Você está na página 1de 8

Journal http://jcn.sagepub.

com/
of Child Neurology

Effects of Methylphenidate on Impulsive Responding in Children With Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity


Disorder
Molly A. Malone and James M. Swanson
J Child Neurol 1993 8: 157
DOI: 10.1177/088307389300800209

The online version of this article can be found at:


http://jcn.sagepub.com/content/8/2/157

Published by:

http://www.sagepublications.com

Additional services and information for Journal of Child Neurology can be found at:

Email Alerts: http://jcn.sagepub.com/cgi/alerts

Subscriptions: http://jcn.sagepub.com/subscriptions

Reprints: http://www.sagepub.com/journalsReprints.nav

Permissions: http://www.sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav

Citations: http://jcn.sagepub.com/content/8/2/157.refs.html

>> Version of Record - Apr 1, 1993

What is This?

Downloaded from jcn.sagepub.com at UNIVERSITY OF WINDSOR on July 12, 2014


Effects of
Methylphenidate on
Impulsive Responding in Children
With Attention-Deficit
Hyperactivity Disorder
Molly A. Malone, PhD; James M. Swanson, PhD

Abstract
The present investigation examined the effects of methylphenidate on impulsivity in children with attention-deficit hyper-
activity disorder (ADHD). A task was designed to measure empirically the ADHD child’s proclivity to blurt out incorrect
answers before giving a final and/or correct response. Twenty-six ADHD children referred for double-blind placebo-
controlled assessment of medication responsiveness and 14 non-ADHD controls were given a visual search word-matching
task to assess impulsive responding. An analysis of covariance showed that ADHD children on methylphenidate made
fewer impulsive errors than ADHD children on placebo. The control group made fewer impulsive errors than the ADHD
children in the placebo condition, but the performance of the ADHD children on medication approximated the perfor-
mance of the children without ADHD. These preliminary findings suggest that the word-matching task may be a useful
tool for assessing impulsive responding and determining the benefits of stimulant medication on impulsivity. ( J Child Neu-
rol
1993;8:157-163).

mpulsivity is one of the primary features con- quences of an action are disregarded and there is a
Isidered to characterize children with attention- lack of tolerance for any delay in gratification, and a
deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). 1,2 Although cognitive impulsivity, in which answers are given
this has been recognized for many years, there contin- without adequate consideration of alternative re-
ues to be a need for the development of methods to sponses. The Matching Familiar Figures Test (MFFT)
adequately assess ADHD symptoms such as impul- was designed to assess cognitive impulsivity.9 There

sivity, as well as different aspects of attention and have been inconsistent findings, however, with
overactivity.3 Evidence from follow-up studies has in- some reports of a fast, inaccurate response style in
dicated that the poor inhibitory control of ADHD chil- children with ADHD10 and other investigations indi-
dren persists into adolescence and adulthood.4-5 cating greater inaccuracy but no change in the MFFT
Deficits in self-regulatory behavior (and thus the re- response time. 11-13 Similarly, reports of stimulant
sulting impulsive response tendencies) have been as- treatment effects have been mixed with some re-
sociated with, and may directly produce, a number of searchers finding decreased errors but no change in
problems, including low academic achievement, peer response latency following medication,14,15 while
rejection, and delinquency.6-8 others have found a decrease in MFFT errors and an
Two classes of impulsiveness have been identi- increase in response time.l6-ls
fied : a behavioral impulsivity, in which the conse- A pattern of high MFFT errors with an absence
of concomitant fast responses in ADHD children
Received Jan 27, 1992. Received revised Aug 10, 1992. Ac-
may be indicative of reduced efficiency in cognitive
cepted for publication Aug 17, 1992.
From the Division of Neurology, The Hospital For Sick Chil- processing.l9 For example, the effects of meth-
dren, Toronto, Canada (Dr Malone), and the Department of Pedi- ylphenidate on self-paced learning has shown that
atrics, University of California, Irvine, CA (Dr Swanson). ADHD children take about the same amount of time
Address correspondence to Dr Malone, Child Development
Clinic, Division of Neurology, The Hospital For Sick Children,
on drug as on placebo but learn more on drug.20 Eye
555 University Avenue, Toronto, Ontario, M5G lX8 Canada. movements of children performing the MFFT have

157

Downloaded from jcn.sagepub.com at UNIVERSITY OF WINDSOR on July 12, 2014


shown that impulsive children make fewer system- manifested by responding prematurely. We wanted
aticcomparisons when matching stimuli2l and seem to try to in a laboratory task the &dquo;jumping-
capture
to demonstrate more global search patterns.22 Hy- the-gun&dquo; phenomenon exhibited by ADHD chil-
peractive children tested on placebo and meth- dren. A visual search word-matching task was
ylphenidate using a visual matching-to-sample task developed and used as a context to operationally de-
were found to be more reflective on drug as indi- fine impulsive responding. Specifically, impulsive
cated by the increased number of systematic com- errors were identified as all incorrect (impulsive) re-

parisons, although no apparent benefit of drug was sponses made by the child before a final response
found (ie, errors were not reduced in conjunction was given during performance of a word-matching
with the increased attentional selectivity).23 task. It was predicted that methylphenidate would
A number of other measures have been used to lead to a decrease in impulsive responses. A de-
examine deficits in response inhibition and to deter- crease in actual errors (final incorrect responses) as a
mine medication effectiveness in ADHD children. function of medication was considered a possible,
Tasks include the Continuous Performance Test,24,25 but not necessary, outcome because these errors
a
Porteus Maze Test,12 delayed reaction time task,16 may not adequately represent a primary deficit in
delayed response task26 stopping task, 21 finger the ability to exert inhibitory control. The effect of
movement detection ’28 and go-no go task.29 None medication on response time was also explored to
of these tasks, with perhaps the exception of the de- determine what, if any, change occurred. Variability
layed response task,26 have been specifically de- of mean latency was expected to decrease in ADHD
signed to measure impulsivity. Instead they have children treated with stimulant drug.16,2’
been adopted from a variety of neurologic, psycho-
logical, and psychiatric paradigms in an attempt to
investigate impulsivity in ADHD children. The Method
MFFT was designed specifically to examine an im-
pulsive response style, but it is unclear what pre- Subjects
cisely the MFFT is assessing in ADHD children. Twenty-six ADHD children, 21 boys and 5 girls ranging in
Although the pathophysiology of ADHD is un- age from 6 to 13 years (mean, 9.1 years; SD, 1.79 years),
known, various brain structures and neural systems participated in the study. Children had been referred to the
have been implicated in theories that have been of- Child Development Clinic at The Hospital For Sick Chil-
fered to help explain the variety of symptoms that dren in Toronto for inattention/hyperactivity problems. De-

constitute ADHD. Particularly relevant to a deficit of velopmental pediatricians in the clinic had identified these
children as meeting the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of
inhibitory control is the behavioral inhibition sys- Mental Disorders, 3rd ed, revised (DSM-111-R) criteria for
tem,3° which has been suggested to be underacti- ADHD and requested that an evaluation of stimulant
vated in ADHD children.31 When activated, this medication (methylphenidate) be completed through the
double-blind assessment service offered by the clinic.
system functions to inhibit actions in response to In addition to a primary diagnosis of ADHD, a num-
frustrative nonreward situations or when circum-
ber of the children exhibited other problems often found to
stances involve conditioned stimuli for punishment, be associated with the disorder. Nine children were found
and it results in passive-avoidance or extinction. to have learning disabilities as identified by the clinic psy-
ADHD and, specifically, defective response inhibi- choeducational assessment team or by school board per-
tion have been linked to another area of the brain: sonnel. Eight children met the DSM-111-R criteria for
the right frontal lobe, particularly as involved in a oppositional disorder, two evidenced aggressive behavior,
and three demonstrated anxious behavior.
right-sided frontal-striatal system. 28,32 Several inves- Children were administered the Wechsler Intelligence
tigators have also postulated or provided evidence Scale for Children-Revised, unless it had been previously
for involvement of frontal areas in the etiology of given. A verbal or performance IQ of 80 or more was a
ADHD. 33-37 minimum requirement for inclusion in this study. The Full-
Scale IQ for participants ranged from 76 to 131 (mean,
In the present investigation, examined the
we
103.4; SD, 15.8). The Verbal IQ ranged from 80 to 131
effects of methylphenidate on a new task designed
(mean, 103.4; SD, 15.0) and the Performance IQ ranged
to measure empirically the ADHD child’s proclivity from 72 to 136 (mean, 102.5; SD, 16.2).
to blurt out incorrect answers before giving a final Eighteen of the children had been treated with meth-
and/or correct response. We attempted to design a ylphenidate previously, whereas the remaining eight sub-
naturalistic task that would closely reflect the diag- jects had no prior history of treatment with stimulant
medication. Scores from the Abbreviated Conners Rating
nostic criteria for impulsivity in ADHD children, Scale’ were available for each subject. A score of 15 or
which describe the impulsiveness of these children more is considered indicative of an ADHD diagnosis, al-

158

Downloaded from jcn.sagepub.com at UNIVERSITY OF WINDSOR on July 12, 2014


though it is currently recognized that associated features of Fourteen non-ADHD control children, nine boys and
aggression and defiance are overrepresented when this cri- five in age from 6 to 11 years old (mean,
girls ranging
terion alone is used to select ADHD children. The mean 9.1 years; SD, 0.94 years) were also given the word-
score of the teacher-administered Conners was 19.3 (SD, matching task. Children were volunteers from the sur-
7.6). The mean score for the parent-administered Conners rounding communities. All were in the grade appropriate
was 17.2 (SD, 7.2). The
majority of subjects (n 17) ob-
=
for their age.
tained ratings of 15 or above from both the teacher and
parent. Some subjects (n 4) received a high rating from
=

the teacher but not the parent, and conversely, some (n = Materials
3) were given a high rating from the parent but not the The word-matching task required the child to make a vi-
teacher. Two subjects were given ratings below 15 on both sual search in order to find the exact match of a centered
the parent- and teacher-administered Conners. However, word from among similar words scattered randomly on the
both children met DSM-111-R criteria for ADHD, showed page. An example of one of the task pages is presented in
functional impairment, and were being treated with stimu- Figure 1. The task included one practice page plus 18 test
lant medication. pages. The stimulus words were typed on 21.7 x 28.0-cm

FIGURE 1
An example of one of the test pages from the word-matching task..

159

Downloaded from jcn.sagepub.com at UNIVERSITY OF WINDSOR on July 12, 2014


white paper. There were 32 words per page, including the tested on one occasion with one version of the task and a
centered to-be-matched standard word. week later with an alternative version of the task. The 22
All stimulus words used for the task were three-letter ADHD subjects who demonstrated a favorable response to
words following a consonant-vowel-consonant configura- medication at a given (optimal) dosage level during the
tion. Four different words were chosen for each page shar- clinical evaluation of medication responsiveness were in-
ing at least one common letter and frequently two common cluded in the favorable/optimal dose group if they im-
letters (eg, bus, but, bat, and bit). Four variations of case proved on a test battery that included the paired-associate
were then produced for each of the four words (eg, bus, learning task.39
Bus, bUs, and buS). The resulting 16 words were then sim- In presenting the word-matching task to subjects, the
ply doubled to obtain a total of 32 words per page. This experimenter placed each page one at a time on the desk in
was done to increase the difficulty level of the task. That is, front of the child. The 18 test sheets were presented to sub-
increasing the number of stimulus words through which jects in a random order, but the same order was used for
the child must search for the answer was believed to create each subject. Each session began with a practice page. The
a more effortful task. child was instructed to find and point to another word that
The standard word, as well as being centered in the was just the same as the word in the middle of the page.
middle of the page, was also set off by typed lines just For each of the 18 test items, the experimenter recorded all
above and below it to make the examples readily apparent incorrect (&dquo;impulsive&dquo;) responses given before a final re-
to the child. The object of the task was to find the correct sponse, incorrect and correct final responses, and first re-
match embedded in similar but nonmatching words, or sponse latency. The testing time involved approximately 20
distractors. Alternate versions of the task were constructed minutes for a given test session.
for testing under placebo and medication conditions by
varying the case of the standard word and the location of
the answer.
Results

Procedure There were no practice effects observed on the task for


ADHD children were tested in the context of a double-
the primary measure of impulsive errors from initial
blind, placebo-controlled assessment for medication re- testing (mean, 1.0) to repeat testing (mean, 1.30)
sponsiveness. During the clinical assessment, children for the control
group, but a complex drug-
typically attended the clinic on the same weekday once per practice interaction effect was noticed in the ADHD
week. Behavior was measured by various laboratory tasks
and questionnaires involving academic, cognitive, and be- group (F[1,20] 4.51; P < .05). Therefore we de-
=

havioral areas. The purpose of the assessment was to pro- cided to perform a between-group analysis compar-
vide information on whether current methylphenidate ing the data for the 11 ADHD children who were
therapy was appropriate or whether the child would bene- given placebo for the first test session, with the 11
fit from a more extended home trial of drug therapy. Chil- ADHD children who were given medication for the
dren were assessed in the clinic for the entire day for a
first test session, along with the data for the control
given medication (or placebo) condition. Therefore, to
overcome the short half-life of methylphenidate, morning group from test session 1. Table 1 presents the mean
and afternoon doses were given to obtain constant effects values for the dependent measures from the word-
across the day. Test data for the word-matching task were
matching task for ADHD children on placebo, ADHD
collected 2 hours following the administration of the after- children on drug, and the control group. An analysis
noon (12:00) dose of medication/placebo.
of covariance was performed on the data for each of
The placebo and medication test sessions were always
on separate days and usually separated by 1 week. The or-
the seven dependent measures with age, sex, and
der of medication and placebo conditions was randomized learning disabilities as the three covariates. The re-
so that approximately half the subjects received meth- sults showed a significant difference among the three
ylphenidate first and the remaining half received placebo groups of subjects, for impulsive errors (F[2,28] _
first. Children were tested on the fixed dose prescribed by
their physician, and the mean dosage of medication given
8.25; P < .01), regular errors (number of incorrect fi-
was 9.4 mg, with a range of 2.5 to 15 mg. The mean dosage
nal responses) (F[2,28] 4.83; P < .05), total errors
=

by weight was 0.34 mg/kg, with a range of 0.1 to 0.54 mg/ (impulsive, plus regular errors) (F[2,28] 10.36; P < =

kg. A small subgroup of children (n 11) was formed by


=
and correct responses (F[2,28]
.001), 4.83; P < .05).
=

those subjects who failed to show a positive effect and The ADHD group on placebo made the most impul-
were tested on an additional higher dose, or who exhibited
sive and regular errors and the fewest correct re-
an adverse or no response across the variety of clinic tasks
on the prescribed doses. The average dose for this sub- sponses. The ADHD group on medication and the
group was 12.01 mg (range, 7.5 to 15 mg), representing a control group made fewer impulsive and regular er-
mean dosage by weight of 0.42 mg/kg (range, 0.33 to 0.50 rors and more correct responses, with the control
mg/kg). Medication was prepared in capsule form by the group showing slightly better performance overall
hospital pharmacy and was administered at the hospital than the ADHD-medication group. Statistical control
upon the subject’s arrival in the morning (about 9:00 AM)
and at noon. for differences in task performance due to age, sex,
Control children were tested at home. They were and learning disabilities was achieved by the analysis

160

Downloaded from jcn.sagepub.com at UNIVERSITY OF WINDSOR on July 12, 2014


TABLE 1
Comparison of the Dependent Measures From the Word-Matching Task for
ADHD Children on Placebo, ADHD Children on Methylphendiate (Optimal
Dose) and Control Children

ADHD =
attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder.
*Mean (SD).

of covariance; all three covariates were significant at effects of medication on the nonspecific attention
the P < .01 level for the four dependent variables dis- problems of these children. Previous studies with
cussed below. the MFFT 14-18 found overall errors to decrease with
There were no significant differences among the drug administration. However, in those studies no
three groups for response latency (time in seconds attempt was made to break the error score into com-
to first response whether correct, incorrect, or im- ponents, as in the current study, to determine
pulsive), within-subject variability (standard devia- whether specific aspects of the error score might be
tion) of response latency, or latency for correct affected by medication.
responses. Overall response latency did not change with
In additional analyses of data for the ADHD medication. However, some evidence for a more
children, latency of impulsive responses under pla- hasty approach to the task was found for the ADHD
cebo was compared to latency for correct responses children during placebo testing. Comparison of the
under placebo (14.9 ± 9.1 seconds versus 18.89 ± reaction time for impulsive responses with the reac-
7.4 seconds). A one-tailed test revealed that impul- tion time for initial correct responses showed that
sive responses were faster than correct responses the ADHD children were faster when they made im-
(t[20] 1.74; P < .05, one-tailed test).
=
pulsive responses than when they made correct re-
sponses. Nonetheless, reaction time for correct
responses in the drug condition was not signifi-
Discussion cantly slower than reaction time for correct re-
Treatment with methylphenidate reduced impulsive sponses in the placebo condition. Thus, treatment
responding in ADHD children on the MFFT-like task with stimulant medication did not seem to slow
designed for this study. By reducing the impulsive down the thinking processes in the ADHD children.
errors, stimulant-induced improvement brought the These data suggest that when ADHD children blurt
task performance of the ADHD children closer to the out an incorrect response, they may do so without
performance of the non-ADHD children. Also, regu- losing any speed in their processing of task informa-
lar errors (incorrect final responses) were signifi- tion. Without medication, they do blurt out incorrect
cantly decreased by drug treatment. The findings of responses more often than when they are on medi-
the present study therefore demonstrated that med- cation. This suggests that medication is operating
ication has specific as well as general effects on per- via an efficiency effect’9,20 rather than a slowing-
formance. It can be concluded that the present study down effect. 16
isolated, in a more precise fashion, the particular de- Response time variability was not significantly
ficiency in inhibitory control that seems to be modi- decreased with medication. Two recent studies
fied by medication in ADHD children. It is difficult have observed decreased variability in task perfor-
to know why medication also affected general errors mance when ADHD children are given meth-
on the task, but this may be related to the beneficial
ylphenidate. The results of these studies suggest

161

Downloaded from jcn.sagepub.com at UNIVERSITY OF WINDSOR on July 12, 2014


that ADHD children perform in a more consistent may be useful in determining the appropriate dos-
fashion on medication, perhaps because they are age. There were no practice effects observed for the
better able to maintain their attentional focus on the control group with repeat testing. However, there
task for a longer period of time. In the current appears to have been some evidence of practice ef-
study, it appeared from the analysis of covariance, fects on the word-matching task for the ADHD chil-
which showed that the age covariate was significant, dren during this study. Therefore, it may be
that variability was related more to patient age, with advisable to use an A-B-A (no-drug/drug/no-drug)
young children being more variable than older chil- design to evaluate the efficacy of medication therapy
dren, than to any specific effect of medication. in individual patients.
In a few studies,16,26 impulsivity has been evalu-
ated using a delayed response paradigm, where an-
ticipatory or premature responses are viewed as Acknowledgment
reflecting poor impulse control. Our word-matching Appreciation is extended to Debra Greenberg for her helpful
task appears to tap somewhat different cognitive comments during the development of the word-matching task.

processes. In this task, the child is not simply wait-


ing to make a response, but instead is engaged in an
active visual search to find the appropriate word References
match. Impulsive errors on the word-matching task 1. Barkley RA: Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder: A Handbook
are presumed to result because insufficient consider- for Diagnosis and Treatment. New York, Guilford, 1990.
ation is given to alternate answers. This may be be-
2. Douglas VI: Stop, look and listen: The problem of sustained
attention and impulse control in hyperactive and normal chil-
cause it is somewhat effortful to continue to search dren. CanJ Behav Sci 1972;4:259-281.
until the correct match is located. 3. Barkley RA: A critique of current diagnostic criteria for atten-
There were not sufficient numbers of ADHD tion deficit hyperactivity disorder: Clinical and research impli-
children with a comorbid diagnosis of learning dis- cations.Dev Behav Pediatr 1990;11:343-352.
4. Minde K, Weiss G, Mendelson N: A five-year follow-up study
ability to examine these children as a specific group, of 91 hyperactive school children. J Am Acad Child Psychiatry
particularly because they did not form a homoge- 1972;11:595-619.
neous group with respect to learning disabilities. 5. Weiss G, Hechtman LT: Hyperactive Children Grown Up. New
Of the 22 ADHD children exhibiting a favorable York, Guilford, 1986.
6. Blackman S, Goldstein K: Cognitive styles and learning dis-
response to medication, eight were identified as abilities. Child Dev 1982;15:106-115.
having a comorbid learning disability. Five of these 7. Pelham WE, Bender ME: Peer relationships in hyperactive
eight children showed a reading/language prob- children: Description and treatment, in Gadow KD, Bialer I
lem while the remaining three children demon- (eds): Advances in Learning and Behavioral Disabilities, vol 1.
strated visual-perceptual learning problems. Individ- Greenwich, CT, JAI Press, 1982, pp 365-436.
8. Satterfield JH, Hoppe CM, Schell AM: A prospective study of
ual inspection of the data for these children revealed
that those ADHD children with a comorbid visual- delinquency in 110 adolescent boys with attention deficit dis-
order and 88 normal adolescent boys. Am J Psychiatry
perceptual learning disability tended to make more 1982;139:795-798.
regular errors on the task than those with a comor- 9. Kagan J, Rosman BL, Day D, et al: Information processing in
bid reading/language disability, and these errors the child: Significance of analytic and reflective attitudes. Psy-
chol Monogr 1964;78 (1. No. 578).
were often of a visual-perceptual nature (eg, wet for
10. Campbell SB, Douglas VI, Morgenstern G: Cognitive styles in
Wet, or cAn for caN). Children with either type of a hyperactive children and the effect of methylphenidate. J
comorbid learning disability made fewer impulsive Child Psychol Psychiatry 1971;12:55-67.
errors, although medication did reduce impulsivity 11. Brown RT, Quay LC: Reflection-impulsivity in normal and be-
when it occurred in these children as well. Statisti- havior disorder children. J Abnorm Child Psychol 1977;4:
457-461.
cally removing the effect of any type of comorbid 12. Homatidis S, Konstantareas MM: Assessment of hyperactiv-
learning disability with analysis of covariance al- ity: Isolating measures of high discriminant ability.J Consult
lowed the significant effects of medication on task Clin Psychol 1981;49:533-541.
measures to emerge more clearly for the ADHD 13. Quay LC, Brown RT: Hyperactive and normal children and
the error, latency and double median split scoring procedures
group without comorbid learning disability.
of the Matching Familiar Figures Test. J School Psychol
The word-matching task appears to be a useful
1980;18:12-15.
tool for assessing the effect of stimulant medication 14. Brown RT, Sleator EK: Methylphenidate in hyperkinetic chil-
on impulsivity in ADHD children. Data from this dren: Differences in dose effects on impulse behavior. Pediat-
task may help identify children who respond poorly rics 1979;64:408-411.
to medication, or in those who respond favorably, it 15. Rapport MD, DuPaul GJ, Stoner G, et al: Attention deficit dis-

162

Downloaded from jcn.sagepub.com at UNIVERSITY OF WINDSOR on July 12, 2014


order with hyperactivity: Differential effects of methylpheni- ylphenidate on inhibitory control in hyperactive children. J
date on impulsivity. Pediatrics 1985;76:938-943. Abnorm Child Psychol 1989;17:437-491.
16. Douglas VI, Barr RG, Amin K, et al: Dosage effects and indi- 28. Voeller KKS, ALexander A, Heilman KM: Defective response
vidual responsivity to methylphenidate in attention deficit inhibition in attention deficit disorder. Neurology 1990;40
disorder.J Child Psychol Psychiatry 1988;29:453-475. (Suppl 1):410.
17. Rapport MD, Stoner G, DuPaul GJ, et al: Attention deficit dis- 29. Trommer BL, Hoeppner JB, Zecker SG: The go-no go test in
order and methylphenidate: A multilevel analysis of dose- attention deficit disorder is sensitive to methylphenidate. J
response effects on children’simpulsivity across settings. J Child Neurol 1991;6(Suppl):S128-S131.
Am Acad Child Psychiatry 1988;27:60-99. 30. Gray JA: The Neuropsychology of Anxiety: An Enquiry into the
18. Sebrecht MM, Shaywitz SE, Shaywitz GA, et al: Components Functions of the Septo-Hippocampal System. New York, Oxford
of attention, methylphenidate dosage and blood levels in chil- University Press, 1982.
dren with attention deficit disorder. Pediatrics 1986;77: 31. Quay HC: The behavioral reward and inhibition systems in
222-228. childhood behavior disorders, in Bloomingdale LM (ed): At-
19. Taylor EA: The Overactive Child. Philadelphia, JB Lippincott, tention Deficit Disorder, vol 3. Oxford, UK, Pergamon, 1988, p
1986. 176.
20. Dalby JT, Kinsbourne M, Swanson JM: Self-paced learning in 32. Heilman KM, Voeller KKS, Nadeau SE: A possible patho-
children with attention-deficit disorder with hyperactivity. J physiologic substrate of attention deficit hyperactivity disor-
Abnorm Child Psychol 1989;17:269-275. der.J Child Neurol
1991;6(Suppl):S76-S81.
21. Ault RL, Crawford DE, Jeffrey WE: Visual scanning strategies 33. Chelune GJ, Ferguson W, Koon R, Dickey TO: Frontal lobe
of reflective, impulsive, fast-accurate and slow-inaccurate disinhibition in attention deficit disorder. Child Psychiatry
children in the Matching Familiar Figures Test. Child Dev Hum Dev 1971;16:221-232.
1972;43:1412-1417. 34. Gualtieri CT, Hicks RE: Neuropharmacology of methylpheni-
22. Zelniker T, Jeffrey WE: Reflective and impulsive children: date and a neural substrate for childhood hyperactivity. Psy-
Strategies of information processing underlying differences in chiatr Clin North Am 1985;8:875-892.
problem solving. Monogr Soc Res Child Dev 1976;41:1-59. 35. Lou HC, Henrikson L, Bruhn P: Focal cerebral hypoperfusion
23. Flintoff MM, Barron RW, Swanson JM, et al: Methylpheni- in children with dysphasia and/or attention deficit disorder.
date increases selectivity of visual scanning in children re- Arch Neurol 984;41:825-829.
ferred for hyperactivity. J Abnorm Child Psychol 1982;10: 36. Swanson JM, Posner M, Potkin S, et al: Activating tasks for
145-161. the study of visual-spatial attention in ADHD children: A cog-
24. Halperin JM, Wolf LE, Pascualvaca DM, et al: Differential as- nitive anatomical approach. J Child Neurol 1991;6(Suppl):
sessment of attention and impulsivity in children.J Am Acad S119-S127.
Child Psychiatry 1988;27:326-329. 37. Zametkin AJ, Nordall TE, Gross M, et al: Cerebral glucose
25. Vitiello B, Stoff D, Atkins M, et al: Soft neurological signs and metabolism in adults with hyperactivity of childhood onset. N
impulsivity in children.J Dev Behav Pediatr 1990;11:112-115. EnglJ Med 1990;323:1361-1366.
26. Solanto MV: The effects of reinforcement and response-cost 38. Conners CK: Rating scales for use in drug studies in children.
on a delayed response task in children with attention deficit Psychopharmacol Bull 1973;9:24-84.
hyperactivity disorder: A research note. J Child Psychol Psychi- 39. Swanson JW, Kinsbourne M: Should you use stimulants to
atry 1990;31:803-808. treat the hyperactive child? Mod Med 1978;46:71-80.
27. Tannock R, Schachar RJ, Carr RP, et al: Effects of meth-

163

Downloaded from jcn.sagepub.com at UNIVERSITY OF WINDSOR on July 12, 2014

Você também pode gostar