Você está na página 1de 9

Food Quality and Preference 15 (2004) 429–437

www.elsevier.com/locate/foodqual

The effect of expectations generated by brand name


on the acceptability of dried semolina pasta
R. Di Monaco *, S. Cavella, S. Di Marzo, P. Masi
Dipartimento di Scienza degli Alimenti, Universit
a degli Studi di Napoli, Federico II––Parco Gussone, 80055 Portici, Italy
Received 6 July 2002; received in revised form 7 March 2003; accepted 1 July 2003

Abstract
Focus group interviews were performed to explore which characteristics drive consumer acceptance of pasta. A questionnaire
was used to evaluate the consumer expectations of the product generated by the brand name. Consumer tests were performed in two
different experimental conditions (blind and informed) to evaluate whether the knowledge of brand name affects consumer per-
ception or not. Discrepancy between expected and blind preference ratings was found, suggesting that disconfirmation of expec-
tations occurred for some brands. Hedonic ratings were different in blind and informed test, proving a significant effect of brand
information on liking. Consumers generally moved their ratings towards expectation, revealing an assimilation effect.
Experimental results showed also that the knowledge of brand name did not affect sensory attributes perception suggesting that,
for dried pasta, the expectations generated by the brand name are essentially of the hedonic-based type.
 2003 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

Keywords: Consumer expectation; Brand; Dried pasta

1. Introduction for pasta producers must be to invest in marketing and


product innovation. For example there is a growing
Dried pasta represents the 88% of the alimentary interest in consumers in organic pasta, made by small
pasta consumption in Italy. The dried pasta market is factories with a premium quality emphasis. Current
characterized by maturity and by elevated competitive- brand promotion is based on emotional advertising
ness and its growth is nowadays related only to the (Mazza, 2000). An understanding of the factors that
export. The high number of competitors is another pe- drive consumer acceptance of these products, such dried
culiarity of this market. Barilla is the market leader semolina pasta, is an important step in developing a
(with labels: Barilla, Selezione Oro and Voiello), and has strategy for successful promotions, particularly for small
about 35% of production in volume. The most impor- companies.
tant competitors are De Cecco, Agnesi and Divella (4– Studying consumer behavior is a multidisciplinary
6% of production in volume). La Molisana, Amato, task involving food science and technology, nutri-
Buitoni and Granoro follow with lower shares. Market tion, psychology and marketing (Inram, 1999). Percep-
fragmentation is more evident in the South of Italy, tion of food product characteristics has been shown to
where local manufacturers generally fulfill the demand be affected by many individual factors, which include
for low price products. sensory attributes that interact with consumer physio-
A recent aspect of this market has been the growth of logical, behavioral, and cognitive factors within the
commercial brands (Torazza, 2000). Annual pro-capita consumer experiences influencing its perception. The
consumption of dried pasta is of about 24.4 kg which is context and background also affect this interaction
a level is unlikely to increase. In this context the priority (Cardello, 1994).
Among these factors, expectations play an important
role, because they may improve or depress the percep-
*
Corresponding author. Tel.: +39-812539328; fax: +39-817754942. tion of a product, even before it is tasted (Deliza &
E-mail address: dimonaco@unina.it (R. Di Monaco). MacFie, 1996).
0950-3293/$ - see front matter  2003 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.foodqual.2003.07.003
430 R. Di Monaco et al. / Food Quality and Preference 15 (2004) 429–437

Expectation can be generated from external attributes 4. To assess the effect of knowledge of brand on the con-
such as the product information (Siret & Issanchou, sumer preference and on the sensory attributes’ per-
2000), nutritional information (Guinard & Marty, 1997; ception.
K€ ahk€
onen & Tuorila, 1998; Tuorila, Andersson, Mar-
tikainen, & Salovaara, 1998; Tuorila, Cardello, & Le-
sher, 1994; Vickers, 1993), price (Dransfield, Zamora, & 2. Materials and methods
Bayle, 1998), packaging and labeling (Guinard, Uotani,
& Schlich, 2001; Lange, Rousseau, & Issanchou, 1999). 2.1. Pasta
Packaging attributes can lead the consumer to purchase
while sensory attributes confirm liking and may deter- Eleven samples of spaghetti available on the market
mine repeat purchase. Products should, therefore, con- were purchased in a local store. Ten of them had been
vey their sensory properties by their packaging and, by indicated during the focus group interviews as the most
doing so, consumer expectations can be met. Packag- popular ones. They were: Agnesi, Amato, Barilla, De
ing, advertising, product information and, in general, Cecco, Divella, Granoro, La Molisana, Pezzullo, Russo di
image attributes generate consumer expectations, and Cicciano and Voiello. The other sample chosen was a
if these expectations are not subsequently met by sen- trade label: SMA, commercialized by Auchan.
sory performance of the product, consumer disconfir-
mation may occur (Murray & Delahunty, 2000). The 2.2. Preparation of Spaghetti and serving conditions
relationship between expectations and product perfor-
mance is very critical in the case of novel food; in fact The cooking time of the spaghetti was that recom-
the risk of disconfirming expectations, generated by too mended on the pack. The cooked spaghetti were sea-
positive or incorrect information, could result in a new soned with extra-virgin olive oil (Bertolli Gentile), and
product failure (Tuorila, Meiselman, Cardello, & Le- presented to the consumers in a white plastic plate. To
sher, 1998). avoid any changing in sensory properties of spaghetti
Two general types of expectation may be distin- during the session, all the samples were not cooked at
guished, a sensory based expectation, or a hedonic ex- once, but one at a time, so that each of them experienced
pectation (Caporale & Monteleone, 2001). In the first the same time–temperature history prior to consumer
type, expectation leads the consumer to believe that the assessment.
product will possess certain sensory characteristics and Ten grams of each sample were presented to the
influences subsequent perception during consumption. consumers and mineral water was provided for rinsing.
The latter is related to consumers like or dislike to a
certain degree.
2.3. Consumer science methodology
The present study focused on consumer expectations
related to dried pasta generated by brand name.
2.3.1. Focus group interviews
Brand name is regarded as being very useful when
Three focus group interviews were performed in the
consumers are choosing between competing items.
meeting room of the Department of Food Science at the
During the buying process consumers seek information
Agricultural University of Naples.
from memory and from external environment, process it
The average number of participants in the focus
and store the results of their purchase in memory to be
group was 7, all of them were regular buyers and users
used later in another similar purchase. However they try
of pasta. Participants were randomly selected from a
to use as little data as necessary in order to make their
database, containing consumers living in Campania, and
decision, and this indicates that consumers strive for
they were recruited by phone. Some demographic details
efficiency by processing a minimum of information. It is
of subjects are listed in Table 1.
there likely that the presence of a well-established brand
The questions to ask the participants were decided in
on the label will be a potent influence in formulating the
advance and were formulated in such way to require an
sensory expectations of consumers as well as influencing
open and discursive answer. To make the subjects feel
their choices.
comfortable, the discussion began with generic ques-
The objectives of this study were:
tions, with references to situations of choice and usual
1. To understand which characteristics the consumers of
pasta process to make their choice, by means of focus Table 1
group interviews. Demographic details of focus groups participants
2. To investigate the consumer preferences for dried Focus group No. of participants No. of men Age (average)
pasta. 1 7 3 46
3. To evaluate the expectations generated by brands 2 8 4 54
3 6 3 32
name.
R. Di Monaco et al. / Food Quality and Preference 15 (2004) 429–437 431

consumption, and then the interview continued with The number of samples tasted was six or seven for
more specific questions (Calder, 1977; Casey & Krueger, each session and consumers were unaware of the num-
1994). ber of products to be evaluated. The first sample eval-
To facilitate the discussion about the sensory attri- uated by consumers was not taken into account in the
butes of the spaghetti, at the end of each interview two analysis. This sample is commonly called ‘‘dummy’’ and
samples of pasta (Barilla and SMA) were provided to its role is to define a reference point for all consumers.
the participants, without revealing their identity (Bogue, Although generally consumers are asked to give
Delahunty, Henry, & Murray, 1999). judgements only on the acceptability or preference, it is
Each interview lasted approximately 1 h and was sometimes interesting to get information regarding
audio tape-recorded and later transcribed. sensory characteristics as well. Thus, consumers were
asked to evaluate first, the overall liking of the pasta
2.3.2. Consumers sample on a scale with the ends anchored as ‘‘very bad’’/
Forty-five consumers, all regular users of spaghetti ‘‘very good’’; then its sensory attributes: color on a scale
(25 females, 20 males; age range 20–40), took part to the ranging from too light to too dark; wheat taste on a
study. They were recruited on the basis of interest and scale ranging from absent to very intense; cooking per-
availability and they were also na€ıve in regard of sensory formance on a scale ranging from too uncooked to too
analysis of pasta products. cooked; stickiness on a scale ranging from not at all
sticky to very sticky; wrinkledness on a scale ranging
2.3.3. Consumer questionnaire from very smooth to very wrinkled; capability of bind-
The questionnaire was prepared and distributed to ing the sauce on a scale ranging from not at all capable
the subjects that were assisted during the filling of the to very capable. All scales were nine-point scales.
form. The questionnaire contained demographic ques- Sensory attributes evaluated during the consumer
tions (gender, age, employment, family members, etc.), tests were those mentioned by focus group interviewed.
questions on food attitude and on frequency at meal
preparation, and questions related to pasta buying 2.4. Data analysis
habits (favorite size, usually acquired brand). The con-
sumers were also asked to evaluate, on a nine-point 2.4.1. Focus group interviews
hedonic scale, the expected liking after having visually The focus group interviews were transcribed from the
inspected empty packages of the 11 different pasta audio tape-recording and summarized, and a list of cited
brands. sensory attributes and brands was reported. Comments
made by the participants were included in the summary
2.3.4. Consumer test to give an accurate perspective of the attitudes and
Consumer tests were carried out in the Sensory Sci- opinions expressed.
ence Laboratory of the Department of Food Science at
the University of Naples. The laboratory consists of an 2.4.2. Consumer questionnaire
experimental kitchen, meeting room and eight booths. The consumer questionnaire data were analysed by
Consumer tests were performed in two different ex- means of SPSS v10.1 package. Analysis of variance and
perimental conditions (blind and informed condition). Duncan’s test (p 6 0:05) were performed on the expected
First blind scores were collected and then informed liking data, with brands as sources of variation. De-
ones. Hedonic ratings were provided by the consumers mographic characteristics and consumption habits of
during two blind-tasting and two informed-tasting ses- interviewed consumers were also used as factors in two-
sions held over two consecutive weeks. way analysis of variance.
A practicing session was performed before the test, to
allow the consumers to become familiar with the use of 2.4.3. Consumer test
the hedonic scale. Total session lengths lasted between Analysis of variance and Duncan’s test (p 6 0:05)
40 and 50 min. In the blind condition, samples were were performed on data relative to the hedonic ratings
served monadically on white plastic plates identified by in both blind and informed conditions, using brands as
three random digit codes. In the informed condition, source of variation.
samples were served monadically on white plastic plates To evaluate the effect of information condition, ex-
identified by the brand name. pected minus blind liking scores (E ) B) and informed
To randomize the order of sample presentation, nine minus blind liking scores (I ) B) were calculated for each
different randomized orders were used. All together the sample and paired t-tests were performed in order to
11 samples were randomized first, and then divided in establish if those differences were significantly different
two subgroups (five or six for each session). from zero (Lange et al., 1999).
The presentation orders were the same for the two Multivariate and univariate analyses of variance
experimental conditions. were performed in order to test the significance of the
432 R. Di Monaco et al. / Food Quality and Preference 15 (2004) 429–437

differences between attributes ratings across the two Table 2


different experimental conditions and across the 11 Sensory attributes and brand names mentioned by the focus group
interviewed
brands and to estimate the effect of experimental con-
ditions on each sensory descriptor for each analysed Sensory attributes Brand names
pasta brand. Color Agnesi
Amato
The matrix of mean ratings for the seven sensory
Wheat taste Barilla
attributes across the 11 samples was also analysed by Stickiness De Cecco
means of the principal component analysis on the co- Divella
variance matrix. For the cooking performance and color Capability of binding the sauce Granoro
scores, before performing PCA analysis, the data were La Molisana
Wrinkledness Pezzullo
pre-treated by folding the scales.
Russo di Cicciano
The results of PCA were presented as biplots with Cooking performance Voiello
both the samples and attributes.

trary, they said that the Barilla sample has a better


3. Results and discussion cooking performance and a better capability of binding
the sauce.
3.1. Focus group interviews The attributes and the brand names mentioned in
the course of the focus group interviews are listed in
The focus group interviews were recorded then tran- Table 2.
scribed and summarised. The pasta characteristics
mentioned by the participants to the three different 3.2. Consumer questionnaire
focus groups were quite similar.
In general, the choice of pasta was driven by previous Analysis of variance revealed that the degree of ex-
experience and by the confidence with the brand name, pected liking varied significantly among the 11 pasta
as was hypothesized. All subjects agreed on the impor- samples, ranging from 2.9 to 7.8 (F10;484 ¼ 43:73,
tance of the material used for packaging, in particular p  0:0001).
they said that a transparent pack is to be preferred as In Fig. 1 the mean values of the expected liking scores
this allows one to assess the color of the pasta. In fact for the 11 pasta brands are represented as well as the
according to the focus group participant opinion, the results of Duncan’s test (p 6 0:05). The highest expec-
color is an important attribute to estimate product tations are generated by Barilla and Voiello brands,
quality. followed by De Cecco. High expectations are also in-
Many interviewees felt that price did not influence duced by Agnesi, Amato and Russo di Cicciano brands.
their choice too much, as pasta is not an expensive Instead Divella and Pezzullo received a middle expected
product. However, it was also felt that variability in liking score, whereas low expectations are generated by
price that existed among different brands did not cor- Granoro and La Molisana brands. The pasta SMA
respond to real differences among product qualities. generates the lowest expectations, this is probably due to
Cooking performance was the most frequently men- the fact that SMA is not a popular brand among the
tioned attribute related to pasta quality, the second most
frequently mentioned attribute was the capability of
9 a
binding the sauce. As mentioned before, color was in- ab
8 b
dicated as an important discriminative factor. Stickiness
c c
was indicated as the main defect. 7 c
Expected liking

Consumers declared that they were very interested on 6 d d


the geographical area where pasta was manufactured. 5
e
e
All consumers said to prefer local products to support 4 f
the local economy. Some concern about presence of
3
GMOs and radioactive contamination of wheat was
expressed. 2

At the end of the group interview two samples of 1


Russo di Cic.
Granoro

Pezzullo

SMA
Agnesi

De Cecco
Barilla

La Molisana
Amato

Divella

Voiello

pasta, Barilla and SMA, were presented to the partici-


pants without revealing their name.
By observing and tasting two pastas, some of par-
ticipants noticed differences among them. In particular,
the SMA was judged more wrinkled, more sticky, but Fig. 1. Expected mean liking scores for the 11 pasta brands. Means
also, more tasty (having taste of wheat). On the con- with a same letter are not significantly different (p 6 0:05).
R. Di Monaco et al. / Food Quality and Preference 15 (2004) 429–437 433

Table 3 3.3. Consumer test


Effect of background factors on expected liking
Source of variation p-Values Analysis of variance showed a significant difference
Spaghetti brands 4.8 · 1016 between the sample liking scores when the samples were
Gender 0.344 assessed in the blind condition (F10;484 ¼ 3:14, p <
Age group 0.153
0:001). This differences elicited by sensory assessment
Employment 0.156
Number of people in the household 0.051 were not as large as those elicited by brand name. Fig. 2
Frequency at meal preparation 0.061 indicates that in the tasting condition means range from
Favorite pasta size 0.526 3.6 to 5.1, and only three different groups of pasta are
Food attitude 0.170 revealed (Duncan’s test, p 6 0:05).
Usually acquired brand 0.094
This behaviour suggests that, when the consumers
Spaghetti brands · gender 0.579
Spaghetti brands · age group 0.916 were unaware of the sample identity, they were not able
Spaghetti brands · employment 0.937 to discern among the analysed samples, and they posi-
Spaghetti brands · number of people in the household 0.989 tioned their preference in the middle part of the scale.
Spaghetti brands · frequency at meal preparation 0.752 This is probably due to the similar sensory characteris-
Spaghetti brands · favorite pasta size 0.753
tics of the pasta samples.
Spaghetti brands · food attitude 0.052
All samples, with the exception of Granoro, received a
middle liking score. Granoro, even after tasting it, was
Table 4
the least acceptable pasta. However the pasta SMA,
Usually acquired brands by the respondents to the questionnaire
whose mean expected liking score was very low, once it
Brands Percentage of citations as usu-
ally acquired brand (%)
had been tasted, moved from the lowest liking group to
the highest one.
Agnesi 2.2
Amato 17.8
Analysis of variance applied to informed liking scores
Barilla 57.8 revealed significant differences among the brands
De Cecco 8.9 (F10;484 ¼ 6:17, p < 0:0001). The differences among the
Divella 6.7 samples in terms of overall liking, in this tasting con-
Granoro 0 dition, are a little bit more pronounced than in the blind
La Molisana 0
Pezzullo 0
condition. The results of the Duncan’s test (p 6 0:05)
Russo di Cicciano 6.7 applied to informed-liking ratings are shown in Fig. 3.
SMA 0 Divella is still the most preferred pasta together with
Voiello 0 Barilla, followed closely by Agnesi, De Cecco, Pezzullo
and Voiello; the Granoro and the SMA are the less liked
consumers in the Naples area, and was never mentioned ones.
by focus group participants. When the brand name is known it generates an in-
The subjects rated the expected liking of samples, tense distortion of consumer response to some brands. It
observing their packaging, which were of the same
flexible material except for Barilla that was packaged in 9
a rigid wrap. As mentioned by focus group participants
the packaging has an important role in consumer choice, 8
and the preferred is the transparent one. Both kinds of
7
packaging were transparent.
blind liking scores

As far as the influence of background characteris- 6


a ab ab
tics of interviewed consumers on their expected liking ab ab ab
ab
ab
5 ab
scores, the results of variance analysis are reported in bc
Table 3. Results indicated that demographic charac- c
4
teristics and consumption habits did not affect the
expectation ratings. There were also no significant in- 3
teractions between background variables and spaghetti 2
brands.
Concerning the ‘‘usually acquired brand’’ factor, 1
Russo di Cic.
Molisana
De Cecco

Pezzullo
Divella

Granoro
Barilla

Voiello
Amato
Agnesi

SMA

Table 4 lists the percentage of interviewees who men-


tioned the brands as the most consumed ones. It can be
noticed that Barilla is the most consumed pasta brand,
followed by Amato. No one mentioned Granoro, La
Molisana, Pezzullo, SMA and Voiello as frequently used Fig. 2. Blind mean liking scores for the 11 pasta brands. Means with
brands. a same letter are not significantly different (p 6 0:05).
434 R. Di Monaco et al. / Food Quality and Preference 15 (2004) 429–437

9 Table 5
Effect of expectation on liking scores of pasta
8
Samples E ) Ba I ) Bb I ) Ec

7 Agnesi 1.4 0.5
informed liking scores

a a Disconfirmation Not significant


6 ab ab –
ab ab
bc bc
bc
5 cd Amato 2.2 0.4
d Disconfirmation Not significant
4

3
Barilla 3.1 0.7 –
2 Disconfirmation Assimilation 2.4

1

Russo di Cic.
2.4
De Cecco

De Cecco 0.5
Molisana
Granoro

Pezzullo
Amato

Divella
Barilla
Agnesi

SMA

Voiello
Disconfirmation Not significant

Divella 0.2 0.5


Confirmation Not significant
Fig. 3. Informed mean liking scores for the 11 pasta brands. Means Granoro 0.2 0.1
with a same letter are not significantly different (p 6 0:05). Confirmation Not significant
La Molisana )0.6 )0.2
Confirmation Not significant
is interesting to note that SMA moved to a lower posi-
tion, this means that upon finding out that this pasta Pezzullo 0.4 0.2
had a trade label, some consumers lowered their ratings. Confirmation Not significant
Instead, the Barilla and the Voiello improved their po- Russo di Cicci 1.6 0.0
sitions among the evaluated brands. Granoro is still the ano Disconfirmation Not significant
least liked pasta. )
Table 5 shows the effect of expectations on liking SMA )2.0 )0.7 1.4
scores. When E ) B is significantly different from zero it Disconfirmation Assimilation 

means that a disconfirmation has occurred and when +


I ) B is significantly different from zero it means that Voiello 3.1 0.9 )
brand name affected the liking scores. Then in the case Disconfirmation Assimilation 2.2
of assimilation effect of the brand, I ) E was calculated,


a
when this difference was significantly different from zero, Expected minus blind liking scores.
b
it means that assimilation was not complete. Informed minus blind liking scores.
c
Disconfirmation of expectation occurred for all Informed minus expected liking scores.
*
p < 0:05.
samples except Divella, Granoro, La Molisana and Pez- **
p < 0:01.
zullo. Where present, disconfirmation was negative ex- ***
p < 0:001 (t-test, differences from zero).
cept for SMA, which was perceived to be better than
expected. mation and sensory characteristics of samples as-per-
Providing information on the brand name affects the ceived during the test caused the liking score to be
liking score of Barilla, SMA and Voiello pastas. How- located in between the expected score and blind score.
ever this assimilation was not complete, impying that All regression curves showed the same low value of
both extrinsic and intrinsic characteristics of samples slope. Thus, it may suggest that information effect on
have an impact on the informed liking scores. informed scores was similar whatever the product and
In Fig. 4 the effect of information on liking ratings that the sensory characteristics remain important de-
(I ) B) was represented for each subject as a function of terminants of liking also.
the degree of disconfirmation (E ) B), for all samples To evaluate the effect of the knowledge of brand
that showed disconfirmation, and a regression line was name on sensory attributes perception, multivariate
fitted for each product. The plots confirm that, under all analysis of variance was performed with experimental
conditions of positive and negative disconfirmation, conditions and samples as sources of variation. F -ratios
assimilation takes place, even at a high level of discon- for experimental conditions and brands and their sig-
firmation. These results are in agreement with previous nificance are reported in Table 6. One can notice that the
studies on consumer expectation (Cardello & Sawyer, experimental condition did not affect any sensory attri-
1992; Tuorila et al., 1994). butes except for stickiness. Further univariate variance
The regression equations showed a positive slope. analysis, performed for each brand on each sensory
This suggests that the combined effect of brand infor- descriptor indicated that experimental condition signif-
R. Di Monaco et al. / Food Quality and Preference 15 (2004) 429–437 435

Agnesi Amato Table 6


8 y = 0,25x 8 y = 0,2487x Effect of experimental conditions on sensory attributes scores
2 2
R = 0,107 R = 0,185
6 6
Source of Attributes d.f. F -ratios
4 4 variation
Informed-Blind

Informed-Blind
2 2
Experimental Color 1 1.36
0 0
condition Wheat taste 1 0.002
-2 -2 Cooking performance 1 0.25
-4 -4 Stickiness 1 6.33
-6 -6 Wrinkledness 1 0.36
-8 -8 Capability of binding 1 3.46
-8 -6 -4 -2 0 2 4 6 8 -8 -6 -4 -2 0 2 4 6 8 sauce
Expected-Blind Expected-Blind

De Cecco Brands Color 10 28.28


Barilla
8 8 y = 0,3451x Wheat taste 10 0.86
y = 0,3764x R2 = 0,347
6 R2 = 0,364
6 Cooking performance 10 4.62
4 4 Stickiness 10 2.32
Informed-Blind
Informed-blind

2 Wrinkledness 10 2.41
2
0
Capability of binding 10 2.22
0
sauce
-2 -2
*
-4 ¼ p < 0:05.
-4 **
-6
¼ p < 0:01
-6 ***
¼ p < 0:001.
-8 -8
-8 -6 -4 -2 0 2 4 6 8 -8 -6 -4 -2 0 2 4 6 8
Expected-Blind Expected-Blind to the hedonic ratings (six sensory pasta characteris-
Russo di Cicciano y = 0,3275x SMA tics + overall liking, across the 11 samples) performed by
8 8 R2 = 0,336
y = 0,3284x consumers in both tasting conditions.
6 R2 = 0,240 6
4
In the blind condition (Fig. 5) the first two principal
4
components accounted for 78% of the variance. Liking,
Informed-Blind

Informed-Blind

2
2
0
in the blind test, was positively related to capability of
0
-2 binding the sauce and negatively to stickiness. Liking was
-2
-4 correlated with cooking performance, although to a les-
-4
-6 ser extent. The spaghetti wrinkledness and color did not
-6
-8 affect liking. This result is in contrast to the focus group
-8 -8 -6 -4 -2 0 2 4 6 8
-8 -6 -4 -2 0 2
Expected-Blind
4 6 8
Expected-Blind
results, where respondents reported color to be an im-
portant driver of choice. However in the tasting condi-
Voiello
8 y = 0,2668x
tion the spaghetti have been cooked, and differences in
6
2
R = 0,222 the color were not comparable to those in the raw
4 product.
Informed-Blind

2
0 2.0
-2
-4 Barilla

-6
-8
-8 -6 -4 -2 0 2 4 6 8
Expected-Blind
1.0
Granoro Russo di Cicciano
Pezzullo Color Wheat taste
Fig. 4. Plots of the effect of expectation disconfirm.
SMA
PC2 (32%)

Amato Stickiness Cooking performance


Divella
icantly affected only the sensory evaluation of stickiness 0.0
for the following brands: De Cecco (F1;88 ¼ 4:885, LIKING

p ¼ 0:03), La Molisana (F1;88 ¼ 4:787, p ¼ 0:03) and Agnesi Capability of binding the sauce
Russo di Cicciano (F1;88 ¼ 4:625, p ¼ 0:03). In particular,
De Cecco, La Molisana and Russo di Cicciano result in a -1.0 Wrinkledness

significantly stickier score in the informed condition test


Voiello La Molisana
than in blind one. De Cecco
-1.5
For the other sensory attributes, all wheat taste, in- -2.0 -1.0 0.0 1.0 2.0
dicated significant differences among the samples. PC 1 (46%)
To assess the influence of sensory attributes on con-
sumer liking, principal component analysis was applied Fig. 5. Principal component analysis; blind tasting condition.
436 R. Di Monaco et al. / Food Quality and Preference 15 (2004) 429–437

1.5 cant influence of brand name on consumer response.


De Cecco Voiello
This finding is in accordance with that obtained by
La Molisana several studies. Non-sensory characteristics of food
Wrinkledness Agnesi
products play an important role in the consumer choice
Capability of binding the sauce (Dransfield et al., 1998; Guinard et al., 2001; Lange
LIKING et al., 1999; Murray & Delahunty, 2000).
However knowledge of brand name did not affect the
PC 2 (37%)

Divella
perception of sensory attributes, suggesting that, for
0.0 dried pasta, the expectations generated by the brand
Wheat taste
name are essentially of the hedonic-based type. This
Cooking performance result must be interpreted within the limits of experi-
Stickiness SMA
Color
mental conditions as measures on expected sensory
Russo di Cicciano Barilla intensity were not collected.
Amato
The results of this study revealed also that, when
Pezzullo disconfirmation of expectations occurred for some pasta
Granoro
brands, consumer ratings generally moved toward their
-1.5 expectation, supporting for an assimilation effect.
-2.0 -1.0 0.0 1.0 2.0

PC 1 (46%)
References
Fig. 6. Principal component analysis; informed tasting condition.
Bogue, J. G., Delahunty, C. M., Henry, M. K., & Murray, J. M.
Barilla, Divella, Pezzullo, Russo di Cicciano and SMA (1999). Market-oriented methodologies to optimise consumer
have the best color and exhibit the best cooking per- acceptability of Cheddar-type cheeses. British Food Journal,
formance. De Cecco, La Molisana and Voiello are the 101(4), 301–316.
Calder, B. J. (1977). Focus groups and the nature of qualitative market
most wrinkled pastas. Divella and La Molisana brand
research. Journal of Market Research, 14, 353–364.
exhibit also, the best capability of binding the sauce. Caporale, G., & Monteleone, E. (2001). Effect of expectations induced
Amato and Granoro are the stickiest samples. by information on origin and its guarantee on the acceptability of a
In the informed condition (Fig. 6), the first two prin- traditional food: Olive oil. Sciences des Aliments, 21(3), 243–
cipal components accounted for 83% of the variance. 253.
Cardello, A. V. (1994). Consumer expectations and their role in food
In this condition, liking was strongly correlated with acceptance. In H. J. H. MacFie, & D. M. H. Thomson (Eds.),
stickiness and capability of binding the sauce only. When Measurement of food preference. London: Elsevier Appl. Sci., Cp.
the brand name was known, the cooking performance 10.
was apparently less important to the consumer in for- Cardello, A. V., & Sawyer, F. M. (1992). Effects of disconfirmed
mulating his liking judgment. consumer expectations on food acceptability. Journal of Sensory
Studies, 7, 253–276.
It is interesting to note also, as shown before in Table Casey, M. A., & Krueger, R. A. (1994). Focus group interviewing. In
6, that knowledge of brand name did not affect the per- H. J. H. MacFie, & D. M. H. Thomson (Eds.), Measurement
ception of sensory attributes. In fact, the wrinkledness of of food preference. London: Elsevier Appl. Sci., Chapter 4.
La Molisana, Voiello and De Cecco is again the highest Deliza, R., & MacFie, H. J. H. (1996). The generation of sensory
one. Barilla, Divella, Pezzullo, Russo di Cicciano and expectations by external cues and its effects on sensory perception
and hedonic ratings: A review. Journal of Sensory Studies, 11, 103–
SMA have still the best color and their cooking perfor- 128.
mance, according to the consumers, is still the best one. Dransfield, E., Zamora, F., & Bayle, M. C. (1998). Consumer selection
Amato and Granoro brands were perceived to be the of steaks as influenced by information and price index. Food
stickiest samples, as before. Quality and Preference, 9(5), 321–326.
Divella and La Molisana once more exhibit a good Guinard, J.-X., & Marty, C. (1997). Acceptability of fat-modified
foods to children, adolescents and their parents: Effect of sensory
capability of binding the sauce. properties, nutritional information and price. Food Quality and
These results demonstrate also that consumers are Preference, 8(3), 223–231.
very reliable in evaluating the sensory characteristics of Guinard, J.-X., Uotani, B., & Schlich, P. (2001). Internal and
pasta, even though they do not examine foods as an external mapping of preferences for commercial lager beers:
expert panel does. Comparison of hedonic ratings by consumers blind versus with
knowledge of brand and price. Food Quality and Preference, 12,
243–255.
Inram, N. (1999). The role of visual cues in consumer perception and
acceptance of a food product. Nutrition and Food Science, 5, 224–
4. Conclusions
228.
K€ahk€
onen, P., & Tuorila, H. (1998). Effect of reduced-fat information
The hedonic ratings of consumers changed signifi- on expected and actual hedonic and sensory ratings of sausage.
cantly in the two conditions, demonstrating the signifi- Appetite, 30, 13–23.
R. Di Monaco et al. / Food Quality and Preference 15 (2004) 429–437 437

Lange, C., Rousseau, F., & Issanchou, S. (1999). Expectation, liking Tuorila, H., Andersson, A., Martikainen, A., & Salovaara, H. (1998).
and purchase behaviour under economical constraint. Food Quality Effect of product formula, information and consumer characteris-
and Preference, 10, 31–39. tics on the acceptance of a new snack food. Food Quality and
Mazza, A. (2000). Il richiamo del vecchio continente. Food, 7–8, 74– Preference, 9(5), 313–320.
82. Tuorila, H., Cardello, A. V., & Lesher, L. L. (1994). Antecedents and
Murray, J. M., & Delahunty, C. M. (2000). Mapping consumer consequences of expectations related to fat-free and regular-fat
preference for the sensory and packaging attributes of Cheddar foods. Appetite, 23, 247–263.
cheese. Food Quality and Preference, 11, 419–435. Tuorila, H., Meiselman, H. L., Cardello, A. V., & Lesher, L. L. (1998).
Siret, F., & Issanchou, S. (2000). Traditional process: Influence on Effect of expectations and the definition of product category on the
sensory properties and on consumers’ expectation and liking appli- acceptance of unfamiliar foods. Food Quality and Preference, 9(6),
cation to Ôp^
ate de campagne’. Food Quality and Preference, 11, 217– 421–430.
228. Vickers, Z. M. (1993). Incorporating tasting into a conjoint analysis of
Torazza, V. (2000). Pasta secca: la variante e il riposizionamento. taste, health claim, price and brand for purchasing strawberry
Mark Up gennaio/febbraio:, 167–170. yogurt. Journal of Sensory Studies, 8, 341–352.

Você também pode gostar