Você está na página 1de 5

Epilepsy & Behavior 60 (2016) 81–85

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Epilepsy & Behavior

journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/yebeh

Auditory temporal processing in patients with temporal lobe epilepsy


Azam Navaei Lavasani a, Ghassem Mohammadkhani a,⁎, Mahmoud Motamedi b, Leyla Jalilvand Karimi c,
Shohreh Jalaei d, Fereshteh Sadat Shojaei e, Ali Danesh f, Hadi Azimi g
a
Department of Audiology, School of Rehabilitation, Tehran University of Medical Sciences, Iran
b
Department of Neurology, School of Medicine, Tehran University of Medical Sciences, Iran
c
Department of Audiology, School of Rehabilitation, Shahid Beheshti University of Medical Sciences, Tehran, Iran
d
Department of Physiotherapy, School of Rehabilitation, Tehran University of Medical Sciences, Iran
e
Department of Psychology, School of Psychology, Islamic Azad University, Tehran Central Branch, Iran
f
Department of Communication Sciences and Disorders, Florida Atlantic University, Boca Raton, FL, USA
g
Department of English Language Teaching, School of Medicine, Shahid Beheshti University of Medical Sciences, Tehran, Iran

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: Objective: Auditory temporal processing is the main feature of speech processing ability. Patients with temporal
Received 2 August 2015 lobe epilepsy, despite their normal hearing sensitivity, may present speech recognition disorders. The present
Revised 2 April 2016 study was carried out to evaluate the auditory temporal processing in patients with unilateral TLE.
Accepted 4 April 2016 Materials and methods: The present study was carried out on 25 patients with epilepsy: 11 patients with right
Available online xxxx
temporal lobe epilepsy and 14 with left temporal lobe epilepsy with a mean age of 31.1 years and 18 control
participants with a mean age of 29.4 years. The two experimental and control groups were evaluated via gap-
Keywords:
Temporal processing
in-noise and duration pattern sequence tests. One-way ANOVA was run to analyze the data.
Temporal resolution Results: The mean of the threshold of the GIN test in the control group was observed to be better than that in
Gap-in-noise test participants with LTLE and RTLE. Also, it was observed that the percentage of correct responses on the DPS test
Duration pattern sequencing test in the control group and in participants with RTLE was better than that in participants with LTLE.
Temporal lobe epilepsy Conclusion: Patients with TLE have difficulties in temporal processing. Difficulties are more significant in patients
with LTLE, likely because the left temporal lobe is specialized for the processing of temporal information.
© 2016 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction air release and vocal vibration [3]. The gap-in-noise (GIN) test is a
new test that evaluates temporal resolution [4]. In addition, temporal
Temporal lobe epilepsy (TLE) is one type of epilepsy that can bring ordering or sequencing refers to the processing of two or more auditory
about significant effect on auditory processing. The temporal lobe stimuli in their order of occurrence in time, which has a particular im-
encompasses primary and secondary auditory areas, which require portance in speech perception. One of the tests available to evaluate
anatomical and functional integrity. This integrity is necessary for the ability of temporal sequencing is the duration pattern sequence
correct perception and interpretation of auditory signals. Temporal (DPS) test [5]. It is a specialized test which is sensitive to cortical lesions,
lobe epilepsy can cause numerous electrical discharges in these areas particularly in the temporal lobe.
[1]. Even though patients with TLE may have normal hearing sensitivity, In two studies carried out on patients who underwent anterior tem-
they may show speech recognition or speech processing disorders [2]. poral lobectomy surgery, gap detection threshold was reported to in-
Sensory encoding of temporal characteristics of signals such as duration, crease for the contralateral ear to the lesion [6,7]. In a study on twelve
interval, and the order of stimuli provides information to the nervous participants with brain damage, Musiek et al. revealed that pitch pattern
system for speech perception [3]. sequence test and duration pattern sequence test involve different pro-
Temporal resolution is the minimal time required to segregate cesses of higher auditory areas [5].
acoustic events and plays an important role in speech recognition. For Ehrlé et al. found that patients with left temporal lobe epilepsy
example, in order to discriminate between two phonemes /ba/ and / (LTLE), compared with those with right temporal lobe epilepsy (RTLE)
pa/, the voice onset time (VOT) is essential. This is because the identifi- and healthy participants, had poorer performance in anisonchrony
cation of consonant–vowel syllables depends on the interval between discrimination of auditory rapid sequences [8]. Bamiou et al. reported
that patients with left hemisphere lesions, compared with those with
⁎ Corresponding author at: Audiology Dept., Faculty of Rehabilitation, Tehran University
right hemisphere lesions, had a lower percentage of correct responses
of Medical Sciences, Iran. Tel.: +98 21 88053299. in the DPS test, and both groups had an increased GIN threshold in
E-mail address: mohamadkhani@tums.ac.ir (G. Mohammadkhani). one or both ears [9]. In a study on patients with split brain damage, it

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.yebeh.2016.04.017
1525-5050/© 2016 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
82 A.N. Lavasani et al. / Epilepsy & Behavior 60 (2016) 81–85

was shown that neither hemisphere is able to process temporal patterns acoustic booth and TDH 39 headphones calibrated within one year of the
alone [10]. In another study, it was revealed that patients with temporal evaluation based on the current 2006 ANSI standards. Tympanometry
lobe epilepsy have a processing disorder in duration detection indepen- and acoustic reflex tests (ipsilateral and contralateral) were performed
dent of the side of lesion [2]. Aravindkumar and colleagues studied pa- using a clinical tympanometer (Zodiac 901, Madsen, Denmark) calibrat-
tients with complex partial seizures and mesial temporal sclerosis ed to 2006 ANSI standards.
(MTS) and found that the thresholds of GIN were abnormal for the ma- The GIN and DPS tests were conducted in a sound-treated room.
jority of patients with MTS in at least one ear [11]. Stimuli were routed via a CD and the diagnostic audiometer to TDH-
Since classic gap detection procedures are often time-consuming 39 headphones. Participants were instructed regarding their response
and not available to clinicians, the present study employed the GIN to the GIN and DPS tests by the examiner. Stimuli to the GIN test includ-
test to evaluate temporal resolution [3]. In general, there is little infor- ed a series of 6-second segments of broadband noise that were present-
mation on auditory temporal processing in this clinical population. As- ed at 50 dB SL (sensation level) compared with the speech recognition
sessment of temporal processes are essential for the proper diagnosis threshold separately on each ear. Each of the segments contained
and management of disorders associated with temporal processing anywhere from 0 to 3 gaps of silence. The participants responded to
[12]. These include clinical measures of temporal ordering and temporal the examiner by pushing a button as soon as they perceived a gap.
resolution. In the current study, The GIN and DPS tests were used; both Each segment was separated with a five-second gap, and the order of
of which are sensitive to the measures mentioned. the gap duration was randomized. The durations of the silence
There are many unanswered questions about temporal processing gaps were 2, 3, 4, 5, 6, 8, 10, 12, 15, or 20 ms. The GIN test was composed
abilities in patients with RTLE and LTLE, such as the asymmetry of of four different lists. Each gap was presented 6 times in each GIN list. As
hemispheric performance on a variety of functions. The present study a result, each GIN list contained a total of 60 gaps per list. A practice list
aimed at evaluating auditory temporal processing in patients with consisted of 10 items presented prior to the beginning of the test to en-
unilateral temporal lobe epilepsy utilizing the GIN and DPS tests. sure that the participant understood the task. Participants were
instructed to press a button whenever they perceived the gap. An
2. Methods error response was recorded when the participants missed a gap. Gap
threshold and percentage of the correct responses are two criteria for
2.1. Participants and procedures this test. Gap threshold is the shortest gap duration for which there
were at least “four of six” correct identifications. This performance
The present cross-sectional noninterventional study was conducted was recorded as the participants' threshold for duration. The previous
on 25 participants with TLE with a mean age of ±31.1 years. research [4] supports the sufficiency of only two lists, and thus, the
Participants were comprised of 11 patients with RTLE and 14 with LTLE current study employed only two lists.
as well as 18 age-matched healthy controls with a mean age of The conditions for administrating the DBS test were similar to those
±29.4 years in the Audiology Clinic of the Faculty of Rehabilitation at for GIN (sound-treated room and presentation of stimuli by CD via a di-
Tehran University of Medical Sciences. Experimental participants with agnostic audiometer). The DPS test was composed of three pure tones
temporal epilepsy were diagnosed by a neurologist following presurgical with the same frequency (1000 Hz) and two durations (250 ms or
investigations, including MRI and video-EEG monitoring, and were then short duration and 500 ms or long duration) and a 300-ms interstimu-
referred to the researchers (Table 1). lus interval with 10-ms fall/rise time. Each tone has a longer or shorter
The inclusion criteria were age range between 20 and 50 years, duration compared with the other two tones, and it is presented in 6
normal peripheral hearing sensitivity ≤ 20 dB HL in frequencies from different pattern randomizations (LLS, LSL, SLL, SLS, LSS, and SSL). The
250 to 8000 Hz, bilateral normal tympanograms, and normal acoustic interval between each pattern was 6 s. The test was administrated at
reflexes in both ears. Speech recognition thresholds (SRTs) were also 50 dB SL compared with the threshold at 1000 Hz. Each pattern was rep-
established using standardized spondee words using live voice. The resented 10 times randomly for a total of 60 practice presentations. Each
SRTs were confirmed by pure tone averages in all the participants. ear was separately presented with the stimuli 30 times. Participants
Right-handedness was revealed via Edinburgh Handedness Scale were instructed to respond to the signal patterns verbally (long or
(EHS). Additionally, participants had no previous history of depression short). They were also instructed to guess if they were not certain. The
or brain trauma, which could affect the nervous system. In addition, par- practice list consisted of 10 items, which were presented prior to the
ticipants had no history of brain or brainstem surgery, or otologic, and actual test in order to ensure that the participant understood the task.
audiologic impairment. Subsequently, 30 items were presented to each ear, and participants
All the participants signed a consent form after they were informed were asked to identify the pattern. The data registry criterion for this
about the nature and purpose of the study. The informed consent form test was the percentage of the correct responses.
was approved by the Ethics Committee, Tehran University of Medical
Sciences. Participants underwent an ear inspection by a licensed audiol- 2.2. Statistical analysis
ogist for cerumen impact. Pure-tone audiometry was performed utilizing
a clinical audiometer (AC40, Interacoustics, Denmark) in a sound-treated The SPSS (v. 16) was used to analyze the data. Following the confir-
mation of the normal distribution of the data by Kolmogrov–Smirnov
test, data were analyzed by one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA),
Table 1
paired t-test, and Pearson's correlation coefficient. The statistical
Distribution of epileptic foci based on the hemisphere with temporal lobe epilepsy (25 =
number).
significance was set at p b 0.05.

Groups Location Number Relative 3. Results


frequency

Right temporal lobe epilepsy (n = 11) Anterior 8 72.7% The means and standard deviations of GIN and DPS test parameters
Posterior medial 1 9.1%
in controls and participants with LTLE and RTLE are given in Table 2. The
Occipitotemporal 2 18.2%
Total 11 100% mean of GIN threshold in the right ear and the left ear in the control
Left temporal lobe epilepsy (n = 14) Anterior 8 57.1% group was observed to be better than that in participants with LTLE
Posterior medial 4 28.6% and RTLE, and the percentage of correct responses in GIN evaluation in
Occipitotemporal 2 14.3% the control group was better than that in the participants with LTLE
Total 14 100%
and RTLE. The percentage of the correct responses on the DPS test in
A.N. Lavasani et al. / Epilepsy & Behavior 60 (2016) 81–85 83

Table 2
Comparison of threshold of the GIN test (ms) and the percentage of correct responses of two tests between right and left ears in the three groups of control participants and participants
with RTLE and LTLE (43 = number).

Test measures Mean (SD) in group with LTLE Mean (SD) in group with RTLE Mean (SD) in control
(n = 14) (n = 11) participants (n = 18)

Left ear Right ear Left ear Right ear Left ear Right ear

Threshold of the GIN test (ms) 7.2 (2.6) 6.64 (2.9) 7.18 (2.3) 7.09 (2.2) 5.1 (0.83) 4.77 (0.54)
p-Value 0.18 0.9 0.055
Percentage of correct response [GIN] (%) 58.3 (1.3) 61.06 (1.3) 57.9 (1.3) 59.2 (2.8) 69.2 (8.3) 70.7 (6.4)
p-Value 0.23 0.66 0.28
Percentage of correct response [DPS] (%) 63.5 (2.34) 63.08 (2.2) 93.6 (6.2) 93.6 (6.04) 92.6 (5.5) 94.99 (4.9)
p-Value 0.9 1 0.06

control participants and participants with RTLE was better than that in Significant differences were also observed between the three groups
participants with LTLE. No statistically significant difference was detect- in the DPS test (p = 0.000 & p = 0.000 in the right and left ears, respec-
ed between the control participants and group with RTLE. None of the tively). Moreover, there was a significant difference between partici-
three groups demonstrated a significant difference in the mean scores pants with RTLE and LTLE (p = 0.000 in the right and left ears) and
of the two tests between right and left ears (p N 0.05). between the control group and the group with LTLE (p = 0.000 in the
Based on the studies by Musiek et al. [4,5] and Aravindkumar et al. right and left ears), but no significant difference was observed between
[11], the cutoff criterion between normal and abnormal performance the control group and the group with RTLE (p = 0.96 & 0.97 in the right
was determined by two standard deviations from the mean scores in and left ears, respectively) (Fig. 4).
the control group. The data obtained in the present study showed that There was no significant correlation between the values obtained for
the GIN threshold above 6.16 ms and the percentage of correct re- the DPS test (in percentage) and threshold of the GIN test in the right ear
sponses below 56.32 in the GIN test and 84.67 in the DPS test were con- of patients with RTLE (r = − 0.13, p = 0.7) and control participants
sidered abnormal. Based on the suggested cutoff scores, 56% of the (r = −0.36, p = 0.14) and left ear of the group with LTLE (r = −0.27,
participants with temporal lobe epilepsy (14 out of 25 participants) p = 0.35), according to Pearson's correlation coefficient. The correlation
had an abnormal threshold in the GIN test while 28% (7 out of 25 partic- was observed to be significant in the left ear of control participants
ipants) had an abnormal percentage of correct responses. (r = −0.49, p = 0.04) and patients with RTLE (r = −0.83, p = 0.002)
All the control participants had GIN thresholds better or less than and the right ear of the group with LTLE (r = −0.6, p = 0.02).
6.16 ms. In the group with RTLE, two participants had abnormal scores The ear effect on the GIN and DPS tests was evaluated via paired t-
only in the right ear, two participants only in the left ear, and three test in the three groups. There was no significant difference in mean
participants in both ears. In the group with LTLE, two participants had scores of the two tests between the right and left ears of each group
abnormal scores only in the left ear, and four participants had abnormal (p N 0.05) (Table 2).
scores in both ears (Fig. 1). Abnormal scores in the DPS test were ob-
served in 85.7% of the group with LTLE (11 out of 14 participants in
both ears and 1 in the left ear) and 27.27% of the group with RTLE (3 4. Discussion
out of 11 participants, 2 participants in the right ear, and 1 in the left
ear) (Fig. 2). The present study was designed to evaluate auditory temporal
The ANOVA revealed significant differences between the three processing in patients with unilateral temporal lobe epilepsy utilizing
groups in the mean GIN threshold scores (p = 0.006 & p = 0.007 in the GIN and DPS tests. The results showed that the threshold of GIN
the right and left ears, respectively). This difference was significant be- test was higher and the mean percentage of correct responses in partic-
tween control participants and participants with RTLE (p = 0.01 & ipants with temporal lobe epilepsy was lower than those in control
0.02 in the right and left ears, respectively) and between the control participants. This difference was statistically significant; however,
group and the group with LTLE (p = 0.03 & 0.012 in the right and left there was no significant difference between participants with RTLE
ears, respectively). There was no significant difference between partici- and LTLE. The mean percentage of correct responses in the DPS test in
pants with RTLE and LTLE (p = 0.84 & 0.99 in the right and left ears, the group with LTLE was significantly lower than that in the group
respectively) (Fig. 3). with RTLE and control group, yet no significant difference was found
between the control group and the group with RTLE.

Fig. 1. Frequency of abnormal thresholds of the GIN test (ms) in the right, left, both, and Fig. 2. Frequency of abnormal scores of duration pattern sequence test (percent) in the
none of the ears of participants with right and left temporal lobe epilepsy with cutoff right, left, both, and none of the ears of participants with right and left temporal lobe
criterion of 6.16 ms. epilepsy with cutoff criterion of 84.67%.
84 A.N. Lavasani et al. / Epilepsy & Behavior 60 (2016) 81–85

more in the right hemisphere [15]. The data collected in our study are
compatible with these findings.
In agreement with our study, some studies have shown that left
temporal lobe disorders impair duration discrimination and temporal
perception [16]. Ehrlé et al. stated that auditory rapid and sequence
information processing depends on the integration of left temporal
lobe structures [8].
Also, Bamiou et al. showed that patients with left hemisphere disor-
der, compared with those with right hemisphere disorder, had lower
scores on the DPS test, which is consistent with the theory of cortex spe-
cialization for temporal processing [9]. The data obtained in the present
study are compatible with these findings, too.
Some parts of the right hemisphere may indirectly analyze percep-
tions of time and duration. Duration perception may take place by
some factors unrelated to temporal cognition. The right hemisphere is
Fig. 3. Comparison of the mean of the threshold of the GIN test (ms) between participants specialized for intensity analysis of both verbal and nonverbal sounds,
with RTLE and LTLE and controls. and one probable hypothesis is that duration alterations of brief sounds
(b200 ms) may be perceived as intensity alterations [17,18]. Therefore,
Since left hemisphere disorders lead to a decrease in scores on DPS in the present study, the existence of durations longer than 200 ms in
test in both ears and this test was shown to be insensitive to right the DPS test and shorter than 200 ms in the GIN test could be one of
hemisphere disorders, the DPS test may recognize the site of lesion. the reasons for abnormality in the GIN test, and normal data outcomes
But if there is a disorder in each of the hemispheres, the approximate in the DPS test in participants with RTLE could be attributed to left audi-
threshold of the GIN test will increase in one or both ears, and thus, it tory cortex specialization for temporal ordering.
cannot recognize the site of the lesion. Therefore, if the purpose is to de- The threshold of the GIN test, compared with the percentage of cor-
termine cortical disorders, the GIN test may be more appropriate than rect response index, has a better sensitivity and specificity [4,19]. In the
the DPS test, and when the lesion site is under investigation, the DPS present study, the sensitivity of percentage of correct responses was
test may be preferable. Overall, both tests are useful for the purpose of 28% weaker than that for GIN threshold. In this study, during the GIN
diagnosis and management of auditory processing disorders. test, some patients had abnormal scores in the right ear, some in the
A significant source of information for every auditory signal depends left ear, and some in both ears. However, no significant difference and
on its temporal structure. Perhaps, more than any other type of informa- ear advantage were observed between the mean scores of right and
tion, temporal information is critical in the discrimination and identifi- left ears in the three groups. In the DPS test, too, most of the patients
cation of the signals. The phase-locked activity of neurons enables with LTLE had abnormal scores in both ears, and there was no ear ad-
listeners to discriminate periodic from aperiodic signals, which is im- vantage in any one of the two tests. This finding was similar to those re-
portant in the recognition of complex sounds [13]. Speech processing ported in other studies [1,4,11,19,20].
disorders associated with temporal processing disorders indicate the Efron et al. reported that all their participants with temporal lobe
main role of temporal signs in the formulation and resolution of speech epilepsy who had undergone surgery had an increased threshold in
signals [14]. the contralateral ear to the lesion [7]. The reason for the difference in
The present study revealed that the GIN and DPS tests are processed findings reported in their study and those obtained in the present
differently in the brain and can identify temporal lobe lesions. This find- study can be due to the different types of stimuli used (broadband in
ing is compatible with that reported by Musiek et al. [5]. In a study con- the present study versus narrowband noise in the other study) and
ducted by Zatorre and Belin [15], positron emission tomography (PET) the type of lesion (lobectomy versus epilepsy disease, respectively).
was employed to evaluate auditory cortex response to temporal and Also, it is worth mentioning that our participants did not have any sur-
spectral changes. The results revealed that the core areas of auditory gical intervention for their epilepsy.
cortex of the two hemispheres respond to temporal changes while the Bilaterally impaired gap detection abilities have been reported
superoanterior areas of both temporal lobes respond to spectral chang- secondary to the electrical discharge (seizure) propagation between
es. It is reported that the responses to temporal events are found more the two hemispheres [11]. However, in the previous studies such as
in the left hemisphere and responses to spectral events are observed Bamiou et al. [9], on patients with insular stroke and adjacent areas,
and Musiek and colleagues [4] on patients with central auditory ner-
vous system disorders (cortex and brainstem), bilateral deficit has
been observed. Musiek [4] reported that only 4 patients had
abnormal performance on contralateral ear to the lesion in the GIN
test. Likewise, in the present study, only two patients with RTLE
showed abnormal gap detection performance with the left ear. The
results of the present study are consistent with a previous study car-
ried out by Musiek [4], and the small differences between the two
studies can be related to the differences in locus and the extent of le-
sions and cutoff criteria. Since epilepsy leads to progressive functional
and structural damage of the temporal lobe and the longer duration
of the disease may be associated with a bilateral decrease in hippo-
campal volume, brain glucose metabolism, and memory performance
[2], the differences between the results of our study and those of
other studies can be related to differences in the duration of
epilepsy and, more importantly, to the severity of the disease, locus,
and the extent of lesions. Patients in the present study were pre-
Fig. 4. Comparison of the mean of the correct responses of the duration pattern sequence scribed one or more antiepileptic drugs. Bearing in mind a previous
test (percent) between participants with RTLE and LTLE and controls. study by Han et al. [2], who had shown that antiepileptic drugs do
A.N. Lavasani et al. / Epilepsy & Behavior 60 (2016) 81–85 85

not have any effects on the tests used in this study, we ignored the [4] Musiek FE, Shinn JB, Jirsa R, Bamiou DE, Baran JA, Zaidan E. GIN (gaps-in-noise) test
performance in with confirmed central auditory nervous system involvement. Ear
effects of the drugs on the test results. Hear 2005;26:608–18.
[5] Musiek FE, Baran JA, Pinheiro ML. Duration pattern recognition in normal subjects
5. Conclusions and patients with cerebral and cochlear lesions. Audiology 1990;29:304–13.
[6] Sherwin I, Efron R. Temporal ordering deficits following anterior temporal lobectomy.
Brain Lang 1980;11:195–203.
Based on the findings of the present study, patients with temporal [7] Efron R, Yund EW, Nichols D, Crandall PH. An ear asymmetry for gap detection fol-
lobe epilepsy suffer from disorders in temporal processing perfor- lowing anterior temporal lobectomy. Neuropsychologia 1985;23:43–50.
[8] Ehrlé N, Samson S, Baulac M. Processing of rapid auditory information in epileptic
mances. In keeping with the left hemisphere being the specialized patients with left temporal lobe damage. Neuropsychologia 2001;39:525–31.
hemisphere for temporal processing, these disorders in patients with [9] Bamiou DE, Musiek FE, Stow I, Stevens J, Cipolotti L, Brown MM, et al. Auditory tem-
LTLE are more significant. As an evaluation tool, the GIN test is more ap- poral processing deficits in patients with insular stroke. Neurology 2006;67:614–9.
[10] Musiek FE, Pinheiro ML, Wilson DH. Auditory pattern perception in ‘split brain’ pa-
propriate for determining whether there is an cortical disorder underly-
tients. Arch Otolaryngol 1980;106:610–2.
ing an auditory processing disorder whereas the DPS test is better for [11] Aravindkumar R, Shivashankar N, Satishchandra P, Sanjib Sinha, Saini J,
identifying the lesion site. Both tests are therefore useful for the diagno- Subbakrishana DK. Temporal resolution deficits in patients with refractory complex
sis and management of auditory processing disorders in patients with partial seizures and mesial temporal sclerosis (MTS). Epilepsy Behav 2012;24:
126–30.
temporal lobe epilepsy. [12] Shinn JB. Temporal processing and temporal patterning tests. In: Musiek FE, Chermak
G, editors. Handbook of (central) auditory processing disorder. 1st ed. Amazon: Plural
Conflict of interest statement Publishing Inc.; 2007. p. 231–9.
[13] Allen P. An introduction to acoustic and psychoacoustics. In: Roeser R, Valente M,
editors. Audiology diagnosis. 2nd ed.; 2007: 169–194.
The authors declare no conflict of interests. [14] Lee J. A comparison of four tests of temporal resolution: AFTR, RGDT, BFT and GIN.
Washington: Washington State University; 2004: 1–69.
[15] Zatorre RJ, Belin P. Spectral and temporal processing in human auditory cortex.
Acknowledgment Cereb Cortex 2001;11:946–53.
[16] Ilvonen TM, Kujala T, Tervaniemi M, Salonen O, Naatanen R, Pekkonen E. The pro-
The authors wish to acknowledge the invaluable help of Dr. Frank cessing of sound duration after left hemisphere stroke: event-related potential and be-
havioral evidence. Psychophysiology 2001;38:622–8.
Musiek who provided consultation and assistance to this project. The
[17] Brancucci A, Babiloni C, Rossini PM, Romani GL. Right hemisphere specialization for
current research was supported by Tehran University of Medical intensity discrimination of musical and speech sounds. Neuropsychologia 2005;43:
Sciences, grant no. p/26/d4/192. 1916–23.
[18] Brancucci A, D'Anselmo A, Martello F, Tommasi L. Left hemisphere specialization for
duration discrimination of musical and speech sounds. Neuropsychologia 2008;46:
References 2013–9.
[19] Shinn JB, Chermak G, Musiek F. GIN (gaps-in-noise) performance in the pediatric
[1] Meneguello J, Leonhardt FD, Pereira LD. Auditory processing in patients with tempo- population. J Am Acad Audiol 2009;20:229–38.
ral lobe epilepsy. Braz J Otorhinolaryngol 2006;72:496–504. [20] Weihing J, Musiek F, Shinn JB. The effect of presentation level on the gaps-in-noise
[2] Han MW, Ahn JH, Kang JK, Kang JK, Lee EM, Lee JH, et al. Central auditory processing (GIN©) test. J Am Acad Audiol 2007;18:141–50.
impairment in patients with temporal lobe epilepsy. Epilepsy Behav 2011;20:370–4.
[3] Samelli AG, Schochat E. The gaps-in-noise test: gap detection thresholds in normal
hearing young adults. Int J Audiol 2008;47:238–45.

Você também pode gostar