Você está na página 1de 12

Learning and Instruction 28 (2013) 12e23

Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect

Learning and Instruction


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/learninstruc

School context, achievement motivation, and academic engagement:


A longitudinal study of school engagement using a multidimensional
perspective
Ming-Te Wang a, *, Jacquelynne S. Eccles b
a
University of Pittsburgh, 5940 Wesley W. Posvar Hall, Pittsburgh, PA 15260, USA
b
University of Michigan, USA

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: This longitudinal study adopts a multidimensional perspective to examine the relationships between
Received 30 August 2012 middle school students perceptions of the school environment (structure support, provision of choice,
Received in revised form teaching for relevance, teacher and peer emotional support), achievement motivation (academic self-
18 April 2013
concept and subjective task value), and school engagement (behavioral, emotional, and cognitive
Accepted 20 April 2013
engagement). Participants were from an ethnically diverse, urban sample of 1157 adolescents. The
ndings indicated that student perceptions of distinct aspects of the school environment contributed
Keywords:
differentially to the three types of school engagement. In addition, these associations were fully or
Adolescence
School environment perceptions
partially mediated by achievement motivation. Specically, student perceptions of the school environ-
School engagement ment inuenced their achievement motivation and in turn inuenced all three types of school
Achievement motivation engagement, although in different ways. Moderation effects of gender, ethnicity, and academic ability
Multidimensional perspective were also discussed.
2013 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction student engagement in school is inuenced by the degree to which


they perceive that the school context meets their psychological
Active engagement in school is critical to student educational needs (Connell & Wellborn, 1991; Deci & Ryan, 2000; Krapp, 2005).
success (Finn & Rock, 1997; Wang & Holcombe, 2010). Youth must Stage-environment t and expectancy-value theorists (Eccles et al.,
be actively engaged with their school education in order to acquire 1993; Roeser, Eccles, & Sameroff, 1998) further argue that the failure
the knowledge and skills required for a successful transition into of schools to meet the psychological needs of adolescents often
postsecondary programs and careers (Wang & Eccles, 2012a, leads to declines in academic motivation and interest, which in turn
2012b). School engagement is also a malleable state that can be contributes to decreased school engagement and poor academic
shaped by school context, therefore holding tremendous potential performance as adolescents transition to middle school. Most of the
as a locus for interventions (Appleton, Christenson, Kim, & Reschly, extant research, however, has failed to capture the dynamic and
2006; Jimmerson, Campos, & Grief, 2003). Currently, and particu- interactive nature of engagement. Specically, few empirical
larly at the secondary level, increasing student engagement is an studies have focused on how school characteristics interact with
explicit goal of many school reform efforts aimed at addressing and inuence various forms of school engagement simultaneously.
problems of student boredom and alienation, low achievement, Moreover, research has not yet fully explained the actual process
and high dropout rate (Marks, 2000). that accounts for the effect of the school environment on student
In order to promote school engagement, we must rst better engagement. It is unclear whether various aspects of the school
understand the school factors that inuence student engagement. environment inuence the behavioral, emotional, and cognitive
Self-determination theorists suggest that individuals seek experi- engagement differentially and whether the associations between
ences that fulll their fundamental needs and identities through the school environment and engagement are mediated by more
their interaction with the environment. According to this view, fundamental motivational beliefs within the student. Therefore,
there is a critical need for research that takes an integrative moti-
vational approach to investigate the contextual and psychological
* Corresponding author. Tel.: 4126246945. factors that predict school engagement (Fredricks, Blumenfeld, &
E-mail address: mtwang@pitt.edu (M.-T. Wang). Paris, 2004). Such research could contribute to the knowledge

0959-4752/$ e see front matter 2013 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.learninstruc.2013.04.002
M.-T. Wang, J.S. Eccles / Learning and Instruction 28 (2013) 12e23 13

base that informs effective school practices and to the efforts of individual experiences oneself as the source of action. Autonomy is
researchers and educators who do the important work of identi- supported when a student perceives schoolwork as relevant to his
fying the optimal developmental correlates of school engagement. or her interests and goals or when a student experiences choice in
In this study we adopt a multidimensional perspective, using a determining his or her own behavior (Assor, Kaplan, & Roth, 2002).
large-scale sample of ethnically diverse students to investigate the Finally, relatedness refers to the need to experience oneself as
longitudinal associations among school environment, achievement connected to other people (Connell & Wellborn, 1991). Fulllment
motivation, and school engagement during the middle school of the need for relatedness is likely to occur when teachers and
years. In particular, we expand on previous research by examining peers create a caring and supportive environment.
whether school environment inuences student engagement in Expectancy-value theory provides a theoretical foundation for a
school both directly and indirectly through achievement motiva- mediational model that links school characteristics to school
tion and whether these associations differ by student gender, engagement and performance through student motivational beliefs
ethnicity, and academic ability. The study of school engagement as (i.e., academic self-concept and subjective task values). According
a multidimensional construct, and as an interaction between the to expectancy-value theory, achievement-related choices such as
individual and the school environment, will aid in identifying the school engagement are inuenced psychologically by the in-
particular school characteristics that foster student engagement dividuals expectation for success and subjective valuing of the
and increase our understanding of the mechanisms through which academic work (see Eccles, 2007); students most likely to engage in
they operate. school learning place higher value and have greater condence in
their academic abilities than those who do not. Expectancy-value
1.1. Theoretical frameworks for school engagement theory also links individual differences in motivational beliefs to
the experiences that individuals have in school contexts. Teachers
Self-determination theory and stage-environment t theory posit create opportunities for students to engage in a variety of school
that engagement is manifested in the quality of students in- activities (Eccles et al., 1993; Eccles & Wigeld, 2002), and these
teractions with learning activities and academic tasks (Deci & Ryan, experiences provide students with information regarding their
2000; Eccles, 2004; Skinner & Wellborn, 1994). Engagement is thus competency to succeed, their relatedness to others in that setting,
conceptualized as consisting of multiple distinguishable features, and their autonomy as learnersdallowing them to realize their
including behavior, emotion, and cognition (Fredricks et al., 2004; personal and social identities (Connell & Wellborn, 1991). This in-
Jimmerson et al., 2003). Behavioral engagement refers to the actions formation cumulates to inuence the development of self-concepts
and practices that students direct toward school and learning, of ones ability and subjective task values for the types of activities
including positive conduct and absence of disruptive behavior, as to which the student is exposed. These motivational beliefs, in turn,
well as involvement in learning and academic tasks (Connell, 1990; inuence student engagement in various educational activities
Finn, 1989). Emotional engagement represents a students positive (Simpkins, Davis-Kean, & Eccles, 2006). Drawing on these theo-
affective reactions to, interest in, and valuing of school activities retical frameworks, school engagement results from an interaction
(Voelkl, 1997). Cognitive engagement refers to students cognitive of the individual with his/her context and is responsive to both
investment in learning, including mental efforts directed toward variations in factors of the school environment and motivational
learning, use of self-regulated strategies to learn and master con- characteristics (see Fig. 1).
cepts, and willingness to exert necessary efforts for comprehension
of complex ideas (Corno & Mansinach, 1983; Zimmerman, 1990). 1.2. The link between perception of school environment and school
These three components of school engagement are dynamically engagement
embedded within the individual and provide a rich characteriza-
tion of how students act, feel, and think (Wang & Peck, 2013). Research suggests that the t between adolescents psycholog-
School engagement is optimized when students perceive that ical needs and their school environment inuences both motivation
the school context fullls their needs for competence, autonomy, and school engagement (Alonso-Tapia & Pardo, 2006; Skinner,
and relatedness (Connell & Wellborn, 1991; Deci & Ryan, 2000). Furrer, Marchand, & Kindermann, 2008; Wigeld, Eccles, Davis-
Competence refers to the need to experience oneself as effective in Kean, Roeser, & Scheifele, 2006). Self-determination theorists and
ones interactions with the social environment (Elliot & Dweck, stage-environment t theorists argue that t is optimized when the
2005), and a students need for competence is fullled when they school context provides adequate support for the development and
know how to effectively achieve desired outcomes (Skinner & maintenance of a students sense of competence, autonomy, and
Belmont, 1993). Autonomy refers to the extent to which an relatedness (Deci & Ryan, 2000; Eccles et al., 1993; Wigeld et al.,

School Factors Individual and Psychological Factors Academic Outcomes


Individual
Characteristics
Gender, Race/Ethnicity,
Academic ability

School Characteristics
1. School structure support School Engagement
Achievement Motivation
2. Provision of choice Behavioral engagement
Academic self-concept
3. Teaching for relevance Emotional engagement
Subjective task value
4. Teacher emotional support Cognitive engagement
5. Peer emotional support

Over Time

Fig. 1. Conceptual model.


14 M.-T. Wang, J.S. Eccles / Learning and Instruction 28 (2013) 12e23

2006). Here we integrate the academic and social aspects of the environment, students are more willing to open ideas up for dis-
school environment and discuss how these school characteristics cussion, demonstrate more positive attitudes toward academic
either foster or inhibit the fulllment of the basic psychological studies, and express feelings of enjoyment as they can freely ex-
needs of adolescents, which in turn impacts their engagement in press themselves and count on teachers for support with a range of
school. problems (e.g., Furrer & Skinner, 2003; Shim, Cho, & Wang, 2013).
School structure support. The need for competence is met when Thus, we expect that feeling emotionally supported by teachers will
school is experienced as being optimally structured (Skinner & increase student behavioral, emotional, and cognitive engagement
Belmont, 1993). Structure refers to the school experiences being (Hypothesis 1d).
organized in such a way that a student comes to know what it Peer emotional support is also important for school engagement,
means and what it takes to be successful in that context (Skinner & especially during adolescence when youth have a great need for
Belmont, 1993). When teachers are clear about their expectations, peer relatedness. Several studies have demonstrated that adoles-
provide consistent responses, and adjust instructional strategies to cents who have positive interactions with peers are more engaged
the level of the student, they provide structure, which supports both behaviorally and emotionally in school (e.g., Wentzel, 2003).
greater behavioral participation in academic tasks, and they foster a These associations are most likely due to feeling supported and
stronger sense of connectedness to school amongst students cared for by ones peers, fullling the adolescent need for related-
(Connell, 1990; Urdan & Midgley, 2003). Indeed, Skinner and ness and promoting the development of a sense of satisfaction in
Belmont (1993) found that students whose teachers are clear in school. Although there is little evidence that peer emotional sup-
their expectations and provide consistent responses are more port is related to cognitive engagement, we are inclined to believe
behaviorally and emotionally engaged. Thus, we expect that school that when adolescents feel peer acceptance for academic achieve-
structure support will encourage behavioral engagement and ment, they develop both condence and competence in discussing
enhance emotional engagement (Hypothesis 1a). points of view and critiquing each others work. Thus, we expect
Teachers can support autonomy by allowing students latitude in that peer emotional support will be positively associated with
their learning activities and by making connections between school behavioral, emotional, and cognitive engagement (Hypothesis 1e).
activities and students personal interests and goals (Skinner &
Belmont, 1993; Skinner et al., 2008). We examine two aspects of 1.3. Achievement motivation as a mediator between school
school environment in relation to support of student autonomy: environment and engagement
provision of choice and teaching for relevance to the student.
Provision of choice involves students perceptions that their We draw on expectancy-value theories of motivation (Eccles,
teachers provide opportunities for participation in decision making 2009) in predicting that students motivational beliefs will
related to academic tasks and school governance and that they mediate the association between perceived school characteristics
allow for student input into class discussion. These perceived and engagement. We assume that student motivation will be
characteristics predict greater behavioral engagement with school highest when the academic demands of school are a good t for
tasks and higher school identication (Katz & Assor, 2006; Reeve, student expectations of success, educational values and goals, and
Bolt, & Cai, 1999). Eccles et al. (1993) argued that the lack of individual developmental needs.
decision-making opportunities for students help to explain de- Academic self-concept. Self-concept of academic success refers to
clines in both interest and valuing of school during the transition to an individuals beliefs regarding their capacity to succeed on spe-
middle school. Provision of choice can promote school engage- cic academic tasks (Eccles, 2009). There is growing evidence that
ment: giving students opportunities to practice their decision- positive perceptions of the school environment lead to students
making skills, to regulate their behavior, and to experience a having higher levels of condence in their academic self-concept
sense of personal satisfaction and responsibility as they exert in- (Vollmeyer & Rheinberg, 2000; Wigeld et al., 2006). Teachers
uence upon their learning environment (Assor et al., 2002). who emphasize task structure and relevance foster both a positive
Therefore, we expect that teacher support for student choice and sense of academic self-concept and positive school behaviors,
input will promote behavioral and emotional engagement (Hy- whereas teachers who fail to provide clear structure and guidelines
pothesis 1b). for students and who impose meaningless and uninteresting ac-
Teaching for relevance. Students feel a sense of autonomy when tivities are in danger of hindering such positive developmental
doing work that, rather than simply fullling school requirements, outcomes (Assor et al., 2002; Roeser et al., 1998). Similarly, when
relates to their interests and has personal meaning (Roeser et al., students feel that they are cared for and treated with respect by
1998); that is, when students believe that the content of the cur- their teachers and peers, they are more likely to develop greater
riculum and the design of instruction provide opportunities for condence in their academic abilities (Goodenow, 1993; Murdock &
self-exploration and when the activities provided are meaningful, Miller, 2003). Furthermore, when students perceive classroom
relevant, and related to personal interests and goals (Finn & Voelkl, material as related to personal interests or goals, they feel more
1993), they feel a sense of autonomy. Additionally, meaningful condent about mastering that material. In terms of provision of
curricula and instruction provide an appropriate level of challenge choice, it is not yet known whether providing students with
and a context that can encourage diverse cognitive strategies (e.g., decision-making opportunities regarding school governance and
opinion formulation, comprehension, and connection) and pro- course material inuences academic condence. Finally, students
mote cognitive engagement (Helme & Clarke, 2001; Wigeld et al., sense of academic self-concept inuences how they behave, how
2006). Therefore, we expect that teaching for relevance to the they feel, and how they thinkdthe three dimensions of engage-
student will encourage both emotional and cognitive engagement ment (Eccles, 2009), and indeed, student condence in personal
(Hypothesis 1c). academic ability has been shown to predict level of school
Teacher emotional support has been positively associated with engagement and task involvement (Eccles & Wigeld, 2002;
different indicators of behavioral engagement, including higher Schunk, 1996).
participation in school activities (Battistich, Soloman, Watson, & Subjective task value of school learning. Subjective task value is
Schaps, 1997) and fewer disruptive behaviors (Patrick, Ryan, & composed of beliefs regarding how enjoyable a task will be, how
Kaplan, 2007; Ryan & Patrick, 2001; Wang, Brinkworth, & Eccles, useful a task is for fullling short- and long-term goals, and how
2013). Similarly, in an emotionally supportive and caring school well a task meets personal needs and assists the realization of
M.-T. Wang, J.S. Eccles / Learning and Instruction 28 (2013) 12e23 15

personal identities (Eccles et al., 1993; Eccles & Wigeld, 2002). autonomy can reinforce a sense of competence, autonomy, and
Several studies have shown that students perceptions of their academic condence amongst high performing students while
school environment predict the value that students attach to increasing anxiety and a sense of helplessness amongst low per-
school. Students who experience emotional support and respect forming students (Patrick, Skinner, & Connell, 1993). Low per-
from teachers and peers are more likely to attach greater value to forming students may therefore require greater structure and
learning in school (Roeser et al., 1998). Similarly, when students feel support regarding academic achievement. This support may come
that their teachers convey reasonable and clear expectations, pro- from greater clarication of teacher expectations, consistency and
vide appropriate instrumental help, and support their autonomy, predictability of response, and the employment of less complex
they are more likely to value a task and experience positive feelings teaching strategies (Skinner & Belmont, 1993). Yet few studies have
toward it (Assor et al., 2002; Grolnick, Ryan, & Deci, 1991). examined the moderation effect of student academic ability when
Furthermore, there is strong evidence that the subjective value a examining associations between school environment, motivation,
student places on a task inuences the likelihood of his/her selec- and engagement. Including academic ability as a moderator will
tion and participation in the task (Deci & Ryan, 2000; Updegraff, allow us to determine whether the level of student engagement in
Eccles, Barber, & OBrien, 1996). Research shows that motivation school is commensurate with demonstrated academic ability.
to do schoolwork that is based on interest or value is associated Taken together, it is evident that there is much to be gained by
with various types of school engagement, including active behav- considering individual differences in gender, ethnicity, and aca-
ioral participation, interest, and self-regulated learning (Connell, demic ability when examining the relations among school char-
1990; Elliot, McGregor, & Gable, 1999; Katz & Assor, 2006). acteristics, motivational beliefs, and school engagement. Greater
Accordingly, we expect students who place high subjective task understanding of these relations will enable researchers and edu-
value on academic work at school will report higher levels of all cators to promote the specic contextual factors known to enhance
three types of school engagement. student motivational beliefs and engagement in school among
different types of students.
1.4. Moderation effects of gender, ethnicity, and academic ability
1.5. Research questions and hypotheses
When either achievement patterns or perceived school envi-
ronments are examined by gender, ethnicity, or academic ability, Drawing on self-determination theory, stage-environment t the-
there are often signicant group differences (Malecki & Demaray, ory, and expectancy-value theory, we investigate how student per-
2003; Meece, Glienke, & Burg, 2006; Oates, 2009; Wang, 2009). ceptions of school characteristics predict school engagement when
For instance, overall, girls report higher levels of school engage- demographic characteristics and prior school engagement are
ment than boys regardless of what type of engagement is consid- controlled for. The three specic research questions of the study are
ered (Johnson, Crosnoe, & Elder, 2001; Martin, 2004). However, to examine whether: (Research Question1) student perceptions of
according to extant research, the signicance of the role of group school environment are related differentially to the three di-
differences (e.g., gender and ethnicity) as a moderator of school mensions of school engagement (i.e., behavioral, emotional, and
effects on student engagement is unclear. cognitive engagement); (Research Question 2) students motiva-
Little is known about the role that classroom management or tional beliefs (academic self-concept and subjective task value)
instructional strategies may play in the development of gender mediate the association between school environment and school
differences in motivation and school engagement. As students engagement; and (Research Question 3) the strength of associa-
make the transition to middle school, they perceive school as ori- tions among school environment, motivation, and school engage-
ented toward teacher control and a social comparison-based ment differ by gender, ethnicity, or academic ability.
approach to evaluating student ability (Urdan & Midgley, 2003). For the rst research question, we hypothesize that each aspect
While these perceptions of school characteristics can inhibit the of school environment predicts the three types of school engage-
motivation and engagement of both genders in school, some ment differentially (Hypothesis 1). For the second research ques-
studies suggest that girls do not respond as well to competitive tion, we hypothesize that student perceptions of the school
teaching practices (Eccles, 2007). Recent studies show that girls and environment directly and indirectly predict the level of school
boys experience different levels of support from teachers and peers engagement through personal motivation beliefs (Hypothesis 2).
in school (Rueger, Malecki, & Demaray, 2010); thus, it is likely that Finally, in the absence of a strong empirical basis for making pre-
teacher and peer support may have different effects on academic dictions about whether the associations between perceived school
adjustment for each gender. environment, motivational beliefs, and school engagement will
There is mixed evidence regarding ethnic differences when function differently for boys or girls, for adolescents from different
examining the relationship between school factors and academic ethnic groups, or for adolescents with different academic abilities,
outcomes; some studies suggest that African American students are we treat the gender, ethnicity, and academic ability moderation
less likely to engage in school than their European American analyses as exploratory (Hypothesis 3).
counterparts (Downey & Ainsworth-Darnell, 2002), while other
studies report no ethnic differences in the relationship between 2. Method
school climate and student academic adjustment (Harris, 2006;
Smerdon, 1999; Wang & Huguley, 2012). Still other researchers 2.1. Participants
have found that the impact of positive relationships with teachers
is stronger for African American students than for European Study participants were part of the Maryland Adolescent
American students (Downey & Ainsworth-Darnell, 2002; Ferguson Development in Context Study that was designed to increase un-
& Mehta, 2004). derstanding of the inuences of social context on behavioral
The effect of school characteristics on student motivation and choices and developmental trajectories during adolescence. Par-
engagement may vary as a function of student academic abilities ticipants were from twenty-three public middle schools in a large,
(Elliot et al., 1999; Pintrich, 2000). For instance, high performing ethnically diverse county near Washington D.C. of the United
students are more likely than low performing students to benet States. This study examines two waves of data: Wave 1(N 1157)
from an autonomous learning environment, as provision of collected in early fall of 7th grade and Wave 2 (N 1039) collected
16 M.-T. Wang, J.S. Eccles / Learning and Instruction 28 (2013) 12e23

at the end of 8th grade. Of these respondents, approximately 56% 2.3.2. School characteristics
are African American, 32% are European American, and 12% are The School Environment Measure was used to create ve latent
either biracial or other ethnic minorities. Approximately 52% of the constructs to assess student perceptions of the school characteris-
students are females. Both samples had a normally distributed tics: (a) School Structure Support, (b) Provision of Choice, (c)
range of socioeconomic levels, with the mean pre-tax family annual Teaching for Relevance, (d) Teacher Emotional Support, and (e) Peer
income between $45,000 and $49,999 (range: $5000 to >$75,000) Emotional Support. Items for each scale were adapted from existing
and 86% of primary caregivers reported being employed. well-validated scales (e.g., Midgley et al., 1998; Roeser et al., 1998;
The subsample was based on parent willingness to participate Wigeld, Eccles, & Pintrich, 1996). Previous research has indicated
and on a stratied sampling procedure designed to capture pro- that these scales have good psychometric properties, including
portional representation of families from each of twenty-three internal consistency and predictive and criterion validity (Midgley
middle schools. Wave 2 retained approximately 89% of the orig- et al., 1998). The School Structure Support subscale included four
inal sample from Wave 1. The most common source of attrition was items and assessed teacher clarity of expectations, consistency and
relocation outside of the school district. To examine whether predictability of response, instrumental support, and adjustment of
sample attrition inuenced results, we compared individuals with teaching strategies (e.g., How often do you know what your
complete data or missing data at one wave with individuals with teacher expects of you in school?). The Provision of Choice sub-
missing data at two or more waves on all indicators included in the scale consisted of four items and assessed student opportunities to
analyses. None of the thirty-one comparisons were statistically make decisions related to academic tasks and school governance
signicant. (e.g., How often do you get to choose your partners for group
work?). The Teaching for Relevance subscale included three items
2.2. Procedures and assessed the extent to which the curriculum and instruction
provide opportunities for self-exploration and offer activities that
Seventh graders were recruited through letters to their families. are relevant and related to student personal interests and goals
Families interested in participating in the study were asked to sign (e.g., How often do you discuss problems and issues that are
and return a consent form. The investigators used a mixture of meaningful to you?). The Teacher Emotional Support subscale
questionnaires and school records to collect the data. In this study consisted of three items and assessed student perceived level of
we used data from the target student and the primary caregiver, care and support from teachers (e.g., How often can you depend on
who was most often the mother. Questionnaires with both students teachers to help you out when you have a personal or social
and their primary caregivers were conducted in the home during problem at school?). The Peer Emotional Support subscale
each wave of the study. Questionnaire administrators were pri- included four items and assessed student perceived level of peer
marily women with bachelors degrees and their race was the same acceptance and positive relationships with peers (e.g., How often
as that of the participating adolescent. The questionnaire took can you depend on your friends to help you out when you have a
approximately thirty minutes to complete. Participating students social or personal problem at school?). All items were rated on a
were compensated $20 at each wave of data collection. ve-point scale, ranging from 1 (not very often) to 5 (very often).

2.3. Measures 2.3.3. Achievement motivational beliefs


We used two measures adapted from the expectancy and value
2.3.1. School engagement scales developed by Eccles et al. (1993) to assess student percep-
We adapted existing well-established scales to assess the three tions of achievement motivation. Both scales have been used in
dimensions of student engagement in school (e.g., Finn & Voelkl, prior studies and demonstrated excellent convergent and
1993; Pintrich, 2000; Skinner & Wellborn, 1994). These scales discriminant validity (Anderman, Midgley, Wigeld, & Eccles, 2001;
have been shown to have strong psychometric properties, Jacobs, Lanza, Osgood, Eccles, & Wigeld, 2002). We created two
including internal consistency, convergent and divergent validity, latent constructs to represent student motivational beliefs in
and measurement invariance across gender, ethnicity, and socio- school: Academic Self-Concept and Subjective Task Valuing of
economic status (see Wang, Willett, & Eccles, 2011). All measure- School Learning, with each construct being represented by multiple
ments were scaled appropriately, so that high scores indicate items. The Academic Self-Concept scale included ve items and
higher levels of school engagement. assessed students perceptions of their academic ability to learn or
The Behavioral Engagement scale included ve items and execute courses of action in order to attain educational perfor-
measured the extent to which students follow the school rules and mances (e.g., How good are you in math?). Items were rated on a
participate in learning activities in school. Sample items are How seven-point scale, ranging from 1 (not very good) to 7 (very good).
often do you get schoolwork done on time? and How often do you The Subjective Task Valuing of Learning in School scale included
participate in class discussion actively? Responses were based on a three items and measured students intrinsic interest in and
5-point scale, ranging from 1 (almost never) to 5 (almost always). attainment valuing of academic achievement (e.g., I go to school
The six-item scale of Emotional Engagement assessed students because I enjoy learning in school.). Items were rated on a seven-
feelings of acceptance, interest, and enjoyment with school. Sample point scale in terms of importance, ranging from 1 (not an important
items are I nd schoolwork interesting and I feel excited by the reason) to 7 (very important reason).
work in school. The item responses for the scale range from 1
(strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree). 2.3.4. Covariates
The ve-item scale of Cognitive Engagement measured the We controlled for a vector of important covariates related to
extent to which students use self-regulated learning strategies such student socioeconomic characteristics and prior academic
as self-monitoring and evaluation to help understand learning achievement, motivation, and engagement, including gender
materials. Sample items are How often do you make academic (0 female, 1 male), ethnicity (two dummy variables:
plans for solving problems? and How often do you try to relate 0 European American, 1 African American; 0 European
what you are studying to other things you know about? Item re- American, 1 others), socioeconomic status (parent education
sponses of the scale range from 1 (almost never) to 5 (almost and family annual income), student prior school engagement, and
always). academic achievement (GPA) in the early fall of 7th grade. We
M.-T. Wang, J.S. Eccles / Learning and Instruction 28 (2013) 12e23 17

standardized and added parent education and annual family in- Table 1
come to create a composite measure of socioeconomic status. Means, standard deviations, and intercorrelations among all latent variables
(N 1157).
Student academic grade point average (GPA) was collected from
school records. GPA was an average of student grades in the core Variable 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10
academic subjects (English, math, science, and social sciences). 1. School structure support 1.00
Letter grades were converted into numerical values (A 5, B 4, 2. Provision of choice .36 1.00
C 3, D 2, Failing 1). 3. Teaching for relevance .37 .31 1.00
4. Teacher emotional .30 .29 .25 1.00
support
2.4. Data analyses 5. Peer emotional support .15 .21 .12 .36 1.00
6. Behavioral engagement .29 .19 .11 .21 .18 1.00
We used structural equation modeling (SEM) with Mplus 6.0 to 7. Emotional engagement .40 .23 .29 .24 .26 .32 1.00
8. Cognitive engagement .13 .13 .31 .20 .23 .23 .30 1.00
t the hypothesized relations among the study constructs (see
9. Academic self-concept .24 .13 .21 .18 .17 .28 .37 .38 1.00
Fig. 1). The nested nature of our data (students nested in twenty- 10. Subjective task value .35 .18 .30 .32 .16 .24 .48 .34 .40 1.00
three schools) was accounted for by tting a multilevel model
with random-effects, which produced correctly adjusted standard M 3.57 3.44 3.25 2.73 2.99 2.55 3.62 3.58 5.36 4.31
errors in the model estimations. We dealt with the missing data SD 0.85 0.77 0.98 0.92 0.85 0.52 0.60 0.54 1.01 1.45
through full-information maximum likelihood estimation, which Internal consistency (a) .85 .77 .80 .70 .87 .72 .77 .81 .84 .77

allowed us to include all available data and identied the parameter Note. All coefcients are signicant (p < .01).
values that have the highest probability of producing the sample
data (Baraldi & Enders, 2010).
To address the research questions, we began by tting baseline indices suggest no signicant cross loadings. Student perceptions of
models that assessed the direct effects of the ve constructs of School Structure, Provision of Choice, and Teacher and Peer
school environment on the three constructs of school engagement. Emotional Support were positively associated with Behavioral
After establishing these direct relationships, we introduced the two Engagement (gs .30, .10, .18, and .12 respectively). Students who
constructs of achievement motivation (academic self-concept and indicated positive experiences of School Structure, Provision of
subjective task value) into the path model and tested their medi- Choice, Teaching for Relevance, and Emotional Support from
ating effect on students perceptions of school environment and Teachers and Peers had higher Emotional Engagement (gs .27, .18,
school engagement. In order to test for mediation, we estimated .16, .21 and .22 respectively). Finally, Teaching for Relevance and
indirect effects with delta method standard errors to conrm the Peer Emotional Support were positively associated with Cognitive
mediation effects (MacKinnon, Lockwood, Hoffman, West, & Sheets, Engagement (gs .21 and .20 respectively). Teacher Emotional
2002). Finally, to examine the moderation effect of gender, Support was not associated with Cognitive Engagement. Overall,
ethnicity, and academic ability, we used a stepwise process for the the model accounts for 35% of the variance in Behavioral Engage-
multigroup comparisons in SEM recommended by Bollen (1989) ment, 46% of the variance in Emotional Engagement, and 32% of the
and Kenny (2005) to examine whether the measurement and variance in Cognitive Engagement.
structural relations in the nal model varied by gender, ethnicity, or
student prior academic achievement level. We included a series of
increasingly restrictive constraints on the model parameters and 3.2. Mediation effects of motivational beliefs
examined whether doing so led to a signicant decrease in the
model t by using chi-square difference tests (Chen, 2007). If the The mediation analyses examined the extent to which student
impositions led to a signicant decrease in overall model t, it Academic Self-Concept and Subjective Task Value of Learning
would indicate that there were differences across the groups in the mediated the relationships between perceived school environment
pattern of associations. and school engagement (see Fig. 3). The t of this model is
adequate, c2 (520, N 1157) 1417.30, p < .001, CFI .95, TLI .94,
3. Results RMSEA .04. Tables 2 and 3 present the results of direct, indirect,
and total effects in the nal model. The relations between School
All bivariate correlations among latent variables were statisti- Structure and three of the engagement variables (Behavioral,
cally signicant and in the expected directions (see Table 1). All Emotional, and Cognitive Engagement) were partially mediated by
variables had low to moderate correlations (from .10 to .49), sug- Academic Self-Concept and Subjective Task Value. Moreover, the
gesting little multicollinearity (Kline, 2005). Conrmatory factor relation between Teaching for Relevance and Emotional Engage-
analysis (CFA) veried that the hypothesized constructs measure ment was fully mediated by the motivational variables, while the
discrete, single latent variables. The standardized loadings ranged relation between Teaching for Relevance and Cognitive Engage-
from .38 to .84 and were all statistically signicant at the .05 level. ment was partially mediated by the motivational variables. The
For the assessment of the measurement model, all the latent vari- relations between Teacher Emotional Support and both engage-
ables, including ve school factors, three school engagement fac- ment variables (Behavioral and Emotional Engagement) were
tors, and two achievement motivation factors, were allowed to partially mediated by the motivational variables. Finally, the rela-
intercorrelate simultaneously. The measurement model was found tion between Peer Emotional Support and Behavioral Engagement
to provide adequate t, c2 (445, N 1157) 1116.87, p < .001, was fully mediated by the motivational variables, while the re-
CFI .97, TLI .96, RMSEA .02. lations between Peer Emotional Support and Emotional and
Cognitive Engagement were partially mediated by motivational
3.1. Direct effects of perceived school environment on school variables.
engagement The inclusion of the mediating variables adds 7%, 13%, and 10% to
the explained variance in Behavioral, Emotional, and Cognitive
We present the standardized path coefcients for the nal tted Engagement, respectively. Overall, the variables explain 42%, 59%,
model in Fig. 2. The overall model t is good, c2 (352, N 1157) and 42% of the variance in Behavioral, Emotional, and Cognitive
984.32, p < .001, CFI .95, TLI .94, RMSEA .04. The modication Engagement, respectively. The school environment variables
18 M.-T. Wang, J.S. Eccles / Learning and Instruction 28 (2013) 12e23

Perceptions of School Environment School Engagement


in 7th Grade Early Fall at the end of 8th Grade

R2 = .35
School structure .30***
Behavioral
engagement
.10*
.27*
**
Provision of choice

.18**
R2 = .46
**
.18 Emotional
Teaching for
.16** engagement
relevance
*
.12
.21* .21***
**
Teacher emotional R2 = .32
support
.22 ***
Cognitive engagement

.20**
Peer emotional
support

Fig. 2. Path model depicting direct effect of student perceptions of school environment on school engagement, controlling for prior school engagement, academic achievement, and
demographic characteristics. Only statistically signicant paths were presented in the model for clarity and all coefcients shown were standardized. Paths describing relations from
controlling variables to outcome variables can be obtained in Table 2. *p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001.

account for 55% and 47% of the variance in Academic Self-Concept For moderation analyses involving academic ability, we tested
and Subjective Task Value, respectively. differences across two ability groups: high GPA and low GPA in the
fall of 7th grade (we dened GPA scores above the median as high
3.3. Moderation effect of gender, ethnicity, and academic ability and below the median as low). The measurement part of the model
was rst tested by constraining the factor loadings of the two
We conducted three sets of moderation analyses to determine models to be equal. The Dc2 difference test showed that model t
whether the measurement and structural relations in the nal decrease is not signicant across gender, ethnicity, or academic
model varied by gender, ethnicity, or academic ability. For moder- ability [gender: Dc2 (23, N 1157) 34.67, p .13; ethnicity: Dc2
ation analyses involving ethnicity, we examined differences across (23, N 1157) 33.24, p .21; ability: Dc2 (23, N 1157) 30.58,
two groups: European American and African American students. p .24]. The constrained measurement model provides a good

Fig. 3. Path model depicting mediational effect of motivational beliefs between perceptions of school environment and school engagement. Only statistically signicant paths were
presented in the model and all coefcients shown were standardized. Direct effects between school environment variables and engagement outcomes were not shown in the model
for clarity but can be obtained in Table 2. Paths describing relations from controlling variables to mediating and outcome variables were presented in Table 3. *p < .05. **p < .01.
***p < .001.
M.-T. Wang, J.S. Eccles / Learning and Instruction 28 (2013) 12e23 19

Table 2
Standardized direct, indirect, and total effects for the nal model from perceived school environment and motivational beliefs to school engagement.

Predictor, mediator, and covariate Behavioral engagement Emotional engagement Cognitive engagement

Direct Indirect Total Direct Indirect Total Direct Indirect Total

Predictor variable
School structure support .17** .13** .30*** .15** .14** .29*** .06 .12** .18**
Provision of choice .07 .03 .10* .11* .04 .15* .05 .04 .09
Teaching for relevance .04 .05 .09 .01 .10** .11* .14* .11** .25***
Teacher emotional support .10* .10* .20*** .12* .08* .20*** .05 .08 .13*
Peer emotional support .04 .08* .12* .11* .09* .20*** .12* .11** .23**

Mediator variable
Academic self-concept .29*** e e .18** e e .38*** e e
Subjective task value .19*** e e .37*** e e .16** e e

Covariate
Male .11* e e .03 e e .14* e e
African American .15** e e .17** e e .07 e e
SES .20*** e e .05 e e .10* e e
Prior academic achievement .38*** e e .11* e e .12* e e
Prior behavioral engagement .36*** e e .18** e e .12** e e
Prior emotional engagement .20*** e e .24*** e e .06 e e
Prior cognitive engagement .13* e e .02 e e .38*** e e

Note: *p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001.

model t overall for the three groups respectively. After examining 4. Discussion
measurement equivalence, the paths of the two models were
constrained to be equal sequentially. This study capitalizes on school engagement as a multidimen-
Moderation analyses by gender and ethnicity revealed no sig- sional constructdincluding behavioral, emotional, and cognitive
nicant differences in the structural relations and suggest the engagementdand examines the inuence of multiple school
generality of the overall model across females and males as well as characteristics on all three types of engagement simultaneously.
European Americans and African Americans. For multigroup ana- The ndings provide empirical evidence regarding the malleability
lyses involving academic ability, results indicated signicant dif- of school engagement and demonstrate that student perceptions of
ferences across groups in the relationships between perceived the school environment predict change in patterns of school
school environment and motivation as well as between perceived engagement. For instance, when students experience school as
school environment and school engagement (see Table 4). Further optimal in structure, have opportunities to make choices, experi-
analyses based on the full mediation model in Fig. 3 revealed that ence what they learn as having personal relevance, and feel
the academic ability differences were specic to the relationship emotionally supported in learning by both their teachers and peers,
between School Structure and Academic Self-Concept, Dc2 (1) they are more likely to feel interested and to value learning activ-
15.03, p < .001, as well as Provision of Choice and Behavioral ities in school. Finally, our study extends the past research on the
Engagement, Dc2 (1) 5.45, p < .05. School Structure was more application and integration of self-determination theory, stage-
strongly associated with Academic Self-Concept for low achievers environment t theory, and expectancy-value theory to illustrate the
(b .24, p < .001) than for high achievers (b .15, p < .01). processes by which different features of the school environment
Conversely, for high achievers, increasing Provision of Choice was inuence student behavioral, emotional, and cognitive engagement
associated with greater Behavioral Engagement (b .16, p < .01), in school.
while for low achievers increasing Provision of Choice was associ-
ated with lower Behavioral Engagement (b .09, p < .05). 4.1. Direct effects among school environment, achievement
motivation, and engagement

Table 3
The multifaceted nature of engagement captured in this study
Standardized direct, indirect, and total effects for the nal model from perceived makes it possible to extricate the unique contribution of each
school environment and covariates to motivational beliefs. school factor to the three different types of engagement. As we
Predictor and covariate Academic self-concept Subjective task value predicted, the associations between school characteristics and
school engagement vary across the different types of engagement
Direct Indirect Total Direct Indirect Total

Predictor variable
School structure support .19** e e .29*** e e Table 4
Provision of choice .07 e e .08 e e Moderation results: effects of academic ability on modeled relationships.
Teaching for relevance .12** e e .19** e e
Teacher emotional support .12* e e .16* e e
Constrained path c2 (df) p
(for Dc2)
Peer emotional support .22** e e .12* e e
1. Measurement model 1104.46 (512) ns
Covariate 2. School characteristics / motivational beliefs 1145.32 (534) .01
Male .11* e e .03 e e School structure support / academic self-concept 1156.78 (538) .05
African American .15** e e .06 e e 3. School characteristics / school engagement 1179.45 (550) .05
SES .21*** e e .17** e e Provision of choice / behavioral engagement 1072.31 (554) .05
Prior academic achievement .25*** e e .20*** e e 4. Motivational beliefs / school engagement 1102.90 (581) ns

Note: *p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001. Note. Bolded pathways denote differences across math ability groups.
20 M.-T. Wang, J.S. Eccles / Learning and Instruction 28 (2013) 12e23

(Hypothesis 1). For example, in order to increase behavioral appropriate difculty and when it comes with adequate structure
engagement, modication of school structure or the enhancement support. It is important for teachers who provide choice to match
of teacherestudent relations appears to be more effective than the complexity of the tasks to student academic abilities and
fostering relevant academic tasks. It is critical to keep such dis- perceived competence. Teaching is most effective when directed
tinctions of associations in mind when studying school engage- toward the students zone of proximal development, which is
ment, as evidently, not all characteristics of school environment largely determined by their academic skills.
impact the unique types of school engagement in the same manner. In regard to support for relatedness, we found that students are
The use of a multidimensional perspective allows us to gain a better more likely to behaviorally and emotionally engaged in school
understanding of which aspects of the school environment are when teachers and peers create a caring and socially supportive
most salient, and hence potentially most effective, in increasing the environment (Hypotheses 1d and e; Battistich et al., 1997; Wentzel,
different types of school engagement. 2003). The ndings add to growing evidence that student percep-
As expected, students who report that their teachers provide tions of the nature and quality of the school social environment are
clear expectations and consistent and contingent responses are as important as the academic environment (e.g., academic tasks
more likely to participate in academic tasks and to identify in a and instructional practices) in promoting adaptive achievement
positive way with their school (Hypothesis 1a). Similarly, greater motivation and engagement in school (e.g., Turner, Meyer, Midgley,
interest and the use of self-regulatory strategies in learning are & Patrick, 2003). If focus is placed solely on academic activity, a
more common when the curriculum include meaningful topics that teacher runs the risk of creating a negative social environment
reect students personal goals and interests (Hypothesis 1c). wherein students are less likely to become behaviorally and
Teaching for relevance also predicts higher academic self-concept emotionally engaged, and are more likely to be apprehensive about
and subjective task value. Contrary to our hypotheses, however, making mistakes. Accordingly, if focus is placed on the social di-
perceived provision of choice did not predict students subjective mensions of class only while academic dimensions are neglected,
task valuing of learning at school, nor did it predict their academic students are less inclined to become behaviorally and cognitively
self-concept (Hypothesis 1b). There is evidence that the benets of engaged in learning.
choice-provision for student motivation are likely to be limited if
the choices involve tasks that are not deemed interesting or rele- 4.3. Mediation effect of achievement motivation
vant to a students personal goals and interests (Assor et al., 2002).
Opportunities for decision making or freedom of action are less This study identies the role that motivational beliefs play in
important than the extent to which the decision making and action mediating the relationship between school environment and
opportunities available reect personal goals, interests, and values various dimensions of school engagement, demonstrating that
(Deci & Ryan, 2000). It is possible that decision-making opportu- student perceptions of the school environment directly and
nities do not effectively support student autonomy in the case that indirectly predict the level of school engagement through per-
no relevance to personal goals or interests is detected. In order to sonal motivation beliefs (Hypothesis 2). Consistent with our ex-
make choice and freedom of action motivational, students should pectations, an academically adaptive and socially oriented school
be provided with options to engage in schoolwork that are relevant environment that facilitates person-environment t promotes
to personal goals and interests (Flowerday & Schraw, 2003; Katz & student engagement through its inuence on student academic
Assor, 2006). A benecial strategy, for example, would be for self-concept and subjective task values. In particular, student
teachers to explicitly illustrate and explain the relevance of tasks to academic self-concept and subjective task valuing of learning
the personal goals and interests of their students when providing is enhanced when the school environment provides clarity
them with choices. of expectation, consistency and predictability of response,
emotional support, opportunity to learn and master meaningful
4.2. Moderation effect of academic ability material, and sufcient or appropriate support of students per-
sonal goals and interests. This study informs the understanding
It is plausible that some students have not developed the of the mechanisms by which the school environment inuences
required academic ability to effectively harness the opportunities student engagement, thus highlighting critical points of
afforded to them when choices are made available. In the present intervention.
study, the moderation effect of academic ability provides partial Understanding student engagement in school requires an inte-
support for this assumption as provision of choice was found to grative motivational framework that considers the interaction of
positively predict the behavioral engagement of high performing the psychological and contextual factors in a youths life, as well as
students, but to negatively predict the behavioral engagement of the developmental needs of the youth as he or she matures within
lower performing students (Hypothesis 3). This moderating impact these contexts. Most studies of school engagement and achieve-
of academic achievement on the link between provision of choice ment in motivational psychology focus on a single theoretical
and behavioral engagement suggests that greater behavioral framework rather than taking advantage of the areas rich theo-
engagement is more likely to occur in a school environment that retical landscape. The phenomenon of looking for lost keys under
responds to differing levels of academic ability with the develop- the lamppost rather than in all the other places they might be
mentally appropriate provision of autonomy (Deci & Ryan, 1985; epitomizes this focused, but decient approach. Through the
Patrick et al., 1993). A second moderation effect of academic ability implementation of multiple motivational theories (self-determina-
supplements this nding: the positive effect of school structure on tion theory, stage-environment t theory, and expectancy-value the-
academic self-concept is stronger for low achievers. According to ory) in the design of this research and in the interpretation of the
self-determination theory-based studies (see Deci & Ryan, 2000; results, this study provides a much richer picture of the role of
Skinner & Belmont, 1993), teachers who balance structural support school engagement in adolescent development.
and student autonomy in an effort to maintain an engaging envi-
ronment are more likely to encounter increased learning motiva- 4.4. Limitations, future research, and implications for practice
tion in their students. For choice to have benecial effects, it needs
to be tailored to student academic ability. Taken together, choice Researchers have typically used student self-reports of school
can be motivational when the task choice options are of characteristics in order to comprehend how students themselves
M.-T. Wang, J.S. Eccles / Learning and Instruction 28 (2013) 12e23 21

derive meaning from their own perceptions of the school learning student behavioral, emotional, and cognitive orientation, schools
environment. In fact, a growing number of studies have shown that will be more able to establish practices that prevent disengagement
student perceptions of school experiences are critical components and to foster learning environments that support engagement e
in understanding their developmental outcomes (e.g., Wang & resulting in the mutually benecial outcome of enhanced academic
Holcombe, 2010). However, relying on student self-reports in performance.
assessing perception of school context raises an important validity
concern, which is that students could answer questions about their
behavior or that of their teacher in ways that they perceive to be References
socially desirable. Future inclusion of multiple sources of informa- Al, O., Assor, A., & Katz, I. (2004). Learning to allow temporary failure: potential
tion (teachers, principals, parents), as well as multiple methodol- benets, supportive practices and teacher concerns. Journal of Education for
ogies (interviews, observations, surveys), will provide a more Teaching, 30, 27e41. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/0260747032000162299.
Alonso-Tapia, J., & Pardo, A. (2006). Assessment of learning environment
robust, valid method of identifying school effects related to
motivational quality from the point of view of secondary and high school
engagement. Furthermore, research from our motivational frame- learners. Learning and Instruction, 16, 295e309. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/
works presumes a causal sequence that the perceived school j.learninstruc.2006.07.002.
environment contributes to individual motivational beliefs. How- Anderman, E. M., Midgley, C., Wigeld, A., & Eccles, J. S. (2001). Learning to value
mathematics and reading: relations to mastery and performance-oriented
ever, it also has been suggested that the extent to which students instructional practices. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 26, 76e96.
are motivated in school may also inuence their responses to the http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/ceps.1999.1043.
school environment (Urdan & Midgley, 2003). It is possible that Appleton, J. J., Christenson, S. L., Kim, D., & Reschly, A. L. (2006). Measuring cognitive
and psychological engagement: validation of the student engagement instru-
students with high academic self-concept and particularly high ment. Journal of School Psychology, 44, 427e445. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/
task value are more likely to attend to and appreciate teachers j.jsp.2006.04.002.
reference to relevance. Thus, future research should examine the Assor, A., Kaplan, H., & Roth, G. (2002). Choice is good, but relevance is excellent:
autonomy-enhancing and suppressing teacher behaviors predicting students
prospective reciprocal relations between school environment and engagement in schoolwork. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 72, 261e
adolescent motivational beliefs. Additionally, in this study we 278. http://dx.doi.org/10.1348/000709902158883.
examined the impact of school context on student engagement in Baraldi, A. N., & Enders, C. K. (2010). An introduction to modern missing data
analyses. Journal of School Psychology, 48, 5e37. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/
school; a more comprehensive examination would take into ac- j.jsp.2009.10.001.
count other contexts such as family and neighborhood. Finally, Battistich, V., Soloman, D., Watson, M., & Schaps, E. (1997). Caring school
school engagement is likely to take different forms in the communities. Educational Psychologist, 32, 137e151. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/
s15326985ep3203_1.
elementary and secondary school years (Fredricks et al., 2004).
Bollen, K. A. (1989). Structural equations with latent variables. Hoboken, N.J.: Wiley.
Future comparison of longitudinal studies across different age Chen, F. F. (2007). Sensitivity of goodness of t indexes to lack of measurement
ranges would be of use in the clarication of whether the ndings invariance. Structural Equation Modeling, 14, 464e504. http://dx.doi.org/
identied in this study were unique to this sample at this given 10.1080/10705510701301834.
Connell, J. P. (1990). Context, self, and action: a motivational analysis of self-system
time, or whether the ndings in fact represent an otherwise processes across the life-span. In D. Cicchetti (Ed.), The self in transition: From
generalizable or even universal developmental trajectory for school infancy to childhood (pp. 61e97). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
engagement. Greater precision in the articulation of the develop- Connell, J. P., & Wellborn, J. G. (1991). Competence, autonomy, and relatedness: a
motivational analysis of self-system processes. In M. R. Gunnar, & L. A. Sroufe
mental effects of school engagement will help to inform in- (Eds.). Self processes in development: Minnesota symposium on child psychology,
terventions to support adolescent learning at different levels of Vol. 23, (pp. 43e77). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
schooling. Corno, L., & Mansinach, E. (1983). The role of cognitive engagement in classroom
learning and motivation. Educational Psychologist, 18, 88e108. http://dx.doi.org/
The study of school engagement as a multidimensional 10.1080/00461528309529266.
construct and as an interaction between the individual and the Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1985). Intrinsic motivation and self-determination in human
school environment enabled us to identify which particular school behavior. New York: Plenum Press.
Deci, E., & Ryan, R. M. (2000). What is the self in self-directed learning? Findings
characteristics foster or undermine student engagement and to
from recent motivational research. In G. Staka (Ed.), Conceptions of self-directed
achieve a greater understanding of the psychological mechanisms learning: Theoretical and conceptual considerations). Munster: Waxmann.
through which the school environment inuences student Downey, D. B., & Ainsworth-Darnell, J. W. (2002). The search for oppositional cul-
ture among black students. American Sociological Review, 67, 156e164.
engagement in school. The current study suggests that teachers
Eccles, J. S. (2004). Schools, academic motivation, and stage-environment t. In
have a critical role to play in creating a positive school environment, R. M. Lerner, & L. Steinberg (Eds.), Handbook of adolescent psychology (2nd ed.).
which can in turn help them to optimize their students perceptions (pp. 125e153) Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
of their school environment. The ve aspects of school environ- Eccles, J. S. (2007). Where are all the women? Gender differences in participation in
physical science and engineering. In S. J. Ceci, & W. M. Williams (Eds.), Why
ment examined in this study are inuenced by the academic goals arent more women in science? Top researchers debate the evidence (pp. 199e210).
and social climate emphasized and fostered by teachers in their Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. http://dx.doi.org/
implementation of school policies, selection of instructional prac- 10.1037/11546-016.
Eccles, J. S. (2009). Who am I and what am I going to do with my life? Personal and
tices and academic tasks, organization of the classroom environ- collective identities as motivators of action. Educational Psychologist, 44, 78e89.
ment, and the manner in which they relate to their students. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00461520902832368.
Teachers can promote student learning motivation and engage- Eccles, J. S., Midgley, C., Buchanan, C. M., Flanagan, C., Mac Iver, D., Reuman, D.,
et al. (1993). Development during adolescence: the impact of stage/envi-
ment by creating a supportive school environment that stresses ronment t. American Psychologist, 48, 90e101. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/
and provides opportunities for students to feel competent, auton- 0003-066X.48.2.90.
omous, and emotionally supported (Al, Assor, & Katz, 2004; Reeve, Eccles, J. S., & Wigeld, A. (2002). Motivational beliefs, values, and goals.
Annual Review of Psychology, 53, 109e132. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0003-
Jang, Carrell, Jeon, & Barch, 2004). Such school environments 066X.48.2.90.
enable all students to succeed in the required academic and social Elliot, A. J., & Dweck, C. S. (Eds.). (2005). Handbook of competence and motivation).
tasks at hand and to grasp the role that schoolwork has to play in New York: Guilford.
Elliot, A. J., McGregor, H. A., & Gable, S. (1999). Achievement goals, study strategies,
the realization of their personal goals, interests, and values. The
and exam performance: a meditational analysis. Journal of Educational
present study thus supports the notion that attending to the quality Psychology, 91, 549e563. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.91.3.549.
of both academic and social domains of the school environment is Ferguson, R. F., & Mehta, J. (2004). An unnished journey: the legacy of Brown
important in developing a positive student perception of school, and the narrowing of the achievement gap. Phi Delta Kappan, 85, 656e669.
http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/i20189399.
which in turn promotes student motivation and engagement. With Finn, J. D. (1989). Withdrawing from school. Review of Educational Research, 59, 117e
a thorough understanding of how school characteristics affect 142. http://dx.doi.org/10.3102/00346543059002117.
22 M.-T. Wang, J.S. Eccles / Learning and Instruction 28 (2013) 12e23

Finn, J. D., & Rock, D. A. (1997). Academic success among students at risk for school Theory, research, and application (pp. 451e502). http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/
failure. Journal of Applied Psychology, 82, 221e234. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/ B978-012109890-2/50043-3.
0021-9010.82.2.221. Reeve, J., Bolt, E., & Cai, Y. (1999). Autonomy supportive teachers: how they teach
Finn, J. D., & Voelkl, K. E. (1993). School characteristics related to school engage- and motivate students. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 537e548. http://
ment. Journal of Negro Education, 62, 249e268. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/ dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.91.3.537.
i314505. Reeve, J., Jang, H., Carrell, D., Jeon, S., & Barch, J. (2004). Enhancing students
Flowerday, T., & Schraw, G. (2003). Effect of choice on cognitive and affective motivation by increasing teachers autonomy support. Motivation and Emotion,
engagement. Journal of Educational Research, 96, 207e215. http://dx.doi.org/ 28, 147e169. http://dx.doi.org/10.1023/B:MOEM.0000032312.95499.6f.
10.2307/i27542431. Roeser, R. W., Eccles, J. S., & Sameroff, A. J. (1998). Academic and emotional
Fredricks, J. A., Blumenfeld, P. C., & Paris, A. H. (2004). School engagement: potential functioning in early adolescence: longitudinal relations, patterns, and pre-
of the concept, state of the evidence. Review of Educational Research, 74, 59e diction by experience in middle school. Development and Psychopathology, 10,
109. http://dx.doi.org/10.3102/00346543074001059. 321e352.
Furrer, C., & Skinner, E. (2003). Sense of relatedness as a factor in childrens aca- Rueger, S. Y., Malecki, C. K., & Demaray, M. K. (2010). Relationship between multiple
demic engagement and performance. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95, sources of perceived social support and psychological and academic adjustment
148e161. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.95.1.148. in early adolescence: comparisons across gender. Journal of Youth and Adoles-
Goodenow, C. (1993). Classroom belonging among early adolescent student: re- cence, 39, 47e61. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10964-008-9368-6.
lationships to motivation and achievement. Journal of Early Adolescence, 13, 21e Ryan, A. M., & Patrick, H. (2001). The classroom social environment and
43. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0272431693013001002. changes in adolescents motivation and engagement during middle school.
Grolnick, W. S., Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (1991). Inner resources for school American Educational Research Journal, 28, 437e460. http://dx.doi.org/
achievement: motivational mediators of childrens perceptions of their parents. 10.2307/i360666.
Journal of Educational Psychology, 83, 508e517. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022- Schunk, D. H. (1996). Goal and self-evaluative inuences during childrens cognitive
0663.83.4.508. skill learning. American Educational Research Journal, 33, 359e382. http://
Harris, A. L. (2006). I (dont) hate school: revisiting oppositional culture theory of dx.doi.org/10.2307/i248887.
blacks resistance to schooling. Social Forces, 86, 797e834. http://dx.doi.org/ Shim, S. S., Cho, Y., & Wang, C. (2013). Classroom goal structures, social
10.2307/i405623. achievement goals, and adjustment in middle school. Learning and Instruction,
Helme, S., & Clarke, D. (2001). Identifying cognitive engagement in the mathematics 23, 69e77.
classroom. Mathematics Education Research Journal, 13, 133e153. http:// Simpkins, S. D., Davis-Kean, P., & Eccles, J. S. (2006). Math and science moti-
dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF03217103. vation: a longitudinal examination of the links between choices and be-
Jacobs, J. E., Lanza, S., Osgood, D. W., Eccles, J. S., & Wigeld, A. (2002). Changes in liefs. Developmental Psychology, 42, 70e83. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0012-
childrens self-competence and values: gender and domain differences across 1649.42.1.70.
grades one through twelve. Child Development, 73, 509e527. http://dx.doi.org/ Skinner, E. A., & Belmont, M. J. (1993). Motivation in the classroom: reciprocal effect
10.2307/i370705. of teacher behavior and student engagement across the school year. Journal of
Jimmerson, S. R., Campos, E., & Greif, J. L. (2003). Toward an understanding of Educational Psychology, 85, 571e581. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-
denitions and measures of school engagement and related terms. The Cali- 0663.85.4.571.
fornia School Psychologist, 8, 7e27. Skinner, E., Furrer, C., Marchand, G., & Kindermann, T. (2008). Engagement
Johnson, M. K., Crosnoe, R., & Elder, G. (2001). Students attachment and academic and disaffection in the classroom: part of a larger motivational dynamic.
engagement: the role of the race and ethnicity. Sociology of Education, 74, 318e Journal of Educational Psychology, 100, 765e781. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/
340. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/i326042. a0012840.
Katz, I., & Assor, A. (2006). When choice motivates and when it does not. Educa- Skinner, E. A., & Wellborn, J. G. (1994). Coping during childhood and adolescence: a
tional Psychology Review, 19, 429e442. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10648-006- motivational perspective. In D. Featherman, R. Lerner, & M. Perlmutter (Eds.).
9027-y. Life-span development and behavior, Vol. 12, (pp. 91e133). Hillsdale, NJ:
Kenny, D. A. (2005). Multiple group models. Retrieved 20.04.06, from. http:// Erlbaum.
davidkenny.net/cm/mgroups.htm. Smerdon, B. A. (1999). Engagement and achievement: differences between African-
Kline, R. B. (2005). Principles and practice of structural equation modeling (2nd ed.). American and white high school students. Research in Sociology of Education and
New York, NY: Guilford Press. Socialization, 12, 103e134.
Krapp, A. (2005). Basic needs and the development of interest and intrinsic moti- Turner, J. E., Meyer, D. K., Midgley, C., & Patrick, H. (2003). Teacher discourse and
vational orientations. Learning and Instruction, 15, 381e395. http://dx.doi.org/ sixth graders reported affect and achievement behaviors in two high-mastery/
10.1016/j.learninstruc.2005.07.007. high performance mathematics classrooms. The Elementary School Journal, 103,
MacKinnon, D. P., Lockwood, C. M., Hoffman, J. M., West, S. G., & Sheets, V. (2002). 357e382. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/i241865.
A comparison of methods to test mediation and other intervening variables Updegraff, K. A., Eccles, J. S., Barber, B. L., & OBrien, K. M. (1996). Course enrollment
effects. Psychological Methods, 7, 83e104. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/1082- as self-regulatory behavior: who takes optional high school math course.
989X.7.1.83. Learning and Individual Differences, 8, 239e259.
Malecki, C. K., & Demaray, M. K. (2003). What type of support do they need? Urdan, T., & Midgley, C. (2003). Changes in the perceived classroom goal structure
Investigating student adjustment as related to emotional, informational, and pattern of adaptive learning during early adolescence. Contemporary
appraisal, and instrumental support. School Psychology Quarterly, 18, 231e252. Educational Psychology, 28, 524e551. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0361-476X(02)
http://dx.doi.org/10.1521/scpq.18.3.231.22576. 00060-7.
Marks, H. M. (2000). Student engagement in instructional activity: patterns in Voelkl, K. E. (1997). Identication with school. American Journal of Education, 105,
elementary, middle and high school years. American Educational Research 294e319.
Journal, 37, 153e184. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/i248902. Vollmeyer, R., & Rheinberg, F. (2000). Does motivation affect performance via
Martin, A. J. (2004). School motivation of boys and girls: differences of degree, persistence? Learning and Instruction, 10, 293e309.
differences of kind, or both? Australian Journal of Psychology, 56, 133e146 http:// Wang, M. T. (2009). School climate support for behavioral and psychological
dx.doi.org/10.1080/00049530412331283363. adjustment: testing the mediating effect of social competence. School Psychol-
Meece, J. L., Glienke, B. B., & Burg, S. (2006). Gender and motivation. Journal of ogy Quarterly, 24, 240e251. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/i245239.
School Psychology, 44, 351e373. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jsp.2006.04.004. Wang, M. T., Brinkworth, M. E., & Eccles, J. S. (2013). The moderation effect of
Midgley, C., Kaplan, A., Middleton, M., Maehr, M., Urdan, T., & Hicks Anderson, L. teacherestudent relationship on the association between adolescents self-
(1998). The development and validation of scales assessing students achieve- regulation ability, family conict, and developmental problems. Develop-
ment goal orientations. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 23, 113e131. mental Psychology, 49, 690e705. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0027916.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2011.03.031. Wang, M. T., & Eccles, J. S. (2012a). Adolescent behavioral, emotional, and cognitive
Murdock, T. B., & Miller, A. D. (2003). Teachers as sources of middle school students engagement trajectories in school and their differential relations to educational
motivational identity: variable-centered and person-centered analytic ap- success. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 22, 31e39. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/
proaches. Elementary School Journal, 103, 383e399. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/ j.1532-7795.2011.00753.x.
i241865. Wang, M. T., & Eccles, J. S. (2012b). Social support matters: longitudinal effects of
Oates, G. L. St (2009). An empirical test of ve prominent explanations for the social support on three dimensions of school engagement from middle to high
blackewhite academic performance gap. Social Psychology Education, 12, 415e school. Child Development, 83, 877e895. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-
441. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11218-009-9091-5. 8624.2012.01745.x.
Patrick, B. C., Skinner, E. A., & Connell, J. P. (1993). What motivates childrens Wang, M. T., & Holcombe, R. (2010). Adolescents perceptions of school environ-
behavior and emotion? Joint effects of perceived control and autonomy in the ment, engagement, and academic achievement in middle school. American
academic domain. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 781e791. Educational Research Journal, 47, 633e662. http://dx.doi.org/10.3102/
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.65.4.781. 0002831209361209.
Patrick, H., Ryan, A. M., & Kaplan, A. (2007). Early adolescents perceptions of Wang, M. T., & Huguley, J. P. (2012). Parental racial socialization as a moderator of
the classroom social environment, motivational beliefs, and engagement. the effects of racial discrimination on educational success among African
Journal of Educational Psychology, 99, 83e98. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022- American adolescents. Child Development, 83, 1716e1731. http://dx.doi.org/
0663.99.1.83. 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2012.01808.x.
Pintrich, P. R. (2000). The role of goal orientation in self-regulated learning. In Wang, M. T., & Peck, S. (2013). Adolescent educational success and mental health
M. Boekaerts, P. R. Pintrich, & M. Zeidner (Eds.), Handbook of self-regulation: vary across school engagement proles. Developmental Psychology.
M.-T. Wang, J.S. Eccles / Learning and Instruction 28 (2013) 12e23 23

Wang, M. T., Willett, J. B., & Eccles, J. S. (2011). The assessment of school engage- Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 3. Social, emotional, and personality develop-
ment: examining dimensionality and measurement invariance across gender ment (pp. 933e1002) New York: Wiley.
and race/ethnicity. Journal of School Psychology, 49, 465e480. http://dx.doi.org/ Wigeld, A., Eccles, J. S., & Pintrich, P. R. (1996). Development
10.1016/j.jsp.2011.04.001. between the ages of eleven and twenty-ve. In D. C. Berliner, & R. C. Calfee
Wentzel, K. R. (2003). Sociometric status and adjustment in middle school: a lon- (Eds.), The handbook of educational psychology). New York: Macmillan
gitudinal study. Journal of Early Adolescence, 23, 5e28. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/ Publishing.
0272431602239128. Zimmerman, B. J. (1990). Self-regulated learning and academic achievement:
Wigeld, A., Eccles, J. S., Davis-Kean, P., Roeser, R., & Scheifele, U. (2006). Motivation an overview. Educational Psychologist, 21, 3e17. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/
to succeed. In N. Eisenberg (Series Ed.) & W. Damon (Vol. Ed.) (6th ed.)., s15326985ep2501_2.

Você também pode gostar