Você está na página 1de 7

Brain Research 1004 (2004) 45 – 51

www.elsevier.com/locate/brainres

Research report
Sleep deprivation induced by the modified multiple platform technique:
quantification of sleep loss and recovery
Ricardo Borges Machado, Débora C. Hipólide *, Ana Amélia Benedito-Silva, Sergio Tufik
Psychobiology Department, Universidade Federal de São Paulo (UNIFESP), Rua Napoleão de Barros 925, São Paulo, SP, 04024-002, Brazil
Accepted 14 January 2004

Abstract

Vigilance status was continually monitored in socially stable groups of rats exposed to the modified multiple platform (MMP) technique
for sleep deprivation. For comparison, sleep parameters were also monitored in socially isolated rats deprived of sleep by the single platform
(SP) method. In all cases, sleep was continuously recorded during baseline, during 96 h of sleep deprivation and during 4 days of recovery.
Both multiple- and single-platform techniques completely abolished paradoxical sleep (PS) during the deprivation period, but also resulted in
significant decreases in slow wave sleep (SWS) ( 31% and 37%, respectively). Unexpectedly, animals on large platforms, which are
normally intended as controls, also showed significant reductions in PS and SWS, and these effects were more pronounced in rats deprived in
groups than in animals deprived in isolation. Another control preparation, rats placed on wire-mesh grids in the deprivation tank, also showed
PS reduction ( 39%) but no loss of SWS during the 4 test days. Paradoxical sleep rebound was observed in the first 24 h in all groups,
except for grid controls. Overall, no significant differences were found between single- and multiple-platform procedures during the 4 days of
deprivation. However, sleep rebound was more pronounced in MMP-deprived rats than in SP-deprived rats. Sleep loss in both control groups
may reflect residual effect of stress that remain in the platform technique. These findings indicate that the MMP technique is effective in
inducing PS deprivation (PSD). However, the fact that SWS is also affected may have implications for conclusions on paradoxical sleep
function based upon paradoxical sleep deprivation.
D 2004 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.

Theme: Neural basis of behavior


Topic: Biological rhythms and sleep

Keywords: Sleep deprivation; Paradoxical sleep; Slow wave sleep; Platform technique; Recording

1. Introduction platform into the water and awaken. Because platform


techniques do not require complex instrumentation, this
Sleep loss or fragmentation is a feature of a number of basic method has been widely used in PSD studies.
sleep disorders and other pathological conditions in humans. It is well known, however, that PSD experiments are
Historically, considerable research interest has focused on affected by a number of potentially confounding variables,
the selective effects of paradoxical sleep (PS) loss. Exper- including stress associated with isolation, movement restric-
imentally, most of the available techniques for PS depriva- tion, wetness and muscle fatigue [5,9,14,27]. In addition, a
tion (PSD) take advantage of the atonia that occurs during significant loss of slow wave sleep (SWS) may also occur
this phase of sleep, following the basic technique introduced [10,19]. In order to reduce movement restriction associate
by Jouvet in 1964 for cats [11], and later adapted for rats by with the original platform technique, Van Hulzen and
Cohen and Dement [6]. In these procedures, animals are Coenen [33] introduced a multiple platform procedure where
placed on top of a small platform that is surrounded by one rat is deprived in a large tank containing a number of
water. As animals enter the paradoxical phase of sleep, they platforms. However, significant indices of stress were still
lose postural tone (atonia) and partially or fully slip from the seen with this procedure [5]. The technique was subsequent-
ly modified by placing several rats in large tanks containing
* Corresponding author. Tel.: +55-11-5539-0155; fax: +55-11-5572-
a number of platforms, with the aim of removing social
5092. isolation and restraint factors [20]. However, Suchecki et al.
E-mail address: hipolide@psicobio.epm.br (D.C. Hipólide). [27] demonstrated that rats subjected to this procedure

0006-8993/$ - see front matter D 2004 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.brainres.2004.01.019
46 R.B. Machado et al. / Brain Research 1004 (2004) 45–51

showed augmented ACTH and corticosterone responses and Japan) at a sampling rate of 200 Hz. Recordings were
that this response was attributed to social instability. More visually scored on 30-s epochs according to classical
recently, it has been shown that animals raised in groups and sleep-waking criteria as reflecting either (i) the waking state
sleep-deprived along with their peers in a large tank water (W), which is characterized by low-voltage, fast EEG
exhibit attenuated stress indices compared to animals that cortical and high EMG activity; (ii) slow wave sleep
were sleep-deprived as part of a socially unstable group [26]. (SWS), characterized by continuous high amplitude, slow
A further modification consisted of exposing controls to the EEG activity and low EMG activity; or (iii) paradoxical
same apparatus except that a wire mesh was placed over the (PS), characterized by high EEG cortical activity and
platforms to prevent any contact with the water. The simul- absence of EMG activity [30]. Each 30-s epoch was defined
taneous deprivation of a socially stable group and the wire- by the wave pattern (W, SWS or PS) which occupied more
mesh control group define what has been referred to as the than 50% of the epoch. When artifacts or noise did not allow
modified multiple platform (MMP) technique. characterization of an epoch, assessments were made based
Although both single- [19] and multiple-platform [34] on immediately surrounding epochs.
techniques have been electrophysiologically evaluated, Animals were habituated to the recording system for 3
there has been no direct assessment of changes in vigilance days before a 24-h baseline recording was performed. Base-
state in rats subjected to the modified multiple platform line recordings were performed in group cages (N = 5) for
procedure. The aim of the present study was therefore to animals subjected to the MMP method (Experiment 1) or in
quantify the nature and extent of sleep changes induced by individual home cages (Experiment 2). Following baseline
the modified multiple platform technique and proposed recording, animals were adapted to the sleep deprivation
control procedures, by continuous recording during 4 days procedure for 30 min on 3 consecutive days. Sleep parameters
of sleep deprivation and recovery. For comparative purpo- were obtained in minutes and then converted to percentage of
ses, we also evaluated sleep changes in rats deprived of total recording time (usually 23 h) for each day.
sleep by the conventional single platform (SP) technique.
2.2.1. Experiment 1: modified multiple platform method
Rats were tested in socially stable groups of five in
2. Materials and methods separate water tanks (123  44  44 cm) containing either
18 round platforms of 6.5-cm-diameter (small platforms) or
2.1. Animals 18 14-cm-diameter platforms (large platforms). The ‘‘large
platform’’ group is often used as a control in sleep depri-
Male Wistar rats (300 – 400 g) from our colony were used vation experiments. An additional control group was placed
at present study. The experiments were approved by the in a third tank on a stainless steel wire mesh (2.3-mm
Ethical Research Committee of the Universidade Federal de openings), which allowed rats to lie down without touching
São Paulo and followed international guidelines for the care the water. This is referred to as the grid control group. The
of research animals. Rats were housed and maintained under tanks were filled with water to a level 1 cm below the
a 12-h light– dark cycle with lights on at 7:00 AM. Room surface of the platforms or the grid. Continuous electro-
temperature was kept at 20 F 2 jC and animals had free physiological monitoring was performed in one rat in each
access to water and food at all times. Animals defined as group of five. After 96 h of sleep deprivation, rats were
‘‘socially stable’’ were housed in groups of five. Isolated returned to the home cage, where recording continued for 4
animals were individually housed in hanging wire-mesh additional days. Ten deprivation runs were conducted, with
cages. different animals, to achieve a final N of 10 recorded
Under ketamine – diazepam anesthesia, rats were imp- animals per group.
lanted with two ipsilateral stainless steel screws for electro-
encephalogram (EEG) monitoring, one overlaying the right 2.2.2. Experiment 2: single platform method
lateral fronto-parietal area and the left medial parietal cortex The single platform procedure consisted of placing an
[30]. One additional pair of nickel – chromium fine wire animal on a single small (6.5-cm diameter) platform or a
electrodes was implanted in the dorsal neck muscle for large (14-cm diameter) platform in an enclosed tank
electromyogram (EMG) recording. The electrodes were sol- (23  23  35 cm). As in the group procedure, the platform
dered to a connector, which was fixed to the animal cranium was surrounded by water to about 1 cm below platform
with acrylic dental cement. After surgery, penicillin and surface. After 96 h of sleep deprivation, rats were returned
diclofenac were administered and 15 days were allowed for to the home cage, where recording continued for 4 addi-
recovery. tional days. Twelve animals were tested in each group.

2.2. Sleep recording 2.3. Statistical analyses

Electrophysiological signals were recorded on a digital In all experiments, sleep parameters were obtained in
polygraph (Neurofax QP 223 A nNihon Kohden, Tokyo, minutes and then converted to percentage of total recording
R.B. Machado et al. / Brain Research 1004 (2004) 45–51 47

Table 1
Sleep parameters in rats subjected to the modified multiple platform procedurea
B D1 D2 D3 D4 R1 R2 R3 R4
SP
W 45.2 F 6.4 67.5 F 9.6* 70.3 F 7.7* 71.3 F 8.4* 72.5 F 9.2* 35.5 F 7.8* 48.0 F 9.4 47.2 F 7.7 49.7 F 4.4
SWS 43.8 F 5.3 32.5 F 9.6* 29.6 F 7.7* 28.3 F 8.4* 27.5 F 9.2* 36.1 F 6.1* 38.0 F 7.1 40.3 F 6.3 39.0 F 4.2
PS 11.0 F 3.3 0.0 F 0.0* 0.0 F 0.0* 0.0 F 0.0* 0.0 F 0.0* 28.4 F 3.3* 14.0 F 4.3 12.4 F 2.6 11.3 F 2.3

LP
W 50.2 F 6.1 69.2 F 5.2* 66.9 F 7.6* 66.2 F 8.0* 71.4 F 6.2* 41.0 F 10.7* 50.0 F 9.5 50.7 F 9.8 52.7 F 5.5
SWS 41.2 F 4.4 29.7 F 4.6* 31.2 F 6.9* 31.1 F 5.7* 27.0 F 5.5* 39.2 F 7.1 40.7 F 7.3 40.1 F 7.4 38.0 F 3.4
PS 8.6 F 2.6 1.2 F 1.3* 1.9 F 1.3* 2.7 F 2.9* 1.5 F 1.3* 19.7 F 5.4* 9.3 F 3.4 9.2 F 3.4 9.3 F 2.9

Grid controls
W 48.1 F 5.7 56.7 F 7.6 54.4 F 4.5 58.8 F 6.4* 57.4 F 4.2* 50.2 F 9.4 55.0 F 6.4 55.4 F 5.3 57.6 F 9.3
SWS 41.3 F 5.0 37.5 F 5.9 38.8 F 3.5 35.2 F 5.5 36.2 F 3.4 37.6 F 5.9 36.7 F 5.3 35.4 F 3.9 34.3 F 6.7
PS 10.7 F 2.0 5.8 F 3.3* 6.8 F 2.0* 6.0 F 1.6* 6.3 F 1.7* 12.2 F 4.8 8.4 F 1.8 9.1 F 2.1 8.0 F 3.1
a
Values are percentage of time (mean F S.D.) spent in wakefulness (W), slow wave sleep (SWS) and paradoxical sleep (PS), during baseline (B),
deprivation days (D) and recovery days (R). SP = small platform; LP = Large platform. Ten animals were tested in each group. Differences from basal level are
indicated by *P < 0.05.

time (usually 23 h) for each day. Separate two-way repeated wakefulness as compared to the baseline throughout the 4
measures ANOVAs were conducted using Group (Small days of deprivation ( P < 0.001). The grid control group
Platform, Large Platform or Control) as a between-subject showed small but significant increases in wakefulness on
factor and Days (Baseline, day 1– 4) as a within subject days 3 and 4 compared to baseline ( P < 0.005). No
factor. Post-hoc pairwise comparisons were performed using differences were detected between groups at baseline
Tukey’s HSD tests. ( P >0.05).
Slow wave sleep: Significant main effects of Group ( F
(2,27) = 5.6, P < 0.01) and Days ( F(4,108) = 21.7, P < 0.001)
3. Results were detected, in addition to a significant Group  Days
interaction ( F(8,108) = 2.5, P < 0.05). Percent of time spent
3.1. Experiment 1: modified multiple platform in SWS significantly decreased in both small and large
platform groups on all 4 days of sleep deprivation compared
Results are summarized in Table 1. Fig. 1 shows a to baseline ( P < 0.05). Comparisons between baseline and grid
representative hypnogram illustrating changes in waking, controls showed no difference in SWS percentage at any point
PS and SWS in a rat (small platform group) during baseline ( P >0.05). At baseline, no differences were detected between
(A), on the fourth day of sleep deprivation (B) and on the groups ( P >0.05).
first day of sleep recovery (C). Paradoxical sleep: As shown in Table 1, PS was com-
pletely absent in the small platform group on each of the 4
3.1.1. Changes during the sleep deprivation period deprivation days. Significant Group ( F(1,18) = 28.6, P <
Wakefulness: Significant ANOVA effects were obtained 0.001), Days ( F(4,72) = 39.3, P < 0.001) and interaction
for Group ( F(2,27) = 13.9, P < 0. 001), Days ( F(4,108) = ( F(4,72) = 3.5, P < 0.05) effects were detected. Post-hoc
59.4, P < 0. 001), and the Group  Day interaction ( F tests indicated that both the large platform and grid groups
(8,108) = 5.3, P < 0.001). Tukey HSD tests revealed that had reduced PS compared to baseline throughout the 4 days
both small and large platform groups exhibited increased of sleep deprivation ( P < 0. 001 and P < 0.005, respective-

Fig. 1. Hypnogram showing sleep – wake states in a representative rat (small platform group) in modified multiple platform condition during baseline (A), on
the fourth day of sleep deprivation (B) and on the first day of sleep recovery (C). W—awake; PS—paradoxical sleep; SWS—slow wave sleep.
48 R.B. Machado et al. / Brain Research 1004 (2004) 45–51

ly). No differences were detected between groups at base- platform group on Day 1 ( P < 0.001) of sleep deprivation
line ( P >0.05). compared to the baseline. No differences were detected
between groups at baseline ( P >0.05).
3.1.2. Sleep recovery period Paradoxical sleep: Paradoxical sleep was completely
Wakefulness: Significant effects were detected for Group eliminated in the small platform group. There was a signif-
( F(2,27) = 5.0, P < 0.05) and Days ( F(4,108) = 15.1, P < icant effect of days of sleep deprivation ( F(4,44) = 50.8,
0.001), but not for the Group  Days interaction ( F(8,108) P < 0.001). Compared to baseline, PS percentage was de-
= 1.7, P = 0.10). Compared to baseline, both small and large creased in the large platform group on each of the 4 sleep
platform groups showed decreased wakefulness on Day 1 of deprivation days ( P < 0.001). No differences were detected
recovery ( P < 0.05). between groups at baseline ( P >0.05).
Slow wave sleep: There was no significant Group effect
( F(4,108) = 1.0, P = 0.39), a significant Days effect ( F 3.2.2. Sleep recovery period
(4,108) = 4.5, P < 0.005) and no Group  Days interaction Wakefulness: There was no significant main effect for
( F(8,108) = 1.5, P = 0.17). Post-hoc tests indicated that SWS Groups, ( F(1,22) = 2.1, P = 0.16), a significant Days effect
percentage was decreased in the small platform group on ( F(4,88) = 8.7, P < 0.001) and no interaction ( F(4,88) = 0.4,
Day 1 ( P < 0.05) compared to baseline. P = 0.79). Pairwise comparisons revealed that wakefulness
Paradoxical sleep: There was a significant Group effect, did not change in small platform at any point during the
( F(2,27) = 18.9, P < 0.001), a significant Days effect recovery period; in the large platform group wakefulness
( F(4,108) = 59.1, P < 0.001) and a significant Group  Days decreased on Day 1 ( P < 0.05). No differences were de-
Days interaction ( F(8,108) = 10.1, P < 0.001). For both tected between groups at baseline ( P >0.05).
small and large platform groups the percentage of time Slow-wave sleep: The main effect of Groups was signif-
spent in PS was higher as compared to the baseline only in icant ( F(1,22) = 6.8, P < 0.05), but not the Days ( F(4,88)
the first 24 h of the sleep recovery period ( P < 0.001). = 0.1, P = 0.96) or interaction ( F(4,88) = 1.9, P = 0.12)
effects. Pairwise analyses confirmed that SWS percentage
3.2. Experiment 2: single platform method did not change at any point during the 4 days.
Paradoxical sleep: The ANOVA revealed no significant
3.2.1. Sleep deprivation period Group effect ( F(1,22) = 0.8, P = 0.38), a main effect of Days
Wakefulness: There was a significant effect of Group, ( F(4,88) = 50.5, P < 0.001) and a significant Group  Days
( F(1,22) = 46.6, P < 0.001), a significant Days effect interaction ( F(4,88) = 2.5, P < 0.05). Pairwise tests indicated
( F(4,88) = 69.8, P < 0.001) and a significant Group Days that in both small and large platform groups, PS percentage
interaction ( F(4,88) = 30.0, P < 0.001). Post-hoc tests con- was higher as compared to baseline only in the first 24 h of
firmed that wakefulness increased in both small and large sleep recovery period ( P < 0.001) (Table 2).
platform groups on all days ( P < 0.005). No differences
were detected between groups at baseline ( P >0.05). 3.3. Comparisons between multiple and single platform
Slow-wave sleep: Analyses of variance revealed a signif- effects
icant effects for Groups, ( F(1,22) = 29.7, P < 0.001), Days
( F(4,88) = 24.8, P < 0.001) and the Group  Days interac- As illustrated in Fig. 2A, the multiple and single
tion ( F(4,88) = 11.8, P < 0.001). Pairwise comparisons indi- platform procedures resulted in similar levels of SWS loss
cated that SWS percentage was decreased in the small across the 4 deprivation days. During recovery, the MMP
platform group on all days ( P < 0.001) and in the large procedure resulted in slightly higher rebound levels

Table 2
Sleep parameters in rats subjected to the single-platform procedurea
B D1 D2 D3 D4 R1 R2 R3 R4
SP
W 48.5 F 4.7 73.6 F 6.7* 71.1 F 7.6* 75.1 F 5.8* 78.7 F 7.3* 40.2 F 5.8 48.3 F 7.0 46.7 F 6.7 48.2 F 4.9
SWS 40.5 F 4.6 26.4 F 6.8* 28.9 F 7.6* 24.9 F 5.8* 21.3 F 7.3* 37.7 F 4.9 39.2 F 6.7 41.7 F 6.2 41.3 F 3.7
PS 11.0 F 1.9 0.0 F 0.0 0.0 F 0.0 0.0 F 0.0 0.0 F 0.0 22.1 F 5.3* 12.5 F 2.2 11.7 F 2.5 10.5 F 2.7

LP
W 45.5 F 7.1 68.5 F 9.7* 57.5 F 5.9* 55.4 F 5.5* 56.3 F 5.0* 35.9 F 9.7* 44.5 F 7.9 45.9 F 4.3 47.8 F 10.8
SWS 42.5 F 5.9 29.8 F 9.1* 39.0 F 4.9 40.5 F 4.9 39.0 F 4.1 45.8 F 5.8 43.3 F 6.3 43.0 F 2.8 42.6 F 7.9
PS 12.0 F 3.3 1.6 F 1.4* 3.5 F 1.4* 4.1 F 1.5* 4.7 F 1.6* 18.2 F 5.4* 12.1 F 3.2 11.1 F 3.0 9.6 F 4.1
a
Values are percentage of time (mean F S.D.) spent in wakefulness (W), slow wave sleep (SWS) and paradoxical sleep (PS), during baseline (B),
deprivation days (D) and recovery days (R). SP = small platform; LP = Large platform. Twelve animals were tested in each group. Differences from basal level
are indicated by *P < 0.05.
R.B. Machado et al. / Brain Research 1004 (2004) 45–51 49

on larger diameter platforms in the MMP procedure also


showed significant reductions in both PS ( 77%) and SWS
( 26%) throughout the 4 days of sleep deprivation.
Previous analyses of sleep recording using platform
techniques with isolated animals have produced conflicting
results with respect to effects on SWS. Our findings are
consistent with some studies that have reported that platform
procedures suppress all phases of sleep [10]. On the other
hand, some previous electrophysiological examinations
found that platform techniques affect SWS only in the first
24 h [16,19]. In contrast to these studies, Porkka-Heiskanen
et al. [22] reported selective PS suppression in small
platform groups, with no significant variation in SWS
percentage for either small or large platform groups.
The reasons for significant SWS reductions in our study
are not clear, but it is possible that social interaction among
animals plays a role. Informal observation indicated that it
was not uncommon for two animals to share the same
platform at different points and animals constantly awak-
ened each other in the tank. It is noted that animals
subjected to the single platform technique, which were
tested in isolation, also showed SWS losses. However,
animals on large platforms in the single platform setup
Fig. 2. Mean percentage changes in slow-wave (A) and paradoxical sleep
(B) in rats subjected to multiple or single platform procedures. Each bar is a
showed SWS reductions only in the first 24 h of sleep
mean of 4 days of deprivation or first day of recovery. #P = 0.057; deprivation ( 29%), whereas their counterparts subjected
*P < 0.05. to the MMP method showed SWS reductions throughout the
4 days of sleep deprivation.
( 11% vs. 0.2%, P = 0.058). When only the first day of It is also conceivable that the surface diameter of the
recovery was considered, a very similar pattern was ob- large platforms used in this study were not ideal to fully
served ( 17% vs. 6%, P = 0.057). accommodate the body in a state of relaxation (SWS
Both procedures resulted in total PS abolition during involves a substantial degree of muscular relaxation). In-
the deprivation period (Fig. 2B). During recovery, there deed, animals placed on the grid, which allowed complete
was trend toward higher rebound levels in the multiple freedom to lie down, showed PS reduction ( 39%) but no
platform procedure (68% vs. 31%) although this differ- loss of SWS during the 4 test days.
ence did not reach statistical significance ( P = 0.13). On Our results suggest that it is appropriate to refer to MMP-
the first day of recovery, the average level of PS was exposed animals as being sleep deprived rather than being
significantly higher in the MMP group than in the SP exclusively deprived of paradoxical sleep. However, while
group, as illustrated in Fig. 2B ( + 187% vs. + 104%, SWS was clearly affected during the deprivation period, it
P = 0.047). did not rebound to levels higher than baseline, suggesting
the possibility that the loss during deprivation may not have
been of sufficient physiological significance to engage
4. Discussion compensatory mechanisms.

The present study provides the first direct quantification 4.2. Sleep loss in control animals
of changes in vigilance state in rats subjected to sleep
deprivation using the modified multiple platform (MMP) The present results also suggest that large platforms are
technique. The continuous recording approach also made it not an adequate environmental control for the MMP tech-
possible for the first time to evaluate the grid procedure as a nique, as animals in this group also show significant sleep
control for the MMP method. losses. Sleep loss was also seen in the grid control group.
These may be residual effects of stress that remains in these
4.1. Sleep deprivation period procedures. In fact, it has been demonstrated that cortico-
sterone levels are elevated over basal levels in grid control
Although the multiple platform procedure was effective animals, although to a lesser degree than in small platform
in producing a total suppression of paradoxical sleep, animals [26].
significant SWS loss also occurred throughout the 4 days A great variety of stressful situations are known to
of sleep deprivation ( 31%). In addition, animals placed decrease sleep duration and efficiency [4,12]. Many of these
50 R.B. Machado et al. / Brain Research 1004 (2004) 45–51

studies have reported decreases in active waking, deep rebound of high-amplitude SWS [24]. Palma et al. [21]
sleep, and paradoxical sleep. In contrast, we observed a demonstrated significant PS and SWS rebound ( + 16.9%
selective PS reduction in the grid control group. Further, the and + 9.4%, respectively) after 18 h of sleep deprivation
PS loss observed in this group was not followed by PS sleep induced by the MMP method. On the other hand, Suchecki
rebound, suggesting that the PS reduction in these animals et al. [28] found that 90 h of sleep deprivation produced PS
was not severe enough to engage compensatory effects. rebound ( + 184.7%) accompanied by a reduction in SWS
There are other instances in the literature where control rats time ( 12.2%)—the same pattern as observed in the
were reported to suffer a partial loss of paradoxical sleep present study using 96 h of sleep deprivation ( + 187%
during the deprivation period, but did not show significantly and 17% for PS and SWS, respectively).
greater PS during recovery, as compared to baseline levels
[32]. 4.4. Rebound differences between SP and MMP methods

4.3. Sleep rebound period A surprising observation was that PS rebound was higher
in MMP- than in SP-deprived rats. The reasons for this are
With respect to the recovery period, the present results not clear. Sleep rebound could in theory be affected by stress
show that 24 h was enough for a complete recovery of PS in factors associated with deprivation procedures, as several
MMP and well as in SP groups. Paradoxical sleep rebound studies have documented an increased need for sleep im-
after sleep deprivation by the platform method has been mediately after a stressful experience. Stress per se can
observed in several previous studies [10,16,19,21,28,34]. produce alterations in sleep patterns and these alterations
However, some of these studies also reported SWS rebound seem to depend on stress modality, duration and intensity
[21,34]. [1 – 4,12,13,17,18,21,23]. By eliminating social isolation
The lack of SWS rebound in the present study for both and movement restriction, the MMP procedure was ex-
SP and MMP techniques may be due to several factors. One pected to be less stressful than the SP procedure and hence
possibility, as noted earlier, is simply that the SWS reduc- cause less PS rebound. However, animals deprived by the
tion during deprivation period was not severe enough to SP or by MMP technique show the similar corticosterone
engage compensatory controls and cause rebound. Alterna- levels [29], which suggests that differential stress effects
tively, SWS rebound could have been offset by the large cannot account for the differences in sleep rebound ob-
increase in PS during the recovery period, i.e. PS rebound served for the two procedures.
would predominate over SWS rebound [7,8,31]. The issue
of sleep stage priorities during recovery has been extensive- 4.5. Use of rebound as an index of deprivation
ly discussed in a recent review [24]. Our results are in
accordance with several studies that used total or selective Recent work from our laboratory has focused on sleep
and different sleep deprivation procedures. Endo et al. [7], rebound parameters as an index of the effectiveness PS-
using several selective protocols for sleep deprivation, specificity of the MMP procedure. In addition, comparisons
showed that slow wave or delta activity, which is charac- between baseline and grid group during 6 h of sleep rebound
teristic from SWS, was inhibited by PS increase during the after 90 h of sleep deprivation revealed no sleep parameters
recovery period. differences [28]. Although sleep rebound patterns in these
It is also conceivable that finer analyses might unearth studies were similar to the pattern observed in the present
differences within SWS with respect to rebound effects. study, our results suggest that analyses of sleep rebound in
Two phases can be distinguished within SWS, one sleep deprivation studies do not necessarily indicate that
characterized by low to medium voltage EEG and another only paradoxical sleep was suppressed. Moreover, the
characterized by high-amplitude slow-wave (HASW) EEG absence of PS rebound in the grid control group does not
activity (for review, see Ref. [25]). Many reports have serve as evidence that PS deprivation did not occur. The
described SWS rebound in terms of augmented HASW present result shows that the rats placed on a grid over the
only, both in humans [15] and rats [7] (for review, see water tank floor may not serve as adequate environmental
Ref. [24]). We did not score SWS phases separately in controls for the MMP method as far as sleep loss-related
the present study, and this may conceivably have masked variables are considered.
a selective rebound effect on one of these two SWS
subphases. 4.6. Conclusions
Another point that needs to be considered is the length of
sleep deprivation. It seems that rebound from 1 day of sleep In summary, the present study shows that the MMP
deprivation produces subsequent rebound of slow-wave or method of sleep deprivation is efficient in producing total
high-amplitude EEG activity in SWS, usually accompanied but not exclusive PS suppression, since SWS is also
by substantial PS rebound (for review, see Ref. [24]). In compromised. However, the absence of SWS rebound
contrast, chronic sleep deprivation by the disk-over-water suggests that caution should be used in assessing the
method produces long-lasting rebound of PS with no significance of SWS loss during deprivation. With respect
R.B. Machado et al. / Brain Research 1004 (2004) 45–51 51

to controls, we observed that both control procedures of chronic stress on sleep in rats, Physiol. Behav. 57 (1995)
359 – 365.
induce at least partial paradoxical sleep deprivation. The
[13] M. Koehl, J.J. Bouyer, M. Darnaudery, M. Le Moal, W. Mayo, The
present results also showed that 24 h was enough to effect of restraint stress on paradoxical sleep is influenced by the
recover PS loss in all groups, except for grid controls circadian cycle, Brain Res. 937 (2002) 45 – 50.
which PS deprivation observed was not sufficient to [14] V.M. Kovalzon, V.L. Tsibulsky, REM-sleep deprivation, stress and
induce sleep rebound. While these techniques remain emotional behavior in rats, Behav. Brain Res. 14 (1984) 235 – 245.
useful to model the loss or fragmentation of sleep that [15] F. Lucidi, A. Devoto, C. Violani, P. Mastracci, M. Bertini, Effects of
different sleep duration on delta sleep in recovery nights, Psychophy-
occurs in a number of sleep disorders and in many human siology 34 (1997) 227 – 233.
conditions, their usefulness as tools to generate inferences [16] K.J. Maloney, L. Mainville, B.E. Jones, Differential C-Fos expression
about the specific contribution of selective sleep phases in in cholinergic, monoaminergic, and GABAergic cell groups of the
various conditions may be limited by the lack of specific- pontomesencephalic tegmentum after PS deprivation and recovery,
ity that is attendant to the procedures. J. Neurosci. 19 (1999) 3057 – 3072.
[17] S. Marinesco, C. Bonnet, R. Cespuglio, Influence of stress duration
on the sleep rebound induced by immobilization in the rat: a possible
role for corticosterone, Neuroscience 92 (1999) 921 – 933.
Acknowledgements [18] P. Meerlo, B.J. Pragt, S. Daan, Social stress induces high intensity
sleep in rats, Neurosci. Lett. 225 (1997) 41 – 44.
The authors would like to tank Dr. José N. Nobrega and [19] W.B. Mendelson, R.D. Guthrie, G. Frederick, R.J. Wyatt, The flower
pot technique of rapid eye movement (REM) sleep deprivation, Phar-
Beatriz D. Palma for their revision and comments on the
macol. Biochem. Behav. 2 (1974) 553 – 556.
manuscript. R.B. Machado was fellowship from CAPES [20] G.P. Nunes Jr., S. Tufik, Validation of the modified multiple platform
and D.C. Hipólide is fellowship from CNPq, both from method (MMP) of paradoxical sleep deprivation in rats, Sleep Res. 23
Brazil government. This work was supported by Associacßão (1994) 419.
Fundo de Incentivo a Psicofarmacologia-AFIP. [21] B.D. Palma, D. Suchecki, S. Tufik, Differential effects of acute cold
and footshock on the sleep of rats, Brain Res. 861 (2000) 97 – 104.
[22] T. Porkka-Heiskanen, S.E. Smith, T. Taira, J.H. Urban, J.E. Levine,
F.W. Turek, D. Stenberg, Noradrenergic activity in rat brain during
References rapid eye movement sleep deprivation and rebound sleep, Am. J.
Physiol. 268 (1995) R1456 – R1463.
[1] J. Adrien, C. Dugovic, P. Martin, Sleep – wakefulness patterns in the [23] C. Rampin, R. Cespuglio, N. Chastrette, M. Jouvet, Immobilization
helpless-rat, Physiol. Behav. 49 (1991) 257 – 262. stress induces a paradoxical rebound in rat, Neurosci. Lett. 126 (1991)
[2] J.J. Bouyer, J.M. Deminiere, W. Mayo, M. Le Moal, Inter-individual 113 – 118.
differences in the effects of acute stress on the sleep – wakefulness [24] A. Rechtschaffen, B.M. Bergmann, M.A. Gilliland, K. Bauer, Effects
cycle in rat, Neurosci. Lett. 225 (1997) 193 – 196. of method, duration, and sleep stage on rebounds from sleep depri-
[3] R. Cespuglio, S. Marinesco, B. Baubet, C. Bonnet, B. El Kafi, Evi- vation in the rat, Sleep 22 (1999) 11 – 31.
dence for a sleep-promoting influence of stress, Adv. Neuroimmunol. [25] C. Robert, C. Guilpin, A. Limoge, Automated sleep staging systems
5 (1995) 145 – 154. in rats, J. Neurosci. Methods 88 (1999) 111 – 122.
[4] S.G. Cheeta, J. Ruigt, P. Vanproosdij, P. Willner, Changes in sleep [26] D. Suchecki, S. Tufik, Social stability attenuates the stress in the
architecture following chronic mild stress, Biol. Psychiatry 41 (1997) modified multiple platform method for PS deprivation in the rat,
419 – 427. Physiol. Behav. 68 (2000) 309 – 316.
[5] A.M.L. Coenen, E.L.J.M. Van Luijtellar, Stress induced by three pro- [27] D. Suchecki, L.L. Lobo, D.C. Hipólide, S. Tufik, Increased ACTH
cedures of deprivation of PS, Physiol. Behav. 35 (1985) 501 – 504. and corticosterone secretion induced by different methods of PS dep-
[6] H.B. Cohen, W.C. Dement, Sleep: changes in threshold to electrocon- rivation, J. Sleep Res. 7 (1998) 276 – 281.
vulsive shock in rats after deprivation of paradoxical phase, Science [28] D. Suchecki, B.D. Palma, S. Tufik, Sleep rebound in animals deprived
150 (1965) 1318 – 1319. of PS by modified multiple platform method, Brain Res. 875 (2000)
[7] T. Endo, B. Schwierin, A.A. Borbély, I. Tobler, Selective and total 14 – 22.
sleep deprivation: effect on the sleep EEG in the rat, Psychiatry Res. [29] D. Suchecki, P.A. Tiba, S. Tufik, Hormonal and behavioural res-
66 (1997) 97 – 110. ponses of paradoxical sleep-deprived rats to the elevated plus maze,
[8] I. Feinberg, I.G. Campbell, Total sleep deprivation in the rat transient- J. Neuroendocrinol. 14 (2002) 549 – 554.
ly abolishes the delta amplitude response to darkness: implications for [30] C. Timo-Iaria, C.N. Negrão, R.W. Schimidek, K. Hoshino, C.E.L.
the mechanism of the ‘‘negative delta rebound’’, J. Neurophysiol. 70 Menezes, T.L. Da Rocha, Phases and states of sleep in the rat, Phys-
(1993) 2695 – 2699. iol. Behav. 5 (1970) 1057 – 1062.
[9] W. Fishbein, B.M. Gutwein, Paradoxical sleep and memory storage [31] I. Tobler, A.A. Borbély, The effect of 3-h and 6-h sleep deprivation on
processes, Behav. Biol. 19 (1977) 425 – 464. sleep and EEG spectra of the rat, Behav. Brain Res. 36 (1990) 36 – 73.
[10] S. Grahnstedt, R. Ursin, Platform sleep deprivation affects deep SWS [32] L.L. Tsai, B.M. Bergmann, A. Rechtschaffen, Sleep deprivation in the
in addition to REM sleep, Behav. Brain Res. 18 (1985) 233 – 239. rat: XVI. Effects in a light – dark cycle, Sleep 15 (1992) 537 – 544.
[11] D. Jouvet, E. Vimont, F. Delorme, M. Jouvet, Étude de la privation [33] Z.J.M. Van Hulzen, A.M.L. Coenen, PS deprivation and locomotor
sélective de la phase paradoxale de sommeil chez le chat, C. R. Soc. activity in rats, Physiol. Behav. 27 (1981) 741 – 744.
Biol. 158 (1964) 756 – 759. [34] E.L.J.M. Van Luijtelaar, A.M.L. Coenen, Electrophysiological evalu-
[12] G.J. Kant, R.H. Pastel, R.A. Bauman, G.R. Meininger, K.R. ation of three PS deprivation techniques in rats, Physiol. Behav. 36
Maughan, T.N. Robinson, W.L. Wright, P.S. Covington, Effects (1986) 603 – 609.

Você também pode gostar