Você está na página 1de 18

See

discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.net/publication/259880454

Relevance of Piagetian Cross-Cultural


Psychology to the Humanities and Social
Sciences

Article in The American Journal of Psychology · November 2013


DOI: 10.5406/amerjpsyc.126.4.0477 · Source: PubMed

CITATIONS READS

12 53

1 author:

Georg Oesterdiekhoff
Karlsruhe Institute of Technology
126 PUBLICATIONS 238 CITATIONS

SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

traumzeit der menschheit View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Georg Oesterdiekhoff on 28 September 2017.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Relevance of Piagetian Cross-Cultural Psychology to the Humanities and Social Sciences
Author(s): Georg W. Oesterdiekhoff
Source: The American Journal of Psychology, Vol. 126, No. 4 (Winter 2013), pp. 477-492
Published by: University of Illinois Press
Stable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/10.5406/amerjpsyc.126.4.0477 .
Accessed: 02/12/2013 10:24

Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at .
http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp

.
JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of
content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms
of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact support@jstor.org.

University of Illinois Press is collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to The
American Journal of Psychology.

http://www.jstor.org

This content downloaded from 128.210.151.153 on Mon, 2 Dec 2013 10:25:02 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
Relevance of Piagetian Cross-Cultural
Psychology to the Humanities
and Social Sciences
GEORG W. OESTERDIEKHOFF
Karlsruhe Institute of Technology

Jean Piaget held views according to which there are parallels between ontogeny and the histori-
cal development of culture, sciences, and reason. His books are full of remarks and consider-
ations about these parallels, with reference to many logical, physical, social, and moral phenom-
ena. This article explains that Piagetian cross-cultural psychology has delivered the decisive data
needed to extend the research interests of Piaget. These data provide a basis for reconstructing
not only the history of sciences but also the history of religion, politics, morals, culture, philoso-
phy, and social change and the emergence of industrial society. Thus, it is possible to develop
Piagetian theory as a historical anthropology in order to provide a basis for the humanities and
social sciences.

Jean Piaget on Ontogeny and History clusions needed. Surely he wanted to develop child
Half a century ago, Parsons and Shils (1961) designat- psychology as a tool to reconstruct the world history
ed Jean Piaget as one of the greatest sociologists and of consciousness, reason, and mind, and additionally
psychologists. Moreover, Piaget could have gained or correspondingly the history of society, culture, and
the status of a Darwin or a Newton of the humanities sciences. He recognized the study of ontogenesis as a
and social sciences if he had worked out his develop- laboratory in which to explore the main characteris-
mental psychology more clearly than he actually did, tics of psyche and reason across history and cultures
to a theory of the history of mind, culture, and social since the Pleistocene (Piaget, 1996, p. 142f). Thus, he
development. One could maintain that Piaget failed emphasized his role as a genetic epistemologist and
to reach this goal because he did not do enough to not only a child psychologist so as to express this
develop his approach as a fundamental theory of the further horizon.
humanities and social sciences, to understand history However, he did not do enough to draw these con-
and culture, to describe the development of psyche clusions and to carry out this program in order to reach
and consciousness in history, and to draw all the con- his goal. As a consequence, developmental psychology

American Journal of Psychology


Winter 2013, Vol. 126, No. 4 pp. 477–492 • © 2013 by the Board of Trustees of the University of Illinois

This content downloaded from 128.210.151.153 on Mon, 2 Dec 2013 10:25:02 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
is largely or even solely a theory of ontogenesis and not made numerous remarks about the parallels between
a theory of culture and history, as it was in the time of ontogenetic and historical phenomena. His descrip-
James Mark Baldwin, Jean Piaget, Heinz Werner, or tions of conceptual realism, magic, animism, and
Karl Lamprecht. Therefore, the modern-day scholar of artificialism, the four main phenomena of children’s
history, sociology, economics, ethnology, or any of the worldview, are full of remarks about resemblances to
other social sciences or humanities has little idea of the the core structures of ancient philosophy and to the
role developmental psychology could play in shaping thinking of the so-called primitives.
or providing a basis for these disciplines. It is therefore However, he usually only described the histori-
necessary to resume these endeavors to enhance the cal phenomena in a few sentences or short sections.
role of Piaget and developmental psychology in the Thus, he never wrote a book about the history of law,
realm of scientific disciplines. based on notions gained through child psychology.
Piaget described the ontogenetic development of He never wrote a book about the history of technol-
psyche, reason, and consciousness in relation to the ogy, religion, magic, politics, economics, customs,
complete world understanding such as logic, nature, cultures, and so on, applying developmental con-
social relations, and morality. In most or all of his cepts. Conversely, he wrote his books mainly about
books, and sometimes also in his articles and essays, the development of children, additionally inserting
he hinted at the historical parallels of the phenom- occasional, very short reflections on historical phe-
ena he found among children. His main reference nomena. But there is one exception to this procedure.
points were the worldview and customs of tribal His Psychogenesis and the History of Sciences, with
societies or occidental philosophy, especially classi- Garcia (1989), is fully dedicated to the historical gen-
cal Greek philosophy. For example, his books about esis and development of sciences, using ontogenetic
chance (1975b), causality (1969), morals (1932), and phenomena only as a reference basis. Likewise, his
the worldview of children (1959) are full of parallels three other volumes on the development of sciences
to corresponding phenomena in premodern cultures. (Piaget, 1950/1975a, Vols. 8–10) deviated at least par-
There is hardly any relevant phenomenon to be found tially from the usual procedure described. Thus, only
among children, whose correspondence to histori- in the description of the history of sciences did Piaget
cal data he did not recognize and did not mention. realize his ambitious program to carry out research
He repeatedly described the way that both ancient on children in order to understand the history of hu-
humans and early sciences share the same primitive mankind, societies, and reason. Only in this area did
concepts of causality, chance, number, and physics he complete the transformation of ontogenetic data to
as children apply. These parallels consist not only human history, as the flamboyant approach of genetic
of some main traits but usually refer to the smallest epistemology intended or implied. However, com-
details too (Piaget, 1969, 1950/1975a, 1975b). pared with his research in this area, the deficiencies
For example, in his book about moral develop- in all other areas become more apparent. The main
ment, Piaget (1932) paralleled children’s understand- objective of Piaget would be realized only if scholars
ing of rules to that of ancient humans regarding laws were to reconstruct the history of psyche, reason,
in nature and society and, accordingly, the adolescent mentality, customs, religion, magic, law, economics,
treatment of rules to that of modern societies and and so on, at least to the same degree as Piaget himself
democratic legislation. In the same book, he recog- did in the research of the history of sciences.
nized children’s “objective responsibility” as a key How did Piaget explain the resemblances between
to understanding the main features of ancient law, ontogenetic and historical phenomena? He repeat-
traits of legislation and trials. Correspondingly, he edly said that all human beings have to go through
described the evolution of “subjective responsibility,” the same stages. Therefore, it would be quite normal
the restriction of the free will of sane humans, as the for ancient mentality to share the same structures that
single reference point to the attribution of judicial characterize children’s reason (Piaget, 1950/1975a,
responsibility, both in modern adolescents and in Vol. 9, p. 253; 1984, p. 173f). This answer is reason-
modern law, especially since the Enlightenment. In ably correct but far from being sufficient, because the
his book on the worldview of children, Piaget (1959) question immediately arises as to why the so-called

478 • OESTERDIEKHOFF

This content downloaded from 128.210.151.153 on Mon, 2 Dec 2013 10:25:02 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
primitives and ancient philosophies did not surpass be the last representative of classical sociology. Most
the childlike structures and did not reach the same of the founders of psychoanalysis, such as Carl Gus-
higher levels that characterize modern populations. tav Jung, Sandor Ferenczi, or Karl Abraham, and of
Obviously, humans of premodern societies remain developmental psychology, such as James Mark Bald-
bound to lower stages, whereas modern humans at- win, G. Stanley Hall, William Stern, Karl Zeininger,
tain higher stages. But why does the cognitive devel- or Heinz Werner, adhered to this idea of the child-
opment of premodern humans stop earlier in com- like psyche of ancient humans. Werner (1948) wrote a
parison to their modern counterparts? book in 1926 that was fully dedicated to the compari-
Piaget often said that the so-called primitives are son between premodern humans and modern chil-
only bound to preoperational stages. In most or all dren. He evidenced the similarities in all aspects of
of his books, he determined this classification subse- psyche and personality, ranging from understanding
quently and stepwise with regard to all phenomena nature, logic, and reason to morals and magic. This
relating to psyche and the whole world understand- book, famous in Germany, the United States, and
ing in physics, social relations, and morals. Whereas elsewhere for many decades, unfolded some of the
psyche, reason, and the worldview of premodern theoretical consequences, which formed the motives
peoples are shaped by preoperational structures, the behind the work of Piaget himself (Langer, 1988, pp.
Ionian philosophy is based on the establishment of 68–85; Kearney, 1973; Oesterdiekhoff, 2009c, 1997,
concrete operations, which had obviously originated pp. 7–45; 2000, pp. 49–79; 2011, pp. 25–39).
in the minds of these philosophers at that time. Fi- Furthermore, main representatives of humanities
nally, the historical evolution of the formal operations and social sciences, along with countless ethnogra-
accounts for the rise of mechanical philosophy and phers, voyagers, missionaries, and practitioners in
physical sciences in the 17th and 18th centuries (Piag- developmental regions, supported this idea of the
et, 1950/1975a, Vol. 9, p. 74f, p. 67, p. 96, p. 179, Vol. childlike psyche of primitive humans, among them
10, p. 184; Piaget & Garcia, 1989, p. 30). Only rarely Albert Schweitzer. Herbert Spencer and Leonard
did Piaget also attribute the higher stage of concrete Hobhouse, the leading sociologists in England for
operations to premodern populations but not the some generations, Edward Tylor and James Frazer,
adolescent stage of formal operations. “In particular the fathers of British anthropology and members of
it is quite possible (and it is the impression given by the Folklore Society, and Karl Lamprecht, the most
the known ethnographic literature) that in numerous read historian in Germany 100 years ago, belonged to
cultures adult thinking does not proceed beyond the this group (Oesterdiekhoff, 1997, 2000, 2011, 2012a,
level of concrete operations, and does not reach that 2012b). Moreover, also in the time span from 1970 up
of prepositional [formal] operations, elaborated be- to now there are some scholars who have adhered to
tween 12 and 15 years of age in our culture” (Piaget, this theory and delivered corresponding contribu-
1974, p. 309). tions, among them Habermas (1976), Ibarra (2007),
Accordingly, Piaget would support older theo- Radding (1985), and especially Hallpike (1979, 2004).
ries, which maintained that greater portions of adult However, today such voices are rare and do not
humans in premodern societies do not develop quali- belong to the mainstream of research and to the “of-
tatively beyond the levels of children, whereas only ficial spirit” of the sciences, whereas they expressed
modern humans can attain higher stages. Therefore, leading ideas of the sciences and public opinion from
the adolescent stage of formal operations might be a the era of the Enlightenment to 1945 or to 1970. The
recent historical evolution, originated in socialization ideological climate has changed dramatically, espe-
techniques to be found solely in modern cultures. cially since 1970. Ideologies of “cultural relativism”
Many scholars in several humanities and social sci- and “unity of mind” have replaced older theories of
ences, especially between 1800 and 1945 (to a lesser development and evolution, theories of primitive
extent up to 1970 and later), shared the theory of the mentality and of psychogenesis. However, nobody
childlike psyche of premodern humans. This idea has actually empirically falsified the older theories,
formed the center of the theory of Auguste Comte, although there are some widespread and deep-rooted
the founder of sociology, and of Norbert Elias, held to illusions that this has been achieved (see Berry, 1974).

RELEVANCE OF PIAGETIAN CROSS-CULTURAL PSYCHOLOGY • 479

This content downloaded from 128.210.151.153 on Mon, 2 Dec 2013 10:25:02 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
Piagetian Cross-Cultural Psychology school children of modern, industrialized countries
The easiest way and first step to applying develop- (Piaget & Inhelder, 1941). The formal operations
mental psychology to the historical disciplines would unfold stepwise and gradually between the 10th and
be to carry out research among contemporary bands 20th year of life in modern adolescents. Their origi-
of hunters and gatherers, nomads, peasant societies, nation provides the complete and encompassing for-
and illiterate populations in backward and traditional mation of concrete operations. The evolution of the
milieus in developing countries. Astonishingly, nei- fourth stage is dependent on the full functioning of
ther Piaget nor the scholars of Piagetian cross-cultural the third stage as its inevitable predecessor (Piaget,
psychology (PCCP) regarded this procedure as the 1950; Piaget & Inhelder, 1958, 1969).
main instrument to realize the actual targets of Piaget. Modern humans commonly develop concrete op-
PCCP collected data on numerous developmental erations completely; however, humans of premodern
phenomena from greatly varying social settings (see the cultures do so only partially or not at all. I will very
samplers edited by Poortinga, 1977; Dasen & Berry, briefly present a few related data to illustrate this.
1974; Dasen, 1977; Eckensberger et al., 1979; Kearney, Laurendeau-Bendavid (1977) researched the stage
De Lacey, & Davidson, 1973; Schöfthaler & Gold- development among 559 children in the age range
schmidt, 1984). The researchers and Piaget (1974) him- 5 to 17 years from Rwanda and French Canada. She
self, in his statements about PCCP, saw in the results no investigated four groups of children from Rwanda,
direct and internal link to his ideas about the parallels those with a school attendance of 3 and 6 years, with
between ontogenesis and history. Whenever he (1996) a completed school biography, and illiterate children
spoke about these parallels and the reconstruction of and adolescents, and compared them with children
history, he overlooked the possibilities PCCP was able from Montreal. She applied tests on seriation, num-
to offer, he neglected to base his related considerations ber, surface, causality, animism, and quantification of
on PCCP, and he did not even mention PCCP in this probabilities. The children from Montreal reached
context, as if PCCP had nothing to do with his main the stages some years earlier than the children from
research goal. It seems to me that only a handful of Rwanda. Among the unschooled children from
scholars worldwide identified the far-reaching pos- Rwanda, 5% to 20% reached concrete operations,
sibilities PCCP was able to offer. depending on the tests mentioned. These children
Most test researchers confirmed the universal ap- did not do much better when they were 17 years
plicability of Piagetian stage theory. The peculiarities old than at the age of 13 years. Not even 20% of the
of the early stages are globally identifiable and are 17-year-old teenagers reached concrete operations.
universal traits of all humans. Humans from all cul- Between 10% and 40% of the school children with
tures develop the first two stages roughly the same incomplete school education from Rwanda, with 3
way. Whether humans from exotic cultures reach or or 6 years of related experience, arrived at concrete
fail to reach the higher stages, they nevertheless give operations. In dependence on the test, 53%–85% of
largely the same answers as humans from developed the Rwandese children with completed school atten-
countries give when they reach or fail to reach higher dance, aged 15 or 17 years, attained concrete opera-
stages (De Lemos, 1973, p. 80f). Basing his summary tions (Laurendeau-Bendavid, 1977, p. 156).
on several related studies, Dasen (1973, p. 90) wrote, In his study on Papua New Guinea, Kelly (1977)
“It is generally recognized that the nature of the con- tested 216 unschooled and 216 schooled children in
cepts and the stages through which they develop are the age range 6 to 19 years (mean 11 years) on con-
identical in all Western and non-Western cultures. servation of quantity and length and on formal op-
The rate of development, on the other hand, is af- erations (pendulum task). With regard to the total
fected by environmental influences.” sample of 532 test subjects, the percentages with suc-
cessful conservation and with established concrete
Development of Concrete Operations operations are small (Table 1; Kelly, 1977, p. 186).
Most research was carried out on the development of Hundreds of tests on concrete operations in de-
concrete operations in all five continents. It develops velopmental regions delivered comparable results.
stepwise between the sixth and 12th year of life among According to de Lemos (1973), 50% of adult aborigi-

480 • OESTERDIEKHOFF

This content downloaded from 128.210.151.153 on Mon, 2 Dec 2013 10:25:02 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
p. 192) found that only 20% of illiterate adults from
TABLE 1. Conservation tasks in Papua New Guinea
Sardinia attained conservation of volume at that time.
Generally, European children from the countryside 50
Conservation Conservation
or 60 years ago developed less rapidly than city resi-
Age (yr) Sex of quantity (%) of length (%)
dents (Maistriaux, 1955, p. 442f; Tuddenham, 1969).
6–9 M 17 5 Asymptotic developments regarding concrete
F 21 4 operations are typical for Africans, aboriginal Aus-
10–12 M 43 8 tralians, Eskimos, Native Americans, and Papuans
F 11 3
(Ponzo, 1966; de Lemos, 1974; de Lacey, 1974; Vernon,
1969). According to reports from Peluffo, Maistriaux,
12–19 M 55 20
Goodnow, and Bethon, they were characteristic of
F 34 8 European and Chinese children from weaker milieus
Note. Data from Kelly (1977, p. 186). half a century ago, too. Freitag (1983, p. 354) found
that 50% of illiterate teens aged 16 living in favelas in
São Paulo stayed at the border of preoperational and
nal Australians attain concrete operations with regard concrete operational stages, 41.6% were within the
to quantity and 75% with regard to length. According concrete stage, and only 8.4% moved to the border
to Dasen (1974b, p. 395f ), only 0% to 30% of adult of concrete and formal operational stages.
aborigines attained conservation tasks. The adults The attainment of concrete operations is nearly
often perform worse than their children because they complete among adolescents in most advanced coun-
have even less contact with the White culture and tries. However, half a century ago, asymptotic and
schools (Table 2). incomplete attainments were common in the weaker
In the 1960s, 40% of the illiterate Chinese children social milieus of Western countries, too. There may
in Hong Kong aged 13 reached concrete operations, be illiterate, traditional, and backward regions within
and not one reached formal operations (Goodnow & developing countries that have a minimum of con-
Bethon, 1966). Forty to fifty percent of 15-year-old city crete operations or are even largely preoperational.
dwellers in Zambia attained conservation of weight The usual phenomenon is a certain presence of con-
(Heron & Simonsson, 1974). Fifty percent of the adult crete operations. However, only greater or smaller
illiterate Wolof from Senegal attained conservation of parts of the populations attain this stage and then only
volume (Greenfield, 1966). Roughly the same percent- in specific areas. Concrete operations are far from
ages among illiterate adult Algerians attained conser- dominating the world understanding and reason-
vation of speed and time (Bovet, 1974). Thirty-seven ing of premodern populations. “According to this
percent of teenagers from Iraq aged 13 years reached evidence, it can no longer be assumed that adults of
conservation of speed and 80% reached conservation all societies reach the concrete operational stage”
of length (Al-Fakhri, 1977, p. 211). Peluffo (1962, 1967, (Dasen, 1974a, p. 418).

TABLE 2. Conservation tasks in Australia

Canberra (White children) Hermannsburg (aborigines) Areyonga (aborigines)

Conservation 100% 70% 40% 30% 30%


of quantity (9 years) (16 years) (adults) (15 years) (adults)
Conservation 80% 50% 20% 9% 10%
of weight (12 years) (15 years) (adults) (14 years) (adults)
Conservation 50% 60% 30% 30% 0%
of volume (12 years) (15 years) (adults) (15 years) (adults)
Note. Data from Dasen (1974b, p. 395f).

RELEVANCE OF PIAGETIAN CROSS-CULTURAL PSYCHOLOGY • 481

This content downloaded from 128.210.151.153 on Mon, 2 Dec 2013 10:25:02 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
Development of Formal Operations groups” (Laurendeau-Bendavid, 1977, p. 158). School
The formal operations unfold gradually between the experience must therefore have a certain amount and
10th and 20th years of age in schooled adolescents quality in order to arouse formal operations.
of modern societies (Piaget & Inhelder, 1958, 1969; According to Freitag (1983), only one illiterate
Schröder, 1989). This fourth stage is often divided favela resident among hundreds attained the formal
into substages A and B. Easier forms of syllogisms level. Illiterate people from China, India, Malaysia,
belong to A, and more complicated forms such as Africa, Brazil, Australia, Central Asia, and Europe do
counterintuitive syllogisms, the pendulum task, or not develop the formal operations (Goodnow & Be-
the four-color test belong to B. Nearly all modern thon, 1966; Ashton, 1975; Ember, 1977; Lurija, 1982;
adolescents master the easier forms of syllogisms, but Dasen, 1974b, p. 400; Peluffo, 1967; Were, 1968; Wer-
only 30% to 50% of them accomplish tasks belonging ner, 1979; Kelly, 1977, p. 184; Hallpike, 1979; Flynn,
to B (Schröder, 1989; Mogdil & Mogdil, 1976, Vol. III, 2007, 2008; Oesterdiekhoff, 2006c, 2009b, 2009c,
p. 149; Flynn, 2007, p. 31; Kohlberg & Gilligan, 1971; 2011, 2012a).
Oesterdiekhoff & Rindermann, 2008). Substage A Half a century ago in Sardinia, 55% of people
unfolds between 10 and 15 years of age and B unfolds from the middle classes, 25% of country children
between 15 and 20 years of age, if it appears at all. aged 11 years, and 20% of illiterate adults reached
It is obvious that the weak development of con- the formal operations (combinatorial tests) (Peluffo,
crete operations among premodern populations 1967, p. 195). Whereas 66% of White boys and 44%
affects the possibilities of the formal operations oc- of White girls from sixth grade in New South Wales
curring. If only 0%, 20%, 30%, 50%, or 70% of a re- (Australia) reached the formal operations (pendulum)
spective population reaches concrete operations, the (Philp & Kelly, 1974), not one person among the 532
percentages of those who attain formal operations Papuans accomplished this task (Kelly, 1977, p. 183).
might be much smaller. It can be expected that the In a follow-up study with 1,536 university students
weaker and asymptotic development of the third stage from Papua New Guinea and Malawi, where the pen-
extends to an almost complete lack of the fourth stage. dulum task was replaced by the easier four-factor test
Moreover, the asymptotic development of Substage B (an electronic analogue to the four-chemical test), it
among modern populations is linked both to the lack was found that only a few test subjects were able to
of formal operations and the asymptotic development solve this problem (Kelly, 1977, p. 184).
of concrete operations among premodern popula- The problems of developing even the border of
tions. Only the brighter and more educated milieus concrete and formal operations or the easiest phases
in modern societies develop the formal operations at of formal operations were documented by Lurija
a high rate (Gellathy, 1987, p. 43). and coworkers (1982, p. 108) in their famous survey
According to Laurendeau-Bendavid (1977, p. about the Kashgars in Uzbekistan in 1932 and 1933.
155f), the unschooled Rwandese never reach the for- Eighty to ninety percent of illiterate Kashgars did
mal operations. Only one Rwandese test subject with not understand the easier forms of syllogisms as well
incomplete school attendance reached the formal as children from industrialized countries up to their
stage. Pupils there must attend school for at least 6 10th year of life do. Lurija found the same peculiari-
years without interruption in order to gain the chance ties to cause this failure as Piaget had identified with
to reach the formal level with 13 years. Twenty-five regard to children in their first decade of life. Later
percent of Rwandese children with completed school studies evidenced that illiterate adults of traditional
experience reached the formal stage (probabilities) cultures from all continents answer the same way as
but only one pupil from the three other groups (0, 3, adult Kashgars or modern children (Oesterdiekhoff,
and 6 years of school attendance) did so. “Finally, in 2009b, pp. 73–82). Humans who do not even handle
the three other tasks concerning causal reasoning and syllogisms have no chance of using abstract and de-
formal thinking [causality, animism, probabilities], ductive forms of logic and reasoning. “So it seems
only the fully schooled subjects appear more ad- correct to state that there may be no theoretic syllogis-
vanced than all the others, and there is no difference tic thinking in the cultures that are strictly traditional”
between any of the partially schooled and unschooled (Tulviste, 1979, p. 77).

482 • OESTERDIEKHOFF

This content downloaded from 128.210.151.153 on Mon, 2 Dec 2013 10:25:02 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
The notion that the adolescent stage of formal op- maternal care, preschool education, schools, occu-
erations is largely bound to educated populations of pational improvements, and media have also contrib-
modern, industrial societies has passably or partially uted. But most scholars of PCCP and intelligence
gained the status of a well-known doctrine. “It would research agree on the prevailing role of school educa-
seem that throughout history many societies have tion (e.g., Flynn, 2007; Irvine & Berry, 1988; Neisser,
never manifested combinatorial and prepositional 1998; Oesterdiekhoff, 2009b, pp. 82–98; 2012a, pp.
logic or the other characteristics of formal thinking” 49–78; 2012b, pp. 130–154).
(Ross, 1974, p. 413). In the meantime, hundreds of Thus, the lack of the adolescent stage character-
scholars and testers have confirmed these conclu- izes developmental, traditional, illiterate, or backward
sions (e.g., Chapman, 1988, p. 98; Dasen, 1974a, milieus in current developing countries; European,
pp. 412, 418, 421; 1974b, pp. 395, 400; Segall, Dasen, Western, and Japanese cultures partially 100 years
Berry, & Poortinga, 1990, p. 154; Ibarra, 2007; Hall- ago, largely 200 years ago, and nearly completely 300
pike, 1979; Eckensberger et al., 1979; Werner, 1979; years ago; the famous agrarian civilizations such as
Flynn, 2007; Habermas, 1976; Kohlberg, 1974; Oes- the Chinese Empire, the Roman Empire, India, Per-
terdiekhoff & Rindermann, 2008; Oesterdiekhoff, sia, Arabia, Egypt, and other well-known cultures of
1997, 2009b, 2011, 2012a, 2012b; Piaget, 1974, p. 309; the past; peasant societies and early kingdoms; bands
1950/1975a, Vols. 8–10). of hunters and gatherers; nomadic cultures; and
Pleistocene humans (Oesterdiekhoff, 2009b, 2009c,
Culture and Cognitive Development 2011, 2012a, 2012b).
Formal operations appeared among the intellectual In the view of developmental psychology, the
elite in Europe in the 17th and 18th centuries, when great divergence concerns the difference between
magic, animism, and artificialism (core concepts of modern and premodern societies and not the differ-
preformal, childlike stages) were replaced by me- ence between tribal societies and civilizations. There-
chanical philosophy and physical sciences (Piaget fore, the rise of modern, industrial culture and the
& Garcia, 1989; Piaget, 1950/1975a, Vols. 8–10; Oes- growth of adolescent stages are two sides of the same
terdiekhoff, 2011, in press). The establishment of phenomenon and belong to each other.
compulsory school systems in Europe 100–150 years Some authors have refuted the empirical results
ago, based on scientific curricula, caused the rise of of PCCP. They maintain that the deficit results in
formal operations, at least Substage A, among greater the development of operations would show only a
parts of the population. Secondary school attendance diverging development but would not disclose the
and universities enhanced these evolutions. Today existing ones (Berry, 1974; Cole & Scribner, 1974).
the world faces a spread of school education, based This argumentation extrapolates that instead of op-
on scientific curricula, and thus observes the evolu- erations, exotic peoples would manifest exotic and
tion of higher stages on all continents, unequally but alien cognitions, unknown in Western cultures and
continuously. The 20th century was the era not only opaque to the test procedures practiced in psychol-
of the global spread of modern societies but also of ogy. Thus, these critics support theories of cultural
the higher stages of cognition (Flynn, 2007, 2008; relativism. Some others say the results might show
Oesterdiekhoff, 2009c, 2012a, 2012b; Barber, 2005). only performance deficits but not competence defi-
The internal link between scientific curricula cits. If exotic cultures really needed the operations,
and formal operations is the main reason for the phe- they would apply them. They do not show them in
nomenon that premodern populations never had a test procedures because they have no experience
chance to establish the adolescent stage. They did not with tests and are not accustomed to abstractions and
provide modern, scientific curricula, and therefore deductions because they do not need them in their
they had no formal operations in their minds. Barber everyday life. When really necessary, all people are
(2005) estimated that changes in school education able to apply the achievements and abilities needed
explain two thirds of rising intelligence. Of course, (Cole & Scribner, 1974).
many phenomena of modern cultures have played These relativistic ideas are not supported by the
their part in fostering cognitive growth. Nutrition, empirical data. The tests reveal not only the lack of

RELEVANCE OF PIAGETIAN CROSS-CULTURAL PSYCHOLOGY • 483

This content downloaded from 128.210.151.153 on Mon, 2 Dec 2013 10:25:02 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
higher stages but also the existing and working ones; if the cultural environment offers no particular chal-
that is, the prevalence of the lower stages. Thus, the lenges of its own, development very rarely reaches the
lack of performance is usually rooted in a failing of level of concrete operations. It seems that at the age of
competence. The higher stages are not established 7 years, the unschooled subjects have already reached
because the lower stages are manifest. The people at a level which they do not surpass in subsequent years”
preformal stages display forms of world understand- (Laurendeau-Bendavid, 1977, p. 144). Lurija (1982)
ing, behavior, and performance that developmental found the same in his famous study on the Kashgars.
psychology knows as parts and dimensions of lower In all his tests (perception, generalization, abstraction,
stages. Thus, there are no alien cognitions that re- deduction, inference, problem solving, imagination,
place the lack of formal operations, but instead the and self-analysis), the illiterate adult Kashgars, aged
lower stages of cognition are at work (Oesterdiekhoff, 30 or 50 years, did not exceed the level of children
2009b, pp. 63–129; Hallpike, 1979). aged 6 or 7 years. The answers and reactions of both
The so-called training studies showed that op- groups were identical.
erations cannot be easily learned (Mogdil & Mogdil, Intelligence research has estimated the mental age
1976). The premodern people cannot surmount their of people since the days of Alfred Binet or even before.
magical–animistic schemes, just as children are not able Porteus (1937) computed the mental age of Khoisan
to surmount magic, animism, and other manifestations at 7 and a half years, and Vernon (1969) computed
of their anthropological stage. These phenomena are that of illiterate Brazilians and other groups at 7 years.
deeply rooted in psyche and personality, in psychocog- Maistriaux (1955, pp. 419, 414, 454) compared several
nitive structures. Developmental psychology does not groups of Arabians and Black Africans both in their
deal with “information” and “techniques” but with countries and in France, with different school educa-
anthropological stages. Only when premodern people tion, and estimated their mental age within a range
settle in modern cultures, attend schools, and partici- from 5 to 11 years, respectively. His test subjects did
pate in modern culture early in their childhood can not largely exceed the level of children aged 7 years,
they attain higher stages. Later in life they have difficul- even when their biological age was 20 or 30 years
ties in surmounting their preformal structures. They (Maistriaux, 1955, p. 454). Thus, the psychometric
do not give up their adherence to magic, animism, pre- intelligence approach comes to the same conclusion
causality, and all the other manifestations of the child- as the cognitive developmental approach regarding the
like psyche and preformal stages, as the relevant data mental age of premodern and modern populations.
from ethnography and cross-cultural psychology have However, the developmental approach allows
shown (Dasen, 1974b; Nurcombe, 1973; Werner, 1948; further conclusions. PCCP showed that premodern
Oesterdiekhoff, 2009b, pp. 63–82; 2012a, pp. 49–78; populations stay at preformal stages with regard to the
2012b, pp. 70–97, 172–192; Lévy-Bruhl, 1923, 1931, 1938; understanding of nature, social relations, and mor-
Jahoda, 1974; Hallpike, 1979; Lurija, 1982; Maistriaux, als. The stages cover the whole understanding of the
1955, p. 441). world, the whole development of psyche and person-
ality, and therefore concern the entire anthropological
Anthropological Conclusions structure or summit of a person or a population. The
When modern populations stay at Substage A, their intelligence approach can only measure reasoning
“anthropological summit,” their “developmental age” abilities (mental age); the developmental approach
or “mental age” is roughly spread between 10 and 15 can additionally identify the anthropological summit
years of age. When premodern populations stay largely or the level of the entire personality. Rightly inter-
at preoperational stages, they mainly reach the mental preted, PCCP comes to the conclusion that premod-
age of children up to 7. When they are more on the ern populations stay at the anthropological stages of
concrete level, they are spread between the two other children, whereas only modern populations attain
groups, roughly between 7 and 12 years. Laurendeau- higher or adolescent stages. The psychostructural
Bendavid found that unschooled adolescents aged 15 difference between premodern and modern popula-
or 17 years usually did not develop above an intellectual tions is spread between 0 and 15 developmental years,
level of a 7-year-old child. “If there is no schooling, and usually between 3 and 10 years.

484 • OESTERDIEKHOFF

This content downloaded from 128.210.151.153 on Mon, 2 Dec 2013 10:25:02 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
Numerous scholars have paralleled children forests, and so on, which are completely different
and primitives, as shown earlier. “In this respect the from modern understandings and related emotions.
performance of traditional peoples is closely par- Whichever kind of experience we want to face and
alleled by that of young children in industrialized examine, we always find deviations between our and
countries” (Gellathy, 1987, p. 37). But there must their interpretations, as ethnography and ethnology
be some differences between premodern popula- have described (Evans-Pritchard, 1976; Fortune,
tions and children because children do not usually 1963; Lévy-Bruhl, 1923, 1931, 1938, 1971, 1983, 1985).
build houses and temples, and they do not work in If we want to find a theoretical framework for these
agriculture and trades. However, developmental psy- data and facts, we need look no further than to de-
chology and PCCP cannot identify these differences velopmental psychology. Developmental psychology
because both groups share the same stages, cogni- delivers the long-sought theoretical foundation to
tive structures, ideological worlds, and behavioral ethnology and historical disciplines (Hallpike, 1979;
consequences stemming from them. However, pre- Oesterdiekhoff, 2009b, 2011; Radding, 1985; Ibarra,
modern populations have more life experience and 2007; Habermas, 1976).
knowledge than children have. According to Hallpike It is by no means difficult to combine ethnology
(1979), premodern peoples and children share the and developmental psychology, as many would sur-
same qualitative development (stages and structures) mise. Conversely, ethnological and developmental
but not the quantitative development (knowledge and data correspond to each other in a way that makes it
experience). However, the influence of the qualitative hard to believe that ethnologists from the last genera-
structures on reason and behavior is overwhelming. tions found it so difficult to find this link. Whatever
What is the right formula to express common- child psychologists have written about children’s
alities and differences? The right formula is iden- thinking about earth and sky, magic and ghosts, myths
tified when one says that “premodern peoples stay and fairy tales, life and death, animism and artificial-
at anthropological stages of children and share their ism, conceptual realism and dream understanding fit
cognitive structures down to the smallest details, with the ethnological descriptions detail for detail, some-
regard to the entire world understanding, apart from times word for word, as Hallpike and I elaborated in
some forms of knowledge and life experience.” It is some of our books.
necessary to use such clear words if one wants to I am able to give only some very brief examples
secure a complete understanding of the facts. Having here to provide a concise insight into these coher-
done so, it becomes obvious that this notion is one of ences. Von den Steinen (1894) reports about some
the most important in all the humanities and social Indians from the Brazilian jungle who ran behind a
sciences. This is the only discovery in social sciences slave. They saw a turtle on the way and maintained
and the humanities that corresponds to the discovery that the slave had turned into this animal in order to
of evolutionary theory in biology (Oesterdiekhoff, escape. Many ethnographers have described at length
2011, pp. 206–220; 2012a, pp. 581–602). that primitives around the world fully believed that
Thus, PCCP delivers the basis for historical an- any kind of metamorphosis could take place any-
thropology or historical psychology. where at any time. Stones can transform into humans
and gods, plants and humans can transform into any
PCCP as a Key to Understanding Premodern Societies forms of animals, and so on (Oesterdiekhoff, 2011, p.
Culture and worldview, religion and magic, conduct 110; Lévy-Bruhl, 1931, 1938, 1971, 1983, pp. 57–61):
and customs, reasoning and emotions, morals and
politics of premodern societies can be understood No metamorphosis, however ridiculous it
fully by reference to the notions about preformal might appear to us, is therefore ruled out sim-
structures that developmental psychology and PCCP ply as physically impossible. . . . According
have described. Primitives have understandings of to our way of thinking, if we could suppose it
birth and death, sickness and health, childhood and possible for a man to change at a substitution of
adulthood, females and males, animals and plants, one kind of outward and visible appearance for
earth and heaven, rivers and oceans, mountains and another—from smooth-skinned biped to shaggy

RELEVANCE OF PIAGETIAN CROSS-CULTURAL PSYCHOLOGY • 485

This content downloaded from 128.210.151.153 on Mon, 2 Dec 2013 10:25:02 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
quadruped. Certainly something more than that Piaget (1959, 1969) showed that animism, an-
would seem to be involved: the new dog would thropomorphism, and personification are stronger
seem scarcely likely to have much in common, the younger the children are. Children only in their
mentally speaking, with the former man. More, first decade tend to animism. Adolescents in mod-
we are so convinced of this incongruity that any ern societies replace the animistic understanding of
such change is necessarily considered by us to the world with the mechanical worldview and ani-
be possible only in the realm of fancy; noth- mistic explanations with empirical-causal ones. The
ing will ever persuade us that in sober reality a transformation from the animistic to the mechanical
man could turn into a dog, or a dog into a man. worldview is part of the evolution of formal opera-
But with the Australian Aborigines the case is tions. The empirical surveys within PCCP evidenced
different. From their earliest infancy they have that premodern populations maintain their children’s
been accustomed to hear about beings who are animism lifelong and never establish empirical-causal
at the same time both animals and men and can explanations (Mogdil & Mogdil, 1976; Peluffo, 1962,
change with complete ease from the one form 1967; Kälble, 1997; Havighurst & Neugarten, 1955;
to the other. (Lévy-Bruhl, 1983, p. 83) Jahoda, 1958a, 1958b). The evolution of mechanical
philosophy and physical sciences in the 17th century
It would be impossible to convince a modern was the first step in the evolution of formal opera-
person, no matter which kind of technique or con- tions and the modern worldview, thus replacing the
ditioning is used, of the existence of metamorphic animistic worldview of the Middle Ages (Fetz, 1982;
transformations. Children up to their sixth year of Fetz, Reich, & Valentin, 2001; Kälble, 1997; Piaget &
life are the only sane people in modern societies who Garcia, 1989; Piaget, 1950/1975a, Vols. 8–10; Oester-
share the beliefs about metamorphosis. De Vries diekhoff, 1997, 2012a, 2012b).
(1969) put a dog mask on a cat’s head. Beforehand, The tendency to personification and anthropo-
the children tested had sufficient opportunity to be- morphism, as part of animism, is typical for both
come acquainted with the cat presented. Children groups. For generations, child psychologists ex-
aged 4, 5, and 6 years believed that the cat had turned plained that children attribute humanlike reason,
into a dog and expected it to start barking and biting. understanding, and morals to plants and especially to
Older children of 7 or 8 years only saw a cat wear- animals. Only adolescents on the formal operational
ing a dog mask. De Vries (1969) and Flavell (1977) level recognize the differences between animals and
understood the replacement of metamorphosis by humans with regard to willpower, reason, and mor-
the recognition of the invariance of species as part of als (Bühler, 1930; Stern, 1924; Werner, 1948; Piaget,
the establishment of concrete operations, similar to 1959; Zeininger, 1929). Ethnography documented the
the understanding of the invariance of volume and same with regard to premodern peoples around the
other physical phenomena. world. They assume that animals and plants know
Nearly every early child psychologist and n­ early everything about what happens in human settlements
every ethnographer has described the animistic and can understand human language, as if they had
schemes in the reasoning of their subjects. Children humanlike reason. They fully believe that their talk-
and premodern peoples regard stones, rocks, moun- ing to plants makes them grow and that plants can
tains, plants, forests, rivers, oceans, stars, sun, moon, walk if they want (Fortune, 1963; Lévy-Bruhl, 1971).
clouds, rainfalls, and so on as living beings. They do The Azande ask the termites about future activities
not differentiate between dead matter and living be- to help them plan or ask about the truth of past oc-
ings. The premodern peoples adore and sacrifice to currences (Evans-Pritchard, 1976). The peoples take
rivers and forests, mountains and moon, regarding blood revenge against predators and horses of hostile
them as persons and gods. They entertain dialogs tribes. If they have courts they accuse, convict, and
with sun and moon, rivers and rainfalls, plants and punish animals as if they were delinquents. Up to
rocks (Tylor, 1871; von den Steinen, 1894; Evans- the time of the Enlightenment, medieval courts con-
Pritchard, 1976; Lévy-Bruhl, 1971, 1985; Oesterdiek- victed animals by the same methods as were applied
hoff, 2007a; 2011, pp. 96–101; 2009b, pp. 211–223). against criminals (Fischer, 2005; Oesterdiekhoff,

486 • OESTERDIEKHOFF

This content downloaded from 128.210.151.153 on Mon, 2 Dec 2013 10:25:02 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
2009d, 2011, pp. 102–109). “The capital punishment If we only knew from these peoples their magical
of a dumb animal for its crimes seems to us so irra- beliefs and practices, this would provide sufficient
tional and absurd that we can hardly believe that sane evidence of their childlike psyche and personal-
and sober men were ever guilty of such folly” (Evans, ity in the deepest sense possible. The elimination
1906, p. 157). Judicial procedures against animals and of magical beliefs since the rise of mechanical phi-
related complete misunderstandings of the capacities losophy, physical sciences, and the Enlightenment
of animals clearly show not only the intellectual weak- manifests the origination of formal operations and the
ness of these peoples but also their childlike psyche psychocognitive maturation of humankind beyond
and personality in the deepest sense possible. preformal levels for the first time in history. The cor-
Child psychologists described magical beliefs respondence to the emergence of adolescence among
and procedures as inevitable parts of children’s modern populations is complete.
psyche. Children in their first decade of life cannot Feuerbach (1985) was the first to maintain that
avoid grasping the order of the world as if organized the childlike psyche of premodern humankind ex-
by magic. The world was created by magic, occur- plains religion, whereas the maturation of human-
rences and regularities originated in magic. Adults kind accounts for the modern elimination of religion
and gods steer and control the world by their magic. and the rise of atheism. In my opinion, my book on
Full belief in magic is a part of the preoperational religion (in press) will be the first comprehensive
stage. The last remains of magic usually last until the book that explains the nature of religiousness and
tenth year of life. With the formal operational stage, atheism, including all relevant aspects, in terms of
the empirical-causal explanations eliminate magic. In developmental psychology, thus following Feuer-
comparison with the child, the modern adolescent bach’s approach. The core concepts of all religions
has surmounted magical beliefs (Piaget, 1959, 1969, are as follows: (1) Gods or ancestors created the
1950/1975a, Vol. 5; Zeininger, 1929; Werner, 1948; cosmos by magic. (2) The running of the world is
Stern, 1924; Bühler, 1930; Jahoda, 1974; Oesterdiek- ruled by gods. (3) Gods punish or reward humans
hoff, 2009b, pp. 203–211; 2011, pp. 112–132). on earth and in heaven. (4) Humans communicate
Ethnography found out that there is no premod- with their gods by prayers and sacrifices (to feed
ern society without the full belief in magic with far- them). (5) Humans have a life after death, preferably
reaching and astonishing practical consequences. in heaven or paradise, or they reincarnate. (6) An-
The peoples understand the world as ruled by magic, cient humans are capable of creating myths that tell
carried out by humans, ghosts, and gods. They rec- biographies about gods and to believe in such tales.
ognize every incident in terms of magic, from strong Religiousness is thus rooted in a psyche, which is
rainfalls, accidents, and sickness to birth and death. susceptible to myths. (7) Divinities are imaginary
Deaths by accident or disease are in reality assas- giants or deceased ancestors.
sinations by magicians. Ordeals are used to detect Children’s adherence to magic and artificialism
them, and the relatives punish the murderers. The account for the religious belief into divine creation
villages are full of alleged murderers who are believed and rulership regarding cosmos and world (1, 2).
to have killed others, who actually died of natural Children’s expectations toward parents explain the
causes. The people buy charms at great cost in order belief in divine reward and punishment, in prayers
to kill people, to seduce partners, to grow plants, to and sacrifices (3, 4). Cognitive egocentrism and child-
catch fish, to find gold, and to defeat enemies. They like wishful thinking disclose the belief in the immor-
kill or punish people who are believed to have caused tality of the soul, in paradise and heaven (5). Further-
harm by magic. They really believe that people are more, child psychologists describe the age between
capable of arousing storms on oceans, turning into 4 and 8 as the time when children are keen on myths
other species, causing rainfalls or sunshine, or bring- (Stern, 1924; Bühler, 1930; Zeininger, 1929; Piaget,
ing about births and deaths magically (Fortune, 1963; 1950/1975a, Vol. 5). Countless ethnologists disclosed
Evans-Pritchard, 1976; Mair, 1969; Lévy-Bruhl, 1923, that premodern peoples have the same attitudes to-
1931, 1938; Oesterdiekhoff, 2002, 2006a, 2007c, 2012a, ward myths as children have. Thus, the mythical
2012b, in press). belief in gods is rooted in the childlike m ­ entality, as

RELEVANCE OF PIAGETIAN CROSS-CULTURAL PSYCHOLOGY • 487

This content downloaded from 128.210.151.153 on Mon, 2 Dec 2013 10:25:02 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
Wundt (1914), Fetz et al. (2001), Campbell (1996), and magic, animism, and artificialism in modern adoles-
many others had already detailed (6). cents, he delivered the decisive tools to explain reli-
The most important gods in all premodern societ- gion and atheism as well. Thus, my theory can fully
ies are the deceased parents, grandparents, and other confirm and extend the early analysis of Feuerbach.
relatives (7). Ancestors usually have been more impor- These short expositions are intended only to de-
tant in cults and sacrifices than any other divinities, liver a first and concise insight into the possibilities
in China, India, Africa, Australia, Polynesia, ancient the developmental approach allows in providing a
America, and Greek–Roman antiquity (Frazer, 1911, foundation for ethnology and historical disciplines.
1922, 1924; Lévy-Bruhl, 1931, 1938, 1971; Durkheim, Understanding of all kinds of behavior and conduct,
1965; Oesterdiekhoff, 2009b, pp. 261–277, 2011, pp. ideologies and philosophies, politics and morals can
147–162, 2012a, 2012b, in press). Piaget (1959), Ze- benefit from being referred to preformal stages of cog-
ininger (1929), and Bovet (1951) evidenced that by nition and psyche.
their sixth year of life, children regard their parents
(and other adults) as gods who master the world. The Conclusions
first skeptical crisis weakens the adoration of parents PCCP is much more important to humanities and so-
in favor of the imaginary god of the established culture. cial sciences than is usually recognized. PCCP reveals
However, the premodern peoples never experience the main objectives behind the lifework of Piaget. It
this crisis but remain bound to the adoration of el- confirms that humankind has developed the same
ders, even after their death. Thus, ancestor worship, way as children and adolescents in modern societies.
the adoration of parents and grandparents, is rooted in Developmental psychology is not only a child psy-
lifelong lasting childlike attitudes and in preoperational chology but also a psychology that reveals the history
stages. The adoration of god the father in heaven is of the development of mind, psyche, and personality,
obviously psychologically not so far removed from the the history of consciousness of the entire humankind.
family religion. Therefore, primitive religions cultivate PCCP implies a psychology of cultures and peoples,
a belief in ancestor gods, god the father in heaven, and a psychology of populations, which lived in tribal
other domain gods at the same time. societies, medieval societies, and antiquity. Scholars
Premodern cultures do not know atheism and such as Baldwin, Lamprecht, Schneider, Wygotski,
agnosticism. Intellectuals of the Middle Ages are all Lurija, Piaget, Brunner-Traut, and Habermas recog-
believers, without exception. Disenchantment of the nized developmental psychology as a key discipline
world, decline of religion and magic, and seculariza- for all historical sciences. They all need developmen-
tion originated in the era of the Enlightenment for tal psychology as their necessary foundation.
the first time. Agnosticism and atheism spread in the Developmental psychology is an instrument for
educated classes of the 19th century and have now explaining religion, magic, customs, politics, and
reached roughly half of the Europeans and Japanese. economics. Premodern societies can be understood
Although 90% of the populations of developing coun- only by reference to the preoperational and concrete
tries are still believers, the situation there is chang- operational stages. Conversely, the evolution of indus-
ing too. Seventy-nine percent of the members of the trial societies, physical sciences, the Enlightenment,
Royal Society of London “deny religion completely,” democracy, and the humanitarian revolution in the
and 97% of them profess to be atheists. Ninety-three past 300 years has to be linked to the rise of anthro-
percent of the members of the American Academy pological stages and formal operations. For example,
of Sciences are atheists (Larson & Witham, 1998, p. the evolution of humane punishment systems in law,
313). My theory (2007a, 2008, 2012a, 2012b, in press) the removal of the ordeal process, and objective re-
explains the rise of atheism according to the rise of sponsibility in legislation and trial are psychocogni-
formal operations and psychocognitive maturation. tive evolutions. Piaget himself (1932, 1950/1975a, Vol.
The century of rising formal operations is also the era 10) saw the links between the rise of formal operations
of rising atheism: It is clear that the decline of religion and the rise of sciences, democracy, and industrial-
is only a manifestation of the rise of anthropological ism. These five unique developments share largely
summits. As Piaget (1959) explained the decline of cognitive-evolutionary aspects, sciences, enlighten-

488 • OESTERDIEKHOFF

This content downloaded from 128.210.151.153 on Mon, 2 Dec 2013 10:25:02 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
ment, and humanism purely and totally, industrial- aborigines. In P. Dasen & J. W. Berry (Eds.), Culture
ism and democracy partially (Oesterdiekhoff, 2005, and cognition. Readings in cross-cultural psychology (pp.
381–408). London, England: Methuen.
2006b, 2007b, 2009a, 2011, 2012a, 2012b). Thus, it is
Dasen, P. (1977). Piagetian cross-cultural psychology. New
apparent that the rise of anthropological stages is the
York, NY: Gardner.
hand, whereas the five transformations, which took Dasen, P., & Berry, J. W. (Eds.). (1974). Culture and cognition.
place in roughly the same time span and in the same Readings in cross-cultural psychology. London, England:
world region, namely in the Western world between Methuen.
1650 and 1950, are only the five fingers of this hand. De Lacey, R. D. (1970). A cross-cultural study of classificatory
ability in Australia. In P. Dasen & J. Berry (Eds.), Culture
NOTE and cognition (pp. 353–366). London, England: Methuen.
Address correspondence about this article to Georg W. De Lemos, M. M. (1973). The development of conservation.
Oesterdiekhoff, Karlsruhe Institute of Technology, Institute In G. E. Kearney et al. (Eds.), The psychology of aborigi-
of Sociology, P.O. Box 6980, 76128 Karlsruhe, Germany (e- nal Australians (pp. 71–88). New York, NY: Wiley.
mail: Oesterdiekhoff@t-online.de). De Lemos, M. M. (1974). The development of spatial concepts
in Zulu children. In P. Dasen & J. Berry (Eds.), Culture and
REFERENCES cognition (pp. 367–380). London, England: Methuen.
Al-Fakhri, S. (1977). The development of the concept of De Vries, R. (1969). Constancy of generic identity in the years
speed among Iraqi children. In P. Dasen (Ed.), Piagetian three to six. Society for Research in Child Development
psychology (pp. 203–215). New York, NY: Gardner. Monographs, 34(4, no. 127), 1–67.
Ashton, P. T. (1975). Cross-cultural Piagetian research. An Durkheim, É. (1965). The elementary forms of religious life.
experimental perspective. Harvard Educational Review, New York, NY: Free Press.
45, 475–506. Eckensberger, L., et al. (Eds.). (1979). Cross-cultural contribu-
Barber, N. (2005). Educational and ecological correlates tions to psychology. Amsterdam, the Netherlands: Swets
of IQ: A cross-national investigation. Intelligence, 33, & Zeitlinger.
273–284. Ember, C. R. (1977). Cross-cultural cognitive studies. Annual
Berry, J. W. (1974). Radical cultural relativism and the concept Review of Anthropology, 6, 33–56.
of intelligence. In P. Dasen & J. Berry (Eds.), Culture and Evans, E. P. (1906). The criminal prosecution and capital
cognition (pp. 225–230). London, England: Methuen. punishment of the animals. London: Faber & Faber.
Bovet, M. C. (1974). Cognitive processes among illiterate chil- Evans-Pritchard, E. E. (1976). Witchcraft, oracles, and magic
dren and adults. In P. Dasen & J. Berry (Eds.), Culture among the Azande. Oxford, England: University Press.
and cognition (pp. 311–334). London, England: Methuen. Fetz, R. L. (1982). Naturdenken beim Kind und bei Aristo-
Bovet, P. (1951). Le sentiment religieux et la psychologie de teles. Grundfragen einer genetischen Ontologie [Aristo-
l’enfant [Religious sentiment and child psychology]. tle’s and the child’s reasoning about nature]. Tijdschrift
Paris: Delachaux & Niestlé. voor Filosofie, 44, 473–512.
Bühler, K. (1930). Die geistige Entwicklung des Kindes [The Fetz, R. L., Reich, K. H., & Valentin, P. (2001). Weltbildent-
mental development of the child]. Jena, Germany: Gustav wicklung und Schöpfungsverständnis. Eine strukturgene-
Fischer. tische Untersuchung bei Kindern und Jugendlichen [De-
Campbell, J. (1966). The masks of God. New York, NY: Pen- velopment of worldview and belief in creation]. Stuttgart,
guin. Germany: Kohlhammer Verlag.
Chapman, M. (1988). Constructive evolution: origins and de- Feuerbach, L. (1985). The essence of Christianity. New York,
velopment of Piaget’s thought. Cambridge, MA: University NY: Harper & Collins.
Press. Fischer, M. (2005). Tierstrafen und Tierprozesse [Punish-
Cole, M., & Scribner, S. (1974). Culture and thought. New ment and trials against animals]. Hamburg, Germany:
York, NY: Wiley. Lit-Verlag.
Dasen, P. (1973). Piagetian research in Central Australia. In Flavell, J. (1977). Cognitive development. Upper Saddle River,
G. E. Kearney et al. (Eds.), The psychology of aboriginal NJ: Prentice Hall.
Australians (pp. 89–96). New York, NY: Wiley. Flynn, J. (2007). What is intelligence? Cambridge, England:
Dasen, P. (1974a). Cross-cultural Piagetian research: A sum- Cambridge University Press.
mary. In P. Dasen & J. Berry (Eds.), Culture and cogni- Flynn, J. R. (2008). Lurija und das Paradox massiver IQ-
tion. Readings in cross-cultural psychology (pp. 409–424). Gewinne längs der Zeit [Lurija and the paradox of mas-
London, England: Methuen. sive IQ gains over time]. In G. W. Oesterdiekhoff & H.
Dasen, P. (1974b). The influence of ecology, culture and Eu- Rindermann (Eds.), Kultur und Kognition (pp. 111–132).
ropean contact on cognitive development in Australian Hamburg, Germany: Lit-Verlag.

RELEVANCE OF PIAGETIAN CROSS-CULTURAL PSYCHOLOGY • 489

This content downloaded from 128.210.151.153 on Mon, 2 Dec 2013 10:25:02 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
Fortune, R. F. (1963). Sorcerers of Dobu. New York, NY: Dut- Kelly, M. (1977). Papua New Guinea and Piaget: An eight year
ton. study. In P. Dasen (Ed.), Piagetian cross-cultural psychol-
Frazer, J. G. (1911/1922/1924). Belief in immortality and the ogy (pp. 169–202). New York, NY: Gardner.
worship of the dead (3 vols.). London, England: Macmil- Kohlberg, L. (1974). Studien zur kognitiven Entwicklung
lan. [Studies on cognitive growth]. Frankfurt am Main, Ger-
Freitag, B. (1983). Der Aufbau kindlicher Bewusstseinsstruk- many: Suhrkamp.
turen im gesellschaftlichen Kontext [Children’s reasoning Kohlberg, L., & Gilligan, C. (1971). The adolescent as a phi-
in its social context]. Munich, Germany: Fink Verlag. losopher: The discovery of the self in a postconventional
Gellathy, A. R. H. (1987). Acquisition of a concept of logical world. Daedalus, 100, 1051–1086.
necessity. Human Development, 30, 32–47. Langer, J. (1988). A note on the comparative psychology of
Goodnow, J., & Bethon, G. (1966). Piaget’s tasks: The effects mental development. In S. Strauss (Ed.), Ontogeny, phy-
of schooling and intelligence. Child Development, 37, logeny, and historical development (pp. 68–85). Norwood,
573–582. NJ: Ablex.
Greenfield, P. (1966). On culture and equivalence. In J. Brun- Larson, E. J., & Witham, L. (1998). Leading scientists still
er et al. (Eds.), Studies on cognitive growth (pp. 270–318). reject God. Nature, 394, 313.
New York, NY: Wiley. Laurendeau-Bendavid, M. (1977). Culture, schooling, and
Habermas, J. (1976). Zur Rekonstruktion des Historischen cognitive development: A comparative study of children
Materialismus [Reconstruction of historical materialism]. in French Canada and Rwanda. In P. Dasen (Ed.), Piaget-
Frankfurt am Main, Germany: Suhrkamp. ian psychology (pp. 123–168). New York, NY: Gardner.
Hallpike, C. (1979). Foundations of primitive thought. Ox- Lévy-Bruhl, L. (1923). Primitive mentality. New York, NY:
ford, England: Clarendon. Macmillan.
Hallpike, C. (2004). The evolution of moral understanding. Lévy-Bruhl, L. (1931). Le surnaturel et la nature dans la
London, England: Prometheus Research Group. mentalité primitive [The supernatural and nature in the
Havighurst, R., & Neugarten, B. (1955). American Indian primitive mentality]. Paris, France: Felix Alcan.
and White children. Chicago, IL: University Press. Lévy-Bruhl, L. (1938). L’expérience mystique et les symboles
Heron, A., & Simonsson, M. (1974). Weight conservation in chez les primitives [Mystical experience and symbols
Zambian children: a nonverbal approach. In P. Dasen & among aborigines]. Paris, France: Felix Alcan.
J. Berry (Eds.), Culture and cognition (pp. 335–350). Lon- Lévy-Bruhl, L. (1971). The “soul” of the primitive. Chicago,
don, England: Methuen. IL: Henri Regnery.
Ibarra, L. (2007). Creencias, mitos y rituales en el mundo Lévy-Bruhl, L. (1983). Primitive mythology. St. Lucia, Austra-
prehispánico. Una explicación desde la teoría histórico- lia: University of Queensland Press.
genética [Beliefs, myths, and rituals in the pre-Hispanic Lévy-Bruhl, L. (1985). How natives think. Princeton, NJ:
world. An explanation from historico-genetic theory]. Princeton University Press.
Guadalajara, Mexico: Universidad de Guadalajara. Lurija, A. R. (1982). Cognitive development. Its cultural and
Irvine, S. H., & Berry, J. W. (Eds.). (1988). Human abilities social foundations. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University
and cultural context (2 vols.). Cambridge, England: Cam- Press.
bridge University Press. Mair, L. P. (1969). Magie im schwarzen Erdteil [Magic on the
Jahoda, G. (1958a). Child animism: I. A critical survey of black continent]. Munich, Germany: Kindler Verlag.
cross-cultural research. Journal of Social Psychology, 47, Maistriaux, R. (1955). La sous-évolution des noirs d’Afrique.
197–212. Sa nature, ses causes, ses remèdes [The under-evolution
Jahoda, G. (1958b). Child animism: II. A study in West Af- of African Blacks. Its nature, causes, and remedies]. Revue
rica. Journal of Social Psychology, 47, 213–222. de Psychologie des Peuples, 10, 397–455.
Jahoda, G. (1974). Supernatural beliefs and changing cogni- Mogdil, S., & Mogdil, C. (1976). Piagetian research (Vol.
tive structures among Ghanaian university students. In 1–8). London, England: Penguin.
P. Dasen & J. Berry (Eds.), Culture and cognition (pp. Neisser, U. (Ed.). (1998). The rising curve. Long-term gains
141–158). London, England: Methuen. in IQ and related measures. Washington, DC: American
Kälble, K. (1997). Die Entwicklung der Kausalität im Kul- Psychological Association.
turvergleich [The development of causality across cul- Nurcombe, B. (1973). Precausal and paracausal thinking. In
tures]. Opladen, Germany: Westdeutscher Verlag. G. E. Kearney et al. (Eds.), The psychology of aboriginal
Kearney, G. E. (1973). Early psychological studies. In G. E. Australians (pp. 105–124). New York, NY: Wiley.
Kearney et al. (Eds.), The psychology of aboriginal Austra- Oesterdiekhoff, G. W. (1997). Kulturelle Bedingungen kog-
lians (pp. 16–26). New York, NY: Wiley. nitiver Entwicklung. Der strukturgenetische Ansatz in
Kearney, G. E., De Lacey, P. R., & Davidson, G. R. (1973). The der Soziologie [Cultural conditions of cognitive growth].
psychology of aboriginal Australians. New York, NY: Wiley. Frankfurt am Main, Germany: Suhrkamp.

490 • OESTERDIEKHOFF

This content downloaded from 128.210.151.153 on Mon, 2 Dec 2013 10:25:02 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
Oesterdiekhoff, G. W. (2000). Zivilisation und Strukturgen- theory of mind and rationality. Mankind Quarterly, 3(4),
ese. Norbert Elias und Jean Piaget im Vergleich [Civiliza- 346–380.
tion and the genesis of structures]. Frankfurt am Main, Oesterdiekhoff, G. W. (2011). The steps of man towards civi-
Germany: Suhrkamp. lization. The key to disclose the riddle of history. Norder-
Oesterdiekhoff, G. W. (2002). Das archaische Prozess- stedt, Germany: Bod.
und Beweisrecht und die “immanente Gerechtigkeit.” Oesterdiekhoff, G. W. (2012a). Die Entwicklung der Men-
Erklärung von Struktur, Entwicklung und Untergang schheit von der Kindheitsphase zur Erwachsenenreife [The
ordalförmiger Konfliktregelungen [The archaic trial and development of humankind from childhood to adult-
immanent justice]. Zeitschrift der Savigny-Stiftung für hood]. Wiesbaden, Germany: Springer Verlag.
Rechtsgeschichte: Germanistische Abteilung, 1, 175–192. Oesterdiekhoff, G. W. (2012b). Die geistige Entwicklung der
Oesterdiekhoff, G. W. (2005). Entwicklung der Weltgesell- Menschheit [The mental development of humankind].
schaft. Von der Steinzeit zur Moderne [The development Weilerswist, Germany: Velbrück.
of world society]. Hamburg, Germany: Lit-Verlag. Oesterdiekhoff, G. W. (in press). Traumzeit der Menschheit.
Oesterdiekhoff, G. W. (2006a). Irrationale Denk- und Ver- Wesen, Ursprung und Untergang der Religion [Dream-
haltensweisen am Beispiel von Hexerei und Magie und time of humankind].
ihr Stellenwert in der Kulturgeschichte der Menschheit Oesterdiekhoff, G. W., & Rindermann, H. (Eds.). (2008).
[Irrational patterns of reason and behavior exemplified by Kultur und Kognition [Culture and cognition]. Hamburg,
magic and witchcraft]. Sozialwissenschaftliches Journal, Germany: Lit-Verlag.
1(2), 79–104. Parsons, T., & E. Shils, A. (Eds.). (1961). Theories of society.
Oesterdiekhoff, G. W. (2006b). Kognitive Entwicklung und Glencoe, IL: Free Press.
sozialer Wandel. Zur Wechselwirkung von Psyche und Peluffo, N. (1962). Les notions de conservation et de causalité
Gesellschaft im Lichte der strukturgenetischen Soziologie chez les enfants provenant de différents milieux physiques
[Cognitive development and social change]. Sozialwissen- et socio-culturels. Archives de Psychologie, 38, 275–291.
schaftliches Journal, 1(1), 9–37. Peluffo, N. (1967). Culture and cognitive problems. Interna-
Oesterdiekhoff, G. W. (2006c). Kulturelle Evolution des tional Journal of Psychology, 2(3), 187–198.
Geistes. Die historische Wechselwirkung von Psyche und Philp, H., & Kelly, M. (1974). Product and process in cogni-
Gesellschaft [Cultural evolution of mind]. Hamburg, Ger- tive development. British Journal of Educational Psychol-
many: Lit-Verlag. ogy, 44, 248–265.
Oesterdiekhoff, G. W. (2007a). Ancient sun cults: Under- Piaget, J. (1932). The moral judgment of the child. New York,
standing religious rites in terms of developmental psy- NY: Free Press.
chology. Mankind Quarterly, 48(1), 99–116. Piaget, J. (1950). The psychology of intelligence. London, Eng-
Oesterdiekhoff, G. W. (2007b, April). Die Entstehung der land: Routledge & Kegan.
Industriemoderne in Europa. Der strukturgenetische Piaget, J. (1959). The child’s conception of the world. New
Erklärungsansatz [The rise of modern society in Europe]. York, NY: Littlefield, Adams.
Sozialwissenschaftliches Journal, 2(1), 9–35. Piaget, J. (1969). The child’s conception of physical causality.
Oesterdiekhoff, G. W., with Rindermann, H. (2007c, Sum- Totowa, NJ: Littlefield, Adams.
mer). The spread of AIDS in developing countries. A Piaget, J. (1974). Need and significance of cross-cultural stud-
psycho-cultural approach. Journal of Social, Political, ies in genetic psychology. In P. Dasen & J. Berry (Eds.),
and Economic Studies, 32(2), 201–222. Culture and cognition (pp. 299–310). London, England:
Oesterdiekhoff, G. W. (2008). Magic and animism in old Methuen.
religions. The relevance of sun cults to the world-view of Piaget, J. (1975a). Gesammelte Werke [Collected works] (10
traditional societies. Croatian Journal of Ethnology, 45(1), vols.). Stuttgart, Germany: Klett Verlag. (Vols. 8–10 origi-
43–66. nally published 1950)
Oesterdiekhoff, G. W. (2009a). The arena games in the Ro- Piaget, J. (1975b). The origin of the idea of chance in children.
man Empire. A contribution to the explanation of the New York, NY: W.W. Norton.
history of morals and humanity. Croatian Journal of Eth- Piaget, J. (1984). Theorien und Methoden der modernen Er-
nology, 46(1), 177–202. ziehung [Theories and methods of modern education].
Oesterdiekhoff, G. W. (2009b). Mental growth of humankind Frankfurt am Main, Germany: Fischer.
in history. Norderstedt, Germany: Bod. Piaget, J. (1996). In J.-C. Bringuier, Jean Piaget. Im Allge-
Oesterdiekhoff, G. W. (2009c). Psyche und Gesellschaft in der meinen werde ich falsch verstanden [Jean Piaget. Gener-
Entwicklung [Development of psyche and society]. Nor- ally I am wrongly understood]. Hamburg, Germany:
derstedt, Germany: Bod. Europäische Verlagsanstalt.
Oesterdiekhoff, G. W. (2009d, Spring/Summer). Trials Piaget, J., & Garcia, R. (1989). Psychogenesis and the history of
against animals. A contribution to the developmental sciences. New York, NY: Columbia University Press.

RELEVANCE OF PIAGETIAN CROSS-CULTURAL PSYCHOLOGY • 491

This content downloaded from 128.210.151.153 on Mon, 2 Dec 2013 10:25:02 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
Piaget, J., & Inhelder, B. (1941). Le développement des quan- Steinen, K. von den. (1894). Unter den Naturvölkern Zentral-
tités physiques chez l’enfant [Development of physical Brasiliens [Among the native tribes of central Brasil].
quantities among children]. Neuchâtel, Switzerland: Berlin, Germany: D. Reimer.
Delachaux & Niestle. Stern, W. (1924). Psychology of early childhood up to the sixth
Piaget, J., & Inhelder, B. (1958). The growth of logical think- year of age. New York, NY: Holt.
ing from childhood to adolescence. New York, NY: Basic Tuddenham, R. D. (1969). A Piagetian test of cognitive de-
Books. velopment. In W. B. Dockerell (Ed.), On intelligence (pp.
Piaget, J., & Inhelder, B. (1969). The psychology of the child. 49–70). Toronto, ON: Oise.
New York, NY: Basic Books. Tulviste, P. (1979). On the origins of theoretic syllogistic
Ponzo, E. (1966). Acculturazione e detribalizzazione [Social- reasoning in culture and the child. Quarterly Newsletter
ization and detribalization]. Rivista de Psicologia Sociale, of the Laboratory of Comparative Human Cognition, 1,
13, 41–107. 73–80.
Poortinga, Y. H. (1977). Basic problems in cross-cultural psy- Tylor, E. B. (1871). Primitive culture (2 vols.). London, Eng-
chology. Amsterdam, the Netherlands: Swets & Zeitlinger. land: J. Murray.
Porteus, S. D. (1937). Primitive intelligence. New York, NY: Vernon, P. E. (1969). Intelligence and cultural environment.
Macmillan. London, England: Methuen.
Radding, C. M. (1985). A world made by men. Cognition and Were, K. (1968). A survey of the thought processes of New
society 400–1200. Chapel Hill: University of North Caro- Guinean secondary students. Unpublished master’s thesis,
lina Press. University of Adelaide, Australia.
Ross, R. J. (1974). The empirical status of the formal opera- Werner, E. E. (1979). Cross-cultural child psychology. Mon-
tions. Adolescence, 9, 413–424. terey, CA: Brooks/Cole.
Schöfthaler, T., & Goldschmidt, D. (Eds.). (1984). Soziale Werner, H. (1948). Comparative psychology of mental develop-
Struktur und Vernunft [Social structure and reason]. ment. New York, NY: Follet.
Frankfurt am Main, Germany: Suhrkamp. Wundt, W. (1914). Völkerpsychologie. Eine Untersuchung der
Schröder, E. (1989). Vom konkreten zum formalen Denken Entwicklungsgesetze von Sprache, Mythus und Sitte [Psy-
[From concrete to formal thought]. Bern, Switzerland: chology of peoples]. Vol. 5: Mythus und Religion [Myth
Huber. and religion] (2nd ed.). Leipzig, Germany: Kröner Verlag.
Segall, M. H., Dasen, P. R., Berry, J. W., & Poortinga, Y. H. Zeininger, W. (1929). Magische Geisteshaltung im Kindesalter
(1990). Human behavior in global perspective. Boston, und ihre Bedeutung für die religiöse Entwicklung [Magic
MA: Allyn & Bacon. and religion of children]. Leipzig, Germany: Barth Verlag.

492 • OESTERDIEKHOFF

This content downloaded from 128.210.151.153 on Mon, 2 Dec 2013 10:25:02 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
View publication stats

Você também pode gostar