Você está na página 1de 10

International Journal of Psychophysiology 93 (2014) 322–331

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

International Journal of Psychophysiology


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/ijpsycho

The effects of theta transcranial alternating current stimulation (tACS) on


fluid intelligence
Anja Pahor, Norbert Jaušovec ⁎
University of Maribor, Slovenia

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: The objective of the study was to explore the influence of transcranial alternating current stimulation (tACS) on
Received 1 March 2014 resting brain activity and on measures of fluid intelligence. Theta tACS was applied to the left parietal and left
Received in revised form 5 June 2014 frontal brain areas of healthy participants after which resting electroencephalogram (EEG) data was recorded.
Accepted 27 June 2014
Following sham/active stimulation, the participants solved two tests of fluid intelligence while their EEG was re-
Available online 3 July 2014
corded. The results showed that active theta tACS affected spectral power in theta and alpha frequency bands. In
Keywords:
addition, active theta tACS improved performance on tests of fluid intelligence. This influence was more pro-
Fluid intelligence nounced in the group of participants that received stimulation to the left parietal area than in the group of par-
tACS ticipants that received stimulation to the left frontal area. Left parietal tACS increased performance on the
Theta frequency difficult test items of both tests (RAPM and PF&C) whereas left frontal tACS increased performance only on the
EEG easy test items of one test (RAPM). The observed behavioral tACS influences were also accompanied by changes
ERD/ERS in neuroelectric activity. The behavioral and neuroelectric data tentatively support the P-FIT neurobiological
model of intelligence.
© 2014 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction directly toward the structure and functional connectivity of the brain
would have a more pronounced influence on measures of fluid intelli-
Can we improve intelligence? The unanimous answer of researchers gence. A direct approach might also have a methodological advantage
is that, to this date, intelligence cannot be raised in a consistent and since it can rule out similarity effects between assessment and training
long-lasting manner (Haier, 2014; Buschkuehl & Jaeggi, 2010; Jensen, tasks and other environmental variables that are difficult to control or
1998; Gottfredson, 1997). Recently, the debate on whether training exclude. Studies employing direct interventions can be divided into
can increase performance on tests of intellectual ability has once more three broad categories: neurofeedback training, pharmacological inter-
gained popularity. The discussion was triggered by Jaeggi et al. (2008), ventions, and brain stimulation. Several neurofeedback studies have
who showed that working memory (WM) training increased perfor- showed a modest positive influence on different tests of cognitive func-
mance on tests of fluid intelligence (Gf). Cognitive training, which typi- tions such as episodic retrieving (Keizer et al., 2010), mental rotation
cally targets working memory, reasoning, and executive functions, is the (Zoefel et al., 2011), music performance (Egner et al., 2004), creativity
most common form of intervention used to increase performance on and ballroom dancing (Gruzelier, 2009), and intelligence tests in a
tests of intelligence (e.g., Owen et al., 2010; Klingberg, 2010; Morrison group of individuals with an intellectual disability (Surmeli & Ertem,
& Chein, 2011; Chooi & Thompson, 2012; Colom et al., 2012; 2010). It has also been reported that nootropic or cognitive enhancing
Thompson et al., 2013). The reported effects are diverse, showing per- substances modestly raise the performance of children on nonverbal
formance increases (Jaeggi et al., 2008; von Bastian & Oberauer, 2013; tests of intelligence (Schoenthaler et al., 2000), have a positive effect
Stephenson & Halpern, 2013) or no significant effects (Owen et al., on working memory and intelligence tests (Rae, et al., 2003), and in-
2010; Chooi & Thompson, 2012; Colom et al., 2012, 2013b; Thompson crease performance on Raven's Advanced Progressive Matrices
et al., 2013). Some studies have further shown that these interventions (Stough et al., 2011).
also change brain activation patterns (Jaušovec & Jaušovec, 2012). A third approach that can influence brain activity directly is the re-
Because our brain is the source of intelligent behavior (Kolb & cently rediscovered field of transcranial electrical stimulation (transcra-
Whishaw, 2009), it could be speculated that interventions aimed nial direct current, and alternating current stimulation—tDCS and tACS).
It is assumed that tDCS modulates resting membrane potentials and, via
⁎ Corresponding author at: Univerza v Mariboru, Filozofska fakulteta, Koroška 160,
this mechanism, alters spontaneous cortical activity. Anodal tDCS en-
2000 Maribor, Slovenia. Fax: +386 2 258180. hances cortical activity and excitability, while cathodal stimulation has
E-mail address: norbert.jausovec@um.si (N. Jaušovec). the opposite effect (Bindman et al., 1964). On the other hand, tACS is a

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2014.06.015
0167-8760/© 2014 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.
A. Pahor, N. Jaušovec / International Journal of Psychophysiology 93 (2014) 322–331 323

newly developed stimulation technique that modulates cortical activity The second tACS protocol variable that was determined prior to
by affecting neuronal membrane potentials via oscillatory electrical conducting the study was location of stimulation (placement of active
stimulation in specific frequencies. This technique can be used to inter- electrodes). Based on research findings concerning the structure and
act with ongoing rhythmic cortical activities during cognitive processes neuronal origin of intelligence we decided to target left frontal and left
(Kuo & Nitsche, 2012). It has been suggested that tACS influences brain parietal brain areas. According to a model proposed by Duncan et al.
oscillations via interference or entrainment of ongoing oscillations (2000), psychometric g is implemented within the prefrontal cortex,
(Thut & Miniussi, 2009), as demonstrated with rhythmic transcranial whereas a more recent model claims that intelligence depends on dis-
magnetic stimulation (rTMS) in the alpha band (Thut et al., 2011). The tributed system of functionally specialized fronto-parietal cortical re-
effectiveness of tACS in modulating oscillatory brain activity was also gions (Jung & Haier, 2007). In Duncan's model (Duncan, 2001, 2003;
demonstrated by Zaehle et al. (2010). Stimulation applied at partici- Duncan & Owen, 2000) fluid intelligence mainly derives from a specific
pants' individual EEG alpha frequency (IAF) resulted in an enhancement frontal brain network (mid-dorsolateral, mid-ventrolateral and dorsal
of the EEG alpha amplitude, indicating that this stimulation method can anterior cingulate cortex), which is important for the control of diverse
interfere with ongoing brain oscillations in a frequency-specific manner. cognitive functions (e.g., executive control, strategy formation, monitor-
In a recent study, Neuling et al. (2013) replicated and extended the find- ing). Some neuroscience data support this framework, demonstrating
ings of Zaehle et al. (2010) by showing that the tACS-induced alpha am- the involvement of the prefrontal cortex in performance on tests of in-
plitude enhancement remained present for at least 30 min after telligence (Roca et al., 2010; Bishop et al., 2008; Gray et al., 2003;
stimulation offset. Prabhakaran et al., 1997) and executive function (Duncan, 2006;
To our knowledge, just one study has been conducted that investi- Duncan & Owen, 2000). A recent morphometric study further indicated
gates the influence of transcranial electrical stimulation on tests of that structural features of gray matter in the frontal lobes show an over-
fluid intelligence. Santarnecchi et al. (2013) showed that gamma-band lapping cluster with measures of crystallized and fluid intelligence
tACS delivered over the left middle frontal gyrus resulted in a shorten- (Colom et al., 2013a). On the other hand, Jung and Haier (2007)
ing of the time required to find the correct solution in a visuospatial ab- reviewed 37 neuroimaging studies on the structural correlates of intel-
stract reasoning test similar to Raven's matrices (1990). However, ligence and synthesized their findings into a so-called ‘parieto-frontal
reaction time may not serve as a valid indicator of fluid intelligence, es- integration’ (P-FIT) model of intelligence. This neurobiological model
pecially since the standard application of Raven's matrices has no time of intelligence emphasizes the importance of frontal and parietal re-
limitation. On the other hand, several studies have reported positive in- gions and the white matter association tracts that bind these areas
fluences of tDCS and tACS on tests of cognitive processes related to fluid into a synchronized system (Jung & Haier, 2007). Indeed, a number of
intelligence, such as working memory performance (Mulquiney et al., studies have demonstrated the importance of parietal regions in perfor-
2011; Boggio et al., 2008; Fregni et al., 2006; Polanía et al., 2012; mance on tests of general intelligence with the use of neuroimaging
Tseng et al., 2012), the ability to discriminate numerosity (Cappelletti techniques (Lee et al., 2006; Colom et al., 2006; Haier et al., 2004), elec-
et al., 2013), creativity, and problem solving (Cerruti & Schlaug, 2009; troencephalography (Jaušovec & Jaušovec, 2004; Gevins & Smith, 2000),
Chi & Snyder, 2011; Dockery et al., 2009; for a review see Kuo & and lesion mapping (Barbey et al., 2012; Gläscher et al., 2009).
Nitsche, 2012). Based on previous research on the effects of IAF tACS on alpha power
The aim of the present study was to explore the influence of dif- (Zaehle et al., 2010; Neuling et al., 2013) we hypothesized that active
ferent tACS protocols on performance on tests of fluid intelligence theta tACS would increase theta power, but would have no effect on fre-
and to extend the current understanding of the neuronal underpin- quencies in the alpha range. Furthermore, we predicted that theta tACS
nings of intelligence. Fluid intelligence is defined as the capacity to would improve performance on tests of fluid intelligence. In contrast, no
solve novel, complex problems, using operations such as inductive improvement in performance would be seen following sham tACS. In-
and deductive reasoning, concept formation, and classification. It creased performance on IQ tests induced by theta tACS would suggest
has also been suggested that Gf represents the influences of biologi- brain activation patterns similar to those observed in highly intelligent
cal factors and incidental learning on intellectual development individuals. In two EEG studies (Gevins & Smith, 2000; Jaušovec &
(Cattell, 1971; Horn & Cattell, 1966). Even though the influence of Jaušovec, 2004) as well as in one fMRI study (Rypma et al., 2006) indi-
tDCS on higher cognitive functions has been explored to a greater ex- viduals scoring high on IQ tests displayed less frontal but more parietal
tent than the influence of tACS, we decided to use tACS because of the cortical activation. Thus, it was predicted that active tACS positioned
possibility to investigate the relationship between brain oscillations over parietal brain areas would have a more prominent effect on perfor-
and performance on tests of fluid intelligence. Synchronized cortical mance on tests of fluid intelligence than active tACS positioned over
oscillations in different frequency bands have been proposed to be frontal brain areas.
an important mechanism of high-level cognition giving rise to intel-
ligence, for example, theta band activity has been linked to working 2. Method
memory while narrow alpha band frequencies have been related to
attention (Klimesch, 2012). 2.1. Subjects
With respect to the waveform and the specific oscillatory frequency
used in the present study, two aspects were considered: (1) the Individuals participating in the experiment were recruited from a
established relation between the constructs of WM and intelligence re- large scale resting (eyes-closed) EEG study. They were selected because
ported in correlational (Buehner et al., 2005; Colom et al., 2008; their eyes-closed individual alpha peak frequency (IAF), which is need-
Oberauer et al., 2008; Martínez et al., 2011) and experimental ap- ed to determine theta frequency for tACS, and their IQ scores (WAIS-R,
proaches (for a review see Morrison and Chein, 2011; Rabipour & Raz, Wechsler, 1981), were available. The sample included 28 right-handed
2012); and (2) the relation between working memory processes and individuals (20 females; average age = 20 years and 8 months; SD =
neuronal rhythms in the theta band. It has been shown that theta syn- 4.35 months) that were assigned to two groups and equalized with re-
chronizes during working memory processes and serves as a gating spect to IQ: Parietal group (N = 14; MIQ = 105.38, SD = 9.25) and
mechanism, providing optimal neural conditions for specific processing Frontal group (N = 14; MIQ = 105.45; SD = 8.93). The participants
(Sauseng et al., 2010). Therefore, theta oscillation was applied in the had a similar educational background, took no medication, and reported
tACS protocol. In particular, our goal was to examine whether theta no health problems. Due to artifacts in the resting (eyes-closed) EEG
tACS can affect power in theta and alpha frequency bands during a sub- data, 4 individuals were excluded from the resting EEG data analysis
sequent period of rest and during performance on tests of fluid (N = 24; 16 females; average age = 20 years and 7 months; SD =
intelligence. 5.25 months).
324 A. Pahor, N. Jaušovec / International Journal of Psychophysiology 93 (2014) 322–331

2.2. Tasks and procedure figural matrix with the lower right entry missing appeared on the
screen and the participant was asked to determine which of 4 different
The individuals participated in two sessions—sham and active tACS, options fits into the blank space. The test items were exposed for 6 s
which were counterbalanced. The sham and active settings were sepa- (easy) or 10 s (difficult) at an interstimulus interval of 10 or 14 s respec-
rated by 28 days in order to ensure that the females were tested on tively. Following the presentation of the item and a 2 s interval, a cross
the same day of their menstrual cycle. It has been shown that the rela- was presented during which the participant was instructed to press a
tive release of sexual hormones during different phases of the menstrual button on a response pad (1–4) to indicate his or her answer
cycle affects cognitive responses of females (Amin et al., 2006). Specifi- (Jaušovec & Jaušovec, 2012). We used 50 test items—25 easy (Advanced
cally, high levels of estrogen and progesterone have been associated Progressive Matrices Set I: 12 items and the B Set of the Colored Progres-
with positive affect and higher prefrontal cortex activity. Fig. 1A shows sive Matrices) and 25 difficult (Advanced Progressive Matrices Set II:
the timeline of the experiment. During both sessions, the participants items 12 to 36). The 50 test items were divided into 2 parallel forms
were exposed to 15 min of tACS (in the sham setting only for 1 min— consisting of 25 items each that were used interchangeably for sham
see description below) after which they rested with their eyes closed and active conditions. The correlation between the two equivalent test
for 5 min while their EEG was recorded. Subsequently they solved two forms was r = 0.73 (p b .01, N = 120).
tests of fluid intelligence (RAPM and PF&C) during which EEG data The second task used was based on the Paper Folding and Cutting
was also collected. At the end of the experiment the participants an- (PF&C) subtest of the Stanford–Binet IQ test (Rideout & Laubach,
swered a questionnaire about their sensations during the stimulation 1996). According to the authors, the subtest measures visual–spatial
period. reasoning. In a given trial, the left part of a computer screen showed
The RAPM test was based on a modified version of Raven's progres- how a piece of paper was folded and cut while the right side of the
sive matrices (Raven, 1990), a widely used and well established test of screen showed four unfolded papers, one of which corresponded to
fluid intelligence (Sternberg et al., 1996). The correlation between this the folded paper on the left. The participants were asked to judge
modified version of RAPM and WAIS-R was r = .56 (p b .05, N = 97) which of the four figures on the right corresponded to the figure on
(Jaušovec & Jaušovec, 2012). Similar correlations of the order of 0.40 the left (Jaušovec & Jaušovec, 2012). On sham/active settings, two paral-
to 0.75 were also reported for the standard version of RAPM (Court & lel forms consisting of 20 test items were used. The correlation between
Raven, 1995). For this reason it can be assumed that the modified appli- the two equivalent test forms was r = 0.71 (p b .01, N = 120). The pre-
cation of the RAPM did not significantly alter its metric characteristics. sentation was the same as that of the RAPM except that the 20 items
The task was presented on a computer screen, which was positioned ap- were exposed for 7 s. Fig. 1B shows the timeline of the PF&C task,
proximately 80–100 cm away from the participant. In a given trial, a which roughly corresponds to the timeline of the RAPM task. The

Fig. 1. (A) Sequence of the experiment. (B) Procedure of the PF&C test, which also applies to the RAPM test. (C) Experimental setup: tACS target electrodes positioned over left frontal (F3)
or left parietal sites (P3) and a return electrode positioned above the right eyebrow (Fp2) of the 10/20 system.
A. Pahor, N. Jaušovec / International Journal of Psychophysiology 93 (2014) 322–331 325

correlation between performance on RAPM and PF&C tests in this study individually determined in 2.0 Hz steps based on individual alpha
compared to performance on the same tests in a previous study peak frequency (MIAF = 10.07; SDIAF = 1.25) for four bands: upper
(Jaušovec & Jaušovec, 2012) was r = 0.43 (p b .01; N = 122). alpha = [IAF to IAF + 2.0 Hz]; lower-2 alpha = [IAF to IAF − 2.0 Hz];
lower-1 alpha = [IAF − 2.0 Hz to IAF − 4.0 Hz]; and theta =[IAF
2.3. Electrical stimulation − 4.0 Hz to IAF − 6.0 Hz] (Klimesch, 1999).

Transcranial alternating current stimulation (tACS) was applied via 2.6. Task-related EEG data analysis
two sponge electrodes attached to the head underneath an EEG record-
ing cap (Quik-Cap Compumedics Neuromedical supplies, Charlotte, NC, For the EEG data recorded during task performance, epochs were ex-
USA). Fig. 1C shows the placement of the electrodes: the target electrode tracted, ranging from 2000 ms before stimulus presentation to 10,000 ms
was placed either over the left parietal location (P3) or the left frontal lo- after its presentation. Prior to the analysis, a correction for ocular artifacts
cation (F3), while the return electrode was placed above the right was performed and the epochs were automatically screened for artifacts.
orbitofrontal cortex (Fp2)—above the right eyebrow. The electrodes All epochs showing amplitudes above ±80 μV were excluded. Overall,
were placed in a saline soaked sponge. To increase the focus of stimula- 2.1% of epochs were excluded from further analysis. The induced
tion, the size of the target electrode was 7 × 5 cm whereas the size of the event-related desynchronization/synchronization (ERD/ERS) was deter-
reference electrode was 10 × 7 cm (Moliadze et al., 2010). The waveform mined using the method of complex demodulation with a simultaneous
of the stimulation was sinusoidal without DC offset with a 0° relative signal envelope computation (Pfurtscheller, 1999). For this method, raw
phase. The impedance was kept below 10 kΩ. We applied oscillating data for each channel were multiplied, point-by-point, by a pure cosine
theta currents based on the IAF of each participant (theta = IAF-5 Hz; based on the selected center frequency and by a pure sine with the
M = 5.07; SD = 1.25) using a battery-operated stimulator system same center frequency. Both time series (multiplied by a pure sine and
(DC-stimulator plus, Neuroconn, Ilmenau, Germany). In the active con- cosine) were then low pass filtered by the half-bandwidth (1 Hz).
dition, tACS was applied for 15 min. The current was ramped up and The quantification of induced ERD was done using the inter-trial var-
down over the first and last 15 s of stimulation. In the sham condition, iance method (induced, non-phase-locked activity). The formulas used
the procedure was the same except for the duration of stimulation, were as follows (Pfurtscheller, 1999):
which was applied for 1 min and then turned off automatically. Because
N n o2
most participants feel an itching sensation only initially during tACS, 1 X
IV ð jÞ ¼ y f ði; jÞ −y f ð jÞ : ð1Þ
this procedure prevents awareness of the stimulation conditions N−1 i¼1
(Nitsche et al., 2008).
The magnitude of the stimulating current was based on individually In Eq. (1) N is the total number of trials, yf(i,j) is the j-th sample of i-th
determined thresholds for skin sensations induced by tACS. To determine trial data, and y f ð jÞ is the mean of the j-th sample over all trials. ERD (IV)
these thresholds, we applied tACS stimulation at the individual theta fre- data were used to calculate the ERD/ERS values that were defined as the
quency for 1 min (ramping up and down for 15 ms) at a time and in- percentage change of the power at each sample point (Aj) relative to the
creased the amplitude stepwise by 250 μA starting with 1000 μA until a average power during the resting 1000 ms reference interval
maximum of 2250 μA was reached. Participants were asked to keep (R) preceding the stimulus onset (−1500 ms to −500 ms):
their eyes closed and to indicate the presence of a sensation. For the re-
maining experiment, stimulation intensity was kept at 250 μA below R−Aj
ERD=ERSð jÞ % ¼ : ð2Þ
the threshold for skin sensations (Modus = 1750 μA peak-to-peak; R
Range = 1000 μA to 2250 μA). The tACS parameter characteristics were
in the range suggested by Zaghi et al. (2010) for the (1) stimulation du- A positive ERD indicates a power decrease, and a negative ERS indi-
ration 5 min–30 min, (2) stimulation intensity 500 μA–2000 μA, and cates a power increase (Pfurtscheller, 1999). Due to statistical con-
(3) scalp current density 24 μA/cm2–29 μA/cm2. straints, we did not conduct separate ERD/ERS analyses for easy and
difficult RAMP items. The ERD/ERS values were determined for six
2.4. EEG recording 1000 ms time windows (from stimulus onset till 6000 ms) and were
collapsed for different electrode locations, distinguishing the hemi-
EEG was recorded using a Quick-Cap with sintered (Silver/Silver spheres as well as frontal, central, and parietal brain areas. The electrode
Chloride; 8 mm diameter) electrodes. Using the 10–20 Electrode Place- positions were aggregated as follows: frontal left (Fp1, F3, F7, Fz), fron-
ment System of the International Federation, EEG activity was moni- tal right (Fp2, F4, F8, Fz), central left (T3, C3, Cz), central right (T4, C4,
tored over 19 scalp locations (Fp1, Fp2, F3, F4, F7, F8, T3, T4, T5, T6, Cz), parietal left (T5, P3, O1, Pz), and parietal right (T6, P4, O2, Pz).
C3, C4, P3, P4, O1, O2, Fz, Cz, and Pz). All leads were referenced to linked
mastoids (A1 and A2), and a ground electrode was applied to the fore- 3. Results
head. Additionally, vertical eye movements were recorded with elec-
trodes placed above and below the left eye. Electrode impedance was 3.1. Resting EEG
maintained below 5 kΩ. The digital EEG data acquisition and analysis
system (SynAmps RT) had a bandpass of 0.15–100.0 Hz. At cutoff fre- To explore the influences of tACS on power measures, a GLM for re-
quencies, the voltage gain was approximately −6 dB. The 19 EEG traces peated measures was conducted separately for the Parietal and the Fron-
were digitized online at 1000 Hz with a gain of 10 × (accuracy of tal group. The following variables were defined: tACS (sham/active),
29.80 nV/LSB in a 24 bit A to D conversion) and stored on a hard disk. hemisphere (left/right), and location (frontal, central, parieto-occipital).
In the Frontal group, the only significant influence of theta tACS on
2.5. Resting EEG data analysis power measures was observed in the lower-2 alpha band. The GLM
showed a significant interaction effect between tACS and location
In order to examine whether sham and active stimulation led to (F(2,22) = 3.80; p = 0.04; partial eta2 = 0.26). Fig. 2 shows that the de-
different baseline activity, resting EEG data was analyzed in terms crease in lower-2 alpha power was most prominent in frontal brain areas.
of spectral power. Comprised were epochs of 4096 data points. A The GLM for the Parietal group showed significant differences be-
Fast Fourier Transformation (FFT) was performed on the epoched tween sham and active settings in theta, lower-1, and lower-2 alpha fre-
EEG data in order to derive estimates of absolute spectral power quency bands, all showing a significant interaction between tACS and
(uV) in different frequency bands. The frequency bands were location (theta: F(2,22) = 5.90; p = 0.009; partial eta2 = 0.35; lower-
326 A. Pahor, N. Jaušovec / International Journal of Psychophysiology 93 (2014) 322–331

Fig. 2. (A) Topographic power maps of sham and active resting eyes-closed neuroelectric activity in the theta band and (B) alpha band (collapsed for the lower-1 and lower-2 alpha bands)
after theta tACS—target electrode positioned over the left parietal site (P3).

1 alpha: F(2,22) = 7.79; p = 0.003; partial eta2 = .42; lower-2 alpha: On the other hand, in the two alpha bands, participants in the active set-
F(2,22) = 11.10; p = 0.001; partial eta2 = 0.50). As can be seen in ting showed decreased alpha power mainly in the posterior brain areas
Fig. 3A, in the active setting participants showed increased theta (Fig. 3B). Note that in Fig. 3B the power spectra for the lower-1 and
power mainly in left frontal brain areas compared to the sham setting. lower-2 alpha frequency bands are collapsed.

Fig. 3. Topographic power map of sham and active resting eyes-closed neuroelectric activity in the lower-2 alpha band after theta tACS—target electrode positioned over left frontal site
(F3).
A. Pahor, N. Jaušovec / International Journal of Psychophysiology 93 (2014) 322–331 327

3.2. Behavioral data the variables tACS, location and group (F(2,52) = 4.39; p = .02; η2 =
.14) were observed.
The questionnaire data were analyzed with a Wilcoxon nonpara- To gain a deeper insight into the influences of tACS on brain activa-
metric test for two related samples (sham/active). The test showed tion patterns during problem solving, a GLM was conducted separately
no significant differences between participants' sensations during for each group: tACS (sham/active) × task (RAPM/PF&C) × time (six
sham and active tACS (Z(11) = .06; p = 0.98). Performance data 1000 ms segments) × hemisphere (left/right) × location (frontal, cen-
were analyzed with a general linear model (GLM) for repeated tral, occipito-parietal).
measures—tACS (sham/active) × task (RAPM/PF&C) × group (Parie- In the Frontal group, active compared to sham tACS significantly af-
tal/Frontal). The GLM showed a significant main effect of tACS fected power in two bands: theta and lower-1 alpha. Namely, in the
(F(1,26) = 27.19; p b .001; η2 = .51), and a significant interaction ef- theta band, significant interaction effects between the variables tACS
fect between tACS and group (F(1,26) = 6.19; p = .02; η2 = .19). Ac- and task (F(1,13) = 4.36; p = .05; η2 = .25); tACS and location
tive tACS improved performance on tests of fluid intelligence. This (F(2,26) = 5.34; p = .01; η2 = .29); and among the variables tACS,
influence was more pronounced for the Parietal group. Subsequent task, and time (F(5,55) = 2.88; p = .04; η2 = .21) were observed. Dif-
paired-sample t tests between sham and active settings, which were ferences in theta ERD/ERS between active and sham settings were more
FDR-corrected (Benjamini & Hochberg, 1995), were significant both pronounced for the RAPM test. Fig. 4 shows that in the active setting,
for RAPM and PF&C tests. These differences were more pronounced participants displayed a time related (3000–6000 ms) decrease in
for the difficult test items than for the easy items. In the active setting, theta power mainly in frontal brain areas while solving the RAPM task.
participants scored notably higher on both tests, mainly by correctly In the lower-1 alpha band, there was a significant interaction effect
solving more difficult test items than in the sham setting. among tACS, task, and hemisphere (F(1,13) = 8.77; p = .01; η2 = .40).
Participants in the Frontal group also showed a significant increase As can be seen in Fig. 5, during the RAPM test, participants in the active
in performance, however, subsequent paired sample t-tests between setting showed a decrease in lower-1 alpha power over the left hemi-
the sham and active settings for the RAPM and PF&C total test scores sphere compared to the sham setting.
were not significant. As can be seen in Table 1, the only significant differ- In the Parietal group, significant differences in power as a factor of
ence between sham/active settings observed for this group was for the tACS were observed only in one band: lower-2 alpha. Specifically, a sig-
easy RAPM test items. In the active setting the participants correctly nificant main effect of tACS (F(1,13) = 14.45; p = .002; η2 = .53),
solved more easy RAPM test items than in the sham setting. and an interaction effect between the variables tACS and location
(F(1,13) = 4.19; p = .03; η2 = .24) were observed. As can be seen in
Fig. 6, participants in the active setting showed higher lower-2 alpha
3.3. Task-related EEG ERD than in the sham setting, which was more pronounced in frontal
brain areas.
In order to examine whether the site of stimulation influenced oscil-
latory power during performance on tests of fluid intelligence, a GLM for 4. Discussion
repeated measures with the following factors was conducted: tACS
(sham/active) × task (RAPM/PF&C) × time (six 1000 ms segments) × The results showed that active tACS in the theta frequency signifi-
hemisphere (left/right) × location (frontal, central, occipito-parietal) × cantly influenced resting brain oscillatory activity. Theta tACS applied
group (Parietal/Frontal). A significant effect of the factor group was ob- to left frontal and parietal areas significantly decreased lower-1 and
served in the theta, lower-1 and lower-2 alpha frequency bands. In the lower-2 alpha power in brain areas located close to or beneath the site
theta frequency band, the GLM analysis showed a significant interaction of stimulation. Furthermore, theta tACS delivered to the left parietal
effect among thevariables tACS, task, and group (F(1,26) = 8.00; p = area significantly increased theta power in frontal brain areas. The effect
.009; η2 = .24); and among the variables tACS, location, and group of active tACS compared to sham stimulation was also observed on per-
(F(2,52) = 4.51; p = .02; η2 = .15). In the lower-1 alpha frequency formance on tests of fluid intelligence. Active theta tACS improved per-
band, a significant interaction effect among the variables tACS, task, formance on Raven's progressive matrices and on the Paper Fold & Cut
hemisphere and group (F(1,26) = 4.30; p = .04; η2 = .14) was observed. test. The improvement was more pronounced in the group of partici-
In the lower-2 alpha frequency band a significant main effect of tACS pants that received stimulation to the left parietal area than in the
(F(1,26) = 6.87; p = .01; η2 = .21); and an interaction effect among group that received stimulation to the left frontal area. Spectral power
calculated during performance on these tests showed that the parietal
Table 1 stimulation had a general influence on ERD/ERS patterns, whereas fron-
Descriptive statistics (means and standard deviations) for RAPM and PF&C tests (distin- tal stimulation showed a task dependent influence that was related to
guished between total scores and scores on easy/difficult items) in sham and active tACS improved performance on the RAPM test.
settings for the Parietal and Frontal groups.

Sham setting Active setting df(13) 4.1. tACS effects on resting EEG
M SD M SD
The observed effects of theta tACS on resting neuronal electric activ-
Parietal
RAPM—total 13.50 3.35 15.79 2.01 t = 2.88; p = .01⁎; d = .77 ity demonstrate that tACS can be used to specifically modulate oscillato-
RAPM—easy 8.86 1.70 9.64 .63 t = 1.76; p = .10; d = .47 ry activity in particular brain areas—most likely by interfering with the
RAPM—difficult 4.64 2.27 6.14 1.92 t = 3.07; p = .009⁎; d = .82 ongoing rhythmic activity of local pacemaker networks. Interestingly,
PF&C—total 10.29 2.13 12.64 3.43 t = 3.16; p = .008⁎; d = .77
the after-effects of theta tACS were more prominent when stimulation
PF&C—easy 4.93 1.27 5.57 1.79 t = 1.04; p = .41; d = .28
PF&C—difficult 5.36 1.87 7.07 1.92 t = 3.97; p = .002⁎; d = 1.06
was applied to the left parietal site compared to the left frontal site. Pa-
rietal stimulation led to an increase in theta power in left frontal areas
Frontal
and a decrease in lower-1 alpha and lower-2 alpha power in posterior
RAPM—total 14.00 2.72 14.86 3.13 t = 1.06; p = .18; d = .28
RAPM—easy 8.71 1.44 9.72 1.33 t = 2.39; p = .03⁎; d = .64
areas. The functional importance of stimulating in theta frequency
RAPM—difficult 5.29 1.68 5.14 2.63 t = .22; p = .83; d = .06 should be considered in the light of the findings that (1) local theta ac-
PF&C—total 11.71 2.27 12.50 3.16 t = 1.01; p = .31; d = .27 tivity increases during working memory processes, and (2) interregional
PF&C—easy 5.86 1.17 5.36 1.15 t = 1.29; p = .22; d = .34 synchronization of theta oscillations connects different neural struc-
PF&C—difficult 5.85 1.56 7.14 2.88 t = 1.66; p = .12; d = .44
tures that are involved in working memory processes (Sauseng et al.,
⁎ FDR adjusted significance. 2010). The finding that theta tACS applied to the left parietal area
328 A. Pahor, N. Jaušovec / International Journal of Psychophysiology 93 (2014) 322–331

Fig. 4. Topographic power map of neuroelectric activity in the theta frequency band recorded during the RAPM test after sham and active theta tACS—target electrode positioned over the
left frontal site (F3).

increased theta power in frontal areas supports the idea that theta oscil- region might have had a more prominent effect on brain activity due
lations reflect a general integrative mechanism in the brain (Sauseng to the fact that it has been identified as one of the brain regions
et al., 2010). On the other hand, tACS applied to the left frontal area re- (termed hubs) that plays a central role in the organization of neural net-
sulted in a decrease in lower-2 alpha power in frontal areas, but did not works (Van den Heuvel and Sporns, 2013; Langer et al., 2012; Achard
affect theta power or any other alpha bands. Stimulating the left parietal et al., 2006).

Fig. 5. Topographic power map of neuroelectric activity in the lower-1 alpha frequency band recorded during the RAPM test after sham and active theta tACS—target electrode positioned
over the left frontal site (F3). The power maps are averaged over 6000 ms.
A. Pahor, N. Jaušovec / International Journal of Psychophysiology 93 (2014) 322–331 329

Fig. 6. Topographic power map of neuroelectric activity in the lower-2 alpha frequency band recorded during the RAPM and PF&C tests after sham and active theta tACS—target electrode
positioned over the left parietal site (P3). The power maps are averaged over 6000 ms.

4.2. Behavioral results stimulation in parietal and frontal areas differently influenced cognitive
processes involved in fluid intelligence. One possible explanation may
Stimulation applied to the left parietal brain area had a more pro- be that left parietal tACS mainly increased WM storage capacity thereby
nounced effect on performance on tests of fluid intelligence than stimu- improving performance on difficult test items, whereas left frontal tACS
lation applied to the left frontal area. This finding is in line with research mainly influenced the attentional components required for successful
demonstrating the relationship between performance on tests of intel- performance on easy test items. Support for the former interpretation
ligence and brain activity in parietal areas (Lee et al., 2006; Haier et al., comes from the relation between working memory processes and neu-
2003; Wharton et al., 2000) and lends further support to the assump- ronal rhythms in frontal and parietal brain areas (Sauseng et al., 2010)
tion that working memory storage capacity largely accounts for the re- and from studies employing tACS (Jaušovec & Jaušovec, 2014;
lationship between the psychological constructs of working memory Jaušovec et al., 2014). Support for the latter claim comes from a recent
and intelligence (Colom et al., 2008; Buehner et al., 2005). The impor- study in which active tDCS applied to the prefrontal cortex resulted in
tance of the left parietal lobe for working memory storage capacity an improvement in target detection performance (Nelson et al., 2014).
has been demonstrated by several neuroimaging (Cowan et al., 2011; According to the authors, active tDCS enhanced sustained attention,
Cowan, 2011; Majerus et al., 2010; Xu & Chun, 2006; Todd & Marois, which improved task performance. However, given the fact that a grow-
2004), rTMS (Postle et al., 2006), and tDCS (Tseng et al., 2012) studies. ing number of studies consider working memory and attention as over-
In a recent study by Jaušovec and Jaušovec (2014), theta tACS applied lapping constructs (Gazzaley & Nobre, 2012; Chun, 2011; Postle, 2006;
to the left parietal area significantly increased participants' working Awh et al., 2006), specific conclusions about the effects of tACS on
memory capacity. Furthermore, the ERP analysis indicated a decrease task-related mental processes cannot be made.
in P300 latency suggesting an improvement in the participants' ability
to rapidly allocate attention resources needed to solve the task. A similar 4.3. Task-related EEG
finding was also reported by Tseng et al. (2012), who showed that tDCS
over the right parietal cortex mainly improved the ability to suppress ir- The observed effects of active tACS on behavioral variables were also
relevant distractors in poor performers. accompanied by changes in neuroelectric activity during performance
On the other hand, stimulating the left frontal area had a minor effect on tests of fluid intelligence. Active stimulation applied to the left pari-
on test performance. On the RAPM test, participants scored higher on etal area was associated with a global decrease in lower-2 alpha power
easy test items in the active stimulation session compared to the sham in the whole brain, which was the strongest in left frontal areas. Re-
session. There were no differences in performance on the PF&C test be- search suggests that a reduction of power in lower alpha reflects general
tween the two sessions. This finding is in line with research showing task demands, particularly attentional processes (Klimesch et al., 1999;
that low-ability subjects rely more exclusively on frontal regions during Doppelmayr et al., 2002). In contrast, frontal stimulation was associated
performance on tests of intelligence, whereas high-ability individuals with a task-dependent decrease in theta power in left frontal areas
make relatively greater use of parietal regions (Lee et al, 2006; along with improved performance on easy RAPM test items. During
Jaušovec & Jaušovec, 2004; Gevins & Smith, 2000). Furthermore, the RAPM test, participants in the active setting also showed a decrease
Langer et al. (2012) demonstrated that the more intelligent a person in lower-1 alpha power over the left hemisphere. Active theta tACS ap-
is, the more his or her brain networks resemble a small world network. plied to the left frontal area either increased the participants' focus of at-
In particular, the parietal cortex was identified as a main hub within tention or improved binding in working memory, which resulted in
such small world networks. improved performance on the easy RAPM test items.
The analysis of the influence of tACS on easy and difficult test items There are two prominent neurobiological models that explain the
revealed that left parietal tACS had a more pronounced influence on dif- relation between intelligence and brain activity: the parieto-frontal in-
ficult test items (RAPM and PF&C), whereas left frontal tACS had a more tegration (P-FIT) model (Jung and Haier, 2007) and Duncan's adaptive
pronounced influence on easy test items (RAPM). It can be assumed that coding model (Duncan, 2001, 2003). On the whole, the results of the
330 A. Pahor, N. Jaušovec / International Journal of Psychophysiology 93 (2014) 322–331

present study lend support to the P-FIT model proposed by Jung and Colom, R., Burgaleta, M., Román, F.J., Karama, S., Álvarez-Linera, J., Abad, F.J., Haier, R.J.,
Haier (2007), suggesting that discrete brain regions play a role in gener- 2013a. Neuroanatomic overlap between intelligence and cognitive factors: mor-
phometry methods provide support for the key role of the frontal lobes. NeuroImage
al intelligence, with an emphasis on the parietal cortex and do not lend 72, 143–152.
support to Duncan's (2003) assumption that psychometric g is entirely Colom, R., Román, F.J., Abad, F.J., Shih, P.C., Privado, J., Froufe, M., Escorial, S., et al., 2013b.
executed within processing networks of the prefrontal cortex. Adaptive n-back training does not improve fluid intelligence at the construct level:
gains on individual tests suggest that training may enhance visuospatial processing.
The main limitation of the current study is that we did not record Intelligence 41, 712–727.
pre-stimulation resting EEG data. Comparing pre- and post-stimulation Court, J.H., Raven, J.C., 1995. Manual for Raven's Progressive Matrices and Vocabulary
EEG data would have given further insight into the effects of theta tACS Scales. Section 7: Research and References: Summaries of Normative, Reliability,
and Validity Studies and References to All Sections. Oxford Psychologists Press/San
on brain activity. Another shortcoming of the study is the predominate- Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation, Oxford.
ly female structure of the sample. Given the immense body of evidence Cowan, N., 2011. The focus of attention as observed in visual working memory tasks:
with regard to sex related differences in general, as well as differences in making sense of competing claims. Neuropsychologia 49 (6), 1401–1406.
Cowan, N., Li, D., Moffitt, A., Becker, T.M., Martin, E.A., Saults, J.S., Christ, S.E., 2011. A neu-
specific abilities (e.g., Johnson & Bouchard, 2007; Nyborg, 2005), the
ral region of abstract working memory. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 23 (10), 2852–2863.
structure of the sample might have influenced the results. Furthermore, Dockery, C.A., Hueckel-Weng, R., Birbaumer, N., Plewnia, C., 2009. Enhancement of plan-
tACS was delivered only in the theta frequency band, therefore it cannot ning ability by transcranial direct current stimulation. J. Neurosci. 29 (22),
be assumed that the obtained results are specific for theta stimulation. 7271–7277.
Doppelmayr, M., Klimesch, W., Stadler, W., Pöllhuber, D., Heine, C., 2002. EEG alpha power
Nevertheless, the results of the study demonstrate for the first time and intelligence. Intelligence 30 (3), 289–302. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0160-
that theta tACS increases resting theta power and, contrary to existing 2896(01)00101-5.
research (Zaehle et al., 2010; Merlet et al., 2013), also affects power in Duncan, J., Owen, A.M., 2000. Common regions of the human frontal lobe recruited by di-
verse cognitive demands. Trends Neurosci. 23 (10), 475–483.
frequencies other than the stimulating frequency, namely in the alpha Duncan, J., Seitz, R.J., Kolodny, J., Bor, D., Herzog, H., Ahmed, A., Emslie, H., 2000. A neural
band. Moreover, the results showed that theta tACS applied to the left basis for general intelligence. Science 289 (5478), 457–460. http://dx.doi.org/10.
parietal brain area moderately increases performance on tests of fluid 1126/science.289.5478.457.
Duncan, J., 2001. An adaptive coding model of neural function in prefrontal cortex. Nat.
intelligence. The observed behavioral and neuroelectric data tentatively Rev. Neurosci. 2 (11), 820–829. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/35097557.
support the neurobiological P-FIT model of intelligence. Future research Duncan, J., 2003. Intelligence tests predict brain response to demanding task events. Nat.
could examine whether theta tACS combined with working memory Neurosci. 6, 207–208.
Duncan, J., 2006. Brain mechanisms of attention. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 59, 2–27.
training further improves performance on tests of fluid intelligence.
Egner, T., Zech, T.F., Gruzelier, J.H., 2004. The effects of neurofeedback training on the
spectral topography of the electroencephalogram. Clin. Neurophysiol. 115 (11),
2452–2460.
References Fregni, F., Boggio, P.S., Nitsche, M.A., Rigonatti, S.P., Pascual-Leone, A., 2006. Cognitive ef-
fects of repeated sessions of transcranial direct current stimulation in patients with
Achard, S., Salvador, R., Whitcher, B., Suckling, J., Bullmore, E., 2006. A resilient, low- depression. Depress. Anxiety 23 (8), 482–484.
frequency, small-world human brain functional network with highly connected asso- Gazzaley, A., Nobre, A.C., 2012. Top-down modulation: bridging selective attention and
ciation cortical hubs. J. Neurosci. 26 (1), 63–72. http://dx.doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI. working memory. Trends Cogn. Sci. 16 (2), 129–135. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.
3874-05.2006. tics.2011.11.014.
Amin, Z., Epperson, C.N., Constable, R.T., Canli, T., 2006. Effects of estrogen variation on Gevins, A., Smith, M.E., 2000. Neurophysiological measures of working memory and indi-
neural correlates of emotional response inhibition. NeuroImage 32 (1), 457–464. vidual differences in cognitive ability and cognitive style. Cereb. Cortex 10 (9),
Awh, E., Vogel, E.K., Oh, S.-H., 2006. Interactions between attention and working memory. 829–839. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/cercor/10.9.829.
Neuroscience 139 (1), 201–208. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience.2005.08. Gläscher, J., Tranel, D., Paul, L.K., Rudrauf, D., Rorden, C., Hornaday, A., et al., 2009. Lesion
023. mapping of cognitive abilities linked to intelligence. Neuron 61, 681–691.
Barbey, A.K., Colom, R., Solomon, J., Krueger, F., Forbes, C., Grafman, J., 2012. An integrative Gottfredson, L.S., 1997. Mainstream science on intelligence: an editorial with 52 signato-
architecture for general intelligence and executive function revealed by lesion map- ries, history, and bibliography. Intelligence 24 (1), 13–23.
ping. Brain 135 (4), 1154–1164. Gray, J.R., Chabris, C.F., Braver, T.S., 2003. Neural mechanisms of general fluid intelligence.
Benjamini, Y., Hochberg, Y., 1995. Controlling the false discovery rate: a practical and Nat. Neurosci. 6 (3), 316–322.
powerful approach to multiple testing. J. R. Stat. Soc. Ser. A 57, 289–300. Gruzelier, J., 2009. A theory of alpha/theta neurofeedback, creative performance enhance-
Bindman, L.J., Lippold, O.C.J., Redfearn, W.T., 1964. The action of brief polarizing currents ment, long distance functional connectivity and psychological integration. Cogn. Pro-
on the cerebral cortex of the rat (1) during current flow and (2) in the production cess. 10 (S1), 101–109.
of long-lasting after-effects. J. Physiol. 172, 369–382. Haier, R.J., White, N.S., Alkire, M.T., 2003. Individual differences in general intelligence cor-
Bishop, S.J., Fossella, J., Croucher, C.J., Duncan, J., 2008. COMT val158met genotype affects relate with brain function during nonreasoning tasks. Intelligence 31 (5), 429–441.
neural mechanisms supporting fluid intelligence. Cereb. Cortex 18, 2132–2140. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0160-2896(03)00025-4.
Boggio, P.S., Sultani, N., Fecteau, S., Merabet, L., Mecca, T., Pascual-Leone, A., Fregni, F., Haier, R.J., Jung, R.E., Yeo, R.A., Head, K., Alkire, M.T., 2004. Structural brain variation and
2008. Prefrontal cortex modulation using transcranial DC stimulation reduces alcohol general intelligence. NeuroImage 23, 425–433.
craving: a double-blind, sham-controlled study. Drug Alcohol Depend. 92 (1–3). Haier, R.J., 2014. Increased intelligence is a myth (so far). Front. Syst. Neurosci. 8. http://dx.doi.
Buehner, M., Krumm, S., Pick, M., 2005. Reasoning = working memory ≠ attention. In- org/10.3389/fnsys.2014.00034.
telligence 33 (3), 251–272. Horn, J.L., Cattell, R.B., 1966. Refinement and test of the theory of fluid and crystallized in-
Buschkuehl, M., Jaeggi, S.M., 2010. Improving intelligence: a literature review. Swiss Med. telligence. J. Educ. Psychol. 57, 253–270.
Wkly. 140 (19–20), 266–272 (doi:smw-12852). Jaeggi, S.M., Buschkuehl, M., Jonides, J., Perrig, W.J., 2008. From the cover: improving fluid
Cappelletti, M., Gessaroli, E., Hithersay, R., Mitolo, M., Didino, D., Kanai, R., Walsh, V., 2013. intelligence with training on working memory. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 105 (19),
Transfer of cognitive training across magnitude dimensions achieved with concurrent 6829–6833.
brain stimulation of the parietal lobe. J. Neurosci. 33, 14899–14907. Jaušovec, N., Jaušovec, K., 2004. Differences in induced brain activity during the perfor-
Cattell, R.B., 1971. Abilities: Their Structure, Growth, and Action. Houghton Mifflin, mance of learning and working-memory tasks related to intelligence. Brain Cogn.
New York. 54 (1), 65–74.
Cerruti, C., Schlaug, G., 2009. Anodal transcranial direct current stimulation of the prefron- Jaušovec, N., Jaušovec, K., 2012. Working memory training: improving intelligence—
tal cortex enhances complex verbal associative thought. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 21 (10), changing brain activity. Brain Cogn. 79 (2), 96–106.
1980–1987. Jaušovec, N., Jaušovec, K., 2014. Increasing working memory capacity with theta transcra-
Chi, R.P., Snyder, A.W., 2011. Facilitate insight by non-invasive brain stimulation. In: nial alternating current stimulation (tACS). Biol. Psychol. 96, 42–47.
Bishop, D. (Ed.), PLoS ONE, 6(2), p. e16655. Jaušovec, N., Jaušovec, K., Pahor, A., 2014. The influence of theta transcranial alternating
Chooi, W.-T., Thompson, L.A., 2012. Working memory training does not improve intelli- current stimulation (tACS) on working memory storage and processing functions.
gence in healthy young adults. Intelligence 40 (6), 531–542. Acta Psychol. 146, 1–6.
Chun, M.M., 2011. Visual working memory as visual attention sustained internally over time. Jensen, A.R., 1998. The g Factor. Westport. Praeger, Westport, Connecticut.
Neuropsychologia 49 (6), 1407–1409. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia. Johnson, W., Bouchard Jr., T.J., 2007. Sex differences in mental ability: g masks the dimen-
2011.01.029. sions on which they lie. Intelligence 35, 23–39.
Colom, R., Jung, R.E., Haier, R.J., 2006. Distributed brain sites for the g-factor of intelligence. Jung, R.E., Haier, R.J., 2007. The Parieto-Frontal Integration Theory (P-FIT) of intelligence:
NeuroImage 31 (3), 1359–1365. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.01. converging neuroimaging evidence. Behav. Brain Sci. 30 (02), 135.
006. Keizer, A.W., Verschoor, M., Verment, R.S., Hommel, B., 2010. The effect of gamma en-
Colom, R., Abad, F.J., Quiroga, M.Á., Shih, P.C., Flores-Mendoza, C., 2008. Working memory hancing neurofeedback on the control of feature bindings and intelligence measures.
and intelligence are highly related constructs, but why? Intelligence 36 (6), 584–606. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 75 (1), 25–32.
Colom, R., Quiroga, M.Á., Solana, A.B., Burgaleta, M., Román, F.J., Privado, J., Karama, S., Klimesch, W., 1999. EEG alpha and theta oscillations reflect cognitive and memory
2012. Structural changes after videogame practice related to a brain network associ- performance: a review and analysis. Brain Res. Brain Res. Rev. 29 (2–3),
ated with intelligence. Intelligence 40 (5), 479–489. 169–195.
A. Pahor, N. Jaušovec / International Journal of Psychophysiology 93 (2014) 322–331 331

Klimesch, W., Vogt, F., Doppelmayr, M., 1999. Interindividual differences in alpha and theta Rabipour, S., Raz, A., 2012. Training the brain: fact and fad in cognitive and behavioral re-
power reflect memory performance. Intelligence 27 (4), 347–362. http://dx.doi.org/ mediation. Brain Cogn. 79 (2), 159–179.
10.1016/S0160-2896(99)00027-6. Rae, C., Digney, A.L., McEwan, S.R., Bates, T.C., 2003. Oral creatine monohydrate supple-
Klimesch, W., 2012. Alpha-band oscillations, attention, and controlled access to stored in- mentation improves brain performance: a double–blind, placebo–controlled, cross–
formation. Trends Cogn. Sci. 16 (12), 606–617. over trial. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences
Klingberg, T., 2010. Training and plasticity of working memory. Trends Cogn. Sci. 14 (7), 270 (1529), 2147–2150. http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2003.2492.
317–324. Raven, J.C., 1990. Advanced Progressive Matrices: Sets I, II. Oxford University Press.
Kolb, B., Whishaw, I.Q., 2009. Fundamentals of Human Neuropsychology (Sixth.). Worth, Rideout, B.E., Laubach, C.M., 1996. EEG correlates of enhanced spatial performance follow-
New York, NY. ing exposure to music. Percept. Mot. Skills 82 (2), 427–432.
Kuo, M.F., Nitsche, M.A., 2012. Effects of transcranial electrical stimulation on cognition. Roca, M., Parr, A., Thompson, R., Woolgar, A., Torralva, T., Antoun, N., Duncan, J., 2010. Ex-
Clin. EEG Neurosci. 43 (3), 192–199. ecutive function and fluid intelligence after frontal lobe lesions. Brain 133 (1),
Langer, N., Pedroni, A., Gianotti, L.R.R., Hänggi, J., Knoch, D., Jäncke, L., 2012. Functional 234–247. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/brain/awp269.
brain network efficiency predicts intelligence. Hum. Brain Mapp. 33 (6), Rypma, B., Berger, J.S., Prabhakaran, V., Bly, B.M., Kimberg, D.Y., Biswal, B.B., D'Esposito, M.,
1393–1406. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/hbm.21297. 2006. Neural correlates of cognitive efficiency. NeuroImage 33, 969–979.
Lee, K.H., Choi, Y.Y., Gray, J.R., Cho, S.H., Chae, J.H., Lee, S., Kim, K., 2006. Neural correlates Santarnecchi, E., Polizzotto, N.R., Godone, M., Giovannelli, F., Feurra, M., Matzen, L., Rossi,
of superior intelligence: stronger recruitment of posterior parietal cortex. S., 2013. Frequency-dependent enhancement of fluid intelligence induced by trans-
NeuroImage 29, 578–586. cranial oscillatory potentials. Curr. Biol. 23, 1449–1453.
Majerus, S., D'Argembeau, A., Martinez Perez, T., Belayachi, S., Van der Linden, M., Collette, Sauseng, P., Griesmayr, B., Freunberger, R., Klimesch, W., 2010. Control mechanisms in
F., Maquet, P., 2010. The commonality of neural networks for verbal and visual short- working memory: a possible function of EEG theta oscillations. Neurosci. Biobehav.
term memory. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 22 (11), 2570–2593. Rev. 34 (7), 1015–1022.
Martínez, K., Burgaleta, M., Román, F.J., Escorial, S., Shih, P.C., Quiroga, M.Á., Colom, R., Schoenthaler, S.J., Bier, I.D., Young, K., Nichols, D., Jansenns, S., 2000. The effect of vitamin-
2011. Can fluid intelligence be reduced to “simple” short-term storage? Intelligence mineral supplementation on the intelligence of American schoolchildren: a random-
39 (6), 473–480. ized, double-blind placebo-controlled trial. J. Altern. Complement. Med. 6 (1), 19–29.
Merlet, I., Birot, G., Salvador, R., Molaee-Ardekani, B., Mekonnen, A., Soria-Frish, A., Stephenson, C.L., Halpern, D.F., 2013. Improved matrix reasoning is limited to training on
Wendling, F., 2013. From oscillatory transcranial current stimulation to scalp EEG tasks with a visuospatial component. Intelligence 41, 341–357.
changes: a biophysical and physiological modeling study. PLoS ONE 8 (2), e57330. Sternberg, R.J., Ferrari, M., Clinkenbeard, P., Grigorenko, E.L., 1996. Identification, instruc-
http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0057330. tion, and assessment of gifted children: a construct validation of a triarchic model.
Moliadze, V., Antal, A., Paulus, W., 2010. Boosting brain excitability by transcranial high Gifted Child Quart. 40 (3), 129–137.
frequency stimulation in the ripple range. J. Physiol. 588 (24), 4891–4904. Stough, C., Camfield, D., Kure, C., Tarasuik, J., Downey, L., Lloyd, J., Reynolds, J., 2011. Im-
Morrison, A.B., Chein, J.M., 2011. Does working memory training work? The promise and proving general intelligence with a nutrient-based pharmacological intervention. In-
challenges of enhancing cognition by training working memory. Psychon. Bull. Rev. telligence 39 (2–3), 100–107.
18 (1), 46–60. Surmeli, T., Ertem, A., 2010. Post WISC-R and TOVA improvement with QEEG guided
Mulquiney, P.G., Hoy, K.E., Daskalakis, Z.J., Fitzgerald, P.B., 2011. Improving working neurofeedback training in mentally retarded: a clinical case series of behavioral prob-
memory: exploring the effect of transcranial random noise stimulation and transcra- lems. Clin. EEG Neurosci. 41 (1), 32–41.
nial direct current stimulation on the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. Clin. Thompson, T.W., Waskom, M.L., Garel, K.-L.A., Cardenas-Iniguez, C., Reynolds, G.O.,
Neurophysiol. 122 (12), 2384–2389. Winter, R., Gabrieli, J.D.E., 2013. Failure of working memory training to enhance cog-
Nelson, J.T., McKinley, R.A., Golob, E.J., Warm, J.S., Parasuraman, R., 2014. Enhancing vigilance nition or intelligence. In: de Beeck, H.P.O.p. (Ed.), PLoS ONE, 8(5), p. e63614.
in operators with prefrontal cortex transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS). Thut, G., Miniussi, C., 2009. New insights into rhythmic brain activity from TMS-EEG stud-
NeuroImage 85, 909–917. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.11.061 (Part 3). ies. Trends Cogn. Sci. 13, 182–189.
Neuling, T., Rach, S., Herrmann, C.S., 2013. Orchestrating neuronal networks: sustained Thut, G., Veniero, D., Romei, V., Miniussi, C., Schyns, P., Gross, J., 2011. Rhythmic TMS
after-effects of transcranial alternating current stimulation depend upon brain states. causes local entrainment of natural oscillatory signatures. Curr. Biol. 21, 1176–1185.
Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7. http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2013.00161. Todd, J.J., Marois, R., 2004. Capacity limit of visual short-term memory in human posterior
Nitsche, M.A., Cohen, L.G., Wassermann, E.M., Priori, A., Lang, N., Antal, A., Pascual-Leone, parietal cortex. Nature 428 (6984), 751–754.
A., 2008. Transcranial direct current stimulation: state of the art 2008. Brain Stimul. 1 Tseng, P., Hsu, T.-Y., Chang, C.-F., Tzeng, O.J.L., Hung, D.L., Muggleton, N.G., Juan, C.-H.,
(3), 206–223. 2012. Unleashing potential: transcranial direct current stimulation over the right
Nyborg, H., 2005. Sex-related differences in general intelligence g, brain size, and social posterior parietal cortex improves change detection in low-performing individuals.
status. Personal. Individ. Differ. 39, 497–509. J. Neurosci. 32 (31), 10554–10561.
Oberauer, K., Süβ, H.-M., Wilhelm, O., Wittmann, W.W., 2008. Which working memory Van den Heuvel, M.P., Sporns, O., 2013. Network hubs in the human brain. Trends Cogn.
functions predict intelligence? Intelligence 36 (6), 641–652. Sci. 17 (12), 683–696. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2013.09.012.
Owen, A.M., Hampshire, A., Grahn, J.A., Stenton, R., Dajani, S., Burns, A.S., Ballard, C.G., von Bastian, C.C., Oberauer, K., 2013. Distinct transfer effects of training different facets of
2010. Putting brain training to the test. Nature 465 (7299), 775–778. working memory capacity. J. Mem. Lang. 69, 36–58.
Postle, B.R., 2006. Working memory as an emergent property of the mind and brain. Neu- Wharton, C.M., Grafman, J., Flitman, S.S., Hansen, E.K., Brauner, J., Marks, A., Honda, M.,
roscience 139 (1), 23–38. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience.2005.06.005. 2000. Toward Neuroanatomical Models of Analogy: A Positron Emission Tomography
Prabhakaran, V., Smith, J.A.L., Desmond, J.E., Glover, G.H., Gabrieli, J.D.E., 1997. Neural sub- Study of Analogical Mapping. Cognitive Psychology 40 (3), 173–197.
strates of fluid reasoning: an fMRI study of neocortical activation during performance Wechsler, D., 1981. Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale—Revised. Psychologica Corporation,
of the Raven's Progressive Matrices Test. Cogn. Psychol. 33, 43–63. San Antonio, TX.
Pfurtscheller, G., 1999. Quantification of ERD and ERS in the time domain. In: Xu, Y., Chun, M.M., 2006. Dissociable neural mechanisms supporting visual short-term
Pfurtscheller, G., Lopes da Silva, F.H. (Eds.), Handbook of Electroencephalography memory for objects. Nature 440 (7080), 91–95.
and Clinical Neuropsychology. vol. 6. Elsevier, New York; Amsterdam, pp. 89–105. Zaehle, T., Rach, S., Herrmann, C.S., 2010. Transcranial alternating current stimulation en-
Polanía, R., Nitsche, M.A., Korman, C., Batsikadze, G., Paulus, W., 2012. The importance of hances individual alpha activity in human EEG. PLoS ONE 5 (11), e13766.
timing in segregated theta phase-coupling for cognitive performance. Curr. Biol. 22 Zaghi, S., Acar, M., Hultgren, B., Boggio, P.S., Fregni, F., 2010. Noninvasive brain stimulation
(14), 1314–1318. with low-intensity electrical currents: putative mechanisms of action for direct and
Postle, B.R., Ferrarelli, F., Hamidi, M., Feredoes, E., Massimini, M., Peterson, M., Alexander, alternating current stimulation. Neuroscientist 16, 285–307.
A., Tononi, G., 2006. Repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation dissociates working Zoefel, B., Huster, R.J., Herrmann, C.S., 2011. Neurofeedback training of the upper alpha
memory manipulation from retention functions in the prefrontal, but not posterior frequency band in EEG improves cognitive performance. NeuroImage 54 (2),
parietal, cortex. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 18, 1712–1722. 1427–1431.

Você também pode gostar