Você está na página 1de 5

Brazilian Journal ofhypoactivity

Restraint-induced Medical and Biological Research (2000) 33: 79-83 79


ISSN 0100-879X Short Communication

Restraint-induced hypoactivity
in an elevated plus-maze

C.M. Padovan and Departamento de Farmacologia, Faculdade de Medicina de Ribeirão Preto,


F.S. Guimarães Universidade de São Paulo, Ribeirão Preto, SP, Brasil

Abstract

Correspondence Rodents submitted to restraint stress show decreased activity in an Key words
F.S. Guimarães elevated plus-maze (EPM) 24 h later. The objective of the present · Stress
Departamento de Farmacologia study was to determine if a certain amount of time is needed after stress · Restraint
FMRP, USP
for the development of these changes. We also wanted to verify if · Elevated plus-maze
Av. Bandeirantes, 3900 Behavioral changes
14049-900 Ribeirão Preto, SP
behavioral tolerance of repeated daily restraint would be detectable in ·
· Tolerance
Brasil this model. Male Wistar rats were restrained for 2 h and tested in the
Fax: +55-16-633-2301 EPM 1, 2, 24 or 48 h later. Another group of animals was immobilized
E-mail: fsguimar@fmrp.usp.br daily for 2 h for 7 days, being tested in the EPM 24 h after the last
restraint period. Restraint induced a significant decrease in the percent
Research supported by FAPESP of entries and time spent in the open arms, as well as a decrease in the
(No. 98/10639-7), FINEP and CNPq.
number of enclosed arm entries. The significant effect in the number
C.M. Padovan and F.S. Guimarães
of entries and the percentage of time spent in the open arms disap-
are recipients of FAPESP (No. 95/
4900-6) and CNPq fellowships,
peared when the data were submitted to analysis of covariance using
respectively. the number of enclosed arm entries as a covariate. This suggests that
the restraint-induced hypoactivity influences the measures of open
arm exploration. The modifications of restraint-induced hypoactivity
are evident 24 or 48 h, but not 1 or 2 h, after stress. In addition, rats
Received December 9, 1998
stressed daily for seven days became tolerant to this effect.
Accepted November 4, 1999

Exposure to environmental stress has been gion and in the hippocampus and raphe nu-
implicated in the etiology of many human clei (7) have also been reported. In addition,
disorders such as depression, anxiety and some studies suggest that chronic restraint
cardiovascular disease (1). could induce tolerance to some of the behav-
In laboratory animals, long-lasting stress- ioral (8,9) or neurochemical (7) stress-in-
induced effects on behavior have also been duced changes.
demonstrated. For example, rats submitted We have recently shown that the restraint-
to 2 h of restraint show an exploratory deficit induced decreased activity is also evident
of new environments such as an open arena when the animal is exposed to the elevated
24 h after the stress (2-5), suggesting that plus-maze, an animal model of anxiety (2-5).
previous stressful experiences may modify Using this model we have demonstrated that
the animal response to new aversive stimuli the restraint-induced deficit is attenuated by
(2). Neurochemical changes such as an in- hippocampal injection of serotonin agonists
crease in benzodiazepine receptor affinity in (2,3) or cycloheximide, a protein synthesis
the frontal cortex (6) or an increase in the inhibitor (4).
5HIAA serotonin metabolite in the same re- It has been suggested that stress-induced

Braz J Med Biol Res 33(1) 2000


80 C.M. Padovan and F.S. Guimarães

long-lasting behavioral changes involve plas- plus-maze was elevated 50 cm above the
tic alterations in the central nervous system floor. Experiments were carried out in a
(1) which might need some time to develop. sound-attenuated, temperature-controlled
The objective of the present study was to room, illuminated by two 40-W fluorescent
investigate if a certain time is needed for the lights placed 1.3 m away from the elevated
development of restraint-induced decreased plus-maze. The observer stayed in the same
activity in an elevated plus-maze. We also room, 1 m or so away from the maze.
wanted to determine whether behavioral tol- The groups consisted of animals tested 1,
erance to restraint stress will also be detect- 2, 24 or 48 h after the 2-h period of acute
able in our model. immobilization. An additional group was
Male Wistar rats (200-220 g) were housed restrained daily for 2 h for 7 days, and tested
in pairs in a temperature-controlled room in the plus-maze 24 h after the last restraint
(23 ± 1oC) under standard laboratory condi- period. The restraint period was chosen based
tions with free access to food and water and on previous findings from this and other
a 12-h light cycle (lights on at 6:30 a.m.). laboratories showing that it is effective to
The animals (N = 11-16/group) were immo- change exploratory activity in the elevated
bilized for 2 h in a wire chamber and tested plus-maze and/or an open arena (2-5,9,10).
in the elevated plus-maze. The number of entries with the four paws,
The restraint chamber (6.3 x 19.3 cm) and time spent in the open or enclosed arms
was made of wire and had an adjustable roof. of the elevated plus-maze were recorded for
The wood plus-maze consisted of two open 5 min. Control animals (N = 65) remained in
and two enclosed arms of equal length and their home cages throughout the experiment.
width (50 x 10 cm). The open arms had a 1- The percent of open arm entries (100 x
cm high Plexiglas edge while the enclosed open/total entries) and of time spent in the
arms had 40-cm high wooden sides. The open arms (100 open/open + enclosed) was
calculated for each rat. These data and the
Figure 1 - Elevated plus-maze ex-
ploration by rats (N = 11-16) 1, 2, 8 number of enclosed and open arm entries
were analyzed by one-way ANOVA followed
Enclosed arm entries

24 or 48 h after a 2-h restraint


period, as compared to non- 6 by the Duncan test for multiple compari-
stressed animals (Control, N =
65). A further group (Chronic) 4
sons. Since a significant effect on the num-
*
was tested 24 h after seven con- ber of enclosed arm entries was observed,
secutive daily periods of 2-h re- 2 analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) using
straint (N = 16). The upper panel
shows the mean (± SEM) num-
this variable as covariate was also applied to
0
ber of entries into enclosed the percentage of entries and time spent in
arms. The lower panel shows open arms (11).
the mean (± SEM) percentage of 30
entries (open bars) and time
The results are shown in Figure 1. Re-
spent (hatched bars) in the open 25 strained rats showed a decreased number of
arms. *P<0.05 compared to con- * entries (F5,134 = 4.84, P<0.01; Duncan test,
trol (ANOVA followed by Duncan
20 * P<0.05), percentage of entries (F5,134 = 2.7,
% Open/total

test).
P<0.05; Duncan test, P<0.05) and percent-
15
age of time spent (F5,134 = 3.9, P<0.01;
123
123
123
123
Duncan test, P<0.05) in the open arms, as
10 123
1234
1234
1234
123
123
123
compared to non-immobilized animals, 24
1234 123 123
5
1234
1234
1234
123
123
123
123
123
123
1234
1234
1234
and 48 h after stress. The former group also
1234 123 123 *
123 1234
1234
1234
1234
123
123
123
123
123
123
1234
*
1234
1234
123
123
123
1234
1234
1234
showed a decreased number of entries into
1234 123 123 1234 123 1234
0 enclosed arms (F5,134 = 5.4, P<0.01; Duncan
Control 1 h 2h 24 h 48 h Chronic
test, P<0.05). No effect was found in ani-

Braz J Med Biol Res 33(1) 2000


Restraint-induced hypoactivity 81

mals tested in the plus-maze 1 or 2 h after mitted to ANCOVA using the number of
restraint stress. ANCOVA using the number enclosed arm entries as a covariate, the re-
of entries into enclosed arms as the covariate sults indicate that the restraint stress resulted
showed that, although there was a trend in in a decrease in general activity.
the number of entries into open arms (F5,133 = A decrease in general exploratory activ-
2.22, P = 0.056), no significant effect was ity in an open arena after restraint stress has
found on this variable or on the percentage been previously described (9). In the el-
of time spent in the open arms. Repeated evated plus-maze most studies found a de-
daily immobilization periods for one week crease in the percentage of open arm entries
induced tolerance to the restraint-induced and/or time spent in them (2-5,10). How-
decreased activity (Duncan test, P>0.10). ever, results concerning the number of en-
The elevated plus-maze is a widely used closed arm entries are conflicting, with re-
animal model of anxiety (11). In this test, the ports of no change (2,4,5) or decreased (3,10)
percentage of open arm entries and time activity. One explanation for these conflict-
spent in the open arms has been validated as ing results could be that an anxiogenic effect
a measure of anxiety (11). of restraint enhanced the aversive novelty
The maze used in the present experiment aspect of the maze, inducing secondary
was modified by the addition of a small edge changes in general activity. Similar reduc-
to the open arms. Although this procedure tions in general activity have been found
has been shown to reduce the anxiolytic after anxiogenic drugs or procedures such as
effects of benzodiazepines in a different rat FG-7142 (14) or ethanol withdrawal (15).
strain (12), we have been able to detect both Exposure to a previous aversive experience
anxiolytic and anxiogenic effects of differ- could conceivably increase anxiety when the
ent treatments, including benzodiazepine animal is confronted by a new threatening
agonists and inverse agonists (13). This modi- situation represented by the novelty aspects
fication may have been responsible for a of the elevated plus-maze. To test this possi-
reduction in the percentage of time spent in bility it would be interesting to repeat in
the open arms in our laboratory as compared previously stressed rats the factorial analysis
to others. The results for the nonstressed study performed by File (11) in nonstressed
group in the present study, however, were animals.
not significantly different from those reported Other groups have also found interfer-
in several similar studies performed in our ence of previous stressful situations in ani-
laboratory (3,4,6). mal models of anxiety 24 h later. For ex-
Exposure to 2 h of restraint stress modi- ample, 15 min of restraint or forced swim-
fied the rat behavior in the elevated plus- ming were able to decrease open arm explo-
maze 24 h later, decreasing the exploration ration of the elevated plus-maze (16), whereas
of open arms. A similar effect was found 30 min of immobilization increased defen-
when the animals were tested 48 h, but not sive postures in a social test (17).
when they were tested 1 or 2 h after stress. The lack of behavioral changes when the
However, the restraint procedure also de- rats were tested 1 or 2 h after immobilization
creased the number of enclosed arm entries. shows that a certain time is needed for the
Based on experimental evidence, this pa- appearance of behavioral consequences of
rameter has been proposed to reflect general restraint stress in the elevated plus-maze
exploratory activity (11). Considering that performance. The mechanisms underlying
the significant effects on the number of en- this phenomenon are unknown. However,
tries and percentage of time spent in the open plastic changes in the central nervous system
arms disappeared when the data were sub- are probably needed for its development. It

Braz J Med Biol Res 33(1) 2000


82 C.M. Padovan and F.S. Guimarães

has already been shown that restraint and erant animals were more prone to showing
other forms of stress induce expression of signs of the “serotonergic syndrome” after
immediate early genes such as c-fos or c-jun systemic administration of a serotonin ago-
in several brain regions, including the hippo- nist, suggesting that stress tolerance was ac-
campus (18-20). These genes are proposed companied by signs of serotonin supersensi-
to act as “third messengers”, leading to alter- tivity. This may involve the hippocampus
ation of target genes expression related to since our group has shown that administra-
some stress-induced long-lasting changes in tion of 5HT1A agonists into this region at-
animal and human behavior (1). We have tenuates the restraint-induced deficit of plus-
also recently shown that cycloheximide, a maze open arm exploration (2,3).
protein synthesis inhibitor, after either icv or In conclusion, our results showed that a
hippocampal administration, attenuates the restraint stress experience induces behavior-
behavioral consequences of restraint stress al changes in rats, expressed as a deficit in
(4,5). open and enclosed arm exploration of an
The behavioral changes induced by re- elevated plus-maze 24 or 48 h later. They
straint stress on plus-maze performance dis- also suggest that daily repeated restraint in-
appeared when the animals were submitted duces tolerance to this effect.
to repeated 2-h daily immobilization peri-
ods. A similar effect was found by Kennett et Acknowledgments
al. (9), using an open arena instead of the
elevated plus-maze. Both data suggest that The authors acknowledge the helpful tech-
animals become tolerant to repeated restraint nical support provided by J.C. de Aguiar.
stress. Interestingly, in Kennett’s model tol-

References

1. Post RM (1992). Transduction of psycho- 6. Farabollini F, Fluck E, Albonetti ME & File chopharmacology, 116: 173-182.
social stress into the neurobiology of re- SE (1996). Sex differences in benzodiaz- 11. File SE (1992). Behavioural detection of
current affective disorder. American Jour- epine binding in the frontal cortex and anxiolytic action. In: Elliot JM, Heal DJ &
nal of Psychiatry, 149: 999-1010. amygdala of the rat 24 hours after re- Marsden CA (Editors), Experimental Ap-
2. Guimarães FS, Del Bel EA, Padovan CM, straint stress. Neuroscience Letters, 218: proaches to Anxiety and Depression. John
Mendonça Netto S & Titze-de-Almeida R 177-180. Wiley & Sons, New York, 25-44.
(1993). Hippocampal 5-HT receptors and 7. Clement HW, Kirsch M, Hasse C, Opper 12. Fernandes C & File SE (1996). The influ-
consolidation of stressful memories. Be- C, Gemsa D & Wesemann W (1998). Ef- ence of open arm ledges and maze expe-
havioural Brain Research, 58: 133-139. fect of repeated immobilization on sero- rience in the elevated plus-maze. Pharma-
3. Mendonça Netto SE & Guimarães FS tonin metabolism in different rat brain ar- cology, Biochemistry and Behavior, 54:
(1996). Role of hippocampal 5-HT1A re- eas and on serum corticosterone. Journal 31-40.
ceptors on elevated plus-maze explora- of Neural Transmission, 105: 1155-1170. 13. Russo AS, Guimarães FS, de Aguiar JC &
tion after a single restraint experience. 8. Albonetti ME & Farabollini F (1992). Be- Graeff FG (1993). Role of benzodiazepine
Behavioural Brain Research, 77: 215-218. havioral responses to single and repeated receptors located in the dorsal periaque-
4. Mendonça FH & Guimarães FS (1998). restraint in male and female rats. Behav- ductal grey of rats in anxiety. Psychophar-
Intra-hippocampal administration of cyclo- ioural Processes, 28: 97-110. macology, 110: 198-202.
heximide attenuates the restraint-induced 9. Kennett GA, Dourish CT & Curzon G 14. Cole BJ, Hillmann M, Seidelmann D,
exploratory deficit of an elevated plus (1987). Antidepressant-like action of 5- Klewer M & Jones GH (1995). Effects of
maze. Behavioural Brain Research, 91: HT1A agonists and conventional antide- benzodiazepine receptor partial inverse
207-211. pressants in an animal model of depres- agonists in the elevated plus maze test of
5. Mendonça FH, Félix FH, Del Bel EA & sion. European Journal of Pharmacology, anxiety in the rat. Psychopharmacology,
Guimarães FS (1996). Intraventricular cy- 134: 265-274. 121: 118-126.
cloheximide attenuates the restraint-in- 10. McBlane JW & Handley SL (1994). Ef- 15. Baldwin HA, Rassnick S, Rivier J, Koob
duced long-lasting effect on plus maze fects of two stressors on behaviour in the GF & Britton KT (1991). CRF antagonist
exploration. Brazilian Journal of Medical elevated X-maze: preliminary investigation reverses the “anxiogenic” response to
and Biological Research, 29: 501-505. of their interaction with 8-OH-DPAT. Psy- ethanol withdrawal in the rat. Psychophar-

Braz J Med Biol Res 33(1) 2000


Restraint-induced hypoactivity 83

macology, 103: 227-232. ogy and Behavior, 57: 431-437. roscience, 11: 2321-2331.
16. Martijena ID, Calvo N, Volosin M & Molina 18. Lino de Oliveira C, Guimarães FS & Del 20. Titze-de-Almeida R, Lino de Oliveira C,
VA (1997). Prior exposure to a brief re- Bel EA (1997). c-jun mRNA expression in Shida HW, Guimarães FS & Del Bel EA
straint session facilitates the occurrence the hippocampal formation induced by re- (1994). Midazolam and the N-methyl-D-
of fear in response to a conflict situation: straint stress. Brain Research, 753: 202- aspartate (NMDA) receptor antagonist 2-
behavioral and neurochemical correlates. 208. amino-7-phosphono-heptanoic acid (AP-7)
Brain Research, 752: 136-142. 19. Sharp FR, Sagar SM, Hicks K, Lowenstein attenuate stress-induced expression of c-
17. Albonetti ME & Farabollini F (1995). Ef- D & Hisanaga K (1991). c-fos mRNA, Fos, fos mRNA in the dentate gyrus. Cellular
fects of single restraint on the defensive and Fos-related antigen induction by hy- and Molecular Neurobiology, 14: 373-380.
behavior of male and female rats. Physiol- pertonic saline and stress. Journal of Neu-

Braz J Med Biol Res 33(1) 2000

Você também pode gostar