Você está na página 1de 8

Music Therapy Perspectives Advance Access published February 28, 2016

Music Perception Training for Pediatric Cochlear


Implant Recipients Ages 3 to 5 Years: A Pilot Study
SOO JI KIM, PHD, MT-BC Ewha Womans University
EUN YEON KIM, PHD Myongji University
GA EUL YOO, MME, MT-BC Ewha Womans University

Appropriate sensory input is critical for young children with timbre perception training (Driscoll, 2012). After training, they
cochlear implants (CIs) and facilitates brain plasticity of the popula- may develop their own strategies to make coherent judgments by
tion (Giraud, Price, Graham, & Frackowiak, 2001; Lazard, Giraud, relying on the specific attributes (e.g., short attack time, as in per-
Gnansia, Meyer, & Sterkers, 2012). In addition to the research sup- cussive instruments) of different sounds that they can effectively

Downloaded from http://mtp.oxfordjournals.org/ at AMTA Member Access on December 31, 2016


porting the existence of a sensitive period for successful cochlear perceive (Fujita & Ito, 1999; Pressnitzer, Bestel, & Fraysse, 2005).
implantation, a growing number of studies also emphasize the Given the variability in the contextual musical environment
need for early intensive auditory rehabilitation (Abdi, Khalessi, and in the developmental features of preschool children with
Khorsandi, & Gholami, 2001; Yucel, Sennaroglu, & Belgin, 2009). CIs, the application of age-specific interventions and accom-
The literature consistently indicates that capacity to process audi- modations is recommended. These differ from those used not
tory information can affect not only cognitive and language skills, only with adults with CIs, but also with school-age children
but psychosocial needs and quality of life for young children and with CIs (Gfeller, Driscoll, Kenworthy, & van Voorst, 2011).
adolescents (Stacey, Fortnum, Barton, & Summerfield, 2006). Children with CIs who received music training at less than
Perception of temporal features of speech in which words with six years of age showed a stronger correlation between dura-
similar phonetic features but of different lengths are discriminated tion of training and pitch perception skills than children who
was found to be significantly related to the rhythm task perfor- received music training at an older age (Chen et  al., 2010).
mance of seven-year-old children with CIs (Yucel & Belgin, 2006). This indicates that clinical outcomes for auditory rehabilita-
The ability to process pitch direction information was found to tion and auditory plasticity may differ depending on the age
affect the ability to recognize speech prosody and the intended of participation in the intervention. Also, considering the age
emotion of speakers (Hopyan, Gordon, & Papsin, 2011; Luo, Fu, of the population, the opportunity to experience success and
& Galvin, 2007). Furthermore, timbre perception was found to enjoyment in a familiar musical environment has been sug-
relate to word recognition in quiet environments (Won, Drennan, gested to be an important factor for maintaining engagement
Kang, & Rubinstein, 2010), recognition of CNC (consonant–vowel in music perception tasks (Gfeller et al., 2011; Hsiao & Gfeller,
nucleus–consonant) words in noisy environments (Gfeller et al., 2012) and developing music preferences (Hwang, 2011).
2002), and decoding of emotions conveyed by speech prosody These findings indicate that age-specific structured training
(Thompson, Shellenberg, & Husain, 2004). would maximize expected improvement in concert with posi-
Along with these developmental issues related to music tive changes in perceptual acuity and attitude toward music
perception, the effects of music perception training have been in young children with CIs (Chen et al., 2010; Nakata et al.,
increasingly investigated as a means to facilitate the development 2005; Trehub, Vongpaisal, & Nakata, 2009). However, there
of music perception and speech language skills of children with has been fairly limited application of structured task-specific
CIs who were prelingually deafened (Chen et al., 2010; Petersen, interventions for preschool children ages three to five years
Mortensen, Hansen, & Vuust, 2012). Although young children that intensively target and assess music perception skills (Abdi
with CIs engage in music-related activities, including music lis- et al., 2001). Therefore, this study aimed to develop an age-
tening during TV watching, similarly to their peers with normal specific music training program that assists young children
hearing, incidental exposure to music does not lead to enhanced with CIs to develop the ability to process musical information.
perceptual accuracy (Gfeller, Witt, Spencer, Stordahl, & Tomblin, This study examined the potential effectiveness of this training
1999). Meanwhile, recent research demonstrated that structured program, answering the following question: Are there differ-
music training improved music perception–related skills of young ences in music perception skills of pediatric CI recipients after
children with CIs. After training, perception of pitch intervals of receiving intensive music perception training?
preschool children with CIs was improved (Chen et  al., 2010;
Yucel et al., 2009). More recent studies reported that although Method
poor perception of timbre by CI users is attributed to limitations
in CI technology, individuals with CIs can benefit from structured Participants
Pediatric CI recipients ages three to five years were recruited
Address correspondence concerning this article to Ga Eul Yoo, MT-BC, Ewha
Womans University, Helen Hall 312, Ewhayeodaegil 52, Seodaemun gu, Seoul from an otorhinolaryngology department in a hospital located
03760, Korea. E-mail: bbird27@hotmail.com. Phone: 82-10-5692-8845. in Seoul that they visited for follow-up examination and
© the American Music Therapy Association 2016. All rights reserved. habilitative intervention. Children with CIs were assessed and
For permissions, please e-mail: journals.permissions@oup.com
referred to the program by a speech language pathologist who
doi:10.1093/mtp/miw009

1
2 Music Therapy Perspectives

Table 1.
Demographic Information of Participants

Participant Gender Age (yrs) Age at CI (yrs) CI use (yrs) Implant devices
1 M 5.4 4.3 1.0 Freedom
2 F 5.8 2.3 3.4 N5
3 M 3.5 1.9 1.6 Freedom
4 F 2.8 1.6 1.2 N5
5 M 4.8 1.4 2.5 Freedom
6 M 3.9 3.9 0.1 N5

had more than 10  years of experience with CI recipients in Table 2.


the area of intensive auditory rehabilitation. Caregivers of six Examples of Target Rhythm Patterns Depending on the
children with CIs voluntarily agreed to participate in this study. Level of Training
The mean age of the participants was 4.4  years (SD  =  1.2),

Downloaded from http://mtp.oxfordjournals.org/ at AMTA Member Access on December 31, 2016


and they had 1.8 years (SD = 1.4) of CI experience before par-
ticipation in this study (see Table 1). They were reported to be
exposed to musical stimuli on a daily basis, in both their home
and preschool environments, but had limited prior music
training for language development. Ethical issues with regard
to all relevant procedures were reviewed, and informed con-
sent was obtained from parents of all participants.

Music Perception Training Table 3.


The music perception training was developed for this study Examples of Melodic Patterns Used in a Melodic Contour Training
and targeted three- to five-year-old children who had at least one
month of cochlear implant use. Individual attributes of music
including rhythm, melody, and timbre and the skills to perceive
and process these attributes were targeted in terms of detection,
discrimination, and identification. Details about the tasks and
materials used during the training are presented in the Appendix.
Sound detection.  The ability to detect the presence of sound
is a prerequisite to attending to auditory input and to developing
skills to process more complex auditory stimuli (Fritz, Elhilali,
David, & Shamma, 2007). Repetitive or successful intentional
attention to sounds leads to further processing of information. Melody perception.  In this program, melodic contour and
For the purpose of training sound-detection skills, children were pitch discrimination were targeted. With regard to percep-
instructed to perform an age-appropriate task, such as moving to tion of melodic contour, five-note and eight-note sequences
music, playing an instrument, or operating a small object (e.g., of basic melodic contours (i.e., rising and falling) with notes
shaking scarves or putting small pebbles in a basket), in response between C4 (262 Hz) and C5 (534 Hz) played at 80 bpm were
to hearing music. The balanced use of various instruments made used (see Table 3). For pitch discrimination, broader intervals
from different materials (e.g., wood, metal, plastic, and skin) was such as the interval of 12 semitones (e.g., C4 and C5) were
considered when selecting instruments based on the literature presented initially, and the smaller interval was targeted once
indicating that for normal development, exploration of a variety the children were confirmed to discriminate the larger interval
of sounds establishes the base upon which the ability to discrim- successfully. Matching to different pitches, movements (e.g.,
inate different sound qualities is developed (Lee, 2006). standing and crouching while pointing to the toes) were also
Rhythm perception.  In consideration of the age and devel- applied to demonstrate the concept of high and low. A set of
opmental stage of preschool children, rhythm perception– instruments in which a different pitch was attributed to a differ-
related training in this study was primarily based on changes in ent length or height of each component (e.g., step bells, reso-
the frequency of beat to be played within the four-beat rhythm nator bells, and other adaptive instruments) were also used to
patterns. Participants were encouraged to attend to the number facilitate the children’s processing of the relevant information.
of beats (i.e., one to four beats) played and then imitate, dis- Based on a previous study showing that a relatively simpler
criminate, and identify the rhythm patterns through instrumen- spectral structure of timbre of the organ increased discrimina-
tal playing (see Table 2). The utilization of movements or visual tion and identification performance (Galvin, Fu, & Shannon,
aids matching the number of beats to be played facilitated 2009), the timbre of MIDI-generated organ was used to pre-
understanding of the abstract concept of changes in rhythm. sent the task of discriminating or identifying pitch information.
Presentation of discrete beats was required in the selection of Timbre perception. Considering that exposure to more
instruments, since long resonance after playing would interfere variety of sound characteristics would contribute to gener-
with accurately recognizing the interval between beats. alization of timbre perception to different environments and
Music Perception Training for Pediatric Cochlear Implant Recipients Ages 3 to 5 Years: A Pilot Study 3

stimuli, traditional Korean instruments, along with Western Procedure


instruments, were used in this study. Traditional Korean instru- This pilot study was conducted using a one-group pre-
ments from three instrumental families (i.e., string, woodwind, test-posttest design. Two or three children were assigned to
and percussion) were selected because of their cultural sig- a training group based on their age and level of language
nificance and because of their distinctive differences in sound development; children received the music perception
features with regard to distributions of fundamental and higher training in these small groups. One of the investigators,
harmonics and attack time in sound production (Nam, 2008). a board-certified music therapist with clinical experiences
In order to facilitate the pediatric CI recipients’ development with this population, implemented the program in a quiet
of their own strategy to rely on timbral cues, instrumental room within the hospital where participants were recruited.
groups showing greater differences in saliency in their sound The music perception training was conducted weekly for
features (e.g., percussive instruments with a rapid and strong 30 to 40 minutes during 24 weeks following the outline
attack time versus woodwind instruments that reach their peak of the training program (see the Appendix), and pretest
amplitude in a relatively longer time) were presented initially. and posttest were administered at the first and 24th week,
Singing skills. This singing-based training targeted the abil- respectively. During training, the period of implementation
ity to perceive and imitate pitch and rhythmic information for each level depended on how accurately a child per-
from familiar songs with repetition of simple patterns and

Downloaded from http://mtp.oxfordjournals.org/ at AMTA Member Access on December 31, 2016


formed the tasks without additional prompts at a certain
fewer changes in rhythmic and melodic sequences, appropri- level (see Figure 1). Considering that the participants were
ate for the developmental age of a child. The initial exposure young children, parents’ involvement was also encouraged
to a targeted song consisted of the original recording of the by giving them a CD that included musical stimuli and
song. Movement or playing of instruments to the recorded song songs used during training to help them actively engage
encouraged the children to become familiar with the musical in the children’s habilitative process. Feedback from par-
environment. Once the children with CIs became familiar with ents was obtained to ascertain the extent to which hav-
the song, a part of the song was introduced with live accom- ing music resources at home facilitated their observing
paniment emphasizing limited target words, salient rhythm, or responses from their children and monitoring changes in
melodic pattern within the song. Then targeted aspects were their children.
expanded to process and produce the entire song. The visual
representation of target words, rhythmic pattern, pitch infor-
mation, and/or melodic contour was also utilized in order to
present the multisensory cues that facilitate this population to
integrate the presented information (Hsiao & Gfeller, 2012).
Internal validity. In order to evaluate the validity of the
constructed training program for intervening with pediatric
CI recipients, six relevant professionals assessed the content
validity of the training, including age-appropriateness, goal-
specificity, and applicability; assessors included two speech
language pathologists with an average of 10 years of clinical
practice with children with CIs, two otorhinolaryngologists
with an average of five years in treatment, and two audiolo-
gists with an average of 11 years of clinical practice. The con-
tent validity index computed was .93 (Kappa = .85), indicating
the program has sufficient validity. Figure 1.  Outline of training process.

Table 4.
Subtest Items in Music Perception Measurement

Category Test content


Discrimination
 Rhythm To discriminate whether two rhythm patterns (modified from PMMA items) are the same or different
  Pitch (Melodic pattern) To discriminate whether two melodic patterns (modified from PMMA items) are the same or different
 Timbre To discriminate whether two instrumental timbres (from different instrumental families of traditional
Korean instruments) are the same or different
Identification
 Rhythm To identify the presented beat pattern (one to four beats) by selecting the corresponding picture that
visually presents the beat pattern
  Pitch (Melodic contour) To identify the presented melodic contour (ascending or descending) by selecting the corresponding
picture that visually presents the beat pattern
 Timbre To identify the presented instrument timbre (one of hand-percussion instruments) by selecting the
corresponding instrument
 Song To identify the recorded children’s song by selecting the picture that visually presents the title of the song
4 Music Therapy Perspectives

Table 5.
Changes in Discrimination Task Performance After Training

Discrimination task performance (%)


Pitch Timbre
Rhythm (Melodic pattern) (Korean instruments)
Participant Pre Post Pre Post Pre Post
1 20.0 50.0 30.0 100.0 33.3 67.0
2 40.0 50.0 60.0 60.0 50.0 50.0
3 30.0 80.0 50.0 50.0 25.0 50.0
4 40.0 60.0 20.0 50.0 67.0 50.0
5 30.0 50.0 40.0 60.0 33.3 67.0

Measures

Downloaded from http://mtp.oxfordjournals.org/ at AMTA Member Access on December 31, 2016


Seven subtests were administered at pretest and posttest. Each
child was instructed to discriminate whether two presented musi-
cal stimuli (pertaining to each musical attribute, such as rhythm,
pitch, and timbre) were the same or different and to identify the
presented musical stimuli (pertaining to each musical attribute,
such as rhythm, pitch, timbre, and song). To minimize the test-
retest effect, the test items were constructed to be different from
what was used during the training. For the pitch and rhythm dis-
crimination tests, lower levels of 10 test items from each pitch
and rhythm subtest of the Primary Measures of Music Audiation
(PMMA, Gordon, 1979) were selected, given the age and level
of development of the participants. The other subtest items were
constructed by the investigator to examine the children’s per-
formance on targeted perception-based skills. All the musical
stimuli were prerecorded instead of using live musical stimuli.
Figure  2.  Performance rate for discrimination tests at pretest and
This served to increase the reliability of the measurements with
posttest.
regard to sound recognition; control the confounding variables,
especially related to visual cues (e.g., gestures of evaluators that
may indicate the auditory source); and generalize the effect of showed the increases in overall performance rate with regard
training to the environment most characteristic of daily living. to discrimination of rhythm, pitch, and timbre at posttest com-
The test measures are summarized in Table 4. pared to pretest (see Table  5 and Figure  2). Increased perfor-
mance on the identification tests was also observed at posttest
Data Collection and Analysis (see Table 6 and Figure 3). Analyses using the Wilcoxon signed-
At pretest and posttest, one professional music therapist and rank test showed that statistical significance was achieved only
one music therapy graduate student observed and evaluated for the rhythm-related tests (p = .039 for rhythm discrimination
the task performance of each participant. The two assessors test and p = .041 for rhythm identification test). Changes in the
received three hours of training, and more than .90 inter- pitch, melodic contour, timbre, and song recognition tests did
observer reliability was obtained prior to engagement in the not reach statistical significance (see Table 7).
evaluation of participants. During pretest and posttest, the task Participant 1 showed the greatest improvement in pitch-
performance rates of each participant were evaluated on each related perception. He discriminated the presented two
test. The inter-observer reliability between the two trained melodic patterns accurately for all of the test items at post-
observers was .96, indicating a high level of agreement. The test. He also showed high levels of melodic pattern identifica-
task performance rates of the group at pretest and posttest tion. The child’s mother reported that, after participation in the
were compared by conducting the Wilcoxon signed-rank test. program, he showed increases in distinguishing the different
intonation between asking a question and stating a declara-
Results tive phrase while she read a children’s book to him. With
A total of six children with CIs participated in this study, and regard to discrimination, Participant 2 showed slight improve-
seven subtests for discrimination and identification of individ- ment in rhythm-related task performance, while changes in
ual musical components were administrated for each partici- other subtests were minimal. Feedback from the child’s car-
pant. The task performance (%) during each test was measured egiver showed that their reciprocal engagement in music (i.e.,
at pretest and posttest. The data from one of the participants singing together) increased after the training. Participant 3
(Participant 6)  were excluded from further analysis due to showed the greatest improvement in rhythm discrimination
insufficient length of CI use. Results of the changes in music task performance. For identification tasks, his performance
perception test measures by the remaining five participants rates increased in all subtests. The child’s mother reported
Music Perception Training for Pediatric Cochlear Implant Recipients Ages 3 to 5 Years: A Pilot Study 5

Table 6.
Changes in Identification Task Performance After Training

Identification task performance (%)


Pitch Timbre
Rhythm (Melodic contour) (Western instruments) Song recognition
Participant Pre Post Pre Post Pre Post Pre Post
1 62.5 87.5 50.0 87.5 50.0 100.0 100.0 100.0
2 62.5 100.0 87.5 87.5 75.0 87.5 100.0 100.0
3 50.0 75.0 50.0 87.5 25.0 62.5 50.0 75.0
4 50.0 62.5 12.5 62.5 37.5 87.5 25.0 50.0
5 62.5 100.0 50.0 62.5 100.0 100.0 50.0 75.0

identification tasks. Although participants also showed

Downloaded from http://mtp.oxfordjournals.org/ at AMTA Member Access on December 31, 2016


increased performance on other subtests, significant changes
were observed only in rhythm perception. This supports research
findings showing that rhythm perception was improved in indi-
viduals with CIs more than other areas after music perception
training (Innes-Brown, Marozeau, Storey, & Blamey, 2013).
The research also conforms to the research finding that rhyth-
mic cues could be a primary factor in successful processing
of sound information when multiple cues exist, as in familiar
melody recognition tasks (Galvin, Fu, & Nogaki, 2007). Young
children with CIs can successfully rely on rhythmic information
when reproducing remembered songs (Nakata, Trehub, Mitani,
& Kanda, 2006), while pitch patterns are not successfully pro-
cessed. These findings indicate that rhythm perception plays an
important role in auditory information processing in pediatric
Figure  3.  Performance rate for identification tests at pretest and CI recipients. Results from this study also support that rhythm
posttest. perception may be facilitated or developed initially among
music perception–related skills, presenting clinical implica-
tions for the utilization of rhythmic aspects for successful per-
that his attempts to imitate the words of others increased after formance of related music perception for this population.
participation in the program. Participant 4 showed increased For increased effectiveness of pitch and timbre perception
performance on all subtests except timbre discrimination. training, more controlled application of the stimuli should
According to the child’s mother, the child engaged more in be the target of future studies. In terms of pitch information,
singing and the child’s attitude toward music was deemed large intervals between notes should be presented in order
more positive after participating in the training. Participant to avoid smaller intervals being conveyed via a single elec-
5 showed increased performance in all subtests except the trode, which are more difficult for individuals with CIs to
timbre identification test, in which he already showed a high differentiate. Spectral information of the timbre that a child
level of performance. His mother reported that after participat- needs to differentiate or identify should also be considered
ing in the program, he began to request that his mother read in order to modulate the complexity of the stimulus more
a storybook that included buttons to activate sounds to him systematically.
and his attempts to imitate the words of others also increased. Although the results of this study should be confirmed via
larger studies, this study presents clinical implications with
Discussion regard to materials to be used for music perception train-
In this pilot study, the potential effectiveness of music per- ing. Integrative application of musical elements (e.g., sing-
ception training for pediatric CI recipients was examined. After ing) and multisensory cues (e.g., visual and kinesthetic cues)
training, participants showed increased task performance rates may provide contextual cues for young children with CIs to
on all subtests in terms of discrimination and identification attend to the presented auditory information more accurately.
of musical elements. This implies that pediatric CI recipients In this study, there was an increase in pitch discrimination
could benefit from intensive music perception–based training performance in the PMMA pitch subtest with a combina-
for specific musical attributes when presented in an age-appro- tion of diverse pitches and melodic patterns, not limited to
priate context. Particularly, it points to the potential applica- single pitch or basic melodic contour. Also, increased song
bility of a structured music perception training program for recognition was observed, even in the song recognition sub-
young CI recipients who need early and continuous interven- test, which included new children’s songs not targeted during
tion along with follow-up treatment in medical settings. the training program. These results indicate that the structured
A notable finding is that improvements were found on program facilitates pediatric CI recipients to rely on specific
the rhythm-related subtests for both discrimination and informational cues and pair the cues with what they have
6 Music Therapy Perspectives

Table 7.
Comparison of Task Performance Rates During Each Subtest at Pretest and Posttest

Pretest Posttest
Subtest performance rate M SD M SD Z p
Discrimination test
 Rhythm 32.0 8.4 58.0 13.0 –2.060 .039*
  Pitch (Melodic pattern) 40.0 15.8 64.0 20.7 –1.604 .109
  Timbre (Korean) 41.7 16.8 56.8 9.3 –1.473 .141
Identification test
 Rhythm 57.5 6.8 85.0 16.3 –2.041 .041*
  Pitch (Melodic contour) 50.0 26.5 77.5 13.7 –1.841 .066
  Timbre (Western) 57.5 30.1 87.5 15.3 –1.841 .066
 Song 65.0 33.5 80.0 20.9 –0.816 .414

Downloaded from http://mtp.oxfordjournals.org/ at AMTA Member Access on December 31, 2016


a. Wilcoxon signed-rank test
* p < .05

remembered when required to process a song with multiple Fritz, J. B., Elhilali, M., David, S. V., & Shamma, S. A. (2007). Auditory attention:
cues (Galvin et al., 2007). Also, the use of ethnic instruments Focusing the searchlight on sound. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 17, 1–19.
doi:10.1016/j.conb.2007.07.011
could be considered for development of both general and cul-
Fujita, S., & Ito, J. (1999). Ability of nucleus cochlear implantees to recognize music.
ture-dependent music preferences in young populations with Annals of Otology, Rhinology, and Laryngology, 10, 634–640.
CIs (Hwang, 2011; Kim, Cho, Kim, & Yoo, 2015). Galvin, J. J. III, Fu, Q., & Nogaki, G. (2007). Melodic contour identification by
The results, however, should be considered with caution cochlear implant listeners. Ear & Hearing, 28(3), 302–319. doi:10.1097/01.
because of the lack of a control group. Along with the baseline aud.0000261689.35445.20
data in terms of developmental level of speech and language, Galvin, J. J. III, Fu, Q., & Shannon, R. V. (2009). Melodic contour identification and
music perception by cochlear implant users. Annals of the New York Academy
the inclusion of a larger sample may corroborate the applicabil-
of Sciences, 1169, 518–533. doi:10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009.04551.x
ity of this program with regard to how systematic music-based Geers, A., Tobey, E., Moog, J., & Brenner, C. (2008). Long-term outcomes
interventions should operate in facilitating and improving skills of cochlear implantation in the preschool years: From elementary
to process musical information. Intervening at an early age in grades to high school. International Journal of Audiology, 47(2), 21–30.
cases of cochlear implantation is strongly correlated with lan- doi:10.1080/14992020802339167
guage performance (Dettman, Pinder, Briggs, Dowell, & Leigh, Gfeller, K., Driscoll, V., Kenworthy, M., & van Voorst, T. (2011). Music therapy for
2007; Geers, Tobey, Moog, & Brenner, 2008), and additional preschool cochlear implant recipients. Music Therapy Perspectives, 29, 39–49.
doi:10.1093/mtp/29.1.39
music training may promote age-appropriate development in
Gfeller, K., Witt, S., Adamek, M., Mehr, M., Rogers, J., Stordahl, J., & Riggenberg,
efficient ways. Structural elements in music may be effectively S. (2002). Effects of training on timbre recognition and appraisal by postlin-
used to facilitate the perception of temporal and spectral infor- gually deafened cochlear implant recipients. Journal of the American Academy
mation of auditory and language input (See, Driscoll, Gfeller, of Audiology, 13, 132–145.
Kliethermes, & Oleson, 2013). Also, it would enhance the Gfeller, K., Witt, S., Spencer, L. J., Stordahl, J., & Tomblin, B. (1999). Musical involve-
developmental process, leading to more successful processing ment and enjoyment of children who use cochlear implants. Volta Review,
of auditory and speech information (Yucel et al., 2009). Future 100(4), 213–233.
Giraud, A. L., Price, C. J., Graham, J. M., & Frackowiak, R. S. (2001). Functional
studies are needed to examine how direct and specific music
plasticity of language-related brain areas after cochlear implantation. Brain,
perception and speech perception training outcomes generalize 124, 1307–1316.
to areas that were not directly trained, such as more complex or Gordon, E. E. (1979). Primary measures of music audiation. Chicago: GIA.
novel stimuli. Further investigation is needed to determine how Hopyan, T., Gordon, K. A., & Papsin, B. C. (2011). Identifying emotions in
the structured intervention facilitates not only successful engage- music through electrical hearing in deaf children using cochlear implants.
ment in and processing of music-related tasks, but also general Cochlear Implants International, 12(1), 21–26. doi:10.1179/1467010
10X12677899497399
enjoyment of the musical environment for children with CIs.
Hsiao, F., & Gfeller, K. (2012). Music perception of cochlear implant recipients with
implications for music instruction: A review of literature. Update: Applications
of Research in Music, 30(2), 5–10. doi:10.1177/8755123312437050
References
Hwang, S. Y. (2011). Comparative study on music preference between school-aged
Abdi, S., Khalessi, M. H., Khorsandi, M., & Gholami, B. (2001). Introducing music children with cochlear implant and normal hearing. Journal of Music and
as a means of habilitation for children with cochlear implants. International Human Behavior, 8(2), 47–64.
Journal of Pediatric Otorhinolaryngology, 59(2), 105–113. doi:10.1016/ Innes-Brown, H., Marozeau, J. P., Storey, C. M., & Blamey, P. J. (2013). Tone, rhythm,
S0165-5876(01)00460-8 and timbre perception in school-age children using cochlear implants and
Chen, J. K., Chuang, A. Y. C., McMahon, C., Hsieh, J., Tung T., & Li, L. P. (2010). hearing aids. Journal of the American Academy of Audiology, 24(9), 789–806.
Music training improves pitch perception in prelingually deafened children doi:10.3766/jaaa.24.9.4
with implants. Pediatrics, 1254, 793–800. doi:10.1542/peds.2008-3620 Kim, S. J., Cho, Y. S., Kim, E. Y., & Yoo, G. E. (2015). Can young adolescents with coch-
Dettman, S. J., Pinder, D., Briggs, R. J.  S., Dowell, R. C., & Leigh, J. R. (2007). lear implants perceive different timbral cues? Cochlear Implants International,
Communication development in children who receive the cochlear implant 16(2), 61–68. doi:10.1179/1754762814Y.0000000083
younger than 12  months: Risks versus benefits. Ear & Hearing, 28(2), 11–18. Lazard, D. S., Giraud, A. L., Gnansia, D., Meyer, B., & Sterkers, O. (2012).
doi:10.1097/AUD.0b013e31803153f8 Understanding the deafened brain: Implications for cochlear implant rehabil-
Driscoll, V. D. (2012). The effects of training on recognition of musical instruments itation. European Annals of Otorhinolaryngology, Head, and Neck Diseases,
by adults with cochlear implants. Seminars in Hearing, 33(4), 410. 129(2), 98–103. doi:10.1016/j.anorl.2011.06.001
Music Perception Training for Pediatric Cochlear Implant Recipients Ages 3 to 5 Years: A Pilot Study 7

Lee, S. Y. (2006). Music activities for facilitation of developmental domains for See, R. L., Driscoll, V. D., Gfeller, K., Kliethermes, S., & Oleson, J. (2013). Speech
children from age three to six. Journal of Music and Human Behavior, 3(1), intonation and melodic contour recognition in children with cochlear implants
29–62. and with normal hearing. Otology & Neurotology, 34(3), 490–498. doi:10.1097/
Luo, X., Fu, Q., & Galvin, J. J. (2007). Vocal emotion recognition by normal hear- MAO.0b013e318287c985
ing listeners and cochlear implant users. Trends in Hearing, 11(4), 301–315. Stacey, P. C., Fortnum, H. M., Barton, G. R., & Summerfield, A. Q. (2006). Hearing-
doi:10.1177/1084713807305301 impaired children in the United Kingdom, I: Auditory performance, communica-
Nakata, T., Trehub, S. E., Mitani, C., & Kanda, Y. (2006). Pitch and timing in the songs tion skills, educational achievements, quality of life, and cochlear implantation.
of deaf children with cochlear implants. Music Perception, 24(2), 147–154. Ear & Hearing, 27(2), 161–186. doi:10.1097/01.aud.0000202353.37567.b4
doi:10.1525/mp.2006.24.2.147 Thompson, W. F., Shellenberg, E. G., & Husain, G. (2004). Decoding speech pros-
Nakata T., Trehub S. E., Mitani C., Kanda Y., Shibasaki A., & Schellenberg, E. G. ody: Do music lessons help? Emotion, 4, 46–64. doi:10.1037/1528-3542.4.1.46
(2005). Music recognition by Japanese children with cochlear implants. Trehub, S. E., Vongpaisal, T., & Nakata, T. (2009). Music in the lives of deaf children
Journal of Physiological Anthropology and Applied Human Science, 24, 29–32. with cochlear implants. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1169,
doi:10.2114/jpa.24.29 534–542. doi:10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009.04554.x
Nam, U. (2008). Timbre of Korean traditional instruments. In C. Kim (Ed.), Korean Won, J. H., Drennan, W., Kang, R. S., & Rubinstein, J. T. (2010). Psychoacoustic abil-
traditional instruments acoustics seminar (pp. 66–98). Seoul, South Korea: ities associated with music perception in cochlear implant users. Ear & Hearing,
National Gugak Center. 31(6), 796–805. doi:10.1097/AUD.0b013e3181e8b7bd
Petersen, B., Mortensen, M. V., Hansen, M., & Vuust, P. (2012). Singing in the key Yucel, E., & Belgin, E. (2006). Speech and musical perception in cochlear implant
of life: A  study on effects of musical ear training after cochlear implantation. and hearing aid user adolescents. Journal of International Advanced Otology,

Downloaded from http://mtp.oxfordjournals.org/ at AMTA Member Access on December 31, 2016


Psychomusicology: Music, Mind, and Brain, 22(2), 134–151. doi:10.1037/ 2(1), 27–35.
a0031140 Yucel, E., Sennaroglu, G., & Belgin, E. (2009). The family-oriented musical train-
Pressnitzer, D., Bestel, J., & Fraysse, B. (2005). Music to electric ears: Pitch and tim- ing for children with cochlear implants: Speech and musical perception results
bre perception by cochlear implant patients. Annals of the New York Academy for two-year follow-up. International Journal of Pediatric Otorhinolaryngology,
of Sciences, 1060, 343–345. 73(7), 1043–1052. doi:10.1016/j.ijporl.2009.04.009

Appendix. Outline of Tasks and Instruments Used Depending on Level of Training

Level Target Primary task Materials used


1 Rhythm discrimination Playing one to four beats with cues Percussive instruments with
less resonating sounds such as
woodblock or hand drum
Melodic pattern discrimination Exploring rising and falling patterns Pitched instruments with a
different color or size for each
note such as hand bells or step
bells
Singing Making movements to recorded children’s song Recorded music of children’s
familiar songs
2 Rhythm discrimination Discriminating one from two to four beats Percussions with less resonating
sounds such as woodblock or
hand drum
Melodic pattern discrimination Discriminating eight-note rising and falling patterns Pitched instruments
Timbre discrimination Discriminating the percussion timbres Percussion made of different
materials
Singing Learning target words of a selected song Guitar or keyboard as
accompaniment instruments
3 Rhythm discrimination Learning basic and subdivided beat patterns Percussive instruments with less
resonating sounds
Melodic contour discrimination Discriminating 5-note rising and falling patterns Recording of melodic patterns
played by the organ
Pitch discrimination Discriminating two pitches over 7-semitone intervals Recording of single pitches
played by the MIDI-generated
organ
Timbre discrimination Exploring traditional Korean instrument timbres Recordings of the gayageum
(string), daegeum (woodwind),
and janggu (percussion)
Singing Learning target words and rhythm of a song Guitar or keyboard as
accompaniment instruments
accompaniment instruments
8 Music Therapy Perspectives

Appendix  Continued

Level Target Primary task Materials used


4 Rhythm identification Identifying basic beat and subdivided beat patterns Percussion with less resonating
sounds
Melodic contour identification Identifying rising and falling patterns Recording of 5-note melodic
patterns played by the organ
Pitch discrimination Discriminating two pitches under 7-semitone intervals Recording of single pitches
played by the organ
Timbre discrimination Discriminating traditional Korean instrumental timbres Recordings of the gayageum
(string), daegeum (woodwind),
and janggu (percussion)
Singing Learning the melody of the song Accompaniment instruments
5 Rhythm identification Identifying/imitating different beat patterns Percussion with less resonating
sounds

Downloaded from http://mtp.oxfordjournals.org/ at AMTA Member Access on December 31, 2016


Melodic contour identification Identifying rising and falling patterns Recording of 5-note melodic
patterns played by the organ
Pitch discrimination Discriminating two pitches under 7-semitone intervals Recording of C4 to C5 played by
the organ
Timbre identification Identifying traditional Korean instrumental timbres Recordings of the gayageum
(string), daegeum (woodwind),
and janggu (percussion)
Singing Singing the learned song independently Accompaniment instruments

Você também pode gostar