Você está na página 1de 14

Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice Copyright 2002 by the Educational Publishing Foundation

2002, Vol. 6, No. 3, 229 242 1089-2699/02/$5.00 DOI: 10.1037//1089-2699.6.3.229

Role Ambiguity, Role Efficacy, and Role Performance:


Multidimensional and Mediational Relationships Within
Interdependent Sport Teams

Mark R. Beauchamp Steven R. Bray


University of Birmingham University of Lethbridge

Mark A. Eys and Albert V. Carron


University of Western Ontario

Male secondary school rugby players (N 271) participated in a study examining role
ambiguity, role efficacy, and role performance. A multidimensional measure was used
to assess 4 manifestations of role ambiguity in offensive and defensive contexts.
Multiple role ambiguity dimensions explained variance in efficacy and performance.
Consistent with theorizing by A. Bandura (1997) and R. L. Kahn, D. M. Wolfe, R. P.
Quinn, J. D. Snoek, and R. A. Rosenthal (1964), negative relationships observed
between role ambiguity and role performance were mediated by competence (role
efficacy) beliefs. Findings support the multidimensional operationalization of role
ambiguity and role efficacy as a generative mechanism through which role ambiguity
can affect role performance.

Role ambiguity refers to a lack of clear infor- should be particularly dysfunctional in situa-
mation associated with a particular role (Kahn, tions where individual roles are characterized
Wolfe, Quinn, Snoek, & Rosenthal, 1964). A by a high degree of interdependence. That is,
large body of research evidence indicates role when role occupants responsibilities are inter-
ambiguity may have problematic consequences woven with those of other individuals in an
for role occupants. For example, within the or- organizational system, the impact of role ambi-
ganizational domain, research has found role guity on the role occupant personally should be
ambiguity to be consistently associated with more problematic compared with when role oc-
negative affect (e.g., Lagace, 1988; Terry, cupants function more or less independently of
Nielsen, & Perchard, 1993) and impaired per- others. Furthermore, in situations characterized
formance (e.g., Bauer & Green, 1994; Sohi, by interdependence of roles, ambiguity could be
Smith, & Ford, 1996; Szilagyi, 1977; Williams, predicted to influence the thoughts and behav-
Podsakoff, & Huber, 1992) among employees. iors of both the role occupant and the other
Although role ambiguity has been associated individuals (e.g., group members) with whom
with various negative outcomes within organi- he or she interacts (Forsyth, 1999). Indeed,
zations, Kahn et al. (1964) theorized that it Kahn et al. (1964) provided an illustration of
this point in their role episode model when they
observed that change in any one part of the
Mark R. Beauchamp, School of Sport and Exercise Sci- [role] system creates changes in other parts as
ences, University of Birmingham, Birmingham, England; well . . . ambiguity in many parts of the organi-
Steven R. Bray, Department of Kinesiology and Physical zation are almost inevitably the outcome (pp.
Education, University of Lethbridge, Lethbridge, Alberta,
Canada; Mark A. Eys and Albert V. Carron, School of
76 77).
Kinesiology, University of Western Ontario, Toronto, On- It is interesting to note that despite the fact
tario, Canada. that research has focused extensively on role
Correspondence concerning this article should be ad- ambiguity and its correlates within generic
dressed to Mark R. Beauchamp, who is now at the
Institute for Sport and Exercise Sciences, University of
work roles (e.g., Berkowitz, 1980; Organ &
Leeds, Leeds LS2 9JT, United Kingdom. E-mail: Green, 1981) few studies have examined role
mrbeauchamp@hotmail.com ambiguity within the interdependent group con-
229
230 BEAUCHAMP, BRAY, EYS, AND CARRON

text. Given the pervasiveness of groups that measuring role ambiguity is not only inconsis-
have task interdependence as a fundamental fea- tent with theory, it has minimal utility in terms
ture (e.g., projects teams, service groups, and of its potential application. For example, King
sport teams), research aimed at examining the and King (1990) criticized unidimensional ap-
extent to which role ambiguity perceptions af- proaches to measurement on the grounds that
fect role occupants cognitions and behaviors is they elicit little knowledge about the myriad
particularly salient. Thus, one general focus of types of ambiguity role occupants might expe-
our study was to examine role ambiguity and rience and that they provide limited information
cognitive and behavioral correlates within inter- to help guide intervention.
dependent teams. A second general focus was to Although Beauchamp and Bray (2001) and
establish a conceptually sound protocol for as- Eys and Carron (2001) both assessed role am-
sessing role ambiguity. Insofar as this second biguity as a multidimensional construct, two
purpose is concerned, the present study builds markedly different approaches were taken.
on recent efforts to examine role ambiguity in Beauchamp and Bray, using the work of
sport teams. Rhoads, Singh, and Goodell (1994) and Singh
In one study, Beauchamp and Bray (2001) (1993) as their basis, measured role ambiguity
examined role ambiguity as well as role conflict in terms of the major behavioral contexts in
perceptions among elite university athletes from which sport team members have formal role-
a cross-section of interdependent team sports related responsibilities. In sports, the major con-
(e.g., basketball, rugby, and soccer). Results texts are related to offense and defense.
indicated that athletes who reported greater lev- Beauchamp and Brays results supported their
els of role ambiguity and role conflict had lower proposition that these two contexts formed def-
levels of efficacy with regard to performing inite distinguishable dimensions of team play
their primary role responsibilities. Furthermore, for each team member, as role ambiguity per-
consistent with theorizing by Kahn et al. (1964), ceptions were found to differentiate across
the relationship between role conflict and role contexts.
efficacy was mediated by role ambiguity. In contrast, Eys and Carron (2001), using the
In another study, Eys and Carron (2001) in- Kahn et al. (1964) theoretical model as their
vestigated the relationship between role ambi- basis, operationalized role ambiguity as a mul-
guity and both task cohesion and task self- tidimensional construct composed of ambiguity
efficacy within six university basketball teams. about (a) the scope of responsibilities, (b) the
It was found that individuals who were unclear behaviors necessary to carry out those respon-
about their role responsibilities perceived their sibilities, (c) how role responsibilities are eval-
team to be less integrated in terms of its task uated, and (d) the consequences of not fulfilling
approach to team play and reported lower levels role responsibilities. Consistent with theory,
of attraction to the team. In addition, consistent their results showed differential patterns of pre-
with the findings of Beauchamp and Bray diction across the various forms of ambiguity
(2001), individuals who reported greater ambi- relating to task cohesion as well as task
guity were less efficacious about performing self-efficacy.
tasks associated with their responsibilities on In combination, the findings of Beauchamp
the team. and Bray (2001) and Eys and Carron (2001)
In addition to identifying important correlates provide initial support for the multidimension-
of role ambiguity, the Beauchamp and Bray ality of role ambiguity within the interdepen-
(2001) and Eys and Carron (2001) studies also dent sport team context. However, limitations
made a potentially important contribution to the and strengths to both approaches should be
study of the construct through the operational noted. One limitation of the Beauchamp and
definitions they used. Despite theoretical argu- Bray measure is that it did not represent multi-
ments that role ambiguity should be conceptu- dimensional role ambiguity as theorized by
alized and measured multidimensionally (Kahn Kahn et al. (1964). In short, although multidi-
et al., 1964; King & King, 1990), researchers mensionality was supported, their approach
have traditionally assessed it as a unidimen- lacked the theory that provided the basis for the
sional construct (e.g., Rizzo, House, & Lirtz- Eys and Carron approach. Conversely, a limita-
man, 1970). A unidimensional approach to tion of the Eys and Carron measure was that it
ROLE AMBIGUITY 231

failed to differentiate among the various con- such as team clown, social facilitator, or moti-
texts (i.e., offense and defense) in which the vator (Carron & Hausenblas, 1998). Formal
four manifestations of role ambiguity (i.e., roles are directly prescribed to group members,
scope of responsibilities, etc.) might be experi- which in the sport team setting encompass spe-
enced. Specifically, in the Eys and Carron mea- cific task-related behaviors determined by the
sure, questions in the scales representing each teams coach (Bray, 1998; Carron & Hausen-
of the four manifestations of ambiguity focused blas, 1998). Because formal roles dictate the
on 10 broad contexts that included members task-related responsibilities that are directly as-
offensive responsibilities, defensive responsi- sociated with task performance (e.g., mastery of
bilities, leadership responsibilities, and respon- specific maneuvers), only formal roles were
sibilities to organize functions for teammates. assessed.
With such an approach, there is a risk that Third, different types of formal roles may
important information will be overlooked. For exist (e.g., offense, defense, and leadership).
example, an individual may be quite clear about However, as offense and defense form the ma-
his or her scope of role responsibilities for of- jor behavioral contexts in which all members of
fense but be unclear about his or her responsi- interdependent sport teams have formal roles,
bilities for team leadership, defense, and so we limited our measure to these two specific
forth. As a consequence, the average score re- contexts. That is, we did not seek to assess
flecting scope of responsibilities would not cap- formal roles associated with group-related re-
ture the differences in ambiguity across differ- sponsibilities such as leadership (e.g., team cap-
ent contexts. tain) or social organization (e.g., team social
Given the limitations associated with both secretary).
approaches, it is clear that neither the Beau- Finally, the measure focused on the four
champ and Bray (2001) nor the Eys and Carron manifestations of ambiguity identified by Eys
(2001) measure is fully satisfactory. However, and Carron (2001): scope of responsibilities,
the two approaches are highly complementary, role behaviors, role evaluation, and role conse-
and their strengths should also be highlighted. quences. Scope of responsibilities refers to a
Specifically, Beauchamp and Bray provided a lack of clear information about the breadth of
measure that taps the two major contexts in ones responsibilities. Role behaviors refer to a
which all interdependent sport team members lack of clear information about the behaviors
have formal roles, namely offense and defense. associated with ones role. Role evaluation re-
The Eys and Carron approach, on the other fers to a lack of clear information about how
hand, taps four theory-driven dimensions across ones responsibilities are evaluated. Role con-
which role ambiguity can be manifested. Thus, sequences refer to a lack of clear information
in the present study a preliminary attempt was about the consequences of a failure to fulfill
made to develop a multidimensional measure of ones role responsibilities.
role ambiguity that combined the strengths of The general purpose of the present study was
both approaches. to examine correlates of role involvement in a
To guide the instrument development pro- team sport characterized by a high degree of
cess, we considered four fundamental role-re- interdependence (i.e., rugby). Strong theoretical
lated issues. First, role ambiguity was theorized and empirical bases led us to focus on four
to encompass both subjective and objective specific questions. The first pertained to the
components (Kahn et al., 1964). However, con- relationship between role ambiguity (using a
sistent with the majority of published role am- measure of the construct that was based on the
biguity research (cf. Van Sell, Brief, & Schuler, conceptual framework outlined above) and role
1981), our attempt to assess role ambiguity was performance effectiveness. Previous meta-anal-
limited to subjective ambiguity (i.e., the percep- yses within the organizational domain (e.g.,
tion of role ambiguity held by a particular Abramis, 1994; Fisher & Gitelson, 1983; Jack-
person). son & Schuler, 1985) have found role ambiguity
Second, roles can also be formal or informal to be negatively associated with performance.
(Mabry & Barnes, 1980). Informal roles de- However, in a more recent meta-analysis con-
velop through processes of interpersonal inter- ducted by Tubre and Collins (2000), the nega-
action within the group. They can include roles tive relationship between role ambiguity and
232 BEAUCHAMP, BRAY, EYS, AND CARRON

performance was found to be more pronounced tionships among the three constructs. Jackson
(i.e., a larger effect size was found) among and Schuler (1985) theorized that role ambigu-
individuals whose roles were characterized by a ity affects performance via the weakening of
high degree of interdependence compared with motivation in terms of both effort-to-perfor-
those whose role requirements were carried out mance and performance-to-reward expectan-
independently of others. Team sports such as cies. According to Bandura (1997), efficacy ex-
rugby involve dynamic team play in which pectations are motivational. Thus, role efficacy
member roles are highly interdependent. Thus, represents a cognitive mechanism that may ac-
it was hypothesized that role performance count for the relationship between role ambigu-
would be negatively associated with role ity and role performance. Consistent with this
ambiguity. reasoning, we hypothesized that the relationship
The second research question concerned the between role ambiguity and role performance
relationship between role ambiguity and role would be mediated by role efficacy.
efficacy. Role efficacy refers to a specific form
of self-efficacy relating to a team members Method
confidence in his or her capabilities to carry out
interdependent role functions (Bray, 1998; Bray Participants
& Brawley, 2000). According to self-efficacy
theory (Bandura, 1997), information derived Male secondary school rugby players (mean
enactively, vicariously, persuasively, or physi- age 15.38 years, SD 1.56; N 271)
ologically is processed to develop beliefs of representing 17 teams and head coaches repre-
personal efficacy. If an individual perceives a senting 12 of those teams participated in the
lack of clear information in terms of what is study. Athletes had an average of 5.26 years
expected of him or her in order to effectively (SD 2.38) playing experience in competitive
carry out his or her role responsibilities, per- rugby. The level of competition represented in
sonal efficacy related to these role behaviors is the sample was of a high standard, with many
likely to suffer (Bandura, 1997; Kahn et al., athletes having played for county or divisional
1964). This proposition has been supported by representative teams. For example, as part of
the findings of both Beauchamp and Bray their regular team engagement, participants
(2001) and Eys and Carron (2001). Thus, it was took part in structured practices 35 times per
hypothesized that role ambiguity would be neg- week.
atively associated with role-related efficacy.
The first two research questions acknowledge Measures
the potential relationships between role ambi-
guity and both role performance and efficacy, Identification of primary role responsibilities.
respectively. The third research question in- In order to have players focus on their primary
volved an examination of the relationship be- role responsibilities for offense and defense, we
tween the latter two constructs. According to used a three-stage process developed by Bray
self-efficacy theory (Bandura, 1997), decre- and colleagues (Beauchamp & Bray, 2001;
ments in personal efficacy are likely to be pre- Bray, 1998; Bray & Brawley, 2002). First, play-
dictive of decrements in performance. In their ers were provided with a specific definition of
recent review of the efficacy literature in sports, roles and were required to focus on their overall
Feltz and Chase (1998) found support for this role on their team. Second, players were asked
proposition in that higher self-efficacy was con- to differentiate specific offensive and defensive
sistently associated with more positive perfor- role responsibilities within their overall role.
mance. Thus, it was hypothesized that role ef- Instructions made clear that ones overall role
ficacy would be positively associated with role could be made up of several interdependent
performance in the present study. responsibilities for offense and defense. Exam-
Drawing from theory (Bandura, 1997; Kahn ples of specific role responsibilities from rugby
et al., 1964), the bivariate interrelationships pro- were also provided breaking the gain line off
posed between role ambiguity, role perfor- set-piece plays, closing down space between
mance, and role efficacy led to an additional myself and my opponent and putting in a strong
research question, namely, the triadic interrela- tackle, et cetera. Finally, players were required
ROLE AMBIGUITY 233

to provide written descriptions of their four In Stage 3, the resulting pool of items was
primary interdependent role responsibilities for submitted to five experts in the field of group
offense and defense, separately. dynamics to further assess content validity.
Role ambiguity. Using their primary offen- Consistent with procedures outlined by Es-
sive and defensive role responsibilities as tabrooks and Carron (2000), the following
frames of reference, athletes responded to a criteria were used to incorporate the feedback
battery of questions regarding the degree to from the group dynamics experts. Items were
which they perceived that their responsibilities reworded or eliminated if they were judged
were ambiguous. Role ambiguity was assessed not to reflect the intended role ambiguity di-
using two 20-item scales (i.e., one for offense mension. Items judged to be awkwardly
and one for defense) designed to assess the worded were removed from the content pool.
degree of ambiguity and lack of clarity associ- Items considered to possess jargon or com-
ated with (a) the scope of personal responsibil- plex terminology were eliminated. Finally,
ities (5 items), (b) the behaviors necessary to items considered to be almost identical in
carry out those responsibilities (5 items), (c) content were removed. The final scales are
how performance associated with those respon- presented in the Appendix.
sibilities is evaluated (5 items), and (d) the Athletes rated their agreement with each role
consequences of a failure to successfully carry ambiguity statement on a 9-point Likert-type
out those responsibilities (5 items). scale anchored by 1 (strongly disagree) and 9
Content validity of each of these scales was (strongly agree). Thus, higher scores reflected
assessed ad hoc through a three-stage process. greater clarity (i.e., lower ambiguity) and lower
First, on the basis of conceptual definitions pro- scores reflected greater ambiguity (i.e., lower
vided by theory (Kahn et al., 1964), items were clarity). Acceptable internal consistencies
developed that were believed to represent the (Cronbachs alpha) of greater than .78 were
four manifestations of ambiguity identified by recorded for each of the role ambiguity scales
Eys and Carron (2001). Second, elite athletes (Nunnally, 1978). Internal consistencies for
(n 9), recreational athletes (n 6), and expe- each of the role ambiguity scales are reported in
rienced coaches (n 4) from a variety of inter- Table 1.
dependent sport teams reviewed and provided Role efficacy. Role efficacy was assessed
feedback on the items, in terms of both wording using procedures outlined by Bray and col-
and content. Any items deemed awkwardly leagues (Beauchamp & Bray, 2001; Bray, 1998;
worded were rephrased. Any items that held Bray & Brawley, 2002). For each of the four
little meaning for the athletes were reworded or offensive and four defensive primary role re-
removed. sponsibilities identified in the role identification

Table 1
Descriptive Statistics for Multidimensional Role Ambiguity, Role Efficacy, and
Role Performance Effectiveness
Context and variable M SD Range
Offense
Scope of responsibilities 6.95 1.20 3.009.00 .79
Behavior 6.85 1.27 1.409.00 .83
Evaluation 6.50 1.52 1.809.00 .90
Consequences 7.24 1.23 2.009.00 .81
Role efficacy 73.50 14.54 10.00100.00
Role performance effectiveness 6.97 1.25 3.5010.00
Defense
Scope of responsibilities 7.08 1.28 2.609.00 .85
Behavior 6.99 1.33 1.499.00 .87
Evaluation 6.81 1.46 1.409.00 .90
Consequences 7.33 1.34 1.209.00 .85
Role efficacy 71.43 16.56 10.00100.00
Role performance effectiveness 6.68 1.34 3.5010.00
234 BEAUCHAMP, BRAY, EYS, AND CARRON

process, athletes rated their confidence in their immediately prior to nor immediately following
ability to perform that responsibility. Responses competition.
were provided using a 0% (not at all confident)
to 100% (extremely confident) scale. The mean Results
score from each athletes (a) four-item defen-
sive role responsibility scale and (b) four-item Confirmatory Factor Analyses
offensive role responsibility scale formed the
defensive role efficacy and offensive role effi- Given that a newly developed inventory was
cacy scores, respectively. Intraclass correlations used to measure the four hypothesized dimen-
of .76 and .78 were obtained for the offensive sions of role ambiguity, we conducted two con-
role efficacy and defensive role efficacy scales. firmatory factor analyses for both offense and
These correlations indicate a high degree of defense to ensure that the items loaded on each
within-subject consistency, thus giving support of the four factors as expected. In the present
for item aggregation. study, a maximum likelihood method of estima-
Role performance effectiveness. The head tion was computed using AMOS (Version 4.0)
coaches from 12 of the 17 participating teams software (Arbuckle, 1999). As recommended
(not all coaches participated) identified the four by Hu and Bentler (1995), several fit indices
primary role responsibilities that they expected were used to assess model fit. First, the chi-
each of their athletes to perform for offense and square test was considered. A nonsignificant
defense, separately. Once these role responsi- chi-square is typically representative of a good
bilities were identified, the coaches rated how fitting model. For both offensive and defensive
well each athlete was performing with regard to models, the chi-square statistic was significant:
each of these responsibilities at that stage in the offense, 2(164, N 271) 420.91, p .001;
season. Responses were provided on an 11- defense, 2(164, N 271) 347.42, p .001.
point scale anchored by 0 (very ineffective) However, numerous authors have suggested
and 10 (very effective). Offensive and defensive that a nonsignificant chi-square result is unreal-
role performance effectiveness scores were cal- istic (e.g., Bentler & Bonett, 1980; Joreskog,
culated from the means of the four defensive 1969) and consider the ratio between the chi-
items and four offensive items, respectively. square and the degrees of freedom to be a more
Intraclass correlations of .85 and .85 were re- appropriate measure of model fit. Carmines and
corded for the offensive and defensive rating McIver (1981) indicated that ratios in the range
scales, indicating a high degree of internal con- of 2 to 1 or 3 to 1 are indicative of an acceptable
sistency among the respective offensive and fit between the hypothetical model and the sam-
defensive item response sets. ple data (p. 80). In the present study, the ratios
obtained for both offensive (2/df 2.57) and
Procedure defensive (2/df 2.12) data indicated accept-
able model fit of less than 3 to 1.
Participants were recruited by Mark R. In addition to the chi-square, several other
Beauchamp through initial contact with each indices were used to assess model fit, including
teams head coach. To provide time for players the standardized root-mean-square residual
formal interdependent role responsibilities to (SRMR), the comparative fit index (CFI), the
become established, we administered the ques- TuckerLewis index (TLI), and the root-mean-
tionnaires approximately 1 month into the com- square error of approximation (RMSEA). Re-
petitive season during a 1-week period. Ques- sults of both the offensive (SRMR .06;
tionnaires were completed at a team meeting CFI .92; TLI .91; RMSEA .08) and
that was neither immediately before nor imme- defensive (SRMR .04; CFI .95; TLI .95;
diately after competition in order to avoid com- RMSEA .06) models showed that the four-
petition-specific bias. The athletes were in- factor solution provided an adequate fit to the
formed of the voluntary nature of the study and data. Collectively, these results indicate that the
were assured of confidentiality, and both paren- role ambiguity instrument used in this study is a
tal and participant consent was obtained. Coach good measure of the four hypothesized ambigu-
ratings of performance were obtained during the ity manifestations. However, it should be noted
same week, again at a time that was neither that these results are restricted to the present
ROLE AMBIGUITY 235

sample. Consequently, future research should

.36**
.32**
.35**

.31**
.73**

.26**

.37**
.19*
12


.04

.16
.17
examine the construct validity of the instrument
with participants from other sports, perfor-
mance levels, and age groups (cf. Carron &

.51**
.49**
.46**
.34**
.73**
.37**

.57**
.55**
.47**
.48**
Brawley, 2000).

11


Descriptive Statistics

.49**
.51**
.53**
.66**
.41**

.69**
.67**
.67**
10


.15
Descriptive statistics are presented in Ta-
ble 1. For both offense and defense, relatively
high mean scores were observed for all role

.58**
.61**
.76**
.52**
.41**

.74**
.75**
ambiguity dimensions (note that higher scores

.16
reflect greater clarity and less ambiguity). The

Intercorrelations Between Offensive and Defensive Role Ambiguity, Role Efficacy, and Role Performance Effectiveness
participants also reported relatively high levels
of role efficacy for both offense (M 73.50,

.62**
.75**
.62**
.55**
.44**

.83**
SD 14.54) and defense (M 71.43,

.17
SD 16.56). In addition, coach ratings re-
vealed that the athletes sampled were achieving

.67**
.66**
.61**
.55**
.49**
.25**
a moderately high level of success in perform-


7
ing both their offensive (M 6.97, SD 1.25)
and defensive (M 6.68, SD 1.34) role
responsibilities.
.44**
.34**
.30**

.39**
Bivariate correlations between the variables

are presented in Table 2. The correlations .03


showed a general trend toward higher levels of
role clarity (i.e., lower role ambiguity) being
.54**
.42**
.46**
.29**

5

associated with higher role efficacy scores (e.g.,


r .54 for the relationship between offensive
scope of responsibilities and offensive role ef-
ficacy) and higher role performance effective-
.53**
.50**
.54**

4

ness scores (e.g., r .44 for the relationship


between offensive scope of responsibilities and
offensive role performance).
.65**
.65**

3

Relationships Between Role Ambiguity


and Role Efficacy
.72**

2

Note. N 271; for role performance, N 135.

To determine the relationship between role


ambiguity and role efficacy, role efficacy was
regressed on the four role ambiguity dimensions

1

(Table 3). Separate multiple regression analyses


were carried out for the two contexts (i.e., of-
fense and defense). Assessment of both the vari-
1. Scope of responsibilities

7. Scope of responsibilities

ance inflation factors and the tolerance statistics


Context and variable

(Hair, Anderson, Tatham, & Black, 1998) re-


6. Role performance

12. Role performance

* p .05. ** p .01.

vealed that multicollinearity was not at a level


4. Consequences

10. Consequences
5. Role efficacy

11. Role efficacy

to evoke concern (i.e., collinearity did not ex-


3. Evaluation

9. Evaluation
2. Behaviors

8. Behaviors

plain more than 10% of the variance in any


independent variable).
For offense, ambiguity related to scope of
Table 2

Defense
Offense

responsibilities was the primary predictor of


variance in role efficacy ( .43, p .001).
236 BEAUCHAMP, BRAY, EYS, AND CARRON

Role evaluation ambiguity was also signifi-

5.27***

2.80**

3.11**
cantly related to role efficacy in the model (

2.36*
0.02

0.64

0.10
1.80
t .21, p .01). The full model accounted for 30%
of the variance in offensive role efficacy (ad-
justed R2 .30), F(4, 264) 29.59, p .001.
A similar pattern of findings was obtained in
Adjusted R2

the defensive analysis. That is, ambiguity asso-


.30***

.34*** ciated with scope of responsibilities was the


major predictor of role efficacy ( .30, p
.01). However, in this analysis, role behavior
ambiguity also contributed to the explained
variance ( .22, p .05). The full model
.001

accounted for 34% of the variance in defensive


.43

.21
.04

.30
.22
.01
.13

role efficacy (adjusted R2 .34), F(4, 262)


35.83, p .001.
0.98
0.91
0.70
0.75

1.24
1.19
0.94
0.91
SEB

Relationship Between Role Ambiguity and


Role Performance Effectiveness
Although 271 athletes completed their ques-
5.16
0.01
1.96
0.48

3.87
2.81
0.09
1.65

tionnaires, 5 of the 17 teams head coaches were


B

either unable (2 were absent from school for 2


weeks because of illness) or chose not (n 3)
to provide ratings on role performance for their
Regression Analyses for Variables Predicting Offensive and Defensive Role Efficacy

athletes (coach participation was voluntary). As


Defensive role ambiguity (complete model)
Offensive role ambiguity (complete model)

a consequence, coach ratings of role perfor-


mance were obtained for a subsample of the
athletes (n 135).
As was the case for the role efficacy analyses,
ratings of role performance were regressed on
Scope of responsibilities

Scope of responsibilities
Predictor

the four role ambiguity dimensions in separate


multiple regression analyses for offense and
defense (Table 4). For offense, ambiguity re-
Consequences

Consequences

lated to scope of responsibilities was the prin-


Evaluation

Evaluation
Behaviors

Behaviors

cipal predictor of role performance ( .41,


p .001), although role consequences ambigu-
ity also accounted for significant variance (
.20, p .05) in offensive role performance.
The full model accounted for 20% of the ex-
plained variance (adjusted R2 .20), F(4,
130) 9.55, p .001. In the defensive analy-
*** p .001.
Defensive role efficacy
Offensive role efficacy

sis, ambiguity related to scope of responsibili-


ties was the only significant predictor of role
Criterion

performance effectiveness ( .30, adjusted


R2 .05), F(4, 130) 2.63, p .05.
* p .05. ** p .01.

Relationship Between Role Efficacy and


Note. N 271.

Role Performance Effectiveness


Equation

To examine the relationship between role ef-


Table 3

ficacy and role performance, role performance


1

was regressed on role efficacy for both offense


ROLE AMBIGUITY 237

and defense. For offensive role performance,

3.66***

2.31*

2.19*
role efficacy accounted for 15% of the variance

0.74
0.73

0.79
0.03
0.88
t (adjusted R2 .15), F(1, 133) 24.44, p
.001, whereas for defensive role performance,
role efficacy accounted for 13% of the variance
(adjusted R2 .13), F(1, 131) 20.53, p
Adjusted R2

.001.
.20***

.05*
Role Efficacy as a Mediator in the Role
AmbiguityRole Performance Relationship
In accordance with Baron and Kennys
.41
.09
.08
.20

.30
.11
.00
.09
(1986) prescription for testing mediation, linear

regression analyses were conducted to examine


role efficacy as a mediator of the relationship
between role ambiguity and performance. As
0.14
0.14
0.09
0.10

0.18
0.18
0.14
0.13
SEB

was the case above, separate analyses were con-


ducted for offense and defense. Baron and
Kenny suggested that first, the mediator should
Regression Analyses for Variables Predicting Offensive and Defensive Role Performance Effectiveness

be regressed on the independent variable; sec-


0.51
0.11
0.07
0.23

0.39
0.14
0.00
0.11

ond, the dependent variable should be regressed


B

on the independent variable; and third, the de-


pendent variable should be hierarchically re-
gressed on the mediator and then the indepen-
Defensive role ambiguity (complete model)
Offensive role ambiguity (complete model)

dent variable.
Results of the defensive analyses are pre-
sented in Table 5. In the first equation, role
ambiguity accounted for significant variance
(adjusted R2 .22, p .001) in role efficacy. In
Scope of responsibilities

Scope of responsibilities
Predictor

the second equation, role ambiguity accounted


for significant variance (adjusted R2 .05, p
.05) in performance. Results of the third equa-
Consequences

Consequences

tion revealed that role efficacy accounted for


13% of the variance ( p .001) in role perfor-
Evaluation

Evaluation
Behaviors

Behaviors

mance. However, role ambiguity did not ac-


count for significant additional variance ( p
.05) in role performance beyond that accounted
for by role efficacy.
Results of the offensive analyses are also
presented in Table 5. In the first equation, role
Defensive role performance
Offensive role performance

ambiguity accounted for significant variance


(adjusted R2 .31, p .001) in role efficacy. In
the second equation, role ambiguity accounted
Criterion

for significant variance (adjusted R2 .20, p


.001) in role performance. In the third equation,
* p .05. *** p .001.

role efficacy explained 15% of the variance


( p .001) in role performance. After control-
ling for the effect of role efficacy, role ambigu-
Note. N 135.

ity also contributed an additional 8% of the


explained variance ( p .001) in role perfor-
Equation

mance. Thus, in both offensive and defensive


Table 4

analyses, mediation was supportedthe rela-


1

tionships between role ambiguity and role per-


238 BEAUCHAMP, BRAY, EYS, AND CARRON

Table 5
Mediational Analyses for Variables Predicting Offensive and Defensive Role Performance Effectiveness
Equation Criterion Predictor F df Adjusted R2
Defense
1 Role efficacy Role ambiguity (complete model) 10.32 (4, 128) .22***
2 Role performance Role ambiguity (complete model) 2.62 (4, 130) .05*
3 Role performance
Step 1 Role efficacy 20.53 (1, 131) .13***
Step 2 Role ambiguity (complete model) 4.88 (5, 127) .13
Offense
1 Role efficacy Role ambiguity (complete model) 15.87 (4, 130) .31***
2 Role performance Role ambiguity (complete model) 9.55 (4, 130) .20***
3 Role performance
Step 1 Role efficacy 24.44 (1, 133) .15***
Step 2 Role ambiguity (complete model) 9.19 (5, 129) .23***
Note. N 135. For Equation 3 of the defensive model, Step 2 R2 .00. For Equation 3 of the offensive model, Step 2
R2 .08.
* p .05. *** p .001.

formance were reduced substantially when the Consistent with our hypotheses, role ambigu-
effects of the mediator (role efficacy) were con- ity was found to be negatively associated with
trolled (Baron & Kenny, 1986). role performance. That is, for both offense and
defense, higher ambiguity scores were associ-
Discussion ated with lower coach ratings of athlete perfor-
mance. Ambiguity associated with scope of re-
The overall objective of this study was to sponsibilities was the major predictor of offen-
examine the relationships among role ambigu- sive and defensive role performance, although
ity, role-related efficacy, and role performance ambiguity related to role consequences also ac-
within an interdependent sport team setting. A counted for significant variance in offensive
multidimensional, theoretically derived (cf. role performance. A possible explanation for
Kahn et al., 1964) measure was developed to the fact that ambiguity related to scope of re-
assess four different manifestations of role am- sponsibilities was the principal predictor of role
biguityscope of responsibilities, role behav- performance for both offense and defense might
iors, role evaluation, and role consequences be that when individuals are unclear about their
within two specific contexts: offense and de- various responsibilities, they may engage in in-
fense. Furthermore, a mediational model was appropriate task-related strategies. In fact, am-
proposed to test the assertion that role efficacy biguity concerning scope of responsibilities can
beliefs are a mechanism through which role even have a detrimental effect on individuals
ambiguity influences role performance. capable of efficient role performance. As Jack-
Theorizing by Kahn et al. (1964) and re- son and Schuler (1985) noted, regardless of
search from the organizational domain (Tubre effort expenditure, the behaviors of such indi-
& Collins, 2000) contribute to the suggestion viduals are most likely to be inefficient, mis-
that ambiguity should be particularly detrimen- directed or insufficient (p. 43).
tal to performance in situations where an indi- Ambiguity concerning scope of responsibili-
viduals role is characterized by high interde- ties was also the major predictor of role efficacy
pendence. Although previous research has ex- for both offense and defense. Of interest, Eys
amined ambiguity within generic work roles and Carron (2001) found that scope of respon-
(e.g., managerial, administrative), our study sibilities was predictive of task self-efficacy for
represents a first step toward investigating the defensive role responsibilities among a sample
relationship between role ambiguity and perfor- of elite university basketball players. However,
mance within the context of interdependent our results showed that in addition to ambiguity
sport teams. related to scope of responsibilities, two further
ROLE AMBIGUITY 239

manifestations of role ambiguity were also pre- examining the efficacyperformance relation-
dictive of role efficacy. Specifically, for offen- ship in both independent and interdependent
sive play, role evaluation ambiguity was asso- sport tasks, our results support the hypothesized
ciated with role efficacy, a finding that is also positive relationship between role efficacy and
consistent with Eys and Carrons. Furthermore, role performance in an interdependent team
role behavior ambiguity was predictive of role setting.
efficacy for defensive responsibilities. Baron and Kenny (1986) defined a mediator
The fact that role evaluation ambiguity was as the generative mechanism through which
associated with offensive role efficacy and that the focal independent variable is able to influ-
role behavior ambiguity was associated with ence the dependent variable of interest (p.
defensive role efficacy may help to illustrate 1173). Although the association between ambi-
how various forms of role ambiguity can be guity and performance has been proposed by
manifested in the differential role requirements theory and supported by empirical research, lit-
placed on members for offense and defense in tle effort has been made to account for theorized
rugby. For example, Escart and Guzman mediators through which ambiguity may affect
(1999) indicated that effective feedback is es- performance. The current findings provide evi-
sential for the maintenance of personal efficacy. dence of such a mechanism in the form of role
For offensive play, rugby players are required to efficacy. That is, role efficacy was found to be a
carry out a number of responsibilities (e.g., set mediator of the role ambiguityperformance re-
plays and counterattacks). If members are not lationship for both offense and defense. The
clear as to how these different responsibilities mediational interpretation of these relationships
are evaluated, appropriate feedback necessary supports Jackson and Schulers (1985) theoriz-
to the maintenance of personal efficacy may be ing that role ambiguity seems to have motiva-
absent. Thus, consistent with theory (Bandura, tional implications for the individual role occu-
1997), lower levels of role-related efficacy will pant. Results are consistent with the interpreta-
be likely. In contrast to offense, when playing tion that a lack of clarity regarding role
defense, rugby players may be required to carry responsibilities may be further manifested in
out a smaller spectrum of responsibilities. How- beleaguered thoughts about capabilities to
ever, if they are unclear about the behaviors perform effectively, which in turn affect
involved in carrying out these responsibilities performance.
(e.g., how to tackle), they may doubt their ca- Although role efficacy fully mediated the re-
pabilities to carry out those responsibilities lationship between role ambiguity and perfor-
(e.g., The opposition are bound to expose my mance for defense (i.e., it accounted for all of
defensive weaknesses) and so lower levels of the variance in performance), for offense, par-
role efficacy will result. When this is considered tial mediation was observed (i.e., role efficacy
in concert with the ambiguityperformance accounted for some, but not all of the variance
findings, it is clear that when individuals fail to in offensive performance previously explained
understand the breadth of their different role by role ambiguity). Thus, additional mediators
responsibilities, both performance and per- may also be present in the latter relationship
ceived capabilities to perform suffer. (Baron & Kenny, 1986). Jackson and Schuler
Previous research from the sport setting has (1985) suggested that role ambiguity may lead
consistently demonstrated a positive relation- to impairments in performance through both
ship between personal efficacy and competitive cognitive and motivational mechanisms. Future
performance (e.g., George, 1994; Lee, 1982; research should explore other possible mecha-
Treasure, Monson, & Lox, 1996). Although the nisms that may explain how role ambiguity
aforementioned research has focused mainly on affects performance.
individuals performing sport tasks indepen- Although this study represents a promising
dently and not interdependently with team- start to examining the relationships between
mates, recent research by Bray and Brawley multidimensional role ambiguity, role efficacy,
(2000, 2002) also found a positive relationship and performance within the interdependent
between role efficacy and role performance in sport team context, a number of limitations
collegiate basketball. Thus, consistent with the- should be noted. The homogeneous nature of
orizing by Bandura (1997) and past research the sample (i.e., male secondary-school rugby
240 BEAUCHAMP, BRAY, EYS, AND CARRON

players) means that the results cannot be gen- and that performance benefits. By failing to do
eralized to other teams, such as university, elite, so, group leaders risk that their subordinates
recreational, or those made up of female ath- will be ill equipped to perform to their best.
letes. Indeed, future research should seek to Although ambiguity related to scope of re-
examine teams from a variety of sports and sponsibilities was the construct most highly as-
levels, as well as female teams. Our role per- sociated with impaired performance and lower
formance measure might also be considered a levels of role efficacy, future research should
limitation in that it was operationalized through also examine potential dependent variables that
coaches subjective rating of role effectiveness might be associated with the socioemotional
(and, therefore, was subject to bias). One aspects of ambiguity. Given the socioemotional
strength, however, was that consistent with the nature of both role consequences and role eval-
recommendations of Bandura (1997), the mea- uation ambiguity (Kahn et al., 1964), it is pos-
sure of performance was intended to represent a sible that these manifestations of ambiguity will
high degree of contextual congruence with the significantly influence affective variables such
specific form of efficacy being assessed. We as role satisfaction and role commitment to a
saw no valid way to assess role performance greater extent than other forms of ambiguity
objectively; this represents a challenge for fu- (e.g., role behaviors). Another important area
ture researchers. In addition, although the re- for future research is the relationship between
sults of this study point toward a potential ambiguity and group (collective) performance.
mechanism (i.e., role efficacy) through which Indeed, if group leaders are particularly poor at
role ambiguity may affect performance, causa- conveying to their subordinates what is ex-
tion cannot be inferred from the concurrent na- pected, not only is it likely that group members
ture of the research design. Indeed, future re- will be unclear about how to perform specific
search should incorporate both experimental systems, but collective performances will also
and longitudinal designs to test for such causal be affected.
relationships (Baron & Kenny, 1986). In conclusion, the present study provides pre-
Balanced against the aforementioned limita- liminary evidence for the multidimensional
tions, however, is the fact that the study does conceptualization of role ambiguity used in this
make a contribution to theory. That is, support study. Furthermore, the results provide evi-
is provided here for a theory-driven multidi- dence for a mechanism (i.e., role efficacy)
mensional conceptualization of role ambiguity through which role ambiguity may affect role
that allows researchers to tap different types of performance. Future research should continue
role ambiguity perceptions held by members of to use this multidimensional approach to mea-
interdependent groups. Furthermore, although a surement and should seek to examine role am-
number of studies from the organizational do- biguity within interdependent groups across a
main have provided evidence for a negative variety of ages, competitive or organizational
relationship between role ambiguity and perfor- levels, and contexts.
mance, our study points toward a mediational
mechanism (role efficacy) that may explain how
References
role ambiguity affects performance.
The fact that all four manifestations of ambi- Abramis, D. J. (1994). Work role ambiguity, job satis-
guity were predictive of role efficacy and role faction, and job performance: Meta-analysis and re-
performance across the different contexts of view. Psychological Reports, 75, 14111433.
offense and defense also has practical implica- Arbuckle, J. L. (1999). AMOS (Version 4.0) [Com-
tions. Group leaders should be made aware of puter software]. Chicago: SmallWaters.
the different types of ambiguity and how they Bandura, A. (1997). Self-efficacy: The exercise of
are perceived by interdependent group mem- control. New York: Freeman.
Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The modera-
bers. Ensuring that group members understand tormediator variable distinction in social psycho-
their scope of responsibilities, how these re- logical research: Conceptual, strategic, and statis-
sponsibilities are to be enacted, and how they tical considerations. Journal of Personality and
are to be evaluated, as well as the costs and Social Psychology, 51, 11731182.
benefits of unsuccessful and successful role per- Bauer, T. N., & Green, S. G. (1994). Effect of new-
formance, will ensure that efficacy is nurtured comer involvement in work-related activities: A
ROLE AMBIGUITY 241

longitudinal study of socialization. Journal of Ap- George, T. R. (1994). Self-confidence and basketball
plied Psychology, 79, 211223. performance: A causal examination. Journal of
Beauchamp, M. R., & Bray, S. R. (2001). Role am- Sport and Exercise Psychology, 16, 381399.
biguity and role conflict within interdependent Hair, J. F., Anderson, R. E., Tatham, R. L., & Black,
teams. Small Group Research, 32, 133157. W. C. (1998). Multivariate data analysis (5th ed.).
Bentler, P. M., & Bonett, D. G. (1980). Significance Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall.
tests and goodness of fit in the analysis of covari- Hu, L., & Bentler, P. M. (1995). Evaluating model fit.
ance structures. Psychological Bulletin, 88, 588 In R. H. Hoyle (Ed.), Structural equation model-
606. ing: Concepts, issues, and applications (pp. 76
Berkowitz, E. N. (1980). Role theory, attitudinal con- 99). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
structs, and actual performance: A measurement Jackson, S. E., & Schuler, R. S. (1985). A meta-
issue. Journal of Applied Psychology, 65, 240 analysis and conceptual critique of research on role
245. ambiguity and role conflict in work settings. Or-
Bray, S. R. (1998). Role efficacy within interdepen- ganizational Behavior and Human Decision Pro-
dent teams: Measurement development and tests of cesses, 36, 16 78.
theory. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Univer- Joreskog, K. G. (1969). A general approach to con-
sity of Waterloo, Waterloo, Ontario, Canada. firmatory maximum likelihood factor analysis.
Bray, S. R., & Brawley, L. R. (2000). Role efficacy Psychometrika, 34, 183202.
and perceived role performance relationships: Kahn, R. L., Wolfe, D. M., Quinn, R. P., Snoek, J. D.,
Longitudinal evidence for reciprocality. Journal of & Rosenthal, R. A. (1964). Organizational stress:
Sport and Exercise Psychology, 22(Suppl.), S22. Studies in role conflict and ambiguity. New York:
Bray, S. R., & Brawley, L. R. (2002). Role clarity, Wiley.
role efficacy, and role performance effectiveness. King, L. A., & King, D. W. (1990). Role conflict and
Small Group Research, 33, 245265. role ambiguity: A critical assessment of construct
Carmines, E. G., & McIver, J. P. (1981). Analyzing validity. Psychological Bulletin, 107, 48 64.
Lagace, R. R. (1988). Role-stress differences be-
models with unobserved variables: Analysis of co-
tween salesmen and saleswomen: Effect of job
variance structures. In G. W. Bohrnstedt & E. F.
satisfaction and performance. Psychological Re-
Borgatta (Eds.), Social measurement: Current is-
ports, 62, 815 825.
sues (pp. 65115). Beverley Hills, CA: Sage.
Lee, C. (1982). Self-efficacy as a predictor of perfor-
Carron, A. V., & Brawley, L. R. (2000). Cohesion:
mance in competitive gymnastics. Journal of Sport
Conceptual and measurement issues. Small Group
Psychology, 4, 405 409.
Research, 31, 89 106.
Mabry, E. A., & Barnes, R. E. (1980). The dynamics
Carron, A. V., & Hausenblas, H. (1998). Group dy- of small group communication. Englewood Cliffs,
namics in sport (2nd ed). Morgantown, WV: Fit- NJ: Prentice Hall.
ness Information Technology. Nunnally, J. C. (1978). Psychometric theory. New
Escart, A., & Guzman, J. (1999). Effects of feedback York: McGraw-Hill.
on self-efficacy, performance, and choice in an Organ, D. W., & Green, C. N. (1981). The effects of
athletic task. Journal of Applied Sport Psychol- formalization on professional involvement: A
ogy, 11, 8396. compensatory process approach. Administrative
Estabrooks, P. A., & Carron, A. V. (2000). The Science Quarterly, 26, 237252.
physical activity group environment questionnaire: Rhoads, G. K., Singh, J., & Goodell, P. W. (1994).
An instrument for the assessment of cohesion in The multiple dimensions of role ambiguity and
exercise classes. Group Dynamics: Theory, Re- their impact upon psychological and behavioral
search, and Practice, 4, 230 243. outcomes of industrial salespeople. Journal of Per-
Eys, M., & Carron, A. V. (2001). Role ambiguity, sonal Selling and Sales Management, 14, 124.
task cohesion, and task self-efficacy. Small Group Rizzo, J., House, R. J., & Lirtzman, S. I. (1970). Role
Research, 32, 356 373. conflict and ambiguity in complex organizations.
Feltz, D. L., & Chase, M. A. (1998). The measure- Administrative Science Quarterly, 15, 150 163.
ment of self-efficacy and confidence in sport. In Singh, J. (1993). Boundary role ambiguity: Facets,
J. L. Duda (Ed.), Advances in sport and exercise determinants, and impacts. Journal of Market-
psychology measurement (pp. 65 80). Morgan- ing, 57, 1131.
town, WV: Fitness Information Technology. Sohi, R. S., Smith, D. C., & Ford, N. M. (1996). How
Fisher, C. D., & Gitelson, R. (1983). A meta-analysis does sharing a sales force between multiple divi-
of the correlates of role conflict and ambiguity. sions affect salespeople? Journal of the Academy
Journal of Applied Psychology, 68, 320 333. of Marketing Science, 24, 195207.
Forsyth, D. R. (1999). Group dynamics (3rd ed.). Szilagyi, A. D. (1977). An empirical test of causal in-
Belmont, CA: Wadsworth. ference between role perceptions, satisfaction with
242 BEAUCHAMP, BRAY, EYS, AND CARRON

work, performance and organization level. Person- between role ambiguity, role conflict, and job perfor-
nel Psychology, 30, 375388. mance. Journal of Management, 26, 155169.
Terry, D. J., Nielsen, M., & Perchard, L. (1993). Effects Van Sell, M., Brief, A. P., & Schuler, R. S. (1981).
of work stress on psychological well-being and job Role conflict and role ambiguity: Integration of the
satisfaction: The stress buffering role of social sup- literature and directions for future research. Hu-
port. Australian Journal of Psychology, 45, 168 175. man Relations, 34, 4371.
Treasure, D. C., Monson, J., & Lox, C. L. (1996). Williams, M. L., Podsakoff, P. M., & Huber, V.
Relationship between self-efficacy, wrestling (1992). Effects of group-level and individual
performance, and affect prior to competition. Sport level variation in leader behaviors on subordi-
Psychologist, 10, 73 83. nate attitudes and performance. Journal of Oc-
Tubre, T. C., & Collins, J. M. (2000). Jackson and Schuler cupational and Organizational Psychology, 65,
(1985) revisited: A meta-analysis of the relationships 115129.

Appendix

Role Ambiguity Scale


Items were worded with reference to the specific Role Evaluation Ambiguity
contexts measured (e.g., offense: I understand the
scope of my offensive responsibilities). (R) indicates I understand the criteria by which my role respon-
items that were reverse scored. sibilities are evaluated.
I understand how my role is evaluated.
It is clear to me how my role responsibilities are
Ambiguity Related to Scope of Responsibilities evaluated
I understand the extent of my responsibilities I am unclear about the way in which my offensive
I understand the scope of my responsibilities role responses are evaluated. (R)
I understand all of my responsibilities. The criteria by which my offensive role is evalu-
I am unclear about the breadth of my responsibil- ated are clear to me.
ities. (R)
I am clear about the different responsibilities that Role Consequences Ambiguity
make up my role.
It is clear to me what happens if I fail to carry out
my role responsibilities.
Role Behavior Ambiguity I understand the consequences of failing to carry
out my role responsibilities.
I understand what adjustments to my behavior
I am unclear about the consequences of failing to
need to be made to carry out my role. carry out my role responsibilities. (R)
I understand the behaviors I must perform to carry I understand the consequences of unsuccessful role
out my role. performance.
I know what behaviors are necessary to carry out I know what will happen if I dont perform my role
my responsibilities. responsibilities.
It is clear what behaviors I should perform to fulfill
my role. Received April 23, 2001
I am unclear what behaviors are expected of me in Revision received March 19, 2002
order to carry out my role. (R) Accepted March 19, 2002

Você também pode gostar