ethno-nationalist civil conflicts.
Because these curvilinear effects were not moderated by reli-
advance our theoretical understanding of complex con-
gious infusion, however, we do not discuss them further.
flict processes and thereby suggest strategies for reducing
3. It is instructive, however, that there are significant chal-
the often staggering costs of intergroup conflict. lenges for third-variable and reverse-causality alternatives to the
hypothesis that religious infusion plays a causal role in conflict.
Author Contributions To be compelling, third-variable alternatives must account not
S. L. Neuberg, C. M. Warner, G. Thomas, M. Winkelman, B. J. only for the interactive patterns of religious infusion with value
Broome, J. Schober, R. E. Millsap, and T. J. Taylor conceptual- incompatibility and with resource-power differential but also
ized the research. S. L. Neuberg, C. M. Warner, S. A. Mistler, for the different patterns of these interactions on the different
R. E. Millsap, G. Thomas, M. Winkelman, B. J. Broome, and forms of conflict observed here. They must also account for
J. D. Johnson designed the methodology and created the expert these findings at the same level of analysis; third variables that
survey. S. L. Neuberg, C. M. Warner, S. A. Mistler, E. D. Hill, can be viewed as mediators of the putative effects of religious
J. D. Johnson, G. Thomas, M. Winkelman, and B. J. Broome infusion do not qualify as alternatives. The reverse-causality
performed the research (i.e., selected sites, recruited respon- alternativethat conflict causes religious infusionneeds to
dents, and programmed and implemented the Internet survey). account not only for the observed interactive patterns of find-
S. A. Mistler, A. Berlin, and G. Filip-Crawford analyzed the data ings but must also explain the origins of the conflict posited as
under the supervision of R. E. Millsap. S. L. Neuberg, C. M. causal. Thus, although we do not claim causal effects of our
Warner, S. A. Mistler, A. Berlin, E. D. Hill, and G. Filip-Crawford predictors, we note that the patterns of data are consistent with
wrote the manuscript. All authors approved the final version of such an interpretation, whereas alternative accounts struggle to
the manuscript for submission. explain the nuanced patterns of observed findings.

Acknowledgments References
We thank David Siroky, Timothy Byne, Douglas Kenrick, Allen, M. A., & Fordham, B. O. (2011). From Melos to Baghdad:
Deborah Hall, and the participants of the Arizona State Explaining resistance to militarized challenges by more
University Social Psychology Research Institute and the Arizona powerful states. International Studies Quarterly, 55, 1025
State University Center for Law and Global Affairs colloquium 1045.
for their helpful comments on this work. The data reported in Arreguin-Toft, I. (2001). How the weak win wars: A theory of
this article are available from S. L. Neuberg. asymmetric conflict. International Security, 26, 93128.
Atran, S., & Ginges, J. (2012). Religious and sacred imperatives
Declaration of Conflicting Interests in human conflict. Science, 336, 855857.
Atran, S., & Henrich, J. (2010). The evolution of religion: How
The authors declared that they had no conflicts of interest with
cognitive by-products, adaptive learning heuristics, ritual
respect to their authorship or the publication of this article.
displays, and group competition generate deep commit-
ments to prosocial religions. Biological Theory: Integrating
Funding Development, Evolution, and Cognition, 5, 1830.
This research was supported by National Science Foundation Barker, D. C., Hurwitz, J., & Nelson, T. L. (2008). Of crusades
Grant 0729516 (to S. L. Neuberg, C. M. Warner, R. E. Millsap, and culture wars: Messianic militarism and political con-
G. Thomas, M. Winkelman, B. J. Broome, T. J. Taylor, flict in the United States. Journal of Politics, 70, 307322.
206 Neuberg et al.

Berman, E. (2009). Radical, religious and violent: The new eco- Hall, D. L., Matz, D. C., & Wood, W. (2010). Why dont we
nomics of terrorism. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. practice what we preach? A meta-analytic review of reli-
Biernat, M., Vescio, T. K., Theno, S. A., & Crandall, C. S. (1996). gious racism. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 14,
Values and prejudice: Toward understanding the impact 126139.
of American values on outgroup attitudes. In C. Seligman, Hassner, R. E. (2009). War on sacred grounds. Ithaca, NY:
J. M. Olson, & M. P. Zanna (Eds.), The psychology of values: Cornell University Press.
The Ontario Symposium (Vol. 8, pp. 153189). Hillsdale, Hegre, H. (2008). Gravitating toward war: Preponderance may
NJ: Erlbaum. pacify, but power kills. Journal of Conflict Resolution, 52,
Bueno de Mesquita, B., & Feng, Y. (1997). Forecasting 566589.
Chinas political and economic future. Problems of Post- Hooghe, L., Bakker, R., Brigevich, A., de Vries, C., Edwards,
Communism, 44(2), 1427. E., Marks, G., . . . Vachudova, M. (2010). Reliability and
Campbell, D. T. (1965). Ethnocentric and other altruis- validity of the 2002 and 2006 Chapel Hill expert surveys on
tic motives. In D. Levine (Ed.), Nebraska Symposium on party positioning. European Journal of Political Research,
Motivation: Vol. 13 (pp. 283311). Lincoln: University of 49, 687703.
Nebraska Press. Huntington, S. P. (1993). The clash of civilizations? Foreign
Cederman, L. E., Weidmann, N. B., & Gleditsch, K. S. (2011). Affairs, 72, 2249.
Horizontal inequalities and ethnonationalist civil war: A Iannaccone, L. R., & Berman, E. (2006). Religious extremism:
global comparison. American Political Science Review, 105, The good, the bad, and the deadly. Public Choice, 128,
478495. 109129.
Cederman, L. E., Wimmer, A., & Min, B. (2010). Why do ethnic Juergensmeyer, M. (2003). Terror in the mind of God: The global
groups rebel? New data and analysis. World Politics, 62, rise of religious violence. Berkeley: University of California
87119. Press.
Collier, P., Hoeffler, A., & Rohner, D. (2009). Beyond greed Kaplan, B. J. (2007). Divided by faith: Religious conflict and the
and grievance: Feasibility and civil war. Oxford Economic practice of toleration in early modern Europe. Cambridge,
Papers, 61, 127. MA: Harvard University Press.
Crosby, F. (1976). A model of egoistical relative deprivation. Katz, I., & Hass, R. G. (1988). Racial ambivalence and American
Psychological Review, 83, 85113. value conflict: Correlational and priming studies of dual
Ericsson, K. A., Charness, N., Feltovich, P. J., & Hoffman, cognitive structures. Journal of Personality and Social
R. R. (2006). The Cambridge handbook of expertise and Psychology, 55, 893905.
expert performance. New York, NY: Cambridge University Kenny, D. A. (1996). Models of non-independence in dyadic
Press. research. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 13,
Fearon, J. D., & Laitin, D. D. (1996). Explaining interethnic 279294.
cooperation. American Political Science Review, 90, 715 Lincoln, B. (2003). Holy terrors: Thinking about religion after
735. September 11. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.
Fearon, J. D., & Laitin, D. D. (2003). Ethnicity, insurgency and Olsen, J. A., & Kenny, D. A. (2006). Structural equation model-
civil war. American Political Science Review, 97, 7590. ing with interchangeable dyads. Psychological Methods, 11,
Fox, J. (2004). Religion and state failure: An examination 127141.
of the extent and magnitude of religious conflict from Russett, B. M., Oneal, J. R., & Cox, M. (2000). Clash of civiliza-
1950 to 1996. International Political Science Review, 25, tions or realism and liberalism dj vu? Some evidence.
5576. Journal of Peace Research, 37, 583608.
Funder, D. C., & West, S. G. (1993). Consensus, self-other Schwartz, S. H., Struch, N., & Bilsky, W. (1990). Values and
agreement, and accuracy in personality judgment: An intro- intergroup social motives: A study of Israeli and German
duction. Journal of Personality, 64, 457476. students. Social Psychology Quarterly, 53, 185198.
Garcia-Retamero, R., Mller, S. M., & Rousseau, D. L. (2012). Sheikh, H., Ginges, J., Coman, A., & Atran, S. (2012). Religion,
The impact of value similarity and power on the perception group threat and sacred values. Judgment and Decision
of threat. Political Psychology, 33, 179193. Making, 7, 110118.
Ginges, J., & Atran, S. (2011). War as a moral imperative (not Sherif, M., Harvey, O. J., White, B. J., Hood, W. R., & Sherif,
just practical politics by another means). Proceedings of the C. W. (1988). The Robbers Cave experiment: Intergroup con-
Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 278, 29302938. flict and cooperation. Middletown, CT: Wesleyan University
Ginges, J., Atran, S., Sachdeva, S., & Medin, D. (2011). Press. (Original work published 1961)
Psychology out of the laboratory: The challenge of violent Sternberg, R. J., & Horvath, J. A. (1999). Tacit knowledge in
extremism. American Psychologist, 66, 507519. professional practice. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Ginges, J., Hansen, I., & Norenzayan, A. (2009). Religion and Themnr, L., & Wallensteen, P. (2012). Armed conflicts, 1946
support for suicide attacks. Psychological Science, 20, 224 2011. Journal of Peace Research, 49, 565575.
230. Toft, M. D. (2007). Getting religion? The puzzling case of Islam
Glasgow, G., Lewis, P. G., & Neiman, M. (2012). Local devel- and civil war. International Security, 31, 97131.
opment policies and the foreclosure crisis in California: Walker, I., & Smith, H. J. (2002). Relative deprivation:
Can local policies hold back national tides? Urban Affairs Specification, development, and integration. New York,
Review, 48, 6485. NY: Cambridge University Press.

Você também pode gostar