Você está na página 1de 9

Journal of Affective Disorders 210 (2017) 130–138

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Journal of Affective Disorders


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/jad

Research paper

Attachment representations, patterns of emotion regulation, and social MARK


exclusion in patients with chronic and episodic depression and healthy
controls

C. Bauriedl-Schmidta, ,1, A. Jobsta,1, M. Ganderb, E. Seidla, L. Sabaßa, N. Sarubina,c, C. Mauera,
F. Padberga,1, A. Buchheimb,1
a
Department of Psychiatry und Psychotherapy, Ludwig-Maximilians-University, Munich, Germany
b
Department of Psychology, Clinical Psychology, University of Innsbruck, Austria
c
Hochschule Fresenius, University of Applied Sciences, Munich, Germany

A R T I C L E I N F O A BS T RAC T

Keywords: Background: The experience of social exclusion (ostracism) is linked to the etiology and maintenance of
Attachment representation depression. Most individuals experience emotional stress in states of social exclusion. Insecurely attached
Chronic depression individuals, especially with an unresolved trauma, show maladaptive coping in response to social stress. The
Episodic depression present study examines (a) the differences with regards to attachment representations in episodic (ED) and
Emotion regulation
chronic depressive (CD) inpatients and (b) how ostracism affects their emotional reactions.
Ostracism
Social exclusion
Methods: Patients with CD (n=29) and ED (n=23) and healthy control subjects (n=29) were interviewed using
the Adult Attachment Projective Picture System (AAP), a valid measure to assess attachment representation;
and played a virtual ball tossing game simulating social exclusion (Cyberball). Multiple depression-related risk
and protective factors were considered. We hypothesized that CD patients show the most severe attachment
disorganization and are emotionally most affected by the social exclusion situation. Moreover, we explored the
interaction between ostracism and attachment.
Results: Contradicting our hypotheses, ED and CD individuals were almost akin with regards to their
attachment insecurity/disorganization and reactions to Cyberball. An emotionally altered reaction to social
exclusion was identified in the insecure-disorganized depressive subgroup.
Limitations: Small sample size hampering further subgroup analyses. The ED sample may include single CD
subjects with recent manifestation.
Conclusions: The pattern of emotion regulation in the depressive groups matches with findings from clinical
studies, including attachment research. The relationship between attachment representations and ostracism
should be further investigated in larger samples of depressive individuals.

1. Introduction Koehler et al., 2015) as chronic depressive patients have more and
longer hospital stays (e.g. Schramm et al., 2011; Brockmeyer et al.,
The experience of loss, social exclusion (ostracism) and rejection is 2015; Koehler et al., 2015). The onset of CD is earlier and more CD
linked to the etiology and maintenance of depression from different patients report an early onset before the age of 21 (Klein et al., 2014;
clinical perspectives (Bowlby, 1969; Coyne, 1976; McCullough, 2003; McCullough, 2003), which is associated with more severe impairment
Allen and Badcock, 2003; Eisenberger, 2012; overview: Buchheim (Berndt et al., 2000), a more aversive progression (Klein et al., 1999;
et al., 2012, 2013). The fifth version of the DSM (APA, 2013) Brockmeyer et al., 2015), as well as more relapses (Agosti, 1999) than a
acknowledges that episodic (ED) and chronic (CD) depression seems late start. Persistent depression increases the vulnerability in adoles-
to be fundamentally different diseases (Klein et al., 2006; Moeller et al., cence to social, psychological, somatic and other psychiatric problems
2014). Studies show that CD in comparison to ED is the more severe (Klein et al., 2004; Jonsson et al., 2011; Schramm et al., 2011).
form (Angst et al., 2009; Jonsson et al., 2011; Brockmeyer et al., 2015; Individuals with CD report earlier childhood trauma (Lizardi et al.,


Corresponding author.
E-mail address: christine@bauriedl-schmidt.de (C. Bauriedl-Schmidt).
1
These authors contributed equally to this work.

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2016.12.030
Received 1 May 2016; Received in revised form 29 September 2016; Accepted 17 December 2016
Available online 21 December 2016
0165-0327/ © 2016 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.
C. Bauriedl-Schmidt et al. Journal of Affective Disorders 210 (2017) 130–138

1995; Wiersma et al., 2009), more interpersonal problems (Schramm (Williams, 2009). An immediate trans-individual reflexive impairment
et al., 2011), and emotional as well as cognitive-behavioral avoidance of mood can be differentiated from individually reflected coping, such
(Brockmeyer et al., 2015). as the fight, flight, and socialize responses. Prolonged exclusion may
Since there is a relatively high amount of the documented therapy lead to resignation and depression. The virtual computer game
resistance in (chronic) depression the disorder is a big challenge for the “Cyberball” is a well-studied and described paradigm to efficiently
health system worldwide (e.g. Kornstein and Schneider, 2001; Trivedi and reliably induce and investigate the influence of social exclusion
and Daly, 2008; Holtzheimer and Mayberg, 2011; WHO, 2012). under standardized conditions on affective reactions and behavioral
Therefore specific therapeutic approaches are requested that consider choices (Williams, 2009; Hartgerink et al., 2015), regardless of
the individual mechanism underlying ED and CD to enhance efficacy individual characteristics (Zadro et al., 2004, for groups with psychia-
(e.g. McCullough, 2003; Holtzheimer and Mayberg, 2011; Jobst et al., tric diagnoses: Staebler et al., 2011; Jobst et al., 2014, 2015). To our
2016). A large recent review on psychotherapy in chronic depression knowledge, only one other research group has investigated the effects
provides a meta-analysis of the present literature and recommenda- of ostracism on groups of depressive individuals and identified
tions for evidence-based treatments (Jobst et al., 2016). One of the heightened activity in the ”social pain” brain area during the
highly recommended treatments is CBASP (Cognitive Behavioral Cyberball game as a predictive marker for increased depressive
Analysis System of Psychotherapy, McCullough, 2000) based on the symptoms one year later (Masten et al., 2011).
assumption that early attachment and interpersonal trauma has In this study, psycho-behavioral indicators for the “attachment” and
resulted in dysfunctional mechanisms of derailed affective and motiva- the “ostracism detection” system were investigated in two groups of
tional regulation and a reduction of perceived functionality. comparably severely affected inpatients with CD and ED compared to a
Affective disorders in general are associated with the predominance healthy control (HC) group. On the basis of previous studies, we
of insecure attachment representations (Bakermans-Kranenburg and hypothesized that chronic depressive (CD) individuals would report (1)
van IJzendoorn, 2009; West and George, 2002). Moreover, recent more childhood trauma and would show (2) a higher amount of
research points at the particular relevance that the disorganized unresolved trauma in their attachment representations than episodic
unresolved attachment pattern has for the CD (Buchheim and depressive (ED) individuals. (3) In the social exclusion paradigm, an
George, 2012). In this group unresolved trauma is mostly associated impaired reaction pattern would be more prominent in CD subjects
with experiences of loss of or rejection by a significant other (Bowlby, compared to the ED and HC groups, i.e. they would have more
1980; Buchheim et al., 2013). Bowlby (1969) conceptualized attach- indicators of emotion dysregulation (e.g. negative mood, need threat),
ment as an evolutionarily developed biological-behavioral human and choose more passive behaviors, than ED subjects. (4) On an
system. The individual develops internalized mental representations explorative level, we were interested to investigate in the attachment
of attachment and attachment figures in early infant-caregiver inter- related subgroups of both depression groups the differences in the
actions. These may adapt to continuing experiences gained throughout reactions to the Cyberball paradigm.
the life span, but are nonetheless relatively stable (Bowlby, 1980) and
generalized across various relationship-types, such as with parents or 2. Method
romantic partners (Waters et al., 2015). Secure attachment is char-
acterized by integrated representations of self and others, and is 2.1. Study population and procedure
considered to be a protective factor against mental illness (Bowlby,
1980; Fonagy et al., 1996; West and George, 2002; Ward et al., 2006; A total of 81 individuals (42 male) between 18 and 75 years were
Bakermans-Kranenburg and van IJzendoorn, 2009; Buchheim and included. The groups of CD (n=29) and ED (n=23) were inpatients of a
George, 2011, 2012). If individuals experience incomplete or compro- German psychiatric university hospital. Inclusion criteria for ED were
mised parental protection, they develop internal working models that single or recurrent depressive episodes lasting less than 2 years, both
may contain defensive processes (“deactivation” or “cognitive discon- with inter-episode full recovery. Key inclusion criterion for CD was a
nection”) to exclude the overwhelming experience and to organize DSM-IV diagnoses (APA, 1994, German version: Saß et al., 1998) of (1)
difficult emotions (George and Solomon, 2008; George and West, major depressive disorder (MD), (2) recurrent MD with no full inter-
2012). When the attachment figure is entirely unavailable, e.g. through episode recovery, (3) dysthymic disorder, or (4) MD superimposed on a
loss, or is highly neglectful, abusive or showing failed protection, the dysthymic disorder (double depression) lasting at least 2 years.
individual makes use of a third, more extreme defensive process that Individuals with a history of psychosis, bipolar disorder, acute addic-
Bowlby termed “segregated systems” (Bowlby, 1980). This process is tion, suicidal state, severe organic disease, or current pregnancy, were
linked to disorganized representations also called “unresolved trauma” excluded.
(Main, 1995; George et al., 1999). Especially disorganized defensive Healthy controls (HC) (n=29) were recruited via announcements
processes turn out to be maladaptive later in life (George and West, and were matched in age, gender, education and marital status (see
2012; Buchheim and George, 2012). These maladaptive processes are Table 1) with the two depression groups. A semi-structured telephone
the basis for impaired affect regulation and a low level of interpersonal interview included the screening questions of SCID-I (German version:
functioning (Diamond et al., 2014; Browne and Winkelman, 2007), as Wittchen et al., 1997) supplemented by questions referring to affective
well they are related to impaired capacity to mentalize one's own or disorders, psychosis and organic diseases. Moreover the Beck
other's states of mind in attachment relevant contexts (Fonagy et al., Depression Inventory (BDI-II, German version: Hautzinger et al.,
1998), and are risk factors for psychiatric illnesses (Buchheim and 2006) was administered (inclusion score: 10 maximum). Our included
George, 2011; George et al., 1999). participants met the general criteria, never developed addiction (life-
Another factor underlying the development of depressive symptoms time), and had no psychotherapy or severe organic disease within the
are experiences of social exclusion (Allen and Badcock, 2003). Williams last ten years. Persisting organic diseases should be under medicinal
(2009) proposes an evolutionarily grown “ostracism detection system” control. Only n=1 participant showed an accentuation of personality
that senses even tiny markers of social exclusion. A lot of research features (high score in SCID-II, German version: Wittchen et al., 1997).
proves the assumption, that ostracism threatens four fundamental Moreover instruments assessing the degree of functioning were
human needs to belong, to self-esteem, to control and to meaningful administered (Fig. 1) to all participants: Severity of symptoms was
existence and thus deprives the individual from the satisfaction of ascertained by using validated clinical instruments: The Hamilton
needs that are relevant for the motivation, survival, efficacy (overview: Rating Scale for Depression (HDRS, Hamilton, 1960), and BDI-II.
Williams et al., 2000). According to the temporal need-threat-model Resilience was determined by using the commonly used Connor-
the reactions to social exclusion are separated in three phases Davidson-Resilience Scale (CD-RISC, 25 Items, Connor and

131
C. Bauriedl-Schmidt et al. Journal of Affective Disorders 210 (2017) 130–138

Table 1
(a) Sample characteristics (socio-demographic, clinical) and (b) early childhood trauma (CTQ).

A Characteristics CD ED HC

n (%) n (%) n (%) Comparisons

Gender: male 17 (58.6) 10 (43.5) 15 (51,7) Pearson χ2=1.178, df=2, p=.555


Married/partnership 12 (44.4) 9 (39.2) 6 (20.7) Pearson χ2=.988, df=6, p=.610
Educational status: A-level 19 (65.5) 10 (43.5) 16 (55.2) Kruskal-Wallis-Test: χ2=1.774, df=2, p=.412
Comorbid Personality Accentuation Cluster C 10 (37.0) 8 (38.1) Exact Fisher Test (1-sided exact significance): .500
Early Onset 12 (41.4) 5 (21.7) Pearson χ2=2.249, df=1, p=.076
Amount of prior episodes: 1–2 8 (27.6) 14 (60.8) Z-test with Bonferroni-correction: p < .05
Amount of prior episodes: No information possible 5 (17.2) 0 Mean (SD) episodes in ED: 2.8 (2.2)
Mean (SD) Mean (SD) Mean (SD)

Age (years) 46.7 (14.2) 43.1 (11.5) 46.3 (13.6) ANOVA: F2,78=.545, p=.582
Hospitalization due to depression, lifetime (weeks) 82.0 (82.7) 82.3 (33.9) U(nCD=26,nED=22)=251.000, p=.235
Psychotropic medication (amount) 2.9 (1.0) 2.7 (1.0) T=−.831, df=49, p=.205
Amount of Axis I or II Diagnosis 1.1 (1.2) 1.1 (1.7) U(nCD=27,nED=21)=248.000, p=.215
Depression severity (HRSD) 22.3 (9.7) 23.8 (9.5) U(nCD=29,nED=23)=303.000, p=.287
Depression severity (BDI-II) 26.9 (9.7) 26.6 (10.8) T=−.089, df=50, p=.465

Resilience (CD-RISC) 48.5 (20.3) 44.3 (17.1) 71.7 (13.8) ANOVA: F2,72=19.994, p=.000**

CD vs. ED post hoc: 4.231, p=.500b


B CTQ Mean (SD) Mean (SD) Mean (SD) Statistic1 p-value

Emotional Abuse 10.6 (5.0) 12.0 (5.0) 6.2 (1.5) 26.2822 .000**
274.5003 .138
162.0004 .000****a
86.5005 .000****a

Physical Abuse 7.0 (3.7) 9.0 (5.5) 5.5 (1.1) 11,4742 .003**
263.5003 .088
293.0004 .012***a
171.0005 .001****a

Sexual Abuse 6.1 (2.4) 6.0 (2.8) 4.9 (.7) 7.7852 .020*
323.0003 .793
309.5004 .001****a
253.5005 010***a

Emotional Neglect 12.7 (5.2) 13.8 (3.6) 8.9 (3.7) 17.4642 .000**
277.5003 .150
238.5004 .002****a
110.0005 .000****a

Physical Neglect 7.4 (3.2) 9.2 (3.5) 6.3 (1.7) 11.1372 .004**
223.5003 .020***a
331.5004 .075
160.0005 .001****a

Note: CD = chronic depression, ED = episodic depression, HC = healthy control, SD = Standard Deviation, HRSD: Hamilton Rating Scale for Depression, BDI: Beck Depression
Inventory, CD-RISC = Connor-Davidson Resilience Scale, 1 = Statistics refer to overall groups comparison (1. value) and to comparison of CD vs. ED (2. value), 2 = Kruskall-Wallis-Test,
df=2, 3 = Mann-Whitney-U-Test, nCD=29, nED=23, 4 = Mann-Whitney-U-Test, nCD=29, nGK=29, 5 = Mann-Whitney-U-Test, nED=23, nGK=29, *= 2-sided (asymptotic) significance
according to level of significance set at α=.05, **= 2-sided (asymptotic) significance according to level of significance set at α=.01, ***= 1-sided (asymptotic) significance level set at
α=.05, ****= 1-sided (asymptotic) significance according to level of significance set at α=.01, SD = Standard Deviation, a = Bonferroni-Holm-correction, three comparisons, b =
Bonferroni post hoc

Fig. 1. : Procedure of Investigation Annotation: This investigation was part of a larger study and included more measures than displayed here. Therefore it took 3–5 sessions as well as
homework to complete the whole procedure.

132
C. Bauriedl-Schmidt et al. Journal of Affective Disorders 210 (2017) 130–138

Davidson, 2003). Traumatic experiences in the past were assessed with refer to fearful or even traumatizing material (e.g. helplessness), that
the Childhood Trauma Questionnaire (CTQ, 28 items, Bernstein and must be locked away from conscious awareness and thus put indivi-
Fink, 1998). duals at risk for dysregulation. Based on these rating scales, the AAP
Socio-demographic, basic clinical characteristics of the diagnostic derives four main attachment groups: secure, dismissing, preoccupied,
subgroups and the respective test statistics are described in Table 1: and unresolved. Secure attachment (F) is characterized by a high
The depressive subgroups did not significantly differ in terms of early agency of self, a lot of connectedness and synchrony in the narratives.
or late onset of depression, duration of hospitalization lifetime, Insecure-dismissing (Ds) or insecure-preoccupied (E) individuals de-
comorbidity with DSM-IV Axis 1 and/or 2 diagnosis, and amount of monstrate either functional or absent relationships and they primarily
psychotropic medication. Both depressive subgroups showed present use deactivation (in case of Ds) or cognitive disconnection (in case of E)
moderate depression and a comparable low resilience score (CD-RISC) to successfully shift attention away from attachment-related stressors.
(Table 1). A significant difference was found in the frequency distribu- In contrast to these organized attachment patterns (F, Ds, E),
tions of prior depressive episodes (Pearson χ2=8.946, df=4, p=.031). individuals with an unresolved attachment (U) can be considered as
More ED than CD patients reported only a few, i.e. one or two prior disorganized as they fail to re-organize fearful or traumatizing material
episodes. Anyhow, on average, the ED patients reported three former (segregated systems) and thus get flooded by their attachment fears.
episodes. Psychometric properties of the AAP are excellent (George and West,
40.7% of the CD and 57.1% ED patients presented no comorbidity 2001, 2004, 2012). AAP interjudge reliability between independent
with another psychiatric diagnosis. Almost 40% of the CD and ED judges showed agreement of 90% (kappa=.85, p=.000, n=144). Test-
participants showed comorbidity with a Cluster C personality disorder, retest reliability was calculated based on 69 participants (39 females,
that is avoidant, dependent, and obsessive-compulsive disorders. The 30 males) who completed the AAP retest 3 months following the
latter is consistent with previous research (Buchheim et al., 2012, original AAP administration. 58 participants (84%) were classified in
2013; Kornstein and Schneider, 2001). the same attachment group categories (kappa=.78, p=.000). Verbal
The depressive groups were treated with an average amount of intelligence and social desirability were not related to AAP classifica-
three types of psycho-pharmaceuticals: antidepressants, second-gen- tions (George and West, 2012). The comparison with the Adult
eration antipsychotics, mood stabilizers and/or benzodiazepines. Attachment Interview (George et al., 1985; Main and Goldwyn,
82.1% of the CD (82.6% ED) individuals reported intake of additional 1985–1996; George et al., 1996), another established measure to
remedies (predominantly internistical) compared to 37.9% of the HC assess attachment representation (see George and West, 2012;
(Table 1, A). Buchheim and George, 2012), showed convergent validity for the four
This study was approved by the local ethics committee of the major attachment groups of 90% (kappa=.84, p < .000) between the
Medical Faculty of the Ludwig-Maximilians-University, Munich, two attachment measures (AAP and AAI). Convergent agreement for
Germany. All participants gave their written informed consent. two group classifications was 97% (kappa=.88, p < .000) (George and
West, 2011, 2012; Buchheim and George, 2012; George and Buchheim,
2.2. Adult Attachment Projective Picture System (AAP) 2014).

The Adult Attachment Projective Picture System (AAP, George 2.3. Cyberball game
et al., 1999) is a semi-structured attachment interview designed to
assess adult attachment representations. The AAP procedure begins The Cyberball game is a virtual ball tossing game (Williams et al.,
with a neutral picture followed by seven attachment-activating draw- 2000). The standardized experimental setting used in this study has
ings of scenes depicting solitude, separation, illness, death, and been reported in other publications (see Jobst et al., 2014). First, the
maltreatment. The individuals are asked to tell stories about each participants were prepared for a computer game that they play together
picture using three standardized questions (e.g. What are the char- with two other players connected via the internet, though in reality they
acters thinking or feeling? ) Trained psychologist/ psychiatrist admi- are computer-controlled and represented by name and picture (Fig. 1).
nistered the interview procedure. Based on verbatim transcripts, each After getting the ball three times, an inclusion rate of 10%, the
stimulus is coded for content and defense according to the AAP participants were excluded for the rest of the game. Immediately after
guidelines (George and West, 2011). On the content level, coders finishing the Cyberball game participants completed a paper-and-
evaluate in the stories (a) the capacity for attachment relationships to pencil questionnaire that contained the Need-Threat Scale (NTS,
foster productive action (agency of self) and (b) the reported desire to Williams et al., 2000; Zadro et al., 2006; German version: Grzyb,
be in relationships with others (connectedness) for pictures presenting 2005; Staebler et al., 2011) to ascertain impairment of four funda-
characters alone as well as or for pictures portraying characters in mental needs during the game that were measured with three items per
dyads: (c) synchrony, i.e. the balance and mutuality in attachment scale: belonging (e.g. “I felt rejected.”), self-esteem (e.g. “I felt good
relationships. These dimensions are rated on 3-point ordinal scales: about myself.”), control (e.g. “I felt powerful.”) and meaningful
integrated, functional, no capacity. First, integrated agency of self e.g. existence (e.g. “I felt nonexistent.”). Negative items were reverse
refers to themes of thoughtful self-exploration, whereas functional scored: Higher values indicated more need satisfaction. Negative mood
agency refers to functional problem-solving behavior. Second, indivi- was assessed through eight items (e.g. “sad”, “bad”, “happy”). Positive
duals with an integrated connectedness show a propensity to establish items were reverse scored. There were three control items to confirm
intimate relationships (i.e. attachment-caregiving figure), whereas the participants’ perception of being excluded (“I was excluded.”, “I was
those with a functional connectedness rather demonstrate the desire ignored.”, open question to estimate the percentage of throws they
to be with other people more generally (e.g. teacher, doctor). Third, an received during the Cyberball interaction). Unless otherwise stated, all
integrated form of synchrony is reflected in the characters’ reciprocal questions were rated on 5-point Likert scales (1=not at all until 5=very
interactions and mutual enjoyments, whereas in those with functional much so). Responses were averaged to create the overall indexes. The
synchrony, this does not seem to be desirable for the characters. internal consistency of the need items and the resulting Cronbach's α
For all picture stimuli, the AAP coding finally distinguishes between (total scale: α=.83; fundamental needs: α=.51 until α=.83; overall need
three forms of defense: Deactivation is used to move attention away satisfaction: α=.84, mood: α=.82, control items: α=.88) were compar-
from attachment-related stressors by reducing the need for attachment able to former findings (e.g. Williams, 2005; Wirth et al., 2010;
relationships as important (e.g. neutralization). Cognitive disconnec- Staebler et al., 2011).
tion serves to separate emotions from attachment-related situations The behavioral intentions after the game were assessed with a list of
and the people arousing them (e.g. insecurity). Segregated systems choices as previously reported (Staebler, 2008). The participants were

133
C. Bauriedl-Schmidt et al. Journal of Affective Disorders 210 (2017) 130–138

Table 2
Responses to Cyberball in depressive vs. healthy individuals.

Comparisons: CD ED HC Between groups

n=28 n=19 n=28

Mean (SD) Mean (SD) Mean (SD) χ2 p

Fulfillment of needs (NTS)


Overall need satisfaction 2.48 (.72) 2.67 (.75) 2.83 (.74) 2.801 .246
Belonging 2.85 (1.10) 3.09 (1.15) 3.17 (1.16) 1.241 .538
Self-esteem 2.37 (.93) 2.45 (.94) 3.00 (.93) 7.497 .001**
Meaningful existence 3.33 (1.21) 3.44 (1.21) 3.39 (1.26) .115 .944
Control 1.52 (.58) 1.72 (.78) 1.75 (.77) .979 .613
Negative mood 3.14 (.82) 2.93 (.69) 2.27 (.74) 16.383 .000**

Control items
“I felt excluded/ ignored” 3.50 (1.15) 2.94 (1.40) 3.25 (1.34) 2.210 .331
“I owned the ball” (%) 11.62 (6.47) 14.50 (16.69) 9.82 (6.06) 1.145 .564

List of behavioral intentions


Pleasant activities1 .31 (.30) .25 (.25) .36 (.26) 2.024 .364
Passive behavior1 .33 (.31) .40 (.36) .14 (.23) 8.975 .011*

Annotations: Means were compared with Kruskall-Wallis-Test (df =2). For the List of Behavioral Intentions only the comparisons that are statistically significant for one or two groups
are displayed. 1 = scores of behavioral items could range between 0 and 1.
*= 2-sided (asymptotic) significance according to level of significance set at α=.05, **= 2-sided (asymptotic) significance according to level of significance set at α=.01.

asked whether or not they agreed with each of the 21 given behaviors
(e.g. to sleep, to hurt s.b.). The individual intentions were clustered
according to Staebler (2008) to eight categories (e.g. passive intentions,
pleasant activities, aggression towards others, verbalize the rejection).
All mentioned measures are listed in the left column in Table 2.

2.4. Data analysis

SPSS software version 17 was used for statistical analysis (SPSS


Inc., Chicago, IL 60606, USA). The decision on whether to apply a
parametric or non-parametric procedure for the metric variables was
made with the Kolmogorov-Smirnov-test and the assessment of the
respective histogram. The Kruskal-Wallis- or Mann-Whitney-U test as
non-parametric or respectively the F- or T-test as parametric procedure
was applied to contrast the subgroups. For the non-metric variables,
the Pearson χ2-test (or Fisher's Exact Probability Test, when applic-
able) was employed. Bonferroni-Holm-corrections were used in the
three comparisons between the CD, ED, HC groups to avoid accumula-
tion of alpha-errors.

3. Results

3.1. Early childhood trauma (CTQ)

With respect to childhood trauma both CD and ED patients differed


significantly from the HC group. Patients reported more emotional,
physical, and sexual abuse as well as more emotional neglect in the
CTQ. Moreover, the ED participants showed higher scores on physical
neglect than the HC and CD groups (HC: p=.001, CD: p=.040). The ED
and CD subgroups did not differ from each other with regards to
amount of sexual, physical, emotional abuse and emotional neglect
(sexual: p=.793, physical: p=.177, emotional abuse: p=.275, emotional
neglect: p=.301) (Table 1, B).
Fig. 2. a: Distribution of “secure”/”insecure” classifications (percentage) in CD, ED, HC
b: Distribution of “organized”/”disorganized” classifications in CD, ED, HC.
3.2. Distribution of adult attachment representations
Cross tables show that the proportions did not significantly differ
In the ED and CD groups, insecure attachment representation was between the two depressive groups, but between the control and each
predominant: 95.5% of the ED and 96.3% of the CD patients were depression group (p > .05, Pearson χ2=21.344, df=2, p=.000). The
classified as either dismissing, preoccupied, or unresolved. In the HC, predominant representation in the HC group was the organized
the distribution of attachment patterns was more balanced: 48.1% classification (Fig. 2b), i.e. 88.9% (vs. 11.1% disorganized subjects,
secure vs. 51.9% insecure individuals (Fig. 2a).

134
C. Bauriedl-Schmidt et al. Journal of Affective Disorders 210 (2017) 130–138

cross table shows significant difference: p > .05), whereas in the clinical emotional, resilience parameters as well as described in terms of
groups we found a more equal distribution of organized (secure, symptoms, comorbidity, present psychotropic treatment and course
insecure-dismissing, insecure-preoccupied)/ and disorganized indivi- of disease. Furthermore, we evaluated interpersonal trauma using the
duals. 63.6% of the ED and 66.7% of the CD individuals were rated as CTQ questionnaire (Bernstein and Fink, 1998) to analyze differences
organized (Fig. 2b). The cross table demonstrates that the overall among the depressive subgroups.
frequency distribution of organized/disorganized classifications shows The similar extent of impairment in CD and ED subgroups
a non-significant trend between the three groups (χ2=5,050; df=2; regarding symptoms, course of disease, biography, socio-emotional
p=.080). However, there was no difference between CD and ED parameters (CTQ, AAP) contradicts previous findings and our hypoth-
patients in this respect. eses 1 and 2, that CD individuals would report the most childhood
trauma and would show the highest amount of unresolved trauma in
3.3. Reactions to cyberball their attachment representations. We cannot conclude here, that ED is
a less severe form of depression (Lizardi et al., 1995; Wiersma et al.,
The overall need satisfaction as well as the NTS scores in the 2009; Schramm et al., 2011). This might be due to the recruitment of
individual needs for “belonging”, “meaningful existence” and “control” ED from a psychiatric hospital inpatient setting. Accordingly, it can be
did not differ significantly among the three diagnostic groups assumed, that the psychiatric inpatients suffering from ED were more
(overall: p=.246, belonging: p=.538, meaningful existence: p=.944, traumatized and ill (i.e. even more physical neglect than CD inpatients,
control: p=.613). Only the CD, but not the ED individuals showed a average amount of depressive episodes: n=3) than the patients in the
significantly lower score in need for self-esteem than the HC subjects, cited studies above. Because recent research and theory propose that
i.e. reported more need threat for self-esteem (NTS: self-esteem: ED and CD are two fundamental different diseases (APA, 2013; Jobst
U(nCD=28,nHC=28)=233.000, p=.005; U(nED=19,nHC=28)=179.000, et al., 2016), these results indicate that with regards to a subgroup of
p=.028, U(nED=19,nCD=28)=179.000, p=.380). Both depression the severely depressive patients it might be difficult to reliably select
group reported significantly higher scores in negative mood between ED and CD. We might assume that a part of ED patients may
(U(nCD=29,nHC=28)=169.000, p=.000; U(nED=19,nHC=28)=127.500, later develop a chronic form of depression (Brockmeyer et al., 2015).
p=.003, U(nED=19,nCD=29)=188.500, p=.263, 1-sided asymptotic sig- To include the assessment of cognition, motivation and behavior in a
nificance). The subgroups were similar on the extent they felt ignored/ standardized interpersonal situation (e.g. AAP, Cyberball) might
excluded, and on the amount of perceived ball tosses (ignored/ provide additional helpful diagnostic information.
excluded: p=.331, ball tosses: p=.564) (Table 2). However, the expected discrepancies between both depression
Regarding behavioral intentions after Cyberball, CD and ED groups and the HC group could be verified. As described in the
patients chose more passive intentions (e.g. “to sleep”, “to smoke”) literature, the relevance of interpersonal childhood trauma, particularly
than the HC after playing the game (U(nCD=29,nHC=29)=276.000, emotional neglect or abuse, was confirmed: Both depressive groups
p=,009, U(nED=19,nHC=29)=162.500, p=,005, U(nCD=29,nED=19) reported a higher amount of adverse childhood experiences (CTQ),
=249.500, p=,272, 1-sided asymptotic significance, Bonferroni-Holm- particularly emotional trauma, than the HC (Bowlby, 1988; APA, 2000;
corrected for three comparisons) (Table 2). McCullough, 2003; Chapman et al., 2004; Chavustra and Cloitre, 2008;
Due to the small sample sizes of single attachment categories we D’Andrea et al., 2012; Banducci et al., 2013; Holmes, 2013). This is
grouped both CD and ED subjects together in order to investigate the consistent with the description of D’Andrea et al. (2012), who linked
differences in the reactions of depressed individuals with organized vs. interpersonal childhood trauma with subsequently arising interperso-
disorganized attachment representations. Because of the explorative nal problems that might result in the development of depression
character of this section, we tested 2-tailed. The significant differences (Coyne, 1976; Kessler et al., 1995; McCullough, 2003; Jonsson et al.,
and non-significant trends (significance levels ranging between .051 2011). As expected, the accumulation of interpersonal trauma in
and .100) are mentioned here. The organized depressive individuals biography was reflected by higher proportions of unresolved trauma
reported a higher self-esteem (p=.094) in the NTS, though the overall in the AAP narratives and insecure representations in both clinical
need satisfaction did not differ between both groups (p=.143). groups (George et al., 1999; Chapman et al., 2004; Buchheim et al.,
Furthermore the organized subjects picked more intentions to involve 2013; Juen et al., 2013). Only 3.7% for CD (4.5% ED, 48.1% HC)
into pleasant activities (e.g. to meet friends) and to verbalize the individuals were classified as secure. Moreover, the disorganized
rejection (e.g. to ask why s/he was rejected) in the List of Behavioral category is more prevalent in the depressive groups as well: 33% in
Intention than the disorganized individuals (pleasant activities: the CD group (36% ED) contrasted with 11% in the HC subjects. This
p=.048, verbalize the rejection: p=.056). The disorganized depressive tendency fits with former findings, demonstrating that the disorganized
subjects in turn felt more ostracized and reported a higher amount of category is not only relevant in CD (Buchheim et al., 2013; West and
negative mood (NTS: ignored: p=.042, mood: p=.025). Moreover, they George, 2002), but also in psychiatric ED (Bakermans-Kranenburg and
chose more intentions to passive behavior and to aggression towards van IJzendoorn, 2009). These discrepancies might again be explained
others (aggression: p=.082, passive behavior: p=,028, 2-sided asymp- through differences in psychiatric vs. non-psychiatric samples.
totic significance). The aggression towards others variable includes e.g. Therefore hypothesis 1 and 2 could only be affirmed for differences
the intention to hurt somebody or to damage something. Additionally between HC and both depression groups with regards to childhood
the disorganized depressive individuals displayed a higher score in the trauma and unresolved trauma in the attachment representations.
CTQ score emotional neglect (Mean ± SD: organized: 12.10 ± 4.02, Although all groups judged the ball ownership similarly and felt
disorganized: 14.76 ± 5.48, U(norganized=31,ndisorganized=17)=184.000, excluded/ignored to a similar extent only the depressive groups
p=.086) (Table 3). reported significantly higher scores in negative mood during
Cyberball and only in the CD group Cyberball had a significantly
4. Discussion higher impact on self-esteem. The comparable effects of Cyberball on
the overall need satisfaction (i.e. especially on belonging, control,
To our knowledge, this is the first study to investigate the meaningful existence, NTS, Williams et al., 2000, right column in
differences in two groups of CD and ED inpatients with regards to Table 2) of the three groups corresponds with the assumptions of the
the three levels: traumatic experiences in biography, mental represen- temporal need-threat model and previous findings (Williams, 2009),
tations of attachment (AAP, George et al., 1999; George and West, that the automatic need-decreasing reaction after social exclusion is
2012) and their reactions to social exclusion (Cyberball, Williams and trans-individual and independent from personal characteristics.
Jarvis, 2006). The groups were characterized by biographical, socio- Depression-specific imprinting was found too: The lower score in the

135
C. Bauriedl-Schmidt et al. Journal of Affective Disorders 210 (2017) 130–138

Table 3
Responses to cyberball in organized vs. disorganized depressive individuals.

Scales Organized (n=31) Disorganized (n=16) Statistic p-value

Mean ± SD Mean ± SD

Immediate effect of ostracism


Overall need satisfaction (NTS) 2.69 ± .72 2.41 ± .72 U(norganized=26,ndisorganized=15)=156.500 .143
Self-esteem (NTS) 2.60 ± .96 2.10 ± .85 U(norganized=28,ndisorganized=16)=156.500 .094
Negative Mood 2.86 ± .70 3.41 ± .84 U(norganized=28,ndisorganized=16)=132.500 ,025*
“I felt excluded”/ “I felt ignored” 3.00 ± 1.22 3.77 ± 1.18 U(norganized=24,ndisorganized=15)=110.500 ,041*

List of Behavioral Intentions1


Pleasant activities .36 ± .27 .20 ± .28 U(norganized=29,ndisorganized=16)=151.500 .048*
Address the rejection .40 ± .34 .22 ± .41 U(norganized=29,ndisorganized=16)=158.500 ,056
Aggression towards others .02 ± .09 .10 ± .20 U(norganized=29,ndisorganized=16)=189.000 ,082
Passive Behavior .28 ± .32 .50 ± .32 U(norganized=29,ndisorganized=16)=147.500 ,028*

Annotations: Means were compared with Mann-Whitney-U-Test. Only the comparisons that are statistically significant for one or two groups are displayed. 1 = scores of behavioral items
could range between 0 and 1.
*=2-sided (asymptotic) significance according to level of significance set at α=.05, SD = Standard Deviation.

need for self-esteem in CD subjects is seen as characteristic that sizes between the participants of that study due to differing phases in
matches the conceptualization of depression (McCullough, 2003; the whole study. The uneven distribution secure/insecure and orga-
Ravitz et al., 2007; Davison et al., 2007). Both depressive groups nized/disorganized across groups does not allow an analysis of all
displayed a higher extent of negative mood. The predominant inten- attachment representations across all groups. The study should be
tions in the depressive individuals for passive behavior can be inter- replicated with larger, but more continuous and representative samples
preted as a marker for a regressive state that is thought to be linked to a to be able to investigate the interaction between ostracism and
pre-operational status (McCullough, 2003). Overall, our hypothesis 3 attachment in the two depression groups. The psychotropic medication
can be affirmed, that in the social exclusion paradigm, the depressive in inpatients might have affected their ability to experience or react to
individuals responded more dysfunctional and that the CD individuals social pain, as shown for the painkiller paracetamol (Eisenberger,
might be most impaired. 2012; Hartgerink et al., 2015). This should be taken into account. In
Our results indicate that the attachment organization of represen- contrast to previous results, we found that ED and CD participants
tations plays a role in how depressive individuals regulate their were similarly ill and displayed almost identical distributions of
emotions in reaction to a social stressor (Cyberball). The subjects with attachment patterns (Klein et al., 1999; Keller et al., 2000;
organized attachment representations (secure, insecure-dismissing or McCullough, 2003; Wiersma et al., 2011; Jonsson et al., 2011;
insecure-preoccupied) displayed a higher self-esteem (NTS, non-sig- Buchheim and George, 2012; Buchheim et al., 2012). For further
nificant trend) and more constructive behavioral intentions (i.e. research, the ED group should be more homogeneous, for instance
pleasant behavior, address the rejection). This can be seen as a general restricted to one or two former episodes and/or be recruited from an
ability to regulate the emotional state through considering steps that outpatient setting. We did not assess neutral or inclusionary baselines
heigthen chances to feel included again. Moreover, the disorganized for mood, need fulfillment and behavioral intentions in depressive
individuals scored higher in negative mood (NTS), and showed more individuals (Jobst et al., 2015). Thus we cannot specify if the
intentions to engage in passive behavior and aggression, though the differences found between the groups reflect effects of the Cyberball
latter is only a non-significant trend. These findings can be seen manipulation or if they are due to pre-existing negative mood,
carefully as indicators for emotional dysregulation in the disorganized decreased need fulfillment for self-esteem and passivity in depressive
group (e.g. Holtzheimer and Mayberg, 2011). The disorganized in- individuals that in terms of ceiling respectively floor effects might have
dividuals (unresolved trauma) however felt more ostracized and been difficult for Cyberball to affect.
additionally scored significantly higher than the organized patients in
the CTQ dimension emotional neglect. Whether this is associated with
a heightened sensitivity towards cues of social exclusion and rejection 5. Conclusion
should be further investigated (Downey and Feldman, 1996; Staebler
et al., 2008; Jobst et al., 2014). Due to the combination of two emotion inducing paradigms (AAP,
Our findings from the two paradigms AAP and Cyberball can be Cyberball), differing patterns of social stress management were identi-
integrated into psychotherapeutic treatment of severely ill patients. fied in the combined depression group that are in accordance with the
Insecurely attached, especially disorganized depressive individuals will current theory and attachment respectively depression related research
benefit from interventions that support them to acquire abilities to (e.g. Eisenberger et al., 2003, 2011; Eisenberger, 2012; Holtzheimer
reflect the states of mind of self and others (e.g. Fonagy et al., 2004). and Mayberg, 2011; Buchheim and George, 2012; overview: Holmes,
The enhancement of “agency of self”, contributing to self-esteem, will 2013; Gander and Buchheim, 2015). Specific resources for emotion
help the depressive individual to develop a more functional coping with regulation and strategies to cope with the social stress (i.e. feeling of
painful social interactions, like threatening ostracism, and learn how to self-esteem, intention to constructive behavior) as well as dysregulation
increase the chances to feel socially included (Williams, 2009; George (i.e. negative mood, intention for dysfunctional behavior) have been
and West, 2012; Brockmeyer et al., 2015). The assessment of attach- identified that provide fruitful contributions for diagnosis and attach-
ment representations may also stimulate CBASP therapists, e.g. to ment specific therapeutic interpersonal interventions. The specific
adjust expressions of disciplined personal (therapeutic) involvement to effect that social exclusion has on the intra- and interpersonal
the patient's extent of interpersonal traumatization (McCullough, responses of depressive individuals as well as the role of moderator
2003). variables (e.g. attachment organization, rejection sensitivity) should be
We have to consider several limitations in this study. These are clarified in larger samples and a manipulation design that encompasses
overrepresentations of male participants, small and differing sample neutral, inclusion and exclusion conditions.

136
C. Bauriedl-Schmidt et al. Journal of Affective Disorders 210 (2017) 130–138

Role of funding sources Diamond, D., Levy, K.N., Clarkin, J.F., Fischer-Kern, M., Cain, N.M., Doering, S., Hörz,
S., Buchheim, A., 2014. Attachment and Mentalization in female patients With
comorbid Narcissistic and borderline personality disorder. Personal. Disord. 5 (4),
There was no involvement of a funding source. 428–433.
Downey, G., Feldman, S.I., 1996. Implications of rejection sensitivity for intimate
relationships. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 70 (6), 1327–1343.
Conflicts of interest Eisenberger, N.I., 2012. The neural bases of social pain: evidence for shared
representations with physical pain. Psychosom. Med. 74, 126–135.
All authors declare that they have no conflicts of interest. Eisenberger, N.I., Lieberman, M.D., Williams, K.D., 2003. Does rejection hurt? An fMRI
study of social exclusion. Science 302, 290–292.
Eisenberger, N.I., Master, S.L., Inagaki, T.K., Taylor, S.E., Shirinyan, D., Lieberman,
Acknowledgement M.D., Naliboff, B.D., 2011. Attachment figures activate a safety signal-related neural
region and reduce pain experience. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 108 (28),
11721–11726.
We thank Babette Renneberg for her kind help with establishing the
Fonagy, P., Gergely, G., Jurist, E.L., Target, M., 2004. Affektregulierung, Mentalisierung
cyberball paradigm. Moreover, we thank the psychologists, and stu- und die Entwicklung des Selbst. Klett-Cotta, Stuttg., 55–73.
dents, who provided support for the study: Anna Albert, Amalie Fonagy, P., Leigh, T., Steele, M., Steele, H., Kennedy, R., Mattoon, G., Target, M., Gerber,
Boldischar, Tanja Daltrozzo, Eva Holzamer, Constance Jacob, Ariane A., 1996. The relation of attachment status, psychiatric classification, and response
to psychotherapy. J. Consult. Clin. Psych. 64, 22–31.
Klughardt, Aischa Letters, Annika Plöger, Anja Palagyi, and Charlotte Fonagy, P., Steele, M., Steele, H., Target, M., 1998. Reflective function manual:
Wink. This work is part of the PhD thesis of C. Bauriedl-Schmidt. version5.0 for application to the adult attachment interviewUnpublished manual.
University College, London.
Gander, M., Buchheim, A., 2015. Attachment classification, psychophysiology and frontal
References EEG asymmetry across the lifespan: a review. Front. Hum. Neurosci., 9.
George, C., Kaplan, N., Main, M., 1985. The adult attachment interview. Unpublished
Agosti, V., 1999. One year clinical and psychosocial outcomes of early-onset chronic manuscript. University of California, Berkeley.
depression. J. Affect. Disord. 54 (1–2), 171–175. George, C., Buchheim, A., 2014. Use of the adult attachment projective picture system in
Allen, N.B., Badcock, P.B.T., 2003. The social risk hypothesis of depressed mood: psychodynamic psychotherapy with a severely traumatized patient. Front. Psychol. 5,
evolutionary, psychosocial, and neurobiological perspectives. Psychol. Bull. 129, 1–9.
887–913. George, C., Kaplan, N., Main, M., 1996. The Berkeley Adult Attachment Interview.
Angst, J., Gamm, A., Rössler, W., Ajdacic, V., Klein, D.N., 2009. Long-term depression Unpublished manuscriptDepartment of Psychology. University of California,
versus episodic major depression: results from the prospective Zurich study of a Berkeley.
community sample. J. Affect. Disord. 115, 112–121. George, C., Solomon, J., 2008. The caregiving system: a behavioural systems approach to
American Psychiatric Association, 1994. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental parenting. In: Cassidy, J., Shaver, P.R. (Eds.), Handbook of Attachment Theory,
disorders (4th ed., Rev.). Author, Washington, DC. Research, and Clinical Applications. Guilford Press, New York, 833–856.
American Psychiatric Association, 2013. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental George, C., West, M., 2001. The development and preliminary validation of a new
disorders (5th ed.). Author, Washington, DC measure of adult attachment: the Adult Attachment projective. Attach Hum. Dev. 3,
Bakermans-Kranenburg, M.J., van IJzendoorn, M.H., 2009. The first 10,000 adult 30–61.
attachment interviews: distributions of adult attachment representations in clinical George, C., West, M., 2004. The Adult Attachment Projective: measuring individual
and non-clinical groups. Attach. Hum. Dev. 11 (3), 223–263. differences in attachment security using projective methodology. (Personality
Banducci, A.N., Gomes, M., MacPherson, L., Lejuez, C.W., Potenza, M.N., Gelernter, J., assessment)In: Hilsenroth, M.J., Segal, D.L. (Eds.), Comprehensive Handbook of
Amstadter, A.B., 2013. A preliminary examination of the Relationship Between the Psychological Assessment 2. John Wiley & Sons Inc, Hoboken, NJ, 431–447.
5-HTTLPR and childhood emotional abuse on depressive symptoms in 10-12-year- George, C., West, M., 2011. The adult attachment projective picture system: integrating
old youth. Psychol. Trauma 6 (1), 1–7. attachment into clinical assessment. J. Pers. Assess. 93 (5), 407–416.
Berndt, E.R., Koran, L.M., Finkelstein, S.N., Gelenberg, A.J., Kornstein, S.G., Miller, I.M., George, C., West, M., 2012. The adult attachment projective picture system. Guilford
Thase, M.E., Trapp, G.A., Keller, M.B., 2000. Lost human capital from early- Press, New York.
onsetearly-onset chronic depression. Am. J. Psychiatry 157, 940–947. George, C., West, M., Pettem, O., 1999. The adult attachment projective: disorganization
Bernstein, D.P., Fink, L., 1998. Childhood trauma questionnaire: a retrospective self- of adult attachment at the level of representation. In: Solomon, J., George, C. (Eds.),
report manual. Psychol. Coop., San. Antonio Tx.. Attachment Disorganization.. Guilford Press, New York, 318–346.
Bowlby, J., 1969. Attachment and Loss: vol. 1 Attachment. Basic Books, New York. Grzyb, K.R., 2005. Sozialer Ausschluss und automatisches Zielstreben [Social exclusion
Bowlby, J., 1980. Attachment and Loss. Vol. 3 Loss: sadness and Depression. Basic and automatic goal pursuit]. Unpublished diploma thesis, Konstanz University,
Books, New York. Konstanz.
Bowlby, J., 1988. A Secure Base Clinical Applications of Attachment Theory. Routledge, Hamilton, M., 1960. A rating scale for depression. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 23,
London. 56–62.
Brockmeyer, T., Kulessa, D., Hautzinger, M., Bents, H., Backenstrass, M., 2015. Hartgerink, C.H.J., van Beest, I., Wicherts, J.M., Williams, K.D., 2015. The ordinal
Diffentiating early-onset chronic depression from episodic depression in terms of effects of ostracism: a meta-analysis of 120 Cyberball studies. PLoS ONE 10 (5),
cognitive-behavioral and emotional avoidance. J. Affect. Disord. 175, 418–423. e0127002. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.
Browne, C., Winkelman, C., 2007. The effect of childhood trauma on later psychological Hautzinger, M., Keller, F., Kühner, C., 2006. Das Beck Depressionsinventar II. Deutsche
adjustment. J. Interpers. Violence 22, 684–697. Bearbeitung und Handbuch zum BDI-II. Harcourt Test Service, Frankfurt.
Buchheim, A., George, C., 2011. Attachment disorganization in borderline personality Holmes, J., 2013. An attachment model of depression: Integrating findings from the
disorder and anxiety disorder. In: Solomon, J., George, C. (Eds.), Disorganized mood disorder laboratory. Psychiatry 76 (1), 68–86.
Attachment and Caregiving. Guilford Press, New York, 343–382. Holtzheimer, P.E., Mayberg, H.S., 2011. Stuck in a rut: rethinking depression and its
Buchheim, A., George, C., 2012. Das Adult Attachment Interview (AAI) und das Adult treatment. Trends Neurosci. 34 (1), 1–9.
Attachment Projective Picture System (AAP). In: Doering, S., Hörz, S. (Eds.), Jobst, A., Albert, A., Bauriedl-Schmidt, C., Mauer, M.-C., Renneberg, B., Buchheim, A.,
Handbuch der Strukturdiagnostik – Konzepte, Instrumente, Praxis. Schattauer Sabaß, L., Falkai, P., Zill, P., Padberg, F., 2014. Social exclusion leads to divergent
Verlag, Stuttgart, 182–218. changes of oxytocin levels in borderline patients versus healthy subjects. Psychother.
Buchheim, A., Viviani, R., Kessler, H., Kächele, H., Cierpka, M., Roth, G., George, C., Psychosom. 24, 252–254.
Kernberg, O., Bruns, G., Taubner, S., 2012. Changes in prefrontal-limbic function in Jobst, A., Sabass, L., Palagyi, A., Bauriedl-Schmidt, C., Mauer, C.M., Sarubin, N.,
major depression after 15 months of long-term psychotherapy. PLoS ONE 7 (3), Buchheim, A., Renneberg, B., Falkai, P., Zill, P., 2015. Effects of social exclusion on
e33745. emotions and oxytocin and cortisol levels in patients with chronic depression. J.
Buchheim, A., Viviani, R., Walter, H., 2013. Attachment narratives in depression: a Psychiatr. Res. 10, 170–177.
neurocognitive approach. J. Conscious. Stud. 20, 292–305. Jobst, A., Brakemeier, E.-L., Buchheim, A., Caspar, F., Cuijpers, P., Ebmeier, K.P., Falkai,
Chapman, D.P., Whitfield, C.L., Felitti, V.J., Dube, S.R., Edwards, V.J., Anda, R.F., 2004. P., Jan van der Gaag, R., Gaebel, W., Herpertz, S., Kurimay, T., Sabaß, L., Schnell, K.,
Adverse childhood experiences and the risk of depressive disorders in adulthood. J. Schramm, E., Torrent, C., Wasserman, D., Wiersma, J., Padberg, F., 2016. European
Affect. Disord. 82, 217–225. Psychiatric Association Guidance on psychotherapy in chronic depression across
Charuvastra, A., Cloitre, M., 2008. Social Bonds and Posttraumatic Stress Disorder. Europe. Eur. Psychiatry 33, 16–36.
Annu. Rev. Psychol. 59, 301–328. Jonsson, U., Bohman, H., von Knorring, L., Olsson, G., Paaren, A., von Knorring, A.L.,
Connor, K.M., Davidson, J.R.T., 2003. Development of a new resilience scale: the connor 2011. Mental health outcome of long-term and episodic adolescent depression: 15-
davidson resilience scale (CD-RISC). Depress Anxiety 18, 76–82. year follow-up of a community sample. J. Affect. Disord. 130, 395–404.
Coyne, J.C., 1976. Toward an interactional description of depression. Psychiatry 39, Juen, F., Arnold, L., Meissner, D., Nolte, T., Buchheim, A., 2013. Attachment
28–34. disorganization in different clinical groups: What underpins unresolved attachment?
D’Andrea, W., Stolbach, B., Ford, J., Spinazzola, J., van der Kolk, B.A., 2012. Psihologija 46 (2), 127–141.
Understanding interpersonal trauma in children: why We need a developmentally Keller, M.B., McCullough, J.P., Klein, D.N., Arnow, B., Dunner, D.L., Gelenberg, A.J.,
appropriate trauma diagnosis. Am. J. Orthopsychiatry 82 (2), 187–200. Markowitz, J.C., Nemeroff, C.B., Russell, J.M., Thase, M.E., Trivedi, M.H., Zajecka,
Davison, G.C., Neale, J.M., Hautzinger, M., 2007. Klinische Psychologie (seventh ed.). J., 2000. A comparison of nefazodone, the cognitive behavioral analysis system of
Beltz PVU. Basel, Weinheim. psychotherapy, and their combination for the treatment of chronic depression. N.

137
C. Bauriedl-Schmidt et al. Journal of Affective Disorders 210 (2017) 130–138

Engl. J. Med. 342, 1462–1470. Schramm, E., Zobel, I., Dykierek, P., Kech, S., Brakemeier, E.-L., Külz, A., Berger, M.,
Kessler, R.C., Foster, C.L., Saunders, W.B., Stang, P.E., 1995. Social consequences of 2011. Cognitive behavioral analysis system of psychotherapy versus interpersonal
psychiatric disorders, I: Educational attainment. Am. J. Psychiatry 152, 1026–1032. psychotherapy for early-onset chronic depression: a randomized pilot study. J.
Klein, D.N., Schatzberg, A.F., McCullough, J.P., Dowling, F., Goodman, D., Howland, Affect. Disord. 129, 109–116.
R.H., Markowitz, J.C., Smith, C., Thase, M.E., Rush, A.J., LaVange, L., Harrison, Staebler, K., 2008. Emotional Responses in Borderline Personality Disorder.
W.M., Keller, M.B., 1999. Age of onset in chronic major depression: relation to Dissertation, Free University Berlin.
demographic and clinical variables, family history and treatment response. J. Affect. Staebler, K., Renneberg, B., Stopsack, M., Fiedler, P., Weiler, M., Roepke, S., 2011. Facial
Disord. 55, 149–157. emotional expression in reaction to social exclusion in borderline personality
Klein, D.N., Shankman, S.A., Lewinsohn, P.M., Rohde, P., Seeley, J.R., 2004. Family disorder. Psychol. Med. 41 (9), 1929–1938.
Study of Chronic Depression in a Community Sample of Young Adults. Am. J. Trivedi, M.H., Daly, E.J., 2008. Treatment strategies to improve and sustain remission in
Psychiatry 161, 646–653. major depressive disorder. Dialog. Clin. Neurosci. 10 (4), 377–384.
Klein, D.N., Shankman, S.A., Rose, S., 2006. Ten-year prospective follow-up study of the Ward, M.J., Lee, S.S., Polan, H.J., 2006. Attachment and psychopathology in a
naturalistic course of dysthymic disorder and double depression. Am. J. Psychiatry community sample. Attach Hum. Dev. 8 (4), 327–340.
163, 872–880. Waters, T.E.A., Fraley, R.C., Groh, A.M., Steele, R.D., Vaughn, B.E., Bost, K.K.,
Klein, J.P., Willenborg, B., Schweiger, U., 2014. Diagnostik affektiver Störungen im Verissimo, M., Coppola, G., 2015. The latent structure of secure base Script
DSM-5: Neuerungen und Kontroversen. (diagnosis of affective disorders in DSM-5: knowledge. Dev. Psychol. 51 (6), 823–830.
changes and controversies). Fortschr. Neurol. Psychiatr. 82, 655–664. West, M., George, C., 2002. Attachment and dysthymia: the contributions of preoccupied
Koehler, S., Wiethoff, K., Ricken, R., Stamm, T., Baghai, T.C., Fisher, R., Seemüller, F., attachment and agency of self to depression in woman. Attach. Hum. Dev. 4,
Brieger, P., Cordes, J., Malevani, J., Laux, G., Hauth, I., Moeller, H.-J., Zeiler, J., 278–293.
Heinz, A., Bauer, M., Adli, M., 2015. Characteristics and differences in treatment WHO Department of Mental Health and Substance Abuse, Marcus, Marina, Yasamy, M.
outcome of inpatients with chronic vs. episodic major depressive disorders. J. Affect. Taghi, Mark van, Ommeren, Chisholm, Dan, Saxena, Shekhar, 2012. Depression A
Disord. 173, 126–133. Global. Public Health, Burden〈http://www.who.int/mental_health/management/
Kornstein, S.G., Schneider, R.K., 2001. Clinical features of treatment-resistant depression/who_paper_depression_wfmh_2012.pdf〉.
depression. J. Clin. Psychiatry 62 (6), 18–25. Wiersma, J.E., van Oppen, P., van Schaik, D.J., van der Does, A.J., Beekman, A.T.,
Lizardi, H., Klein, D.N., Ouimette, P.C., Riso, L.P., Anderson, R.L., Donaldson, S.K., Pennix, B.W., 2011. Psychological characteristics of chronic depression:depression a:
1995. Reports of the childhood home environment in early-onset dysthymia and a longitudinal cohort study. J. Clin. Psychiatry 72 (3), 288–294.
episodic major depression. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 104 (1), 132–139. Wiersma, J.E., Hovens, J.G., van Oppen, P., Giltay, E.J., van Schaik, D.J., Beekman, A.T.,
Main, M., 1995. Recent studies in attachment. In: Goldberg, S., Muir, R., Kerr, J. (Eds.), Penninx, B.W., 2009. The importance of childhood trauma and childhood life events
Attachment Theory: Social, Developmental, and Clinical Perspectives. Analytic Press, for chronicity of depression. J. Clin. Psychiatry 70, 983–989.
Hillsdale N.J, 467–474. Williams, K.D., 2009. Ostracism: a temporal need-threat model. In: Zanna, M. (Ed.),
Main, M., Goldwyn, R., 1985. Adult-1998. Attachment scoring and classification system, Advances in Experimental Social Psychology 41. Academic Press, New York,
Version 6 .0.]Unpublished manuscript|. Berkeley: University of California. 279–314.
Masten, C.L., Eisenberger, N.I., Borofsky, L.A., McNeally, K., Pfeifer, J.H., Dapretto, M., Williams, K.D., Cheung, C.K.T., Choi, W., 2000. Cyberostracism: effects of being ignored
2011. Subgenual anterior cingulate responses to peer rejection: a marker of over the Internet. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 79, 748–762.
adolescents' risk for depression. Dev. Psychopathol. 23, 283–292. Williams, K.D., Gerber, J., 2005. Ostracism – The making of the ignored and excluded
McCullough, J.P., 2000. Treatment for Chronic Depression: Cognitive Behavioral mind: Interact. Stud.. 6 (3), 359–374.
Analysis System of Psychotherapy (CBASP). The Guilford Press, New York. Williams, K.D., Jarvis, B., 2006. Cyberball: a program for use in research on
McCullough, J.P., 2003. Treatment for chronic depression:depression: cognitive interpersonal ostracism and acceptance. Behav. Res. Methods 38, 174–180.
behavioral analysis system of psychotherapy (CBASP). J. Psychother. Integr. 13 (3/ Wirth, J.H., Sacco, D.F., Hugenberg, K., Williams, K.D., 2010. Eye gaze as relational
4), 241–263. evaluation: averted eye gaze leads to feelings of ostracism and relational devaluation.
MoellerH.J., BandelowB., BauerM., HampelH., HerpertzS.C., SoykaM., BarnikolU.B., Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 36 (7), 869–882.
ListaS., SeverusE., MaierW., 2014. DSM-5 reviewed from different angles: goal Wittchen, H.U., Zaudig, M., Fydrich, T., 1997. Strukturiertes Klinisches Interview für
attainment, rationality, use of evidence, consequences –part 1: general aspects and DSM-IV. Achse I: Psychische Störungen/ Achse II: Persönlichkeitsstörungen.
paradigmatic discussion of depressive disorders. Eur. Arch. Psychiatry Clin. Hogrefe Verlag, Göttingen.
Neurosci. Zadro, L., Williams, K.D., Richardson, R., 2004. How low can you go? Ostracism by a
Ravitz, P., Maunder, R., McBride, C., 2007. Attachment, contemporary interpersonal computer is sufficient to lower self-reported levels of belonging, control, self-esteem,
theory and IPT: An integration of theoretical, clinical, and empirical perspectives. J. and meaningful existence. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 40, 560–567.
Contemp. Psychother., 38 (11), 11–21. Zadro, L., Boland, C., Richardson, R., 2006. How long does it last? The persistence of the
Saß, H., Wittchen, H.U., Zaudig, M., 1998. Diagnostisches und Statistisches Manual effects of ostracism in the socially anxious. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 42, 692–697.
Psychischer Störungen. Hogrefe, Göttingen.

138

Você também pode gostar