Você está na página 1de 18

See

discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.net/publication/303365349

The Scope of Infant Mental Health

Chapter · January 2009

CITATIONS READS

8 329

1 author:

Charles H Zeanah
Tulane University
299 PUBLICATIONS 12,880 CITATIONS

SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

Maltreated Children View project

Screening and primary care early childhood mental health View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Charles H Zeanah on 19 May 2016.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


CHAPTER 1

The Scope of Infant Mental Health

Charles H. Zeanah, Jr.


Paula Doyle Zeanah

nfant mental health has emerged as an in- ment, stigma, and major mental illness. Is it
Iendeavor
creasingly important and visible clinical
during the past 35 years. There are
reasonable to think of infants as having men-
tal health problems? Or does it make more
many ways to trace its origins. In the clini- sense to think about them as being at tisk
cal realm the work of Selma. Fraiberg and for problems later? There are also questions
her colleagues in Michigan (Fraiberg, Adel- about "infant mental health" as a profes-
man, & Shapiro, 1975) was a major early sion. In a multidisciplinary field how is core
contributor, as was research in developmen- knowledge versus specialized knowledge
tal psychology on the power of babies to af- determined? Are infant mental health inter-
fect their caregivers (Bell, 1968). From these ventions qualitatively different from mental
beginnings, the field of infant mental health health" interventions for older children and
has grown dramatically both in terms of its adolescents? Finally, how is infant mental
breadth and its acceptance. In the early 21st health similar to, and distinct from, other
century, the field of infant mental health closely related multidisciplinary fields, such
stands as .a broad-based, multidisciplinary, as developmental psychopathology?
and international effort to enhance the social We begin by defining infant mental health
and emotional well-being of young children and considering its scope. We suggest that
and which includes the efforts of clinicians, die relational framework of infant mental
researchers, and policymakers. health distinguishes it from work with older
Still, as a relatively new field, a number children and adolescents. We review some of
of questions ought to be considered. For ex- the major empirical foundations of the field,
ample, how is infant mental health defined? highlighting the implications of these foun-
Some have expressed puzzlement or even dations for an infant mental health perspec-
aversion to the term "infant mental health." tive. Finally, we emphasize the need for com-
The idea of an "infant," with its associations prehensive approaches· to intervention and
of innocence, beginnings, and hope for a bet- highlight some evidenced-based programs.
ter future, does not seem to fit with "mental Throughout, we emphasize the policy impli-
health," and its associations of maladjust- cations of this work.
5
6 I. DEVELOPMENT AND CONTEXT

DEFINING INFANT health. We also extend the upper age limit


MENTAL HEALTH from 3 to 5 or so years, because much re-
search and many clinical programs extend
A Steering Committee on Infant Mental somewhat beyond the first 3 years.
Health was convened by Zero to Three and Beyond these definitions, several tenets re-
tasked with creating a definition of infant garding the clinical practice of infant mental
mental health. What emerged was a defini- health merit attention. These include a focus
tion of infant mental health as a characteris- on strengths in infants and families, a rela-
tic of the child. That is: tional framework for assessment and inter-
vention, and a prevention orientation.
the young child's capacity to experience, regu- Infant mental health is a strengths-based
late, and express emotions, form close and discipline. This means that clinicians work to
secure relationships, and explore the environ- identify strengths from which to build com-
ment and learn. All of these capacities will be
best accomplished within the context of the
petence and address problems. One could
caregiving environment that includes fam- rightly argue that all mental health profes-
ily, community, and cultural expectations for sionals ought to work from a strengths-
young children. Developing these capacities is based perspective, but it seems especially im-
synonymous with healthy social and emotion- portant in a field that focuses on the crucial
al development. (Zero to Three, 2001) and vulnerable beginnings of parent-child
relationships. Our children are extensions of
This definition seems to have met with ourselves, and when they do not thrive, we
widespread acceptance by the field (Zeanah, experience it as a reflecting profoundly on
Gleason, & Zeanah, 2008). us as parents. Nevertheless, being strengths-
In addition, infant mental health can be based does not mean ignoring liabilities
defined as a multidisciplinary professional (Zeanah, 1998). Clinicians must identify
field of inquiry, practice, and policy, con- problems in young children and in their par-
cerned with alleviating suffering and enhanc- ents unflinchingly in order to address them
ing the social and emotional competence of effectively. Further, there is often a complex
young children. Infant mental health is mul- interrelationship between strengths and
tidisciplinary because the complex, interre- weaknesses, such that strengths may be ob-
lated nature of human development and its scured by weaknesses but also possibly mo-
deviations requires expertise and conceptu- bilized to ameliorate weaknesses.
alizations beyond the capabilities of any par- Infant-caregiver relationships are the pri-
ticular discipline. For the same reason, it is mary focus of assessment and intervention
likely that the field of infant mental health efforts in infant mental health, not only be-
will remain pluralistic, a subspecialty within cause infants are so dependent upon their
a number of different disciplines, rather than caregiving contexts but also because infant
an integrated and distinct discipline itself. competence may vary widely in different re-
A definition is also needed for what we lationships. Assessments in infancy always
mean by the term "infant." In pediatrics, in- are considered a form of intervention, as
fant usually refers to the first year of life. In they may have important impacts on both
mental health, there is a tradition that infant infant and family. Moreover, intervention
refers more broadly to the period from birth efforts always involve prevention, because
to 3 years. In this chapter, however, we use the infant is considered as constantly devel-
an even broader conceptualization. First, oping, and the infant's developmental trajec-
as famously declared in From Neurons to tory must be attended to in addition to here-
Neighborhoods (National Research Coun- and-now adaptation. This means that there
cil and Institute of Medicine, 2000), focus- is a simultaneous focus on relieving current
ing disproportionately on birth to 3 years suffering as well as attending to future de-
"begins too late and ends too soon" (p. 7). velopment, all through attention to primary
Because there is considerable evidence re- caregiving relationships (Zeanah, Stafford,
garding prenatal influences on many clinical Nagle, & Rice, 2005; Zeanah, Stafford, &
problems in early childhood (see Robinson Zeanah, 2005; Zeanah & Zeanah, 2001).
et al., 2008), we include prenatal experience Just as young infants engender hope for a
in our conceptualization of infant mental better future in general, the field of infant
1. The Scope oflnfant Mental Health 7

mental health strives to delineate, establish, and mental health outcomes. The more ad-
and sustain positive developmental trajec- verse experiences individuals reported hav-
tories for young children. In all of these ef- ing, the more likely they were to engage in
forts, the empirical foundations of infant risky health behaviors and to be diagnosed
mental health have broadened and deepened with disorders such as depression, alcohol-
in ways that have important implications for ism and substance abuse, heart disease, can-
practice and policies. cer, chronic pulmonary disease, obesity, and
diabetes, among others (Dube, Felitti, Dong,
Giles, & Anda, 2003; Feletti et al., 1998).
EMPIRICAL FOUNDATIONS These findings remind us that infant mental
OF INFANT MENTAL HEALTH health has important implications for health
as well as mental health outcomes.
Basic knowledge salient to infant mental A related question concerns the ways in
health has been bolstered by research in which the timing of experiences matter, usu-
genetics, basic neuroscience, child develop- ally framed as a "sensitive period" or "criti-
ment, developmental psychopathology, and cal period" hypothesis. Knudsen (2004)
by studies of clinical disorders and their notes that the period during which the ef-
treatment. Investigations in these areas fects of experience on the brain are par-
provide the empirical foundations of infant ticularly strong is referred to as a sensitive
mental health. period, whereas experiences that provide in-
formation that is crucial for normal develop-
ment and alter performance permanently are
Early Experiences Matter
known as critical periods. Animal literature
Considerable research has documented the reveals that sensitive and critical periods in
importance of early experiences for the de- brain development are evident (Knudsen,
veloping person. Brain circuits are being es- 2004).
tablished at an extremely rapid rate in the Knudsen (2004) also notes that sensitive
early years of life, and various experiences and critical periods are actually properties
influence not only how brains function but of neural circuits, though we may be most
also the neural architecture of how they de- interested in how the effects of these vari-
velop. We are only just beginning to attempt ous periods are expressed at the level of be-
to understand the details about how expe- havior. For example, Nelson et al. (2007)
riences influence brain development, but studied children removed from institutional
evidence in humans on this point is grow- care in the first 3 years of life and placed
ing (see Sheridan & Nelson, Chapter 3, this in foster families and reported increases in
volume). IQ. For children removed prior to 24 months
Although mild to moderate stress can be the gains were substantial, but for those re-
growth promoting, so-called toxic stress moved after 24 months, the gains were few.
can impair the proper development of brain For a construct as complex as IQ, we would
circuitry, which may be especially vulner- expect to find an enormous number of cir-
able during early childhood (Middlebrooks cuits with different sensitive or critical peri-
& Audage, 2008). If individuals develop a ods involved.
lower threshold for stress, thereby becom- In keeping with these findings, infant
ing overly reactive to adverse experiences mental health has the importance of infant
throughout life, both physical and mental experience as a core principle. Escalona
health can be compromised (see also Rifkin- (1967) anticipated this emphasis almost
Graboi, Borelli, & Bosquet Enlow, Chapter half a century ago when she noted that it is
4, this volume). For example, in the adverse not infant or environmental characteristics
childhood experiences (ACE) study, adults that matter so much; rather it is the infant's
receiving treatment from a health main- subjective experience of the world. Indeed,
tenance organization (HMO) were inter- developmental psychopathology has demon-
viewed about early childhood experiences of strated that stabler individual differences lie
abuse, neglect and household dysfunction. initially in the infant-caregiver relationship,
The number of childhood risk factors was only later becoming a characteristic of the
linearly related to a large number of health individual child. Further, how an individual
8 I. DEVELOPMENT AND CONTEXT

thinks about relationship experiences-the sensitive resolution of distress, have been


internal representation or working model- linked to more optimal social, emotional,
is crucial because the meanings an individ- and cognitive development (see Crockenberg
ual attributes to experiences may alter their & Leerkes, 2000). In addition, parents who
consequences (Sroufe, 1989; Sroufe & Rut- promote the development of self-regulation
ter, 2000). and minimize problematic behavioral ten-
For the infant mental health clinician, the dencies have children who avoid maladap-
task becomes nothing less than attempting tive trajectories (Degnan, Henderson, Fox,
to understand what an individual child's ex- & Rubin, 2008; Gardner, Sonuga-Barke, &
perience is and to help that child's caregiv- Sayal, 1999). Conversely, parents who have
ers empathically appreciate that experience. problematic relationships with their young
From a policy perspective, even more daunt- children may increase the likelihood of mal-
ing is the challenge of attempting to extend adaptive outcomes in them (Scheeringa &
this appreciation of an infant's experience to Zeanah, 2001).
the level of systems, such as the child protec- Infant-parent relationships moderate in-
tion system or the legal system. How differ- trinsic biological risk factors in infants (Mc-
ent the lives of infants in dire circumstances Carton et al., 1997). That is, infants with
might be if these large and complex systems biological difficulties, such as the complica-
better appreciated and valued their experi- tions of prematurity or adverse temperamen-
ences (Knitzer, 2000). tal dispositions, have better outcomes when
their caregiving environments are support-
ive, and they have more problematic out-
Essential Experiences Involve
comes when their caregiving environments
Caregiving Relationships
are less supportive. Further, attachment re-
The importance of the contexts, or environ- lationships moderate the effects of prenatal
ments, in which infants grow and develop is stress on child fearfulness at 17 months, even
well established. Appreciating the complexi- after controlling for the effects of postnatal
ties and importance of context has enhanced stress, as well as obstetric, social, and demo-
our understanding of infant development graphic factors (Bergman, Sarkar, Glover, &
and our ability to predict developmental O'Connor, 2008).
trajectories (Sameroff & Fiese, 2000). Con- Infant-parent relationships also are the
texts exert their effects from within and conduit through which infants experience
from without, determining which experi- environmental risk factors (Zeanah, Boris,
ences an infant has and how that infant per- & Scheeringa, 1997). That is, infants expe-
ceives those experiences. One of the most rience risk factors such as poverty, maternal
distinctive features of the early years is the mental illness, and partner violence primar-
clear importance of the multiple interrelat- ily through the effects of those factors on
ed contexts (infant-caregiver relationship, infant-parent relationships. Infants are im-
family, cultural, and so forth) within which pacted by the risk factors that characterize
infants develop. For young children, infant- their caregiving environments through their
caregiver relationships are the most impor- specific relationship experiences. The bot-
tant experience-near context for infant de- tom line: Relationships can buffer or exac-
velopment and are the distinctive focus of erbate risk.
the infant mental health field. Finally, increasingly we are learning that
A considerable body of research has docu- the way in which psychopathology is ex-
mented the importance of the quality of the pressed in young children depends on the
infant-caregiver relationship and its impact types of relationships they have with their
on infant development (National Research caregivers (Zeanah et al., 1997). Research
Council and Institute of Medicine, 2000). In has shown that infants, in fact, construct
fact, although individual differences in infant different types of relationships with different
characteristics are readily identifiable, they caregivers (Steele, Steele, & Fonagy, 1996),
are not particularly predictive of subsequent and they also may express symptoms in the
characteristics later in development. Posi- presence of once caregiver but not with an-
tive qualities in infant-parent relationships, other (Zeanah, Bakshi, Boris, & Lieberman,
such as warmth, attentive involvement, and 2000). And, there is evidence that how an
1. The Scope oflnfant Mental Health 9

individual processes relationship experi- complexity are rarely considered in research


ences, through an internal working model, or even in our clinical conceptualizations.
is importantly related to outcomes (Sroufe, Beyond the immediate family of the infant,
1997). still other familial influences are important,
for all of the above reasons, the focus of chief among which are the cultural contexts
infant mental health has been dominated within which infants develop. Cultural be-
by a relational approach. This means that liefs and value systems define the assump-
infants are best understood, assessed, and tions of the group about what is important
treated in the context of their primary care- and the rules about raising children to be a
giving relationships. Or as Sroufe (1989) put certain way. Parenting beliefs, explanations,
it, "Most problems in the early years, while and interpretations of infant behavior are
often manifest poignantly in child behavior, among the most important components of
are best conceptualized as relationship prob- the cultural context of infant development
lems" (p. 70). (Lewis, 2000). These beliefs include some-
Beyond the infant-caregiver dyad, we times subtle cultural assumptions about
must consider infant development in the what facilitates infant development, the
context of the entire family. Not only is causes and amelioration of psychopathol-
infant development related to character- ogy, the roles and relevance of parenting,
istics of the family considered as a whole and many other concerns central to infant
(Minuchin, 1988), but there are important mental health. Cultures typically develop
effects on development from the infants in- adaptively in response to larger environ-
dividualized relationships with various fam- mental characteristics, such as the physical
ily members (Crockenberg, Lyons-Ruth, & resources of the area in which the culture de-
Dickstein, 1993; Favez, Frascorola, Keren, velops. Often differences among cultural be-
& Fivaz-Depeursinge, Chapter 29, this vol- lief systems can be understood within those
ume). For example, considerable evidence larger contexts. In recent decades, however,
indicates that the parents' marital relation- technological advances have thrust different
ship is one of the most important influences cultures together with increasing rapidity
on child development (Cummings & Davies, and led to intense cultural clashes, efforts at
2002). Sibling influences on infant develop- cultural coexistence, and pressures for cul-
ment are less well studied, but they are likely tural integration in the global village. All of
vitally important. Understanding family these factors have significant implications
processes is a complex undertaking. Emde for infant development and mental health.
(1991) has pointed out, for example, that The policy implications of these findings
the numbers of dyadic relationships within are clear and can be simply stated: Policies
families increases dramatically with increas- aimed at supporting families and other care-
ing numbers of children. Whereas two par- giving relationships, such as child care, are
ents and one child have only three dyadic most likely to provide needed supports for
relationships to consider, two parents and infant development (Center on the Develop-
three children have 10 dyadic relationships, ing Child at Harvard University, 2007).
and two parents and five children have 21
dyadic relationships, and so forth. Further,
Supporting Developmental Trajectories
an infant's relationships with various family
members are influenced by various other re- The rapidity and profundity of development
lationships within the family. The numbers in the first 3 years of life is unprecedented in
of dyadic relationships influencing individu- the postnatal human life cycle. In a mere 36
al family members increase from 3 for two months, infants change from totally depen-
parents and one child, to 4 5 for two parents dent newborns to complex creatures who can
and three children, to 210 for two parents come and go as they please; understand that
and five children (Emde, 1991). Obviously, they can share thoughts, feelings, and inten-
one could also consider other levels of com- tions with others; express themselves ab-
plexities, such as how an infant and his or stractly using symbols; and empathize with
her relationships might be affected by the others (Zeanah & Zeanah, 2001). From an
triadic relationship of his or her parents and infant mental health perspective, this devel-
another sibling. Nevertheless, these levels of opmental continuum means not only think-
10 I. DEVELOPMENT AND CONTEXT

ing about where the infant is now but also As noted, the field of infant mental
where the infant has been and where the health has a long tradition of focusing on
infant is going. It also requires understand- strengths and using strengths to minimize
ing not only what capacities are emerging in risks (Knitzer, 2000; Zeanah, 1998). A cen-
the developing child but also the processes tral concern then, for, infant mental health
involved in establishing trajectories of devel- is how to balance the influence of risk and
opment. protective factors and their mutual effects
on a child's particular situation. In addition,
in the first few years of life, it appears that
Risk and Protective Factors
environmental risk and protective factors
Risk and protective factors impact develop- matter more than within-the-infant risk and
mental trajectories, increasing or decreasing protective factors. In the Rochester longi-
the risks of developmental disruptions and tudinal study, for example, highly compe-
psychopathology. These risk factors are used tent infants in high-risk environments fared
to define high-risk groups, such as infants worse in terms of competence at age 4 years
born preterm, infants of depressed mothers, than did low-competent infants in low-risk
and infants raised in institutions. On the environments (Sameroff, Bartko, Baldwin,
other hand, risk factors are neither random- Baldwin, & Seifer, 1998). Thus, identifying,
ly distributed nor unrelated to one another. supporting, and strengthening caregiver and
Complexly interacting risk factors within family strengths is a fundamental principle
groups are the rule rather than the excep- underlying the work of infant mental health
tion. In other words, although intervention practitioners and provides direction for poli-
programs may target single risk factors, such cymakers interested in supporting young
as substance abuse, maternal depression, or children.
early parenthood, most of the time, infants
face multiple risk factors.
Psychopathology May Be Evident Early
Studies of many types of risk factors, from
mild to severe, consistently have been shown Can infants and toddlers experience or ex-
to lead to quite variable outcomes (Sroufe press psychopathology? The existence of psy-
& Rutter, 2000). In fact, it appears that the chopathology in infancy has been the source
number of risk factors rather than the nature of considerable controversy in part because
of any one is the best predictor of outcomes we are reluctant to believe that infants can
(Sameroff & Fiese, 2000). For example, pre- experience or suffer from psychiatric disor-
natal substance exposure is widely accepted ders (Zeanah et al., 1997). Behavioral indi-
to be a risk factor for infant development cators of infant mental health include emo-
(Boris, Chapter 10, this volume). Neverthe- tion regulation, the ability to communicate
less, Carta et al. (2001) studied the effects feelings to caregivers, and active exploration
of prenatal exposure and environmental of the environment. These behaviors lay the
cumulative risks. They found that although groundwork for later social and emotional
both prenatal drug exposure and cumulative competence, readiness to enter school, and
environmental risk predicted children's de- better academic and social performance.
velopmental level and rate of growth, envi- One major approach to studying psycho-
ronmental risk accounted for more variance pathology in the early years is a multidisci-
in developmental trajectories than prenatal plinary endeavor known as developmental
drug exposure. In fact, over time, the ef- psychopathology. It concerns identifying de-
fects of environmental risk outweighed the velopmental trajectories and those risk and
adverse consequences of prenatal substance protective factors and processes that increase
exposure. or decrease the probability of positive devel-
Protective factors may directly reduce the opmental outcomes. Clinical disorders may
effects of risk, may enhance competence, or be less than fully differentiated in infancy
may protect the individual against adver- (but see Angold & Egger, 2007, regarding
sity (Garmezy, Masten, & Tellegen, 1984). preschool children). Developmental psycho-
Protective processes may operate simultane- pathology emphasizes identification of in-
ously or successively even within the same dividuals with developmental delays (devel-
individual in the face of different challenges opment is behind where it ought to be, but
and at different points in development. the child is otherwise normal) or deviance
1. The Scope of Infant Mental Health 11

(development is abnormal) even before an rather than risk for psychopathology, there
actual disorder has emerged. Thus, preven- are increasingly compelling reasons to think
tive interventions, targeted to children with that doing so is a useful approach. For ex-
risk factors but not yet manifesting a disor- ample, most would agree that autism repre-
der, can be developed. Finally, because there sents a disorder, and there are compelling in-
is interest in the process of how disorders dicators that autism as a disorder is evident
develop, the field of developmental psycho- at least as early as the second year of life
pathology studies the ~volution o~ ~isor?ers (see Carr & Lord, Chapter 18, this volume).
over time rather than simply exammmg signs There are almost certainly neurobiological
and symptoms at a single point in time. abnormalities and behavioral differences
Psychopathology often is characterized that are evident even before the second year,
by the inability to change and adapt, but but the reliability of a categorical diagnosis
infants are constantly changing by develop- of autism from about 2 years of age is rea-
ing. This means that infant problems must sonable.
be distinguished from the often large range New studies are beginning to show that
of normal variations in behavior and from many types of psychiatric disorders are
transient perturbations in development. Ob- prevalent in young children. A recent study
viously, one way to address this challenge is of more than 300 two- to five-year-old chil-
to follow children over time and determine dren attending pediatric clinics in Durham,
whether problems persist. On the other North Carolina found that 16% had diag-
hand, it is important to recognize that psy- nosable psychiatric disorders associated
chopathology and maladaptation may not with impairment in functioning (Egger et
produce static symptomatology; rather, the al., 2006). This prevalence rate in nonre-
manifestations of problems may be different ferred preschool children is almost identical
at different times in development. For exam- to the 13 % rate reported in older children
ple, indiscriminate behavior toward unfa- and adolescents (Costello, Mustillo, Erkanli,
miliar adults in early childhood is a predic- Keeler, & Arnold, 2003 ).
tor of serious peer relational disturbances in There also has been progress in distin-
adolescence (Hodges & Tizard, 1989)-the guishing transient individual differences
continuity is in interpersonal disturbances, from true psychopathology. Belden, Thom-
but they manifest differently at different son, and Luby (2008) studied temper tan-
ages. Lawful developmental transformation trums in healthy versus depressed and
of symptomatology, known as heterotypic disruptive preschoolers. They found that
continuity, adds to the complexity of assess- preschoolers diagnosed with disruptive be-
ing psychopathology in infancy and early havior disorders had more tantrums, more
childhood. lasting tantrums, and more violent tantrums
For an individual child, however, risk than other children. Preschoolers diagnosed
factors are less important than the actual with depression, in contrast, displayed more
development and functioning of that indi- self-harm during tantrums than their healthy
vidual child at a given time. Clinicians must or disruptive peers. The conclusion is that
determine whether a given child, at a given children having more violent tantrums and
moment, has sufficient distress or maladap- tantrums associated with self harm require
tive behavior to constitute a disorder that more careful monitoring and perhaps refer-
requires intervention. This area introduces ral for assessment. In addition, separation
the other approach to psychopathology in anxiety as a disorder can be differentiated
infancy, which is to consider that at least from more transient separation anxiety in
some infant problem behaviors are signs and 2-year-old children by the degree of impair-
symptoms of psychiatric disorders. Clini- ment (Egger, 2008).
cians have found the use of categorical di- Despite all of these findings, there has
agnostic approaches to be valuable in young been widespread dissatisfaction among cli-
children, as they allow for conceptualizing nicians about using DSM-IV-TR (American
how clusters of symptoms hang together and Psychiatric Association, 2000) criteria to
provide clearer indicators of "caseness" than diagnose disorders in young children. New
do dimensional scores of various constructs. diagnostic classifications systems have been
Though some still hesitate to describe created to provide more developmentally
early deviant behavior as psychopathology, appropriate criteria, and also to provide a
12 I. DEVELOPMENT AND CONTEXT

basis for studying the construct validity of comes despite high-risk status, who main-
diagnoses. Zero to Three's alternative nosol- tain competent functioning despite stressful
ogy has been recently updated as DC:0-3-R life circumstances, and who recover from
(Zero to Three, 2005), and is in use in many traumatic events and experiences (Masten
parts of the world. In addition, the Research & Coatesworth, 1998). Increasingly, it has
Diagnostic Criteria for Infants and Pre- become clear that resilience, like compe-
schoolers (American Academy of Child and tence, is a multidimensional construct, and
Adolescent Psychiatry, 2003) was developed one that changes over time and context. In
by clinical investigators to enhance unifor- addition, it may be that rather than being re-
mity in research efforts. Finally, the DSM- silient to many problems, individuals may be
V, scheduled for publication in 2012, has resilient to some stressors but not to others
an explicit goal of incorporating a develop- (Rutter, 2000).
mental focus, including age-related subtypes For children in the early years, having a
of disorders where the evidence warrants it relationship with a caregiver who is available
(Pine et al., 2008). This level of activity un- and responsive to their needs, able to help
derscores considerable interest in psychiatric them navigate the demands of development
disorders in young children. over time, is likely to be the most important
We believe that at this early stage of the factor in helping them to achieve positive
science of infant mental health, both the outcomes, maintain competent functioning
risk and protective factor approach of de- under stress, and recover from traumatic ex-
velopmental psychopathology and the cat- periences. Young children who have the ca-
egorical disorder approach of many clinical pacity to elicit support and positive responses
studies have merit and are worthy of further from others may be at an advantage in this
investigation. Each approach, in fact, may regard (Werner & Smith, 2001). Enforcing
complement the other. In addition, we must policies that support families-especially
concern ourselves not only with adverse out- those that have limited resources-from the
comes but also with desired outcomes and time they are expecting through their child's
how to achieve them. This point leads to a early years is the best way to enhance young
discussion of how best to promote healthy children's competent functioning (Center on
outcomes in infant mental health. the Developing Child at Harvard University,
2007).
Social Competence and Resilience
Some Early Problems Are Enduring
Health is sometimes defined as the absence
of disease, although increasingly research- As noted above, not all problem behaviors
ers and clinicians are concerned with health seen in the early years are transient. We turn
promotion, that is, in enhancing individuals next to consider examples of enduring quali-
quality of experience. One aspect of "qual- ties of at least some forms of psychopathol-
ity of experience" is social competence, the ogy and consider the implication of these
ability to adapt successfully to differing findings. We consider first the subsyndromal
social and environmental demands. Social risk factor of aggression and then consider
competence, of course, is an ongoing adap- the categorical diagnosis of posttraumatic
tive capacity that itself may change over time stress disorder.
in relation to different stressors and situa-
tions. A focus on competence also reminds
Aggression
us that symptoms alone do not make a dis-
order; their functional significance for the Aggression, defiance, and temper tantrums
individual also must be considered. Social typically peak in early toddlerhood and de-
competence has emerged as an increasingly crease by school entry; however, some chil-
important outcome in infant mental health, dren do not show this normative decline. In
as well as in studies of developmental psy- the National Institute of Child Health and
chopathology. Human Development (NICHD) study of
A special form of social competence re- child care, investigators identified a cluster
ceiving increasing attention is resilience. of children who exhibited very high levels of
Resilience is demonstrated by infants and aggression at age 2 years and again at age
young children who achieve positive out- 9 years (National Institute of Child Health
1. The Scope of Infant Mental Health 13

and Human Development Early Child Care mixed traumatic experiences 4 months, 16
Research Network, 2004). Family correlates months, and 28 months after the trauma.
of children with stable high levels of aggres- They found significant stability of symptoms
sion included lower social class, less maternal over the 2 years, with almost no diminish-
education, reduced sensitivity to the child, ment of symptoms. Meiser-Stedman, Smith,
harsh and punitive parenting, depressive Glucksman, Yule, and Dalgleish (2008)
symptoms in the parent, and parents having studied 62 preschool children 2-4 weeks
fewer child-centered attitudes. Similarly, in a and 6 months after they had experienced
longitudinal study of 318 children at ages 2, motor vehicle accidents. They found that the
4, and 5 a latent profile analysis resulted in diagnosis of PTSD was moderately stable
two distinct longitudinal profiles of disrup- over the 6-month interval, even though the
tive behavior (Degnan, Calkins, Keane, & initial assessment occurred before a month
Hill-Soderlund, 2008). One high-aggression had passed from the accident.
profile was characterized by high child re- Treatment studies of PTSD that include
activity (children who reacted strongly and control groups also indicate a similar persis-
quickly to frustration) combined with highly tence of symptoms over time. For example,
controlling maternal behavior. Another was Lieberman, Van Horn, and Ippen (2005)
characterized by low child regulation (poor studied the effectiveness of child-parent psy-
efforts to regulate emotions) combined with chotherapy as a treatment of PTSD in young
low levels of maternal control. In both of children exposed to partner violence. The
these studies, aggression is stable over time comparison condition was case manage-
and associated with stable parental charac- ment, involving monthly telephone contact
teristics. with the mothers as well as providing infor-
Aggression in young children is not with- mation about and referrals to, local mental
out consequences. Gilliam (2005) deter- health clinics. Immediately after treatment
mined that state-run pre-K programs have (1 year after the trauma), the group who re-
three times the rate of expulsion of grades ceived child-parent psychotherapy showed
K-12. The reason young children get ex- statistically significant improvements in
pelled from child care centers and pre-K is child posttraumatic stress symptoms, but the
almost always aggression. Longer-term con- group receiving case management showed no
sequences are also important, as aggressive significant diminishment of signs of PTSD.
school-age children may begin a path toward These results show that young children re-
antisocial behavior in adolescence or adult- ceiving case management and sometimes
hood (Frick & Marsee, 2006). referral experienced stability in their symp-
toms over 12 months.
Posttraumatic Stress Disorder
Implications
It is well known that many adults and older
children who have been severely trauma- These findings are selective rather than
tized develop posttraumatic stress disorder comprehensive, but they illustrate that it is
(PTSD), showing signs of hyperarousal, re- no longer acceptable to assume that early-
experiencing the trauma, avoiding remind- a ppearing symptomatology is always, or
ers of the trauma, and/or numbing of re- even usually, transient. Furthermore, there
sponsiveness. A series of studies of young are reasons to believe that intervening earlier
children has demonstrated that these same is more effective-at least for some domains
symptoms are apparent in infants, toddlers, of development.
and preschoolers, although their manifesta- Dishian and colleagues (2008) suggest
tions are different than in older children and three reasons why earlier intervention may
adults because of obvious developmental dif- be more beneficial. First, earlier interven-
ferences (see Scheeringa, Chapter 21, this vol- tions may target child behaviors before they
ume). In addition, two studies that have take on a more serious form. In their focus
followed the course of traumatized young on externalizing problems, they argue that
children indicate that signs and symptoms noncompliant and oppositional behaviors
exhibited following a traumatic event are are easier to remediate than are lying, steal-
not transient. Scheeringa, Zeanah, Myers, ing, and proactive aggression. Second, if
and Putnam (2005) studied 62 children with children are younger, then parents are also
14 I. DEVELOPMENT AND CONTEXT

younger and may have had fewer stressful young children effectively, (2) ensure that
experiences and more capacity to change. families in need of additional services can
Third, the sense of optimism caregivers have obtain them, and (3) increase the ability of
regarding the possibility of parent-child nonfamilial caregivers to identify, address,
relationship change is much higher during and prevent social-emotional problems in
their offspring's early childhood. early childhood. The targets of intervention
Knudsen and colleagues (Knudsen, Heck- can be the child's behavior, the parent's be-
man, Cameron, & Shonkoff, 2006) pointed havior, or even the social context in which
out that there is a convergence of findings the child is developing, but the main focus of
from child development, neuroscience, and infant mental health is on strengthening or
economic research indicating that greater re- improving relationships as they impact the
turn on investments are to be expected when young child's development and behavior.
intervening earlier. Citing studies from all In Figure 1.1, we present a model of in-
three areas of research, they present compel- fant mental health services, based on a pre-
ling evidence that early intervention is more ventive health perspective (Mrazek & Hag-
likely to be effective, providing a basis for gerty, 1994; National Research Council and
policies that support a broad array of early Institute of Medicine, 2000) that represents
childhood initiatives (see Knitzer & Lefkow- an update of a previous conceptualization
itz, 2006). This point leads us to consider (Zeanah, Stafford, Nagle, et al., 2005).
the kinds of early intervention that infant Mrazek and Haggerty (1994) distinguished
mental health recommends. between prevention and treatment services.
Preventive interventions aim to prevent the
initial onset of a disorder, decrease causal
COMPREHENSIVE factors and increase protective factors, and/
INTERVENTIONS ARE NEEDED or decrease the severity or duration of a dis-
order. Specifically, preventive interventions
The goals of the infant mental health field emphasize altering infant and parent behav-
are to reduce or eliminate suffering, to pre- iors and family functioning in order to pre-
vent adverse outcomes (school failure, delin- serve or restore infants to more normative
quency, psychiatric morbidity, interpersonal developmental trajectories. For example,
isolation or conflicts, developmental delays intrinsic infant risk factors such as difficult
and deviance), and to promote healthy out- temperament cannot be prevented, but the
comes by enhancing social competence and adverse consequences of difficult tempera-
resilience. In order to accomplish these ment, such as the emergence of behavior
overarching goals, interventions must (1) problems, can be a focus of prevention ef-
enhance the ability of caregivers to nurture forts.

FIGURE 1.1. Continuum of services at state and local levels.


1. The Scope of Infant Mental Health 15

Mrazek and Haggerty (1994) divided needs such as safe housing, appropriate nu-
preventive interventions into three distinct trition, and availability of health and human
levels. Universal preventions are considered services are met even before other issues can
desirable for everyone in an eligible popu- be addressed.
lation; professional assistance may or may Early child care provides one example of a
not be needed. Selective preventions target universal setting for addressing infant men-
members of a group who have high lifetime tal health. Scarr (1998) declared that there is
or high imminent risk for subsequent prob- an international consensus about what con-
lems. Finally, indicated preventions target stitutes quality child care-namely, warm,
those who manifest minimal but detectable supportive interactions with adults in a safe,
behavioral symptoms that may later become healthy, and stimulating environment. Con-
a full-blown disorder. Treatment of existing siderable evidence supports her assertion.
disorders adds a fourth level to this concep- For example, the NICHD study of early
tualization (see Figure 1.1). child care is a prospective, longitudinal study
Since infants and young children grow and designed to examine concurrent, long-term,
develop within multiple contexts, biologi- and cumulative influences of variations in
cal, social, and relationship issues are often early child care experiences of young chil-
interrelated, and a continuum of services is dren. In this study, 1,364 healthy full-term
needed. Infants and families may seek ser- newborns were recruited in 10 sites around
vices at any point along the continuum or the United States. Investigators examined
more than one point simultaneously. For what aspects of child care were important
example, a young child who requires treat- for promoting child development across a
ment for trauma symptoms related to abuse number of domains by assessing the child,
or neglect may also need preventive health the family, and the child care setting longi-
care; access to services for basic needs such tudinally; among child care variables, qual-
- as food, shelter, or clothing; or specialized ity of care was the most important predictor
developmental services such as speech and of child outcomes. Quality of care is related
language or physical therapy. A child being to cognitive and language outcomes, as well
seen for a well-child visit may be identified as social and behavioral outcomes, in young
as having behavioral problems that warrant children (National Institute of Child Health
more intensive or specialized interventions. and Human Development Early Child Care
Thus, cross-discipline and often cross- Research Network, 2005). In other words,
system collaboration is essential. In fact, in access to quality child care is a vitally im-
the United States, major policy initiatives portant intervention for young children and
in infant mental health are evident in most should be the focus of sustained policy ef-
states, supported by federal and/or state gov- forts to help achieve that goal.
ernments (Rosenthal & Kaye, 2005). An important caveat was that character-
istics of the parent-child relationship were
better predictor of child outcomes than any
Universal Prevention
combination of child care variables (Nation-
Some services are believed to be important al Institute of Child Health and Human De-
for all infants and families, either for pre- velopment Early Child Care Research Net-
vention or for health promotion purposes. work, 2006). This does not mean that child
These universal services seek to avert or care experiences are unimportant. Rather, it
prevent the onset of problems and/or seek to emphasizeS' the importance of all care giving
enhance social-emotional health arid devel- relationships for young children, with spe-
opment. In infant mental health, approaches cial primacy for parent-child relationships.
include education regarding normal infant
health and development, increasing knowl-
Selective Approaches to Intervention
edge about what constitutes healthy parent-
infant relationships, and access or referral Some interventions are provided to families
to additional services as needed. Although of young children who have been selected
most universal services are aimed at indi- because they are "at risk" for poorer so-
viduals or families, in some cases, a commu- cial and emotional outcomes. Some within
nity approach is needed to ensure that basic the group may be functioning well; others
16 I. DEVELOPMENT AND CONTEXT

may be more obviously struggling. Interven- program has yielded significant cost-benefit
tions are presumably developed to address advantages (Aos, Lieb, Mayfield, Miller, &
the risks inherent in the population, and Pennucci, 2004; Karoly, Kilburn, & Can-
typically, specific outcomes are monitored non, 2005).
or measured. Selective interventions may be
delivered in a variety of settings (e.g., health,
Indicated Approaches
mental health, educational, or social servic-
to Preventive Intervention
es), and there is a great range in the structure
of such services, such as frequency or inten- When subsyndromal problems are already
sity, type of intervention provided, skills or evident in young children, indicated inter-
behaviors that are targeted, and amount of ventions may be applied. These interventions
monitoring or follow-up. are aimed at preventing early manifestations
A notable example of a selective preven- of deviance from becoming clinical disorders
tion directed at improving maternal and in later development.
infant outcomes, including the reduction of Insecure and especially disorganized at-
abuse and neglect in a high-risk, impover- tachments between young children and
ished sample, is the work of Olds, Salder, their caregivers are known to be a risk fac-
and Kitzman (2007). They pioneered the tor for subsequent psychosocial adaptation.
Nurse-Family Partnership (NFP), a nurse- Because sensitive and responsive parenting
home visitation intervention for impover- is associated with secure attachment, van
ished first-time mothers. The preventive in- den Boom (1994) developed an intervention
tervention begins prior to the 28th week of designed to enhance secure attachment in
pregnancy and continues through the child's infants believed to be at risk because of tem-
second birthday. Though the NFP program peramental irritability. She delivered three
uses attachment theory, social learning the- home visits to low-income mothers and their
ory, and human ecology theory to ground 6- to 9-month-old temperamentally irritable
the work, the program evolved out of a pub- infants. The intervention focused on increas-
lic health rather than mental health delivery ing mothers' sensitive responsiveness to their
approach. infants' cues. Findings from a randomized
NFP has three major goals: to improve trial of 100 infant-mother pairs demon-
pregnancy health outcomes, to improve in- strated that when infants were 9 months old,
fant health and development outcomes, and program mothers were significantly more re-
to improve maternal life course development. sponsive, stimulating, and visually attentive.
Highly trained nurses use manualized guide- At 12 and 18 months old, children whose
lines to address issues related to personal mothers receiveq the intervention were sig-
health and health, quality of caregiving for nificantly more likely to be securely attached
the infant, maternal life course develop- than control children (van den Boom, 1994,
ment, and social support. Special attention 1995). These findings led Juffer, Bakermans-
is given to the importance of establishing a Kranenburg, and van IJzendoorn (2007) to
trusting, consistent relationship between the develop and evaluate a promising interven-
nurse and the client, and the development tion called the Video-based Intervention to
of a safe, nurturing, and enriched parent- Promote Positive Parenting. This interven-
infant relationship. tion is targeted to dyads at risk for the ad-
Through a series of randomized controlled verse consequences of insecure attachment
trials, NFP has demonstrated significant im- and has been shown to reduce externalizing
pact across a variety of maternal and infant problems in young children.
health and social outcomes, including reduc-
tion in child maltreatment, reductions in se-
Treatment of Established Disorders
rious accidental injuries in children, delays
in subsequent pregnancies, and increased For young children who already have iden-
maternal employment, as well as reductions tifiable disorders, psychotherapeutic services
in child and maternal criminal and antisocial aimed at alleviating suffering or repairing
behaviors as long as 15 years after program or remediating functioning are necessary.
completion (Olds et al., 2007). Importantly, Most often these services are provided by
two independent groups have shown that the mental health professionals trained in spe-
1. The Scope of Infant Mental Health 17

cific infant mental health assessment and in- Rogosh, 1999; Toth, Rogosch, Manly, &
tervention techniques. Treatment of already Cicchetti, 2006) and in maltreated young
identified problems may be focused primar- children (Cicchetti, Rogosch, & Toth, 2006;
ily on changing the infant (Benoit, Wang, & Toth, Maughan, Manly, Spagnola, & Cic-
Zlotki, 2001), the parent and his or her be- chetti, 2002).
havior (McDonough, 2000), or the infant-
parent relationship (Lieberman, Silverman,
Challenges of Infant Mental
& Pawl, 2000). Stern (1995) has argued that
Health Interventions
these different forms of intervention may
use different strategies and different ports of Preventive interventions and treatment ef-
entry into the infant-parent dyad, but all are forts in infant mental health share several
concerned with changing the relationship as challenges. First, it is important to involve
a way of changing infant behavior and ex- families of young children and to listen and
perience. incorporate their concerns into the planning
Treatment of established problems is con- and implementation of interventions. This
cerned with current resolution of symptoms requires the development of a working alli-
and distress but also with infants' develop- ance between parents and intervener-that
mental trajectories. For these reasons, infant is, a shared commitment to work together
mental health treatment is concerned simul- in the best interest of the child. The relation-
taneously with present and future adapta- ship between the parent and the intervener
tion of the child. often becomes a model for the respectful
An increasing number of treatments in in- and empathic way parents learn to relate to
fant mental health are supported empirical- their infant.
ly. Perhaps the best studied is child-parent Second, practitioners must recognize that
psychotherapy. Originally pioneered by Frai- personal, familial, ethnic, cultural, profes-
berg and colleagues (Fraiberg et al., 1975), sional, and organizational values impact
this treatment is a manualized interven- every aspect of interventions. These values
tion used primarily with high-risk families create explicit and implicit lenses through
that have children less than 5 years of age. which relationships are understood. Often,
Child-parent psychotherapy tries to estab- the situations faced by infants and young
lish links between the parents' early child- children evoke strong feelings in the pro-
hood experiences and their current feelings, fessional. Recognizing and understanding
perceptions, and behaviors toward their in- one's own value system as well as how pro-
fants and young children. The therapist acts fessional perspectives impact one's ability to
as a translator of the emotional experience understand the dyad are an ongoing chal-
of parent and child, attending carefully to lenge. Countertransference, including prob-
the parent's stressful life circumstances and lems with boundaries, value judgments, and
culturally derived values. rescue fantasies, can cloud objectivity and
A new generation of clinician research- undermine the potential for the intervention
ers has more fully developed child-parent to succeed. Adequate provider training and
psychotherapy, expanded its application to supervision are viewed as essential precur-
preschool-age children, and systematically sors to developing effective interventions
studied its effectiveness (see Lieberman & (see Hinshaw-Fuselier, Zeanah, & Larrieu,
Van Horn, Chapter 27, this volume); in fact, Chapter 33, this volume).
there are now five randomized controlled tri- A third related challenge, particularly for
als supporting its efficacy. Child-parent psy- professionals who have been taught to focus
chotherapy has been shown to be effective at on individuals, is keeping the focus on the
(1) reducing insecure attachment behaviors infant-parent relationship. The professional
in toddlers of stressed immigrant families must pay attention not only to the behavioral
(Lieberman, Weston, & Pawl, 1991), (2) re- interactions within the dyad, but also must
ducing signs of PTSD in children traumatized appreciate the parent's emotional experience
by marital violence (Lieberman et al., 2005; of the young child, and the young child's ex-
Lieberman, Ippen, & Van Horn, 2006), and perience of the parent. Recognition of each
(3) increasing secure attachments in infants of these perspectives requires a paradigm
of depressed mothers (Cicchetti, Toth, & shift for most early childhood professionals,
18 I. DEVELOPMENT AND CONTEXT

and it requires significant training in order caregiving relationships, which are embed-
to fully understand and to integrate these ded within multiple social and cultural con-
perspectives into clinical work. texts, promote social competence in young
Finally, though the evidence base in infant children, and social competence is associ-
mental health is growing, ongoing research ated with adaptive behavioral, emotional,
into preventive interventions and treatments and cognitive outcomes. The scope of infant
is needed. It is important to identify the mental health includes clinical, research, and
components of the intervention, such as (1) policy efforts and encompasses the theoreti-
the targeted recipient; (2) methods of inter- cal perspectives and knowledge base of mul-
vention; (3) frequency, intensity, and length tiple professional disciplines. The complex-
of services; (4) location of service delivery; ity of the problems of infants and toddlers
and (5) type of service provider. Then it is must be matched by the comprehensiveness
important to link these components with of our efforts to minimize their suffering
anticipated, measurable outcomes (Karoly and enhance their competence.
et al., 2005). Explicating these components
and applying sound research methodology
will enhance the evidence base and even- REFERENCES
tually will allow us in the field to identify
American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psy-
critical elements and combination strate- chiatry Task Force on Research Diagnostic Cri-
gies that make a difference within and pos- teria: Infancy and Preschool. (2003). Research
sibly across programs. For example, Olds diagnostic criteria for infants and preschool chil-
and colleagues (2002) showed that nurses dren: The process and empirical support. Journal
outperformed paraprofessionals in terms of of the American Academy of Child and Adoles-
outcomes achieved, keeping other character- cent Psychiatry, 42, 1504-1512.
istics of the NFP model constant. This find- American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diag-
nostic and statistical manual of mental disorders
ing helps justify the extra cost of using nurs- (4th ed., text rev.). Washington, DC: Author.
es to deliver services in this intervention. Angold, A., & Egger, H. L. (2007). Preschool psy-
There is a particular need for research chopathology: Lessons for the lifespan. Journal
that focuses on the impact of sequential pre- of Child Psychology, Psychiatry, and Allied Dis-
ventive interventions (Mrazek & Haggerty, ciplines, 48, 961-966.
1994). This area has hardly been studied at Aos, S., Lieb, R., Mayfield, J., Miller, M., & Pen-
nucci, A. (2004). Benefits and costs of preven-
all, no doubt partly because it poses signifi-
tion and early intervention programs for youth.
cant fiscal and logistical challenges. Olympia, WA: Washington State Institute for
In developing more refined questions in in- Public Policy.
tervention research, clinicians need to work Belden, A. C., Thomson, N. R., & Luby, J. L.
closely with researchers. Ideally, the latest (2008). Temper tantrums in healthy versus de-
research findings inform clinical practice, pressed and disruptive preschoolers: Defining
and clinical practice informs research de- tantrum behaviors associated with clinical prob-
lems. Journal of Pediatrics, 152, 117-122.
signs by introducing promising approaches. Bell, R. Q. (1968). A reinterpretation of the direc-
The ultimate goal is for clinicians to be able tion of effects in studies of socialization. Psycho-
to select an intervention that is best suited to logical Review, 75, 81-95.
address an individual child's particular prob- Benoit, D., Wang, E. L., & Zlotki, S. H. (2000).
lems and circumstances. Policies ensuring Discontinuation of enterostomy tube feeding by
that families have access to individualized behavioral treatment in early childhood: A ran-
domized controlled trial. Journal of Pediatrics,
services will become increasingly important
137, 498-503.
as our ability to match children and families Bergman, K., Sarkar, P., Glover, V., & O'Connor,
with specific interventions improves. T. G. (2008). Quality of child-parent attachment
moderates the impact of antenatal stress on child
fearfulness. Journal of Child Psychology and
CONCLUSIONS Psychiatry, 49, 1089-1098.
Carta, J. J., Atwater, J.B., Greenwood, C.R., Mc-
Connell, S. R., McEvoy, M. A., & Williams, R.
The field of infant mental health emphasizes (2001). Effects of cumulative prenatal substance
the importance of caregiving relationships exposure and environmental risks on children's
as having major effects on the young child's developmental trajectories. Journal of Clinical
social and emotional experience. Healthy Child Psychology, 30, 327-337.

j I
1. The Scope of Infant Mental Health 19

C~nter on the Developing Child at Harvard Uni- Walter, B. K., & Angold, A. (2006). Test-retest
versity. (2007). A science-based framework for reliability of the Preschool Age Psychiatric As-
early childhood policy using evidence to improve sessment (PAPA). Journal of the American Acad-
outcomes in learning, behavior, and health for emy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 45,
vulnerable children. Available online at www. 538-549.
developingchild.harvard.edu. Emde, R. N. (1991). The wonder of our complex
Cicchetti, D., Rogosch, F. A., & Toth, S. L. (2006). enterprise: Steps enabled by attachment and the
Fostering secure attachment in infants in mal- effect of relationships on relationships. Infant
treating families through preventive interven- Mental Health Journal, 12, 164-173.
tions. Development and Psychopathology, 18, Escalona, S. (1967). Patterns of infantile experience
623-649. and the developmental process. Psychoanalytic
Cicchetti, D., Toth, S. L., & Rogosch, F. A. (1999). Study of the Child, 22, 197-244.
The efficacy of toddler-parent psychotherapy to Felitti, V. J., Anda, R. F., Nordenberg, D., Williams,
increase attachment security in offspring of de- D. F., Spitz, A. M., Edwards, V., et al. (1998).
pressed mothers. Attachment and Human De- Relationship of childhood abuse and household
velopment, 1, 34-66. dysfunction to many of the leading causes of
Costello, E. J., Mustillo, S., Erkanli, A., Keeler, G., death in adults: The Adverse Childhood Experi-
& Angold, A. (2003). Prevalence and develop- ences (ACE) study. American Journal of Preven-
ment of psychiatric disorders in childhood and tive Medicine, 14, 245-258.
adolescence. Archives of General Psychiatry, 60, Fraiberg, S., Adelman, B., & Shapiro, V. (1975).
837-844. Ghosts in the nursery. Journal of the American
Crockenberg, S., & Leerkes, E. (2000). Infant so- Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry,
cial and emotional development in family con- 14, 387-421.
text. In C. H. Zeanah, Jr. (Ed.), Handbook of Frick, P. J., & Marsee, M.A. (2006). Psychopathy
infant mental health (2nd ed., pp. 60-90). New and developmental pathways to antisocial behav-
York: Guilford Press. ior in youth. In C. J. Patrick (Ed.), Handbook of
Crockenberg, S., Lyons-Ruth, K., & Dickstein, psychopathy (pp. 353-370). New York: Guilford
S. (1993). The family context of infant mental Press.
health: II. Infant development in multiple family Gardner, F., Sonuga-Barke, E., & Sayal, K. (1999).
relationships. In C. H. Zeanah (Ed.), Handbook Parents anticipating misbehavior: An observa-
of infant mental health (pp. 38-55). New York: tional study of strategies parents use to prevent
Guilford Press. conflict with behavior problem children. Journal
Cummings, E. M., & Davies, P. T. (2002). Effects of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 40, 1185-
of marital conflict on children: Recent advances 1196.
and emerging themes in process-oriented re- Garmezy, N., Masten, A. S., & Tellegen, A. (1984).
search. Journal of Child Psychology and Psy- The study of stress and competence in children:
chiatry, 43, 31-63. A building block for developmental psychopa-
Degnan, K. A., Calkins, S. D., Keane, S. P., & Hill- thology. Child Development, 55, 97-111.
Soderlund, A. L. (2008). Profiles of disruptive be- Gilliam, W. S. (2005). Prekindergarteners left be-
havior across early childhood: Contributions of hind: Expulsion rates in state prekindergarten
frustration reactivity, physiological regulation, systems. New Haven, CT: The Edward Zigler
and maternal behavior. Child Development, 79, Center in Child Development and Social Policy,
1357-1376. Yale University Child Study Center. Available
Degnan, K. A., Henderson, H. A., Fox, N. A., & at ziglercenter.yale.edu!resources!docs/Nation-
Rubin, K. H. (2008). Predicting social wariness in al%20Prek%20Study_expulsion.pdf.
middle childhood: The moderating roles of child Hodges, J., & Tizard, B. (1989). Social and fam-
care history, maternal personality, and maternal ily relationships of ex-institutional adolescents.
behavior. Social Development, 71, 471-487. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 30,
Dishion, T. J., Shaw, D., Connell, A., Gradner, F., 77-97.
Weaver, C., & Wilson, M. (2008). The family Juffer, F., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., & van
check up with high risk indigent families: Pre- IJzendoorn, M. H. (2007). Promoting positive
venting problem behavior by increasing parents' parenting: An attachment-based intervention.
positive behavior support in early childhood. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Child Development, 79, 1395-1414. Karoly, L. A., Kilburn, M. R., & Cannon, J. S.
Dube, S. R., Felitti, V. J., Dong, M., Giles, W. H., & (2005). Early childhood interventions: Proven
Anda, R. F. (2003). The impact of adverse child- results, future promise. Santa Monica, CA:
hood experiences on health problems: Evidence RAND.
from four birth cohorts dating back to 1900. Knitzer, J. (2000). Early childhood mental health
Preventive Medicine, 37, 268-277. services: A policy and systems development
Egger, H. L. (2008, November). Assessing pre- perspective. In J. Shonkoff & S. Meisels (Eds.),
schoolers with anxiety. Paper presented at the Handbook of early childhood intervention (2nd
annual meeting of the American Academy of ed., pp. 416-438). New York: Cambridge Uni-
Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Chicago. versity Press.
Egger, H. L., Erkanli, A., Keeler, G., Potts, E., Knitzer, J., & Lefkowitz, J. (2006). Pathways to
20 I. DEVELOPMENT AND CONTEXT

early school success issue: Brief No. 1. Helping Minuchin, P. (1988). Relationships within the fam-
the most vulnerable infants, toddlers, and their ily: A systems perspective on development. In
families. New York: National Center for Chil- R. A. Hinde & J. Stevenson-Hinde (Eds.), Re-
dren in Poverty. lationships within families: Mutual inf/.uences
Knudsen, E. I. (2004). Sensitive periods in the de- (pp. 7-26). New York: Oxford University Press.
velopment of the brain and behavior. Journal of Mrazek, P. B., & Haggerty, R. J. (1994). Reducing
Cognitive Neuroscience, 16, 1412-1425. risks for mental disorders: Frontiers for preven-
Knudsen, E. I., Heckman, J. ]., Cameron, J. L., tive intervention research. Committee on Preven-
& Shonkoff, J. P. (2006). Economic, neurobio- tion of Mental Disorders, Institute of Medicine.
logical, and behavioral perspectives on building Washington, DC: National Academy Press.
America's future workforce. Proceedings of the National Institute of Child Health and Human De-
National Academy of Sciences, ·103, 10155- velopment Early Child Care Research Network.
10162. (2004). Trajectories of physical aggression from
Lewis, M. (2000). The cultural context of in- toddlerhood to middle childhood: Predictors,
fant mental health: The developmental niche of correlates and outcomes. Monographs of the So-
infant-caregiver relationships. In C. H. Zeanah, ciety for Research in Child Development (Serial
Jr. (Ed.), Handbook of infant mental health (2nd No. 278), 69, 1-144.
ed., pp. 91-107). New York: Guilford Press. National Institute of Child Health and Human De-
Lieberman, A. F., Ippen, C. G., & Van Horn, P. velopment Early Child Care Research Network.
(2006). Child-parent psychotherapy: 6-month (2005). Child care and child development. New
follow-up of a randomized controlled trial. Jour- York: Guilford Press.
nal of the American Academy of Child and Ado- National Institute of Child Health and Human De-
lescent Psychiatry, 45, 913-917. velopment Early Child Care Research Network.
Lieberman, A. F., Silverman, R., & Pawl, J. (2000). (2006). Child care effect sizes for the NICHD
Infant-parent psychotherapy. In C. H. Zeanah study of early child care and youth development.
(Ed.), Handbook of infant mental health (2nd American Psychologist, 61, 99-116.
ed., pp. 472-484). New York: Guilford Press. National Research Council and Institute of Medi-
Lieberman, A. F., Van Horn, P., & Ippen, C. G. cine. (2000). From neurons to neighborhoods:
(2005). Toward evidence-based treatment: The science of early childhood development.
Child-parent psychotherapy with preschool- Committee on Integrating the Science of Early
ers exposed to marital violence. Journal of the Childhood Development (J. P. Shonkoff and D.
American Academy of Child and Adolescent A. Phillips, Eds.). Washington, DC: National
Psychiatry, 44, 1241-1248. Academy Press.
Lieberman, A. F., Weston, D., & Pawl, J. H. (1991). Nelson, C. A., Zeanah, C. H., Fox, N. A., Mar-
Preventive intervention and outcome with anx- shall, P. J., Smyke, A. T., & Guthrie, D. (2007).
iously attached dyads. Child Development, 62, Cognitive recovery in socially deprived young
199-209. children: The Bucharest Early Intervention Proj-
Masten, A. S., & Coatesworth, D. J. (1998). The ect. Science, 318, 1937-1940.
development of competence in favorable and un- Olds, D. L., Robinson, J., O'Brien, R., Luckey,
favorable environments: Lessons from research D. W., Pettitt, L. M., Henderson, C. R., et al.
on successful children. American Psychologist, (2002). Home visiting by paraprofessionals and
53, 205-220. by nurses: A randomized, controlled trial. Pedi-
McCarton, C. M., Brooks-Gunn,]., Wallace, I. F., atrics, 110, 486-496.
Bauer, C.R., Bennett, F. C., Bernbaum, J. C., et Olds, D. L., Sadler, L., & Kitzman, H. (2007). Pro-
al. (1997). Results at age 8 years of early interven- grams for parents of infants and toddlers: Re-
tion for low-birth-weight premature infants: The cent evidence from randomized trials. Journal of
infant health and development program. Obstet- Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 48, 355-391.
rical and Gynecological Survey, 52, 341-342. Pine, D. S., Costello, E. J., Dahl, R., James, R.,
McDonough, S. (2000). Interaction guidance: An Leckman, J., Leibenluft, E., et al. (2008, March).
approach for difficult to engage families. In C. Increasing the developmental focus in DSM-V:
H. Zeanah (Ed.), Handbook of infant mental Broad issues and specific potential applications
health (2nd ed., pp. 485-493). New York: Guil- in anxiety. Paper presented at the annual meeting
ford Press. of the American Psychopathological Association,
Meiser-Stedman, R., Smith, P., Glucksman, E., New York City.
Yule, W., & Dalgleish, T. (2008). The posttrau- Robinson, M., Oddy, W. H., Li, J., Kendall, G. E.,
matic stress disorder diagnosis in preschool- and de Klerk, N. H., Silburn, S. R., et al. (2008).
elementary school-age children exposed to motor Pre- and postnatal influences on preschool men-
vehicle accidents. American Journal of Psychia- tal health: A large-scale cohort study. Journal
try, 165, 1326-1337. of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 49, 1118-
Middlebrooks, J. S., & Audage, N. C. (2008). The 1128.
effects of childhood stress on health across the Rosenthal, J., & Kaye, N. (2005). State approaches
lifespan. Atlanta: Centers for Disease Control to promoting young children's healthy mental
and Prevention, National Center for Injury Pre- development: A survey of Medicaid, mater-
vention and Control. nal and child health, and mental health agen-
1. The Scope of Infant Mental Health 21

cies. Portland, ME: National Academy for State sive disorder: A randomized preventive trial.
Health Policy. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology,
Rutter, M. (2000). Resilience reconsidered: Con- 74, 1006-1016.
ceptual considerations, empirical findings, and van den Boom, D. C. (1994). The influence of tem-
policy implications. In J. Shonkoff & S. Meisels perament and mothering on attachment and ex-
(Eds.), Handbook of early childhood interven- ploration: An experimental manipulation of sen-
tion (2nd ed., pp. 651-682). New York: Cam- sitive responsiveness among lower-class mothers
bridge University Press. with irritable infants. Child Development, 65,
Sameroff, A. ]., Bartko, W. T., Baldwin, A., Bald- 1457-1477.
win, C., & Seifer, R. (1998). Family and social van den Boom, D. C. (1995). Do first-year inter-
influences on the development of competence. vention effects endure?: Follow-up during tod-
In M. Lewis & C. Feiring (Eds.), Families, risk dlerhood of a sample of Dutch irritable infants.
and competence (pp. 161-186). Hillsdale, NJ: Child Development, 66, 1798-1816.
Erlbaum. Werner, E. E., & Smith, R. S. (2001). Journeys from
Sameroff, A.]., & Fiese, B. (2000). Models of devel- childhood to midlife: Risk, resilience, and recov-
opment and developmental risk. In C.H. Zeanah ery. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press.
(Ed.), Handbook of infant mental health (2nd Zeanah, C. H., Jr. (1998). Reflections on the
ed., pp. 3-19). New York: Guilford Press. strengths perspective. The Signal, 6, 12-13.
Scarr, S. (1998). American child care today. Ameri- Zeanah, C. H., Jr., Bakshi, S., Boris, N. W., &
can Psychologist, 53, 95-108. Lieberman, A. (2000). Disorders of attachment.
Scheeringa, M. S., & Zeanah, C. H., Jr. (2001). In]. Osofsky & H. Fitzgerald (Eds.), WAIMH
A relationship perspective on PTSD in infancy. handbook of infant mental health (pp. 93-122).
Journal of Traumatic Stress, 14, 799-815. New York: Wiley.
Scheeringa, M. S., Zeanah, C. H., Jr., Myers, L., Zeanah, C.H., Boris, N., & Scheeringa, M. (1997).
& Putnam, F. W. (2005). Predictive validity in a Psychopathology in infancy. Journal of Child
prospective follow-up of PTSD in preschool chil- Psychology, Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines,
dren. Journal of the American Academy of Child 38, 81-99.
and Adolescent Psychiatry, 44, 899-906. Zeanah, C. H., Jr., & Zeanah, P. D. (2001). To-
Sroufe, L.A. (1989). Relationships, self and individ- wards a definition of infant mental health. Zero
ual adaptation. In A. J. Sameroff & R. N. Emde to Three, 22, 13-20.
(Eds.), Relationship disturbances in early child- Zeanah, P. D., Gleason, M. M., & Zeanah, C. H.,
hood (pp. 70-94). New York: Basic Books. Jr. (2008). Infant mental health. In M. M. Haith
Sroufe, L. A. (1997). Psychopathology as an out- & ]. B. Benson (Eds.), Encyclopedia of infant
come of development. Development and Psycho- and early childhood development (pp. 301-311).
pathology, 9, 251-268. New York: Elsevier.
Sroufe, L. A., & Rutter, M. (2000). Developmental Zeanah, P. D., Stafford, B., Nagle, G., & Rice, T.
psychopathology: Concepts and challenges. De- (2005). Addressing social emotional develop-
velopment and Psychopathology, 12, 265-296. ment and infant mental health. In Building early
Steele, H., Steele, M., & Fonagy, P. (1996). Associa- childhood comprehensive systems series (Vol.
tions among attachment classifications of moth- 12). Los Angeles: National Center for Infant and
ers, fathers, and their infants. Child Develop- Early Childhood Health Policy.
ment, 67, 541-555. Zeanah, P. D., Stafford, B., & Zeanah, C. H., Jr.
Stern, D. N. (1995). The motherhood constellation. (2005). Clinical interventions in infant mental
New York: Basic Books. health: A selective review. In Building state early
Toth, S. L., Maughan, A., Manly, J. T., Spagnola, childhood comprehensive systems series (Vol.
M., & Cicchetti, D. (2002). The relative efficacy 13). Los Angeles: National Center for Infant and
of two interventions in altering maltreated pre- Early Childhood Health Policy.
school children's representational models: Impli- Zero to Three. (2001). Definition of infant mental
cations for attachment theory. Development and health. Washington, DC: Zero to Three Infant
Psychopathology, 14, 877-908. Mental Health Steering Committee.
Toth, S. L., Rogosch, F. A., Manly, J. T., & Cic- Zero to Three. (2005). Diagnostic classification
chetti, D. (2006). The efficacy of toddler-parent of mental health and developmental disorders
psychotherapy to reorganize attachment in the of infancy and early childhood, revised (DC:0-
young offspring of mothers with major depres- 3R). Washington, DC: Zero to Three Press.

View publication stats

Você também pode gostar