Você está na página 1de 13

10.

1177/0146167203256374
PERSONALITY
Davila, Sargent /AND
LIFESOCIAL
EVENTSPSYCHOLOGY
AND ATTACHMENT
BULLETIN
SECURITY ARTICLE

The Meaning of Life (Events) Predicts


Changes in Attachment Security

Joanne Davila
State University of New York, Stony Brook
Erica Sargent
State University of New York, Buffalo

Building on prior research, which has failed to find consistent sured as categorical patterns or as levels of security, changes
effects of life events on change in self-reported adult attachment over time (e.g., Baldwin & Fehr, 1995; Baldwin, Keelan, Fehr,
security over time, the present study tested the hypothesis that it is Enns, & Koh-Rangarajoo, 1996; Davila, Burge, & Ham-
the meaning people assign to events, rather than the objective men, 1997; Davila, Karney, & Bradbury, 1999; Pierce &
features of events, that is associated with changing levels of secu- Lydon, 2001). Therefore, in addition to guiding inter-
rity. Participants engaged in an 8-week daily diary study, dur- personal functioning, adult attachment also may be
ing which they completed daily self-report measures of attach- guided by the interpersonal environment. This is consis-
ment security, negative life events, perceptions of loss associated tent with Bowlby’s original premise that working models
with events, and mood. Hierarchical linear modeling revealed of attachment accommodate as well as assimilate new
that perceptions of greater interpersonal (but not achievement) information (Bowlby, 1973). It is also consistent with
loss associated with life events were significantly associated with recent conceptualizations of adult attachment security
greater insecurity on a day-to-day basis, even controlling for as a social-cognitive variable that may have some trait-like
objective features of events and for mood. Trait levels of security properties but that also may have state-dependent prop-
did not moderate this association. Results are discussed with erties that are open to and result in change (e.g.,
regard to social-cognitive models of attachment security and the Baldwin et al., 1996; Davila et al., 1999).
utility of understanding the meaning of life events to under- For example, Baldwin and colleagues (e.g., Baldwin,
stand how attachment models may be confirmed or 1999; Baldwin et al., 1996) conceptualize attachment
disconfirmed. patterns as relational schemas, which are defined as
declarative knowledge structures that provide scripts for
interpersonal relating. Their work and that of others has
Keywords: attachment; stress; daily experiences; loss shown that relational schemas can differ across situa-
tions, can be primed by contextual factors, and once
The literature on self-reported adult attachment has primed, affect interpersonal thoughts, feelings, and
behaviors (e.g., Baldwin et al., 1996; Mikulincer, Gillath,
consistently shown that attachment patterns are mean-
ingful individual differences in adulthood and that levels
of security are associated with a variety of important Authors’ Note: The authors would like to thank Jonna Pitts, Dawn
Baumgarden, Monica Howard, Pamela Cocol, Pamela Wickham,
interpersonal outcomes (see Cassidy & Shaver, 1999;
Veronika Peterson, Shiho Manabe, Heather Trigg, Kristin Schulz,
Hazan & Shaver, 1994). Much of this research has consid- Christine Mattison, Tom Lee, and Maribel Montanez for their assis-
ered self-reported attachment in adulthood to be a rela- tance with data collection, coding, and entry; Sandra Murray for help-
tively fixed trait (see also Pierce & Lydon, 2001). And, ful comments on an earlier draft of the article; and the State University
even if researchers have not conceptualized attachment of New York, Buffalo, undergraduates who generously gave their time
to participate in the study. Correspondence may be addressed to Jo-
as such, their measurement of it and research designs
anne Davila, Department of Psychology, SUNY Stony Brook, Stony
have resulted in studying it as such. There are two prob- Brook, NY 11794-2500; e-mail: joanne.davila@stonybrook.edu.
lems with this.
PSPB, Vol. 29 No. 11, November 2003 1383-1395
First, at the conceptual level, there is growing evi- DOI: 10.1177/0146167203256374
dence that self-reported adult attachment, whether mea- © 2003 by the Society for Personality and Social Psychology, Inc.

1383
1384 PERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY BULLETIN

et al., 2001; Mikulincer, Hirschberger, Nachmias, & The present study was designed to address state
Gillath, 2001). Similarly, Davila and Cobb (2003) have changes in self-reported adult attachment security, with
suggested that when we assess self-reported attachment the goal of predicting whether daily experiences
security, we are assessing people’s consciously held account for variance in daily levels of security. In doing
beliefs, which, like any other consciously held beliefs or so, not only can the findings speak to the circumstances
attitudes, are naturally prone to change based on peo- that allow for or promote changes in people’s levels of
ple’s current context (e.g., mood, social circumstances, attachment security but they can also speak to broader
environmental factors) (e.g., Bower, 1981; McGuire, issues regarding the effects of everyday interpersonal
McGuire, Child, & Fujioka, 1978; Morse & Gergen, 1970; experiences on beliefs and feelings about the self and
Nisbett & Wilson, 1977). Hence, a richer conceptualiza- others. There is a growing literature on topics related to
tion of the complexities of people’s beliefs about attach- how self-esteem and social-cognitive processes affect and
ment security and the factors that promote change or are affected by experiences in close relationships (e.g.,
stability over time is necessary. Baldwin, 1999; Leary, Tambor, Terdal, & Downs, 1995;
Conceptualizing attachment security in a more com- Murray, Holmes, & Griffin, 2000; Pierce & Lydon, 2001).
plex way would help bring the adult attachment litera- This literature clearly shows that this is a topic of rele-
ture more in line with how social-personality psycholo- vance to our understanding of interpersonal processes
gists conceptualize other variables that have traditionally and has the potential for important applications in
been viewed as individual differences. For example, the regard to relationship and individual well-being.
literature on self-esteem has moved from conceiving of What type of daily experiences might account for vari-
self-esteem as a stable trait to considering both its trait- ance in daily levels of security? This study tested the
and state-like properties (e.g., Kernis, 1993). Doing so hypothesis that subjective views of daily life experiences,
has yielded important information about how and why or the meaning that people assign to daily events, would
best predict changes in levels of security. One of the orig-
self-esteem fluctuates and has shown that the stability of
inal hypotheses to be tested regarding predictors of
self-esteem, in addition to the level of self-esteem, is a
change in self-reported adult attachment patterns was
unique aspect of self-esteem with distinct associations
that negative interpersonal life events would lead to
with individual functioning (e.g., Greenier et al., 1999;
increased insecurity over time (e.g., Schar fe &
Kernis et al., 1998). Similarly, the literature on personal-
Bartholomew, 1994). Surprisingly, little support was
ity now focuses on personality as a dynamic construct
found for this hypothesis. Except for two studies that
that has both stable and changing components (e.g.,
examined significant romantic relationship experiences
Mischel & Shoda, 1995; Nezlek, Feist, Wilson, & Plesko,
(Davila et al., 1999; Kirkpatrick & Hazan, 1994), no other
2001; R. W. Robins, Fraley, Roberts, & Trzesniewski,
study was able to document associations between life
2001). In doing so, we have developed a better under- events and change in attachment patterns or beliefs
standing of how the individual and the environment (Baldwin & Fehr, 1995; Davila et al., 1997; Scharfe &
interact to produce continuity or change in personality Bartholomew, 1994). This was unusual, especially given
and behavior over time. The literature on adult attach- that studies of change in attachment patterns during
ment may benefit from a similar focus. childhood or from childhood to adulthood generally
The second problem with considering self-reported supported the role of life events in attachment change
adult attachment as a relatively fixed trait is at the mea- (e.g., Beckwith, Cohen, & Hamilton, 1999; Egeland &
surement level. Most studies have examined self- Farber, 1984; Egeland & Sroufe, 1981; Vaughn, Egeland,
reported attachment security at one point in time. Even Sroufe, & Waters, 1979; Weinfeld, Sroufe, & Egeland,
those designed explicitly to examine stability and 2000). This may be due to the fact that such studies used
change have typically examined relatively few data behavioral and interview measures of attachment pat-
points spread over rather long time periods (4 months to terns, which likely tap different aspects of the attach-
2 years) (e.g., Baldwin & Fehr, 1995; Davila et al., 1997, ment system compared to those tapped by self-report
1999; Kirkpatrick & Hazan, 1994; Pierce & Lydon, 2001; (see Bartholomew & Shaver, 1998; Collins & Read, 1994;
Scharfe & Bartholomew, 1994). However, if we construe Crowell, Fraley, & Shaver, 1999; Davila & Cobb, 2003;
self-reported attachment as a more dynamic, social- Shaver, Belsky, & Brennan, 2000).1 Still, it was striking
cognitive construct that may vary over short-term, even that self-reported adult attachment beliefs and patterns
moment-to-moment time frames, then we would need to were not more strongly and consistently associated with
study short-term variations in security to best understand life events. Why might this be the case?
the state-like component of adult attachment (i.e., the One possibility is that it is not simply the occurrence
component that is open to revision and that can change) of events, or the type and number of events, that makes
and the factors to which it is responsive. them salient enough to affect change in adults but rather
Davila, Sargent / LIFE EVENTS AND ATTACHMENT SECURITY 1385

that it is the meaning of the event(s) to the person. in response to life events, especially when assessed over
There is ample evidence that the manner in which peo- relatively long periods of time. It may instead be the case
ple view events, or the meaning that they assign to events, that life experiences are related to changing levels of
is an important determinant of how they will be affected attachment security on a day-to-day basis. Such day-to-
by those events (e.g., Beck, 1967; Lazarus & Folkman, day assessment may capture the natural fluctuation of
1984). Hence, prior research, by examining only the attachment security in response to environmental cir-
type and number of events, may have failed to define cumstances. As such, when examined at the daily level,
events in a way that is truly meaningful to people’s beliefs life events may be associated with fluctuations in levels of
about attachment security. Attachment theorists suggest security.
that attachment beliefs and patterns are likely to change Yet another alternative to the hypothesis that it is the
only in response to interpersonally relevant, emotionally meaning of life events that is associated with daily levels
significant life circumstances (e.g., Collins & Read, of security is that daily self-reported attachment security
1994). To date, researchers have attempted to objec- is a function of daily mood. If this were true, it would
tively define which events fit those criteria. It may be mean that people report feeling more or less secure sim-
more appropriate to consider the subjective experiences ply because they are in a better or worse mood. If the
of the people to whom events occur. The present study findings were to support this possibility, then they would
does so by asking people not only to indicate whether cast doubt on the idea that self-reported attachment
they experienced negative events on a daily basis but also security is a valid, unique construct. However, if predic-
to rate the extent to which each event was associated with tors of daily levels of attachment security were identified,
two types of loss (interpersonal and achievement). above and beyond mood, this would strengthen confi-
Loss, particularly interpersonal loss, was selected as a dence in the validity of the self-reported adult attach-
relevant way to define subjective meaning because of its ment construct. Hence, mood was included as a
centrality to attachment theory and its likelihood for covariate in all analyses.
emotional significance (e.g., Bowlby, 1969, 1973, 1980; Because adult attachment security may have both trait
Brown & Harris, 1978). Attachment theory assigns a and state properties, and because of the extensive evi-
prominent role to interpersonal loss. Proximity and dence for individual differences in attachment security,
availability of attachment figures are key to feelings of we also examined whether attachment security mea-
security, and activation of the attachment system is based sured at the trait level moderated the state-level attach-
on assessments of the availability of the caregiver in times ment effects. Specifically, we examined whether people
of stress. From an attachment perspective, then, inter- who were more or less secure showed different magni-
personal loss is indicative of the unavailability of the tudes of state attachment change over time and whether
attachment figure. We believe that assessments of inter- they showed different associations between events, loss
personal loss resulting from life stress in adulthood may ratings, and daily security. Doing so allows for a consider-
be construed as the extreme perception of the unavail- ation of dynamic/reciprocal associations between peo-
ability of important others and, as such, may result in ple and their environments. Similar to personality and
changes in feelings of security. Therefore, ratings of mood (e.g., Davila, Karney, Hall, & Bradbury, in press;
interpersonal loss associated with daily life events were Hammen, 1991; Snyder & Cantor, 1998), daily attach-
expected to be associated with daily levels of security. ment security may be both responsive to and have an
Data on achievement loss also were collected to deter- effect on daily experiences as people both construct and
mine whether it was interpersonal loss specifically that react to their circumstances. Identifying such associa-
was associated with security, rather than loss more gener- tions may shed light on how working models accommo-
ally defined. date and assimilate information on a day-to-day basis.
An alternative to this hypothesis is that it is the objec- To summarize, this study tested the hypothesis that
tive features of the life events (e.g., type, number) that changes in daily levels of self-reported attachment secu-
are associated with fluctuation in daily levels of security. rity would be associated with the meaning that people
Although prior research largely rules against this possi- assign to daily life events. Specifically, levels of security
bility, prior findings may be due to conceptual and meth- should vary to the extent that events are viewed as involv-
odological limits. As noted earlier, recent social- ing more or less interpersonal (but not achievement)
cognitive conceptualizations of self-reported adult loss. In addition, the study attempted to rule out two
attachment security emphasize the malleable nature of alternative hypotheses. First, that it is the objective,
people’s beliefs about security that renders those beliefs rather than subjective, features of the events that are
prone to constant change. If self-reported adult attach- associated with changing levels of security, and second,
ment beliefs and patterns are construed in this way, then that it is daily mood that best predicts daily levels of secu-
it is not surprising that they do not show lasting change rity. Effects of trait security on the proposed associations
1386 PERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY BULLETIN

also were examined. These hypotheses were tested in an subscales: close, which measures the extent to which
8-week daily diary study in which participants made daily people feel comfortable being close to others; depend,
ratings of life events, the meaning of those events (e.g., which measures the extent to which people are comfort-
loss ratings), mood, and their level of security. able relying on others and believe that others are
dependable; and anxiety, which assesses fears about
METHOD abandonment and of being unloved. Many attachment
scholars consider avoidance of intimacy and anxiety
Procedure
about abandonment to be the two dimensions underly-
Participants took part in an 8-week (56-day) daily ing all attachment styles (e.g., Brennan, Clark, & Shaver,
diary study during the last 10 weeks of the 2001 spring 1998; see Crowell et al., 1999). The close and depend
semester at State University of New York (SUNY), Buf- subscales tap aspects of avoidance of intimacy. As such,
falo. Participants attended an initial in-person labora- their mean was computed as a measure of comfort with
tory session during which they were instructed about intimacy (low scores = avoidance of intimacy). In an
daily diary completion. They were instructed to com- attempt to capture state-like beliefs about security rather
plete one diary each evening for the next 56 days. Diaries than trait-like ones, each day, participants were
were to be returned twice weekly to a drop box in the Psy- instructed to “think about all of your relationships and
chology Department. When diaries were not returned, a respond in terms of how you feel RIGHT NOW.”
reminder phone call was made to participants. Partici-
To measure trait-like security, the means of anxiety
pants were informed that everyone who turned in all dia-
about abandonment and comfort with intimacy were cal-
ries on time would be entered into a lottery to win $100.
culated across the 56 days of the study. Trait-like security
During the week following the last day of the study (day
was thus defined as the average security level over the
56), participants returned to the lab, turned in their
study period. Although this differs from how trait secu-
remaining diaries, completed exit questionnaires, and
rity is usually measured (e.g., with a one-time report of
received debriefing information.
security based on how participants generally feel), it
Participants could be argued that the present assessment is actually a
more valid indicator of trait security because it takes into
Participants were 154 undergraduates enrolled in
account the natural variation in security that people
introductory psychology at the University at Buffalo who
experience.
participated in exchange for experimental credit. They
averaged 19.3 years of age (SD = 2.5). Sixty-five percent Mood. Mood was assessed with the Positive and Nega-
were freshmen, 64% were women, and 60% were Cauca- tive Affect Schedule (PANAS) (Watson, Clark, &
sian. Initially, 204 students consented to participate. Tellegen, 1988). The PANAS lists 10 positive affects and
However, 43 were dropped from analyses because they 10 negative affects. For each item, participants were
provided fewer than 10 days of data. Another 7 partici- instructed to report on “how much you have felt this way
pants were dropped from analyses because on exit ques- today” on a 5-point scale ranging from very slightly or not at
tionnaires they indicated that their data were not trust- all (1) to extremely (5). One negative mood score and one
worthy. Of the 154 in the final sample, 140 provided positive mood score were computed by summing the
complete data. Of them, 22 provided fully on-time data negative and positive item ratings, respectively.
and were entered into the lottery. For the 14 (of 154)
who did not provide complete data, the mean number of Negative life events and loss ratings. Each day, partici-
missing data points (days) was 8.7 (SD = 8.2). Among the pants were presented with a list of 60 negative life events
154 participants, the average number of days on which regarding school (e.g., received an F on an exam, paper,
diaries were turned in late was 14.2 (SD = 12.7, median = or project; found out you were unable to get desired
11, mode = 0). Hence, on average, participants turned in courses or program), employment (e.g., reprimanded at
75% of their diaries on time. work; lost job, laid off, or fired), family (e.g., death of a
Measures parent or guardian; conflict, argument, or disagreement
with parent), friends (e.g., left out of activities by friend;
Each day, participants completed paper-and-pencil conflict, argument, or disagreement with friend), close
questionnaire measures of negative life events, loss asso- (romantic) relationships (e.g., romantic relationship
ciated with events, mood, and attachment security. ended; conflict, argument, or disagreement with roman-
Attachment security. Attachment security was assessed tic partner), illness (e.g., injured or became ill), living
with the 18-item Revised Adult Attachment Scale situations (e.g., conflict, argument, or disagreement
(RAAS) (Collins & Read, 1990). Items were rated on a 5- with roommate/housemate), and extracurricular activi-
point scale ranging from 0 to 4. The RAAS includes three ties (e.g., activity run by your group was a failure).
Davila, Sargent / LIFE EVENTS AND ATTACHMENT SECURITY 1387

Participants were instructed to “circle the events that events. Of interest, these mixed events included conflicts
have happened to you in the last 24 hours.” with romantic partners, parents, friends, and classmates,
For each event circled, participants were asked to which have typically been considered interpersonal
make a series of ratings as to how much loss was associ- events in prior research. Other mixed events included
ated with each event on a 7-point scale ranging from no demoted at work, lost job, fired, or laid off. The number
loss (0) to great loss (6). Four items were used to compute of interpersonal, achievement, and mixed events occur-
a measure of total interpersonal loss (How much did this ring each day were used in the analyses.
event result in loss of emotional support that I had or It is important to note that the above-described nor-
wanted to have; closeness or affection I had or wanted to mative event ratings were used for objective event cate-
have; friendship or companionship I had or wanted to gorizing purposes only. Participants in the diary study
have; trust that I had for someone) and four items were made interpersonal and achievement loss ratings for
used to compute a measure of total achievement loss every event that they reported occurred (of the 60).
(How much did this event result in loss of succeeding at Hence, loss ratings do not pertain to any one type of
what I do; accomplishing what I wanted; reaching my event. If interpersonal loss ratings are significantly asso-
goals or fulfilling my hopes for how I do; meeting my ciated with levels of security, it will indicate that it is rat-
expectations or performing up to my own standards).
ings of loss, independent of the objective type of event,
To determine whether it was the meaning assigned to
that predict ratings of security; that is, regardless of
events (based on the loss ratings) rather than the events
whether a person has interpersonal or achievement
themselves that was associated with attachment security,
events (as categorized by objective standards), it is their
it was necessary to group the events into categories con-
perceptions of the amount of loss associated with those
sistent with the types of loss ratings being made (inter-
events that are important.
personal and achievement). Rather than rely on arbi-
trary categorization, an independent sample was asked
to rate each event according to how much interpersonal DATA ANALYSIS STRATEGY
and achievement loss it entailed (using the scales First, between-subjects descriptive and correlation
described above). To ensure the most normative ratings,
analyses were conducted. The rest of the analyses were
participants first completed measures of dependency
conducted to examine within-subject associations using
and achievement/perfectionism (Sargent, 2000).2 Only
multilevel modeling (also called hierarchical linear mod-
loss ratings from participants who scored within 1 stan-
eling [HLM]) (e.g., Bryk & Raudenbush, 1992) with the
dard deviation of the sample means on these measures
HLM/2L computer program (Bryk, Raudenbush, &
were included (n = 96, 67% men, 95% 18 to 22 years of
age, 50% Caucasian, 61% freshmen). Each participant Congdon, 1994). Benefits of this approach for analyzing
in the independent sample was asked to rate 10 of the 60 diary data have been described in detail elsewhere and
events to minimize fatigue. Each event was thus rated by will not be presented here (see Nezlek, 2001). For the
11 to 17 participants. Mean interpersonal and achieve- present purposes, the multilevel approach to diary data
ment loss ratings for each event were then examined. An allows for the analysis of daily covariation between
event was categorized as interpersonal when mean inter- repeated measures of multiple variables (attachment
personal loss ratings were at least one point higher than security, events, loss, and mood) that are nested within
mean achievement loss ratings. Typical interpersonal person. In these analyses, daily security is treated as an
events included romantic partner was disapproving of outcome variable to be predicted by daily events, loss,
you; death of a parent or close relative; separated from a and mood. In addition, to examine temporal associa-
good friend; conflict, argument, or disagreement with tions between the variables, daily attachment security
roommate/housemate; and conflict with coworkers or was predicted from daily events, loss, and mood, control-
supervisor. An event was categorized as achievement ori- ling for level of security on the prior day. As such,
ented when achievement loss ratings were at least one changes in security from day to day are predicted. Multi-
point higher than mean interpersonal loss ratings. Typi- level modeling also allows for the examination of
cal achievement events included received an F on an between-subject differences in the within-subject associ-
exam, paper, or project; found out you were unable to ations just described. In these analyses, within-subject
get desired courses or program; sought-after promotion intercepts and slopes are treated as outcomes to be pre-
or raise was denied; activity run by your group was a fail- dicted by the between-subject variables. Therefore, we
ure; and received rejection of an application to graduate can examine whether individual differences in trait-like
or professional schools. A number of events did not meet security moderate the within-subject associations
these criteria and were thus categorized as “mixed” between, for example, loss and security.
1388 PERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY BULLETIN

RESULTS Yij = β0j + β1j (Time) + rij, (1)

Preliminary Analyses
where Yij is the attachment security (either anxiety about
Between-subjects descriptive data and correlations. Table 1 abandonment or comfort with intimacy) of individual j
presents the weekly means and standard deviations for at time i, β0j is the attachment security of individual j at
attachment security scores, number of life events, loss Time 0 (i.e., the initial level of security of individual j), β1j
ratings, and mood. Because of space limitations, weekly is the rate of the linear change in security for individual j
rather than daily data were presented (daily data can be (over the 56 days), and rij is the residual variance in re-
obtained from the first author). As the table shows, com- peated measurements for individual j, assumed to be in-
fort with intimacy tended to increase throughout the 8 dependent and normally distributed across subjects.
weeks, whereas all other variables showed decreases. On Unexpectedly, for both anxiety about abandonment
average, participants experienced relatively few events. and comfort with intimacy, the linear parameter was
This is not surprising given that the events assessed were reliable and significant. Its sign indicated that anxiety
relatively major discrete ones and not daily hassles or about abandonment (unstandardized coefficient = –0.02,
ongoing circumstances. On average, participants t = –3.49, p = .001) decreased linearly over the 8-week
reported moderate levels of loss (note that loss averages period and comfort with intimacy increased linearly
were computed only for days on which events occurred). (unstandardized coefficient = 0.02, t = 2.93, p = .004). On
Table 2 presents the correlations between the weekly one hand, this may reflect the tendency for people to
attachment ratings from week to week and from week 1 become relatively more secure over time (e.g., Klohnen
to week 8. These correlations are quite strong, suggest- & John, 1998), although 8 weeks is a relatively short time
ing significant stability even in the face of instructions frame in which to see maturation toward security.
designed to pull for instability (i.e., “respond in terms of Instead, the change may somehow be a product of the
how you feel RIGHT NOW”). However, it is important to study design. Completion of the same items daily for 8
note that these correlations reflect relative stability, weeks may result in systematic changes, or the linear
which refers to the consistency of the rank order of indi- change may reflect a general increase in positivity as the
viduals within a group over two time points. Such corre- semester draws to an end. Because these linear parame-
lations, although appearing to indicate high levels of sta- ters were significant, they were included in all subse-
bility, may be misleading because it is possible to have quent analyses. Hence, the primary analyses provide
high relative stability while still having significant indi- information about the association between daily experi-
vidual change. For example, we computed the within- ences and daily security, controlling for the fact that
subject standard deviations across the 56 study days for security increases linearly over time.
anxiety about abandonment and comfort with intimacy, As described earlier, we also were interested in
resulting in measures of within-person variability on whether trait security affects the trajectory of change in
security. The average within-subject standard deviation daily security over time. The above analyses were
was 2.26 (range = 0 to 7.28, SD = 1.18) for anxiety about reconducted including mean security scores (anxiety
abandonment and 3.24 (range = .51 to 9.12, SD = 1.51) about abandonment and comfort with intimacy) as pre-
for comfort with intimacy, suggesting that despite the dictors of the attachment intercepts and slopes. For the
high relative stability in the sample, there is quite a bit of model predicting changes in anxiety about abandon-
variability at the within-subject level. It is this individual ment, mean anxiety scores were significantly associated
change, we would argue, that is of most relevance to with the trajectory of change (p = .008). People with
understanding changes in attachment security over higher mean anxiety scores showed weaker linear
time. The rest of our analyses were designed to examine declines than other people, suggesting that people
such individual change. higher on trait anxiety about abandonment are more
Within-subject baseline trajectory models. Although the likely to stay anxious over time. For comfort with inti-
attachment variables were not expected to show any sys- macy, mean comfort scores were significantly associated
tematic change over time (but rather vary randomly), with the trajectory of change (p = .001). In this case, peo-
baseline trajectory models were estimated to explore ple with higher mean comfort scores showed stronger
this possibility. Specifically, a model of linear change was linear increases than others, suggesting that people
examined. The linear model can be understood as a higher on trait comfort with intimacy are likely to get
within-subjects regression of an individual’s attachment more comfortable over time.
scores onto the time of each assessment. To evaluate this
model, the following function was specified to describe
the data from each individual:
Davila, Sargent / LIFE EVENTS AND ATTACHMENT SECURITY 1389

TABLE 1: Means and Standard Deviations of all Variables by Week

Variable Week 1 Week 2 Week 3 Week 4 Week 5 Week 6 Week 7 Week 8

Anxiety about abandonment 9.07 (5.91) 8.55 (6.03) 8.59 (6.34) 8.49 (6.11) 8.38 (6.25) 8.22 (6.32) 8.32 (6.38) 8.03 (6.36)
Comfort with intimacy 27.83 (7.83) 28.20 (8.03) 28.16 (8.44) 28.04 (8.14) 28.47 (8.26) 28.74 (8.32) 28.79 (8.67) 28.98 (8.64)
Number of interpersonal
events .13 (.36) .09 (.30) .07 (.26) .09 (.30) .08 (.29) .08 (.28) .06 (.25) .06 (.26)
Number of achievement
events .12 (.34) .09 (.32) .06 (.26) .09 (.30) .05 (.22) .06 (.25) .05 (.24) .06 (.24)
Number of mixed events .13 (.37) .08 (.30) .10 (.32) .09 (.31) .06 (.26) .06 (.24) .07 (.28) .05 (.23)
Interpersonal loss 9.86 (9.63) 9.24 (9.25) 11.17 (9.22) 10.22 (9.35) 8.35 (7.54) 8.39 (7.71) 9.11 (8.48) 8.18 (8.08)
Achievement loss 11.42 (9.80) 12.44 (11.46) 10.83 (10.04) 12.00 (9.70) 10.66 (8.03) 11.36 (9.06) 11.51 (9.15) 12.14 (9.20)
Positive mood 27.17 (8.96) 26.77 (9.04) 26.74 (9.68) 25.84 (8.89) 26.53 (9.46) 26.46 (9.45) 26.99 (9.39) 27.53 (9.28)
Negative mood 16.96 (7.03) 16.51 (7.39) 15.72 (6.52) 16.15 (6.97) 15.60 (6.90) 15.33 (6.63) 15.28 (6.40) 15.36 (6.57)

NOTE: N = 154. Standard deviations are in parentheses.

TABLE 2: Correlations Between Attachment Scores Across Weeks ety about abandonment) for individual j, β2j is the num-
ber of interpersonal events of individual j at time i, β3j is
Anxiety About Comfort
Time Abandonment With Intimacy the number of achievement events of individual j at time
i, β4j is the number of mixed events of individual j at time
Week 1 to Week 2 .92 .94 i, β5j and β6j are the positive and negative mood scores of
Week 2 to Week 3 .96 .94 individual j at time i, β7j is the other type of attachment se-
Week 3 to Week 4 .95 .94
Week 4 to Week 5 .94 .94 curity (in this case, comfort with intimacy, entered as a
Week 5 to Week 6 .97 .95 covariate to control for the association between the two
Week 6 to Week 7 .97 .94 dimensions), β8j is the previous day’s level of attachment
Week 7 to Week 8 .97 .96 security (anxiety about abandonment in this case), and
Week 1 to Week 8 .79 .82
rij is the residual variance in repeated measurements for
NOTE: N = 151. individual j. One analysis for each attachment security
variable was conducted.3
Results of the first analysis, predicting anxiety about
Primary Analyses abandonment (controlling for comfort with intimacy
Before examining whether interpersonal loss ratings and the previous day’s anxiety about abandonment), are
were associated with levels of security, analyses were con- shown in the top section of Table 3. As can be seen, a
ducted to examine the effects of daily mood and number greater number of achievement and mixed events were
of daily events on daily security, controlling for the previ- significantly associated with greater anxiety. Interper-
ous day’s security. Building on the baseline model sonal events were marginally associated. Negative (but
described above, the following function, which included not positive) mood was also a significant predictor.
the event, mood, and security variables as time-varying Results of the second analysis, predicting comfort with
covariates (group mean centered), was specified to intimacy (controlling for anxiety about abandonment),
describe the data from each individual. The example showed a similar pattern, although only mixed events
below describes the model predicting anxiety about predicted less comfort with intimacy and both positive
abandonment: and negative mood predicted changes in comfort with
intimacy (see Table 3, bottom section). These findings
Yij (anxiety about abandonment) = β0j + β1j (time) + indicate that objective features of events and daily mood
β2j (interpersonal events) + β3j (achievement events) + are associated with daily levels of security. Specifically, on
β4j (mixed events) + β5j (positive mood) + β6j (negative days that people have more events and report a worse
mood) + β7j (comfort with intimacy) + β8j (yesterday’s mood, they also report lower levels of security compared
anxiety about abandonment) + rij, (2) to the levels of security on the previous day.4
The next set of analyses tested the primary hypothesis
by examining whether loss ratings made a unique contri-
where Yij is the level of attachment security of individual j bution to security, controlling for mood and events. For
at time i (anxiety about abandonment in this model), β0j these analyses, the two loss variables (interpersonal loss
is the attachment security of individual j at Time 0 (i.e., ratings and achievement loss ratings) were included as
the initial level of anxiety about abandonment of individ- time-varying covariates (group centered) in the models
ual j), β1j is the rate of the linear change in security (anxi- described above. The results for anxiety about abandon-
1390 PERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY BULLETIN

TABLE 3: Results of Multilevel Modeling Predicting Daily Attach- TABLE 4: Results of Multilevel Linear Model Predicting Daily At-
ment Security From Daily Events and Daily Mood tachment Security From Daily Loss Rating, Controlling for
Daily Events and Daily Mood
Effect
a
Coefficient t p Sizeb Effect
a b
Coefficient t p Size
Anxiety about abandonment
Intercept 8.711 19.66 .000 Anxiety about abandonment
Time –0.008 –2.53 .012 .20 Intercept 8.710 19.68 .000
Number of interpersonal Time –0.008 –2.52 .012 .20
events 0.138 1.78 .075 .14 Interpersonal loss 0.242 4.87 .000 .37
Number of achievement Achievement loss –0.043 –0.89 ns
events 0.221 2.75 .006 .22 Number of interpersonal
Number of mixed events 0.449 5.78 .000 .42 events –0.124 –1.11 ns
Positive mood –0.000 –0.11 ns Number of achievement
Negative mood 0.222 7.55 .000 .52 events 0.079 –0.64 ns
Comfort with intimacy –0.173 –25.92 .000 .90 Number of mixed events 0.041 –0.33 ns
Yesterday’s anxiety about Positive mood 0.000 0.00 ns
abandonment 0.357 37.59 .000 .95 Negative mood 0.210 7.09 .000 .50
Comfort with intimacy Comfort with intimacy –0.171 –25.59 .000 .90
Intercept 28.252 48.53 .000 Yesterday’s anxiety about
Time 0.006 1.34 ns abandonment 0.356 37.47 .000 .95
Number of interpersonal Comfort with intimacy
events –0.098 –0.87 ns Intercept 28.255 48.52 .000
Number of achievement Time 0.006 1.33 ns
events 0.126 1.08 ns Interpersonal loss –0.278 –3.87 .000 .30
Number of mixed events –0.611 –5.45 .000 .40 Achievement loss 0.113 1.63 ns
Positive mood 0.075 15.08 .000 .77 Number of interpersonal
Negative mood –0.303 –7.16 .000 .50 events 0.109 0.67 ns
Anxiety about abandonment –0.346 –25.44 .000 .90 Number of achievement
Yesterday’s comfort with events 0.157 0.88 ns
intimacy 0.304 32.15 .000 .93 Number of mixed events –0.222 –1.26 ns
Positive mood 0.075 14.99 .000 .77
NOTE: N = 154, df = 153. Negative mood –0.294 –6.89 .000 .49
a. Unstandardized. Anxiety about abandonment –0.342 –25.09 .000 .90
b. Effect sizes were computed with the following formula (Rosenthal & Yesterday’s comfort with
Rosnow, 1984) and are presented for significant effects only: r = square intimacy 0.303 32.10 .000 .93
root of (t 2 / t 2 + df).
NOTE: N = 154, df = 153.
ment are shown in the top section of Table 4. As pre- a. Unstandardized.
b. Effect sizes were computed with the following formula (Rosenthal &
dicted, higher ratings of daily interpersonal loss were Rosnow, 1984) and are presented for significant effects only: r = square
associated with higher levels of daily anxiety about aban- root of (t 2 / t 2 + df).
donment, controlling for number of events, mood, and
yesterday’s level of anxiety about abandonment. More- accounted for by the amount of interpersonal loss peo-
over, this was not the case for achievement loss ratings, ple perceive in those events. Finally, there was no
suggesting that perceptions of interpersonal loss are evidence that the effects of loss on security were moder-
uniquely related to security levels. Mood did retain a sig- ated by trait levels of security.5
nificant association with security but number of events
(of all types) did not. This suggests that loss ratings Supplementary Analyses
accounted for the effect of events on changes in anxiety An additional alternative explanation of the results
about abandonment. suggests that interpersonal loss ratings are the best pre-
The results of the analysis predicting comfort with dictor of daily security because they assess the same con-
intimacy are shown at the bottom of Table 4. Again, as struct as security. If this were the case, then not only
predicted, higher ratings of daily interpersonal loss were would loss ratings be better predictors of security than
associated with lower levels of daily comfort with inti- are events (as we have shown) but security would be a
macy, controlling for events, mood, and yesterday’s level better predictor of loss ratings than are events; that is, if
of comfort with intimacy. Again, this was not the case for loss ratings are simply a proxy for security, then they
achievement loss ratings. In addition, although mood would have more to do with security levels than with the
remained a significant predictor, number of events (of actual events with which they are associated. To examine
all types) became nonsignificant, again suggesting that this possibility, we conducted an analysis predicting daily
the effect of events on changes in security is largely interpersonal loss ratings from number of daily interper-
Davila, Sargent / LIFE EVENTS AND ATTACHMENT SECURITY 1391

sonal events, number of daily achievement events, num- selves, would be associated with daily variation in secu-
ber of daily mixed events, daily positive and negative rity. This hypothesis was strongly supported.
mood, the prior day’s anxiety about abandonment, the Before describing the results, however, it is important
prior day’s comfort with intimacy, and the prior day’s to note that consistent with prior research, and with con-
interpersonal loss ratings. The prior day’s security vari- ceptualizations of attachment security as a social-
ables were included because we were interested in how cognitive variable (e.g., Baldwin, 1999; Baldwin et al.,
preexisting feelings of security (i.e., feelings of security 1996; Davila et al., 1999), levels of self-reported attach-
present before the events and consequent loss ratings) ment security did indeed vary over time. Of importance,
affected subsequent loss ratings. A variable for time also this variability was more evident at a within-subject level
was included to control for any baseline changes in loss rather than a between-subject level, suggesting that
ratings over time. The analysis revealed that the event between-subject analyses (i.e., relative stability) may miss
variables were all significant predictors of interpersonal some of the variability that people actually experience.
loss (ps < .001). However, of the security variables, only Moreover, this within-person variability was predicted by
the prior day’s anxiety about abandonment was a signifi- daily experiences, supporting the psychological mean-
cant predictor (p < .05); comfort with intimacy was not (p ingfulness of the variability in adult attachment security.
= .48). This suggests that interpersonal loss ratings are The present study is the first to demonstrate such daily
only partly a function of prior levels of security. They are changes, and their existence suggests that change pro-
also a function of the actual events experienced. In addi- cesses need to be explicitly integrated into our models of
tion, negative mood was also a significant predictor. adult attachment.
In another analysis, we included concurrent security As for the primary hypothesis, as noted above, strong
variables, despite the fact that these ratings were made support was found. Although a greater number of nega-
subsequent to the loss ratings. Not surprisingly, the con- tive life events were associated with greater insecurity
current security ratings were significantly associated with when views of events (i.e., loss ratings) were not included
interpersonal loss (p < .001). However, the event vari- in the model, when views were included, events lost their
ables (and negative mood) also were significant (p < predictive power. Specifically, viewing events as resulting
.001). Of interest, in both analyses, when the trait secu- in interpersonal loss was associated with increases in
rity variables were included as moderators, they did not insecurity (less comfort with intimacy and more anxiety
change the effect of daily security on loss but they did about abandonment) on a daily basis regardless of the
moderate the effect of events on loss. Interpersonal and number and type of life events experienced. As such, it
mixed events had a stronger effect on loss ratings for appears that it is the meaning that people assign to life
people with higher mean levels of anxiety about aban- events that is most associated with changing levels of
donment (p < .002). Mixed events had a stronger effect attachment security.
on loss ratings for people with lower mean levels of com- Moreover, it is not just any meaning that is associated
fort with intimacy in the concurrent analysis (p < .001), with daily variation in attachment security; rather, it is
although in both analyses interpersonal events had a viewing life events as resulting in interpersonal loss that
stronger effect on loss ratings for people with higher specifically affects change. Ratings of achievement loss
mean levels of comfort with intimacy (p < .02). were not associated with security. This further confirms
In sum, although security levels do affect how much the importance of relational loss to people’s sense of
loss people perceive when they experience a negative security (Bowlby, 1980). It is also somewhat consistent
event, which is very consistent with a large body of with other recent models of how interpersonal loss
research showing that attachment security guides inter- affects the self, such as the sociometer model of self-
personal expectations and functioning, interpersonal esteem (Leary et al., 1995). Although focused on inter-
loss ratings are also a function of the objective events that personal rejection, which overlaps only partially with
people experience, particularly for insecure people. interpersonal loss, the model suggests that changes in
Attachment security, on the other hand, is much more a state self-esteem are indicators of the current quality of
function of perceived loss than of objective events, interpersonal relations, specifically the extent to which
regardless of trait levels of security. people feel excluded by others. Changes in attachment
security seem to function in a similar manner—as indica-
tors of the extent to which people feel they have lost nec-
DISCUSSION
essary social support.
This study used an 8-week daily diary methodology to The specificity of the findings to interpersonal loss
examine changes in levels of self-reported adult attach- also suggest that no matter what the event, be it one that
ment security. It was predicted that people’s views of we think of as interpersonal (e.g., a relationship breakup)
events, rather than objective features of the events them- or one that we think of as not interpersonal (e.g., failing
1392 PERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY BULLETIN

an exam), it has the potential to be associated with inter- cumstances—how they define events—to best under-
personal loss and, consequently, with variability in feel- stand the effect of those life circumstances on views of
ings of security. An interesting next step would be to self and others, at least on those views that are con-
examine whether perceptions of interpersonal loss and sciously reported. Bowlby (1973) proposed that people
the associated decreases in comfort with intimacy and both assimilate and accommodate new information into
increases in fear of abandonment translate into interper- their working models. Accommodation and, hence,
sonal/relationship behavior and into further changes in change in level or type of attachment security should
security. Again, similar to the sociometer model of self- result when new information disconfirms existing mod-
esteem (Leary et al., 1995) and to Bowlby’s original els. The present research suggests that a focus on the
hypotheses, when security is decreased it may motivate meaning that people assign to their life circumstances
people to behave in ways that attempt to restore security. may help us begin to identify how working models get
Of importance, the results held even when control- disconfirmed and ultimately changed. Presumably, this
ling for daily mood. Although mood was significantly could be true not only for attachment models but for
associated with variations in security, such variations more generalized self- and other views.
were not only a product of changing moods. These find- Future research might be directed at continuing to
ings lend support to the validity of the adult attachment examine how trait-like models of attachment security
construct. affect people’s likelihood for change and also at examin-
We also examined whether the effects of loss on secu- ing how daily variation in self-reported levels of security
rity were moderated by trait-like levels of security. Of in response to perceptions of life events (i.e., state levels
interest, they were not. Regardless of people’s trait level of security) may be related to more long-lasting change
of security, perceptions of greater loss were associated in trait-like working models of attachment. That both
with decreased security. This was true despite the finding state and trait models of attachment security exist is con-
that, on average, trait levels of security are associated sistent with findings that people hold both global and
with trajectories of security over time in a manner that specific attachment, or relational, models (e.g., Pierce &
suggests confirmation of models. Specifically, people Lydon, 2001). The existence of specific relational mod-
who were most anxious about abandonment were least els implies that people may experience different levels of
likely to show decreases in anxiety about abandonment security depending on their circumstances and the spe-
over time. Similarly, people who were most comfortable cific model that is activated. Moreover, that trait models
with intimacy were most likely to show increases in such may be affected by state models over time is consistent
comfort over time. Therefore, although, on average, as with Pierce and Lydon’s (2001) findings that specific
trait levels of security become more extreme they limit relational models predicted change in global models
the range of change, there is still daily variation at the over time. Hence, people’s varying experiences in and
within-person level that occurs in response to how much perceptions of their interpersonal worlds may affect
people construe interpersonal loss in their daily life cir- their more global, trait models of security over time.
cumstances, and this variation does not differ based on It is important to interpret the present results with the
trait levels of security—all people appear to respond to following in mind. First, participants completed paper-
perceptions of loss with fluctuations in security. and-pencil diaries, which were collected twice weekly.
One concern with our analyses is that our measures of This methodology is comparable to other recently pub-
security and perceptions of interpersonal loss may not lished diary studies (e.g., Armeli, Carney, Tennen,
tap distinct constructs. The supplementary analyses help Affleck, & O’Neil, 2000; Bolger, Zuckerman, & Kessler,
allay this concern at least to some degree. If the measures 2000; Nezlek & Leary, 2002; Pierce & Lydon, 2001). In
were not distinct, then not only would loss ratings be the addition, steps were taken to ensure timely completion
best predictor of security compared to events (which we of diaries (e.g., phone calls, lottery) and the majority of
found) but security would be the best predictor of loss the daily diaries were turned in on time. However, unlike
ratings. The latter was not the case. Instead, loss ratings newer, more technologically advanced methods (e.g.,
were predicted by both security and number of events, palm pilot data collection, internet data collection) (see
suggesting that loss ratings are at least in part a response Feldman Barrett & Barrett, 2001; Shiffman & Hufford,
to objective features of life circumstances (whereas secu- 2001), the exact time at which participants completed
rity is not). These findings do, however, support the diaries cannot be determined. Hence, it is possible that
notion of dynamic/reciprocal processes whereby levels some participants may have completed multiple diaries
of security are both a response to and a predictor of daily on the same day rather than one diary per day.
experiences, particularly views of the world. Second, there are other ways of assessing trait security,
In sum, the findings suggest that, in adults, it may be such as interview-based assessments, that may provide
necessary to consider how people construe their life cir- additional information about how trait security affects
Davila, Sargent / LIFE EVENTS AND ATTACHMENT SECURITY 1393

state security over time. Third, the study exclusively or relationship, regardless of whether that relationship
focused on how negative events and perceptions of loss can be construed as an attachment relationship
were related to levels of security. As such, we have no (Baldwin, 1999).
information about whether and how positive events or However, regardless of whether the results are construed
positive perceptions, or for that matter any other type of as pertaining to attachment security or to the broader
perceptions, are associated with variability in security construct of relational security, the take-home message is
over time. This is an important limitation of the present the same. The meaning people assign to life events, par-
work and of the larger literature on attachment stability ticularly the extent to which people believe that they
and change that future research will need to address. have lost interpersonal support or connection, is an
One fruitful way to pursue this issue would be to examine important predictor of people’s daily thoughts and feel-
both positive and negative perceptions about both posi- ing about how comfortable they are being intimate with
tive and negative events. If the logic outlined here is others and how fearful they are of being rejected. This
accurate that it is the meaning people assign to events suggests that to the extent that we can understand the
rather than the events themselves that lead to changes in meaning people find in life circumstances, we may be
attachment security, then any event, depending on how able to better understand what allows them to change or
it is viewed, may be a contender for contributing to remain the same over time.
attachment change. For example, a negative event could
lead to changes toward security if it is viewed in a security-
NOTES
enhancing way or a positive event could lead to changes
toward insecurity if it is viewed in a security-depleting 1. The conceptualization and measurement of different aspects of
the attachment system is a fairly controversial issue, although less so
way. Such an investigation of the meaning of events, both within social and personality psychology than between social and devel-
at the daily level and over longer time periods, has the opmental psychology. However, it is beyond the scope of this article to
potential to shed more light on how people may confirm elaborate these issues in detail.
2. The dependency and achievement/perfectionism measures
or disconfirm their attachment models. (Sargent, 2000) were derived from principal components factor analy-
Fourth, as noted in the earlier discussion about global ses of commonly used measures of sociotropy and autonomy (Beck,
versus specific attachment models, there are questions Brown, Steer, & Weissman, 1991; C. J. Robins et al., 1994) and depend-
ence and self-criticism (Bagby, Parker, Joffe, & Buis, 1994). Internal
about the extent to which levels of attachment security consistency in a sample of 576 undergraduates was .78 for dependency
differ across relationships, with most research suggest- and .83 for achievement.
ing that they do (Baldwin et al., 1996; Bridges, Connell, 3. All results presented from this point forward are from models in
which the time-varying covariates were specified as fixed effects at level
& Belsky, 1988; Cook, 2000; LaGuardia, Ryan, 2. Although it is recommended that time-varying covariates be speci-
Couchman, & Deci, 2000; Lamb, 1977; Main & Weston, fied as random (e.g., Nezlek, 2001), doing so resulted in substantially
1981). In the present study, participants were asked to reduced sample sizes (ns ranged from 81 to 102) and also led to prob-
lems with colinearity and ill-conditioning of the matrices. However,
report about daily security across all relationships, which analyses with the random effects yielded the same pattern of findings as
might best be considered a global attachment model. did those with fixed effects. Hence, the findings were robust regardless
This is reasonable given the goals of the study, which of whether the predictor variables were specified as random or fixed.
4. As noted, mixed events were significant predictors of security but
were to examine how perceptions of general interper- interpersonal events were not—a somewhat surprising result. In an
sonal loss for all types of negative events relate to chang- attempt to determine what it is about mixed events that accounts for
their association with security, we conducted a series of additional anal-
ing levels of security rather than to examine loss or yses. First, because the mixed events contained many interpersonal
events specific to any one relationship. However, the conflict events, which could be relevant to security, we divided the
extent to which the present findings also explain mixed events into conflict events and nonconflict events and included
these variables in the analysis. Both conflict and nonconflict events
variability in specific attachment models must be were significant predictors of security, suggesting that it is not interper-
determined. sonal conflict that accounts for the association between mixed events
Finally, related to the above issue, in assessing global and security. Next, because some mixed events were rated as having
high levels of both interpersonal and achievement loss, whereas other
attachment security, the extent to which participants’ mixed events were rated as having low levels of both, we wondered
security ratings referred to an actual attachment rela- whether the events with high levels of both account for the association
tionship (e.g., one that involves secure base functions) is between mixed events and security (e.g., when an event is character-
ized by high levels of multiple types of loss it may have the greatest
unknown. As such, consistent with Pierce and Lydon effect on security). Therefore, we divided the events into those with
(2001), it may be best to consider the present assessment high levels of both types of loss (high/high) and those with low levels of
of security as relational security rather than attachment both types of loss (low/low). We did this first using median splits on loss
ratings across the mixed events (i.e., medians derived from ratings of
security per se. The concept of relational models and mixed events only) and then using median splits of loss ratings across
relational security is less tied to propositions about all events (i.e., medians derived from ratings of all events). In both
secure base functioning in attachment relationships and cases, both high/high and low/low mixed events were significant pre-
dictors of security (anxiety about abandonment and comfort with inti-
more consistent with the notion of interpersonal macy). Hence, the factors that account for the association between
schemas or scripts that could be held about any person mixed events and security remain unclear.
1394 PERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY BULLETIN

5. Some readers might suggest that a more convincing set of analy- Bridges, L., Connell, J. P., & Belsky, J. (1988). Similarities and differ-
ses would predict today’s security levels from yesterday’s loss ratings. ences in infant-mother and infant-father interaction in the
Although on the surface that seems like a better way to tease apart the strange situation. Developmental Psychology, 24, 92-100.
temporal associations between the variables, we believe that such an Brown, G. W., & Harris, T. O. (1978). Social origins of depression: A study
analysis is conceptually problematic. Yesterday’s loss ratings are based of psychiatric disorder in women. London: Tavistock.
on events that people experienced yesterday. To predict today’s secu- Bryk, A. S., & Raudenbush, S. W. (1992). Hierarchical linear models:
rity from such ratings ignores the fact that people may have had com- Application and data analysis methods. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
pletely different events today that result in completely different types Bryk, A. S., Raudenbush, S. W., & Congdon, R. T. (1994). HLM: Hier-
of loss ratings. We are arguing that people’s security today is a direct archical linear modeling with the HLM/2L and HLM/3L programs.
response to what happened to them today and how they construed it. Chicago: Scientific Software International.
Yesterday’s feelings of loss may, therefore, be completely unrelated to Cassidy, J., & Shaver, P. R. (1999). Handbook of attachment: Theory,
today’s feelings of security. In fact, in analyses designed to examine this research, and clinical applications. New York: Guilford.
issue (the same models as described in the text, except including yes- Collins, N. L., & Read, S. J. (1990). Adult attachment, working mod-
terday’s rather than today’s loss), that is exactly what we found. els, and relationship quality in dating couples. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 58, 644-663.
Whereas some readers might argue that this takes away from the impor-
Collins, N. L., & Read, S. J. (1994). Cognitive representations of
tance of our findings because they do not speak to lasting changes in
attachment: The structure and function of working models.
security, we believe that these findings demonstrate an important
Advances in Personal Relationships, 5, 53-90.
aspect of the dynamics of day-to-day interpersonal social cognition. Cook, W. L. (2000). Understanding attachment security in family
Future research will be necessary to determine the extent to which context. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 285-294.
such day-to-day changes are related to broader changes in security over Crowell, J. A., Fraley, R. C., & Shaver, P. R. (1999). Measurement of
time or even to the construction of stability. individual differences in adolescent and adult attachment. In J.
Cassidy & P. R. Shaver (Eds.), Handbook of attachment: Theory,
research, and clinical applications (pp. 434-468). New York: Guilford.
REFERENCES Davila, J., Burge, D., & Hammen, C. (1997). Why does attachment
style change? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 826-838.
Armeli, S., Carney, M., Tennen, H., Affleck, G., & O’Neil, T. P. (2000). Davila, J., & Cobb, R. (2003). Predicting change in self-reported and
Stress and alcohol use: A daily process examination of the stressor- interviewer-assessed adult attachment: Tests of the individual dif-
vulnerability model. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, ference and life stress models of attachment change. Personality
979-994. and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29, 859-870.
Bagby, R. M., Parker, J. D. A., Joffe, R. T., & Buis, T. (1994). Recon- Davila, J., Karney, B. R., & Bradbury, T. N. (1999). Attachment change
struction and validation of the depressive experiences question- processes in the early years of marriage. Journal of Personality and
naire. Assessment, 1, 59-68.
Social Psychology, 76, 783-802.
Baldwin, M. W. (1999). Relational schemas: Research into the social-
Davila, J., Karney, B. R., Hall, T., & Bradbury, T. N. (in press). Depres-
cognitive aspect of interpersonal experience. In D. Cervone & Y.
sive symptoms and marital satisfaction: Dynamic associations and
Shoda (Eds.), The coherence of personality: Social-cognitive bases of con-
the moderating effects of gender and neuroticism. Journal of Fam-
sistency, variability, and organization. New York: Guilford.
ily Psychology.
Baldwin, M. W., & Fehr, B. (1995). On the instability of attachment
Egeland, B., & Farber, E. A. (1984). Infant-mother attachment: Fac-
style ratings. Personal Relationships, 2, 247-261.
tors related to its development and changes over time. Child Devel-
Baldwin, M. W., Keelan, J. P. R., Fehr, B., Enns, V., & Koh-Rangarajoo,
opment, 55, 753-771.
E. (1996). Social-cognitive conceptualization of attachment work-
Egeland, B., & Sroufe, L. A. (1981). Attachment and early maltreat-
ing models: Availability and accessibility effect. Journal of Personal-
ity and Social Psychology, 71, 94-109. ment. Child Development, 52, 44-52.
Bartholomew, K., & Shaver, P. R. (1998). Methods of assessing adult Feldman Barrett, L., & Barrett, D. J. (2001). An introduction to com-
attachment: Do they converge? In J. A. Simpson & W. S. Rholes puterized experience sampling in psychology. Social Science Com-
(Eds.), Attachment theory and close relationships (pp. 25-45). New puting Review, 19, 175-185.
York: Guilford. Greenier, K. D., Kernis, M. H., McNamara, C. W., Waschull, S. B.,
Beck, A. T. (1967). Depression: Causes and treatments. Philadelphia: Uni- Berry, A. J., Herlocker, C. E., et al. (1999). Individual differences
versity of Pennsylvania Press. in reactivity to daily events: Examining the roles of stability and
Beck, A. T., Brown, G., Steer, R. A., & Weissman, A. N. (1991). Factor level of self-esteem. Journal of Personality, 67, 185-208.
analysis of the dysfunctional attitude scale in a clinical population. Hammen, C. L. (1991). The generation of stress in the course of uni-
Psychological Assessment: A Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychol- polar depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 100, 555-561.
ogy, 3, 478-483. Hazan, C., & Shaver, P. R. (1994). Attachment as an organizational
Beckwith, L., Cohen, S. E., & Hamilton, C. E. (1999). Maternal sensi- framework for research on close relationships. Psychological
tivity during infancy and subsequent life events relate to attach- Inquiry, 5, 1-22.
ment representation at early adulthood. Developmental Psychology, Kernis, M. H. (1993). The roles of stability and level of self-esteem in
35, 693-700. psychological functioning. In R. F. Baumeister (Ed.), Self-esteem:
Bolger, N., Zuckerman, A., & Kessler, A. (2000). Invisible support and The puzzle of low self-regard (pp. 167-182). New York: Plenum.
adjustment to stress. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, Kernis, M. H., Whisenhunt, C. R., Waschull, S. B., Greenier, K. D.,
953-961. Berry, A. J., Herlocker, C. E., et al. (1998). Multiple facets of self-
Bower, G. H. (1981). Mood and memory. American Psychologist, 36, esteem and their relations to depressive symptoms. Personality and
129-148. Social Psychology Bulletin, 24, 657-668.
Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment and loss: Vol. 1. Attachment. New York: Kirkpatrick, L. A., & Hazan, C. (1994). Attachment styles and close
Basic Books. relationships: A four-year prospective study. Personal Relationships,
Bowlby, J. (1973). Attachment and loss: Vol. 2. Separation: Anxiety and 1, 123-142.
anger. New York: Basic Books. Klohnen, E. C., & John, O. P. (1998). Working models of attachment:
Bowlby, J. (1980). Attachment and loss: Vol. 3. Loss. New York: Basic A theory-based prototype approach. In J. A. Simpson & W. S.
Books. Rholes (Eds.), Attachment theory and close relationships (pp. 115-
Brennan, K. A., Clark, C. L., & Shaver, P. R. (1998). Self-report mea- 142). New York: Guilford.
surement of adult attachment: An integrative overview. In J. A. LaGuardia, J. G., Ryan, R. M., Couchman, C. E., & Deci, E. L. (2000).
Simpson & W. S. Rholes (Eds.), Attachment theory and close relation- Within-person variation in security of attachment: A self-
ships (pp. 46-76). New York: Guilford. determination theory perspective on attachment, need fulfill-
Davila, Sargent / LIFE EVENTS AND ATTACHMENT SECURITY 1395

ment, and well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, Nisbett, R. E., & Wilson, T. D. (1977). Telling more than we can know:
367-384. Verbal reports on mental processes. Psychological Review, 84, 231-
Lamb, M. E. (1977). Father-infant and mother-infant interaction in 259.
the first year of life. Child Development, 48, 167-181. Pierce, T., & Lydon, J. E. (2001). Global and specific relational mod-
Lazarus, R. S., & Folkman, S. (1984). Stress, appraisal, and coping. New els in the experience of social interactions. Journal of Personality
York: Springer. and Social Psychology, 80, 613-631.
Leary, M. R., Tambor, E. S., Terdal, S. K., & Downs, D. L. (1995). Self- Robins, C. J., Ladd, J., Welkowitz, J., Blaney, P., Diaz, R., & Kutcher, G.
esteem as an interpersonal monitor: The sociometer hypothesis. (1994). The Personal Style Inventory: Preliminary validation stud-
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68, 518-530. ies of new measures of sociotropy and autonomy. Journal of
Main, M., & Weston, D. R. (1981). The quality of toddler’s relation- Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 16, 277-300.
ship to mother and father: Related to conflict behavior and the Robins, R. W., Fraley, R. C., Roberts, B. W., & Trzesniewski, K. H.
readiness to establish new relationships. Child Development, 52, (2001). A longitudinal study of personality change in young adult-
932-940. hood. Journal of Personality, 69, 617-640.
McGuire, W. J., McGuire, C. V., Child, P., & Fujioka, T. (1978). Rosenthal, R., & Rosnow, R. L. (1984). Essentials of behavioral research:
Salience of ethnicity in the spontaneous self-concept as a function Methods and data analysis. New York: McGraw-Hill.
of one’s ethnic distinctiveness in the social environment. Journal of Sargent, E. L. (2000). Personality Orientation Scale. Unpublished manu-
Personality and Social Psychology, 36, 511-520. script, State University of New York at Buffalo.
Mikulincer, M., Gillath, O., Halevy, V., Avihou, N., Avidan, S., & Scharfe, E., & Bartholomew, K. (1994). Reliability and stability of
Eshkoli, N. (2001). Attachment theory and reactions to others’ adult attachment patterns. Personal Relationships, 1, 23-43.
needs: Evidence that activation of the sense of attachment security Shaver, P. R., Belsky, J., & Brennan, K. (2000). The adult attachment
promotes empathic responses. Journal of Personality and Social Psy- interview and self-reports of romantic attachment: Associations
chology, 81, 1205-1224. across domains and methods. Personal Relationships, 7, 25-43.
Mikulincer, M., Hirschberger, G., Nachmias, O., & Gillath, O. (2001). Shiffman, S., & Hufford, M. (2001). Methods of measuring patient experi-
The affective component of the secure base schema: Affective ence: Paper versus electronic patient diaries [White paper]. Invivodata,
priming with representations of attachment security. Journal of Per- Inc.
sonality and Social Psychology, 81, 305-321. Snyder, M., & Cantor, N. (1998). Understanding personality and
Mischel, W., & Shoda, Y. (1995). A cognitive-affective system theory of social behavior: A functionalist strategy. In D. T. Gilbert, S. T.
personality: Reconceptualizing situations, dispositions, dynamics, Fiske, & G. Lindzey (Eds.), The handbook of social psychology (Vol. 1,
and invariance in personality structure. Psychological Review, 102, 4th ed., pp. 635-679). Boston: McGraw-Hill.
246-268. Vaughn, B. E., Egeland, B. R., Sroufe, L. A., & Waters, E. (1979). Indi-
Morse, S., & Gergen, K. J. (1970). Social comparison, self-consistency, vidual differences in infant/mother attachment at twelve and
and the concept of self. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, eighteen months: Stability and change in families under stress.
16, 148-156. Child Development, 50, 971-975.
Murray, S. L., Holmes, J. G., & Griffin, D. W. (2000). Self-esteem and Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and vali-
the quest for felt security: How perceived regard regulates attach- dation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The
ment processes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 478- PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 1063-
498. 1070.
Nezlek, J. B. (2001). Multilevel random coefficient analyses of event- Weinfeld, N. S., Sroufe, L. A., & Egeland, B. (2000). Attachment from
and interval-contingent data in social and personality psychology infancy to early adulthood in a high-risk sample: Continuity, dis-
research. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 27, 771-785. continuity, and their correlates. Child Development, 71, 695-702.
Nezlek, J. B., Feist, G. J., Wilson, C. F., & Plesko, R. M. (2001). Day-to-
day variability in empathy as a function of daily events and mood. Received April 25, 2002
Journal of Research in Personality, 35, 401-423. Revision accepted November 23, 2002
Nezlek, J. B., & Leary, M. R. (2002). Individual differences in self-
presentational motives in daily social interaction. Personality and
Social Psychology Bulletin, 28, 211-223.

Você também pode gostar