Você está na página 1de 4

Okay, so, we've been talking about our

first order neurons and their organization in the periphery and just a little bit
about their organization as they enter the central nervous system.
We're going to devote an entire tutorial to following the pathway by which the
first order neuron eventually communicates with higher structure in the fore-brain.
So, we won't look at those details now, but I do want to jump to identify what are
those centers, that are involved in processing mechanism sensation in the
for-brain and give you just a little bit about the physiology.
So let's begin with talking about the somatic sensory division of the thalamus.
So, there's a brief tutorial that talks about the thalamus to get you oriented to
this part of the forebrain. For now I'll just remind you that the
thalamus is derived from the diencephalon which is part of the forebrain that the
prosencephalon and it's the more dorsal posterior part of the diencephalon with a
hypothalamus being more ventral interior. Well the thalamus as my other tutorial
goes into it more detailed is divided into a number of nucleoli and those nucleoli
relate to a particular part of the cerebral cortex.
So if we were to look at the cerebral cortex we could identify just about any
region that we may decide to poke at, and ask, what is the thalamic nucleus that
projects to this region of the cerebral cortex?
And, and we'll have a different answer for most different sites in the cerebral
mantle that we might inquire about. Well, if we focus on the postcentral
gyrus, which is right here that's, as we'll talk about in just a moment, is our
primary somatic sensory cortex. There's a particular part of the thalamus
that maintains interconnections with this postcentral gyrus.
That part is exactly the part of the thalamus that receives the ascending
information about mechano-sensation. That part is what we might call the
somatic sensory thalamus. And we have some anatomical names to give
it. We call it the ventral posterior complex
of the thalamus. And that ventral posterior complex is
divided into two main nuclei. There is a division called the ventral
posterior lateral nucleus, and a division called the ventral posterior medial
nucleus. And the division there bascially reflects
the terminations of our, pairs of pathways that are representing the body from the
posterior part of the head on down. And then a different pathway conveying
mechanosensation for the face. So for the body below the back of the
head, we have signals arriving in the ventral posterior lateral nucleus.
Where our signals concerning mechanosensory activation of facial
structures arrive in our ventral posterior medial nucleus.
So now we're ready to consider what happens to the somatic sensory
information. As the thalamus communicates it up to the
level of the cerebral cortex, and that gets us into our postcentral gyrus.
So here, once again, is the postcentral gyrus in my model of the human brain here.
And this is where we're going to find the region of cortex that we call the primary
somatic sensory cortex. The reason why it's primary is that it's
the first part of the cortex to receive input about mechanosensory information.
And in anatomical terms, it's the part of the cortex that receives input from the
somatosensory thalamus. That is, the ventral posterior lateral and
ventral posterior medial nuclei . And as we'll see, the ventral posterior
lateral nucleus projects from the para-central lobule in the mid line of the
hemisphere, along this post central gyrus as it extends across the dorsal and
lateral aspects of the cerebral hemisphere.
Until we get to about two thirds the distance between the mid line and the
lateral fissure. When we get to about this region now the
rest of the postcentral gyrus is receiving input not from the VPL but from the VPM,
the ventral posterior medial nucleus. This explains why, in the mapping of the
body that we're, we will see in just a moment, there's a disjunction between the
representation of the hand and the representation of the face.
It's basically the boundary between the projection zone from the VPL and the VPM.
Okay, well let's look at this now in a bit more detail.
So here we have an illustration from your text which shows us our primary semantic
sensory cortex which we call s1 for short. So it's found in the post central gyrus.
And it's comprised of four cytoarchitect tectoniclly distinct regions of the human
brain that Korbinian Brodmann named over 100 years ago.
Areas two, one, and areas three and workers after Brodmann subdivided area
summary into an area 3A in an area 3B. So from posterior to interior, they're
areas 2, 1, 3b, then 3a right at the depth of the central sulcus.
So in each of these cortical areas we have a complete representation of the
opposite
side of the body, and that representation Is beautifully mapped out.
We call this somatotopy which means body mapping.
And the way the body mapping works is that the distal part of the lower extremity
is
represented in the midline. Right at the medial termination of the
post-central gyrus in this region that we call the paracentral lobule.
So this is essentially where we find the representation of the contralateral foot.
And as we progress over the dorsal rim of the hemisphere And on to the dorsal
lateral surface of the postcentral gyrus, the body map progresses from the foot up
the lower extremity, and then into the region of the trunk.
And then from the region of the trunk, we encounter the shoulder, and then we get
into the upper extremity. And where the central sulcus has a fairly
distinctive curve in it, that's the region where we find an expansive
representation
of the contralateral hand. Now, notice we have not yet encountered
the face. That's because as I just mentioned.
The face is conveyed by inputs from the ventral posterior medial nucleus of the
thalamus, which terminates in this inferior segment of the postcentral gyrus.
So just below this S-shaped bend in the central sulcus we have a junction between.
The representation of the hand and the representation of the face.
The illustration that we have in front of us shows us the detail of that body
mapping and it truly is an exquisite mapping of the body.
With the progression from lower extremity through the upper extremity.
And then, a disjunction, where we then have a break.
And the representation of the face in the inferior third or so of the post central
gyrus. Now, there's a wonderful story about, a
famous neurosurgeon at the Montreal Neurological Institute, Wilder Penfield
and his assistant. And, there's some discussion as to how
much of this is really true or not. But evidently the assistant is the one who
suggested to Dr. Pennfield that this distorted
representation of the human body be represented in a simple cartoon.
And so that's the cartoon that we see here in panel c.
This is called the little man, or the homunculus.
And this is a illustration you've probably seen before.
It's intended to communicate the idea that, in the post central gyrii, taking
the two hemispheres together We have a complete representation of the body, but
that representation is distorted. Those body parts that provide us with the
greatest spacial acuity and the greatest sensitivity to mechano-sensory stimuli are
hugely represented in the brain. And they're represented in these
distinctive features of the postcentral gyrus.
The hands are represented where the central sulcus has that S shape where the
two gyri interdigitate against each other. And then the face is represented in that
inferior segment of the postcentral gyrus. Whereas the remaining parts of the body
are not nearly as magnified. We call this phenomenon cortical
magnification, which has to do with how much more does cortical circuitry magnify
the representation of the body that's otherwise related to the density of
peripheral receptors, the density of neurons at all the antecedent stations in
the somatic sensory pathway. Well the up shot here is that the
homunculus makes very clear that the important somatic sensory surfaces for us
as humans would be our hands and our face especially the facial structures around
the mouth and the tips of our fingers. Now before we leave the primary
somatosensory cortex, I do just want to comment on the functions of these four
paralell body maps. You, you may find that curious, and
frankly, a lot of us find it somewhat curious.
But there do seem to be distinctive receptive field properties that are
represented In these four different maps of the body.
One might suggest that area three, being subdivided into three a and three b is
truly the primary somatic sensory cortex, from a physiological perspective.
Whereas areas one and two seem to process a more higher.
Order aspect of somatic sensation. Area 3a is especially concerned with our
proprioceptors. Those receptors that are found in deeper
structures, such as our muscle spindles, and our golgi tendon organs and our joint
receptors. Where as area 3B is more concerned with
cutaneous receptors. So area 3B is more cutaneous, is going to
be more concerned with like touch for example the then the movements of our
muscles and joints. Now once we get into areas wanted to wear
encountering more complex aspects of somatic sensory processing.
Area one for example is going to respond to complex stimuli activating multiple
skin surfaces. For example, over multiple fingertips and
perhaps responding selectively to a particular pattern of motion.
Our fingers passing over a counter surface from left to right, for example.
Area two seems to be concerned more so with the shapes of objects that we
encounter. So it suggests there is some kind of
computation of lower level signals into more higher order signals that begin to
build up a sense of structure. In our somatic sensory interactions with
the world. Okay, and so finally let's consider what
happens to this information beyond the primary somatic sensory cortex.
What we discovered is that the primary somatic sensory cortex being at this level
here. Has extensive interconnections with other
parts of the brain. There is something that we recognize as a
secondary somatic sensory cortex. That is in the inferior and posterior
aspect of the parietal lobe perhaps also involving the posterior aspect of the
insula. And this secondary somatosensory cortex
receives input from our primary areas. And from there, that information is
further elaborated in a hgher order sort of way, and passed into structures in the
medial part of the temporal lobe that are going to be important for making
memories.
And for interacting with our emotional networks.
So, that involves the hippocampus and the amygdala respectively.
Areas one and two, likewise project into our secondary somatic sensory cortex, but
area two in particular, the one concerned with more complex geometrical aspects of
somatic sensation projects into parts of the parietal lobe that sit just posterior
to it in the superior parietal labial and these parts of the parietal cortex seem
to
be especially concerned with our orientation relative to the environment.
So there's a, a spatial mapping that's going on here in this part of the brain.
And that mapping is used to drive our motor system to interact with the world in
a appropriate way so, for example imagine a visually guided reach that is a means
of
directing or somatic sensory surfaces towards a particular goal.
Well that's going to be integrated through parietal and frontal networks, most
likely
involving the superior parietal lobule region.
Okay. Well, this has been a very quick sense of
cortical processing in the somatic sensory system.
There are so many big questions that we have yet to address.
Such as how does perception actually work. How is it that we can have for example
the
inner grated sensation that one might experience when holding a snow ball or an
ice cube or maybe immersing a finger in a cup of tea.
Consider those experiences were combing a sensation of shape with structure with a
sense of texture and a sense of temperature and just maybe a sense of pain
as well. I've been emphasizing the concept of label
lines and parallel processing But at some point in our perception we unite these
labeled lines into a solitary percept. And it's still not clear to
neuroscientists how that unity of representation occurs.
There may not be an anatomical explanation for that perceptual sense of unity.
There may be other physiological means to build up that percept and this is all
under active investigation. So hopefully this tutorial has given you a
framework for understanding how mechanicization works and will also help
you to put the anatomical pieces together when we look in a different tutorial in
detail at the organization of the somatic sensory pathways.
So I'll see you then when we consider that interestting anatomy that gets us from
skin to cortex. Well, finally, before I leave you to this
tutorial, I want to suggest a, study question that begins to apply this
knowledge in the clinical domain. I want you to consider what would happen
If a patient were to suffer a stroke involving the middle cerebral artery.
And I want you to think about such a stroke occurring in the right hemisphere.
Okay? Now, I've given you a bunch of options to
think through, and they all begin with this word, anesthesia.
And anesthesia implies lack of somatic sensory sensation, particularly in the
domain of mechano-sensation. So consider these options, and go ahead
and indicate in one form or another, what you consider to be the best choice among
this set of options. I'll see you next time.

Você também pode gostar