Você está na página 1de 11

Brain Imaging and Behavior (2017) 11:1862–1872

DOI 10.1007/s11682-016-9660-0

ORIGINAL RESEARCH

Reversal learning reveals cognitive deficits and altered prediction


error encoding in the ventral striatum in Huntington’s disease
Katharina Nickchen 1,2,3 & Rebecca Boehme 2,4 & Maria del Mar Amador 1 &
Thomas D. Hälbig 1 & Katharina Dehnicke 1,5 & Patricia Panneck 1,2 & Joachim Behr 2,6 &
Konstantin Prass 5 & Andreas Heinz 2 & Lorenz Deserno 2,7 & Florian Schlagenhauf 2,7 &
Josef Priller 1,2,8

Published online: 5 December 2016


# Springer Science+Business Media New York 2016

Abstract Huntington’s disease (HD) is an autosomal functional magnetic resonance imaging. We studied 24
dominant neurodegenerative condition characterized by HD patients without central nervous system (CNS)-ac-
a triad of movement disorder, neuropsychiatric symp- tive medication and 25 healthy controls. Twenty HD pa-
toms and cognitive deficits. The striatum is particularly tients and 24 healthy controls were able to complete the task.
vulnerable to the effects of mutant huntingtin, and cell Computational modeling was used to calculate prediction error
loss can already be found in presymptomatic stages. values and estimate individual parameters. We observed that
Since the striatum is well known for its role in rein- gray matter density and prediction error signals during the learn-
forcement learning, we hypothesized to find altered be- ing task were related to disease stage. HD patients in advanced
havioral and neural responses in HD patients in a prob- disease stages appear to use a less complex strategy in the rever-
abilistic reinforcement learning task performed during sal learning task. In contrast, HD patients in early disease stages
show intact encoding of learning signals in the degenerating left
ventral striatum. This effect appears to be lost with disease
Katharina Nickchen and Rebecca Boehme contributed equally to this progression.
work.
Keywords Huntington’s disease . Reinforcement learning .
* Josef Priller Ventral striatum . Gray matter density
josef.priller@charite.de

1
Department of Neuropsychiatry, Charité –Universitätsmedizin Berlin
(Charité Campus Mitte), Charitéplatz 1, 10117 Berlin, Germany
Introduction
2
Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy,
Charité –Universitätsmedizin Berlin (Charité Campus Mitte),
Huntington’s disease (HD) is a rare autosomal dominant neu-
10117 Berlin, Germany rodegenerative condition caused by a CAG trinucleotide re-
3
Fliedner Klinik Berlin, 10117 Berlin, Germany
peat expansion in the Huntingtin gene located on the short arm
4
of chromosome 4 (4p16.3). HD commonly presents in midlife
Center for Social and Affective Neuroscience, Linköping University,
58245 Linköping, Sweden
with a combination of a progressive movement disorder, neu-
5
ropsychiatric symptoms, e.g. depression, irritability, apathy, as
Department of Neurology, Helios Klinikum Bad Saarow, 15526 Bad
Saarow, Germany
well as cognitive deficits with executive dysfunctions and
6
dementia (Barker and Priller 2013). HD is characterized by
Department of Psychiatry, Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics,
Medical School Brandenburg - Campus Neuruppin,
neuronal dysfunction, shrinkage and ultimately neuronal loss
16816 Neuruppin, Germany in a wide range of brain structures. The caudate and putamen
7
Max-Planck-Fellow-Group Cognitive and Affective Control of
appear to be particularly vulnerable and show significant at-
Behavioral Adaptation, Max-Planck-Institute for Human Cognitive rophy even in presymptomatic stages (Aylward et al. 2011;
and Brain Sciences, 04103 Leipzig, Germany Kipps et al. 2005; Weir et al. 2011).
8
Cluster of Excellence NeuroCure, BIH and DZNE, Substantial evidence has implicated frontostriatal circuits
10117 Berlin, Germany in reinforcement learning. Activity of midbrain dopaminergic
Brain Imaging and Behavior (2017) 11:1862–1872 1863

neurons was shown to encode the difference between received not finish the fMRI task because of a panic attack, and did not
and expected outcome (Montague et al. 1996; Schultz et al. undergo further physical and neuropsychological examina-
1997), termed the ‘reward prediction error’ (RPE). This is in tion. Three HD patients and one HC were not able to complete
line with findings in humans using functional magnetic reso- the fMRI task.
nance imaging (fMRI). Coding of such RPEs was repeatedly In the end, 20 HD patients and 24 HC were included in the
shown in the striatum, which receives dopaminergic input from final analysis. The mean age of this final HD cohort was
the midbrain (D’Ardenne et al. 2008; O’Doherty et al. 2003). 47.5 years ± 10.3 years. The median number of CAG-repeats
Given this involvement of the striatum in reinforcement was 43 (range 40–50). The median Unified Huntington’s dis-
learning and its early dysfunction in HD, deficits in rein- ease rating scale (UHDRS) motor score was 18 (range 1–58).
forcement learning in HD patients are to be expected. The median total cognitive score was 206 (range 120–319).
Indeed, a behavioral study showed deficits in probabilistic Twelve HD patients had a total functional capacity (TFC)
discrimination and reversal learning in HD patients com- between 11 and 13, eight patients had a TFC ≤ 10. The mean
pared to controls (Lawrence et al. 1999). However, to date age in HC was 46.8 years ± 11.3 years. The median total cog-
only few studies have investigated reinforcement learning nitive score in HC was 316.5 (range 265–406) (Table 1).
in HD patients in relation to neurobiological measures. None of the investigated subjects showed clinical signs of
Using a Monetary Incentive Delay task, Enzi et al. reported depression or psychosis. The median mini-mental state exam-
an impaired association of activity in the left ventral stria- ination (MMSE) scores were 28.5 (range 22–30) in HD pa-
tum with outcome anticipation in pre-HD subjects near to tients, and 29 (range 26–30) in HC. One HD patient showed
disease onset (Enzi et al. 2012). Recently, another study mild signs of dementia reaching 22 points on the MMSE.
using the Monetary Incentive task found evidence for dif- Overall, HD patients had received a median 14 years of edu-
ferentially enhanced reward-related fMRI signaling in the cation (range 8–18), whereas HC had received a median
ventral striatum and orbitofrontal cortex/anterior insula in 17 years of education (range 12–20) (Table 1).
prodromal HD (Malejko et al. 2014). Two patients had UHDRS motor scores < 5 and were there-
Palminteri et al. used an instrumental learning paradigm fore classified as pre-manifest HD mutation carriers with dis-
and combined this behavioral task with structural measures ease burden (CAG index) scores of 210 and 264, respectively.
in HD patients (Palminteri et al. 2012). Presymptomatic HD One HD patient and 5 HC consumed ten or more cigarettes
subjects with accentuated dorsal striatal atrophy were im- per day. Alcohol use disorder was an exclusion criterion.
paired in punishment learning but not reward learning. In con-
trast, symptomatic patients (mostly medicated with antipsy- Neuropsychological testing
chotic drugs) with atrophy in ventral and dorsal striatum
showed impairments in learning from reward and punishment After fMRI, patients and controls performed four neuropsy-
compared with presymptomatic HD subjects and healthy con- chological tests for reaction time and attentional function
trols. The behavioral deficits were associated with striatal de- based on the Test battery for Attentional Performance (TAP,
generation following a dorsoventral gradient pattern of gray Zimmermann and Fimm, Psychologische Testsysteme,
matter loss. Herzogenrath, Germany):
So far, neuronal representation of learning signals like
RPEs has not been investigated in HD. Here, we investigated 1) Alertness without warning tone (simple reaction to a vi-
24 symptomatic HD patients without CNS-active medication sual stimulus): In this test, the average time a subject
and 25 healthy controls in a reversal learning task using fMRI. needs to press a button after a white cross (x) appears
We used computational modeling of reinforcement learning to on a black computer screen is measured.
describe changes in learning dynamically, and to provide 2) Alertness with warning tone (reaction to a visual stimulus
quantitative fits and parameters for individual behavior. We after a warning tone): This test includes the same task as
hypothesized that ventral striatal RPEs signals are reduced in in 1), but a warning tone appears before the cross is shown
HD patients, and that this reduction is associated with the on the screen.
stage of disease and the degree of ventral striatal atrophy. 3) Divided attention (reaction to simultaneous visual and
auditory stimuli): In this task, white crosses and points
move in a four-by-four pattern while a sequence of tones
Material and methods in two different frequencies is played. Subjects are
instructed to press a button whenever four crosses appear
Subjects in the form of a square on the screen and whenever two
tones of the same frequency follow each other. Reaction
Twenty-four HD patients and 25 age-/sex-matched healthy times to visual and auditory stimuli are measured
controls (HC) were included in this study. One patient could separately.
1864 Brain Imaging and Behavior (2017) 11:1862–1872

Table 1 Sample characteristics


HD (n = 20) HC (n = 24) HD vs. HC
P (t-test)

Age (mean ± SD) 47.5 ± 10.3 years 46.8 ± 11.3 years P = 0.8
Sex (male/female) 14/6 14/10
Educational years (median) 14, range 8–18 17, range 12–20 P = 0.006
CAG repeats (median) 43, range 40–50 n.a.
CAG index (median) 351, range 210–663 n.a.
UHDRS motor score (median) 18, range 1–58 n.a.
TFC (median) 11, range 4–13 n.a.
UHDRS total cognitive score (median) 206, range 120–319 317, range 265–406 P < 0.0001
MMSE (median) 28.5, range 22–30 29, range 26–30 P = 0.014

4) Go/NoGo (Selective attention): Subjects are instructed to reversal occurred with a probability of 20 % during one of the
press a button only when the letter Bx^ appears on the next trials after the criterion was fulfilled.
screen, but not after the sign B + ^ appears. Errors are Twenty patients and 24 controls were included in the final
measured. analysis of the fMRI data. One patient had to discontinue the
MRI examination because of a panic attack. To ensure that
only participants who understood the task were entered into
the final analysis, three HD patients and one HC had to be
Probabilistic reversal learning task excluded from the analysis because they achieved less than 10
reversals. All three excluded HD patients had advanced dis-
A probabilistic reversal learning task was administered during ease stages with low TFC scores (7, 5, 3) and high UHDRS
fMRI. This task required the subjects to detect a reversal in motor scores (29, 52, 72).
reward probabilities and to adapt their behavior accordingly.
To maximize reward, participants had to maintain the advan- Reinforcement learning algorithm
tageous response even when probabilistic errors occurred.
Each participant was introduced to the task in a training ses- A reinforcement learning model was applied to dynamically
sion without reversals. Subjects were instructed to try to win quantify learning, and to generate prediction errors as
as much money as possible and to always try to find the
currently best symbol. After the first training run, we
interviewed the participants to ensure they understood the
task. In case they did not, the instruction and the training were
repeated. Afterwards, they performed the task for the duration
of approximately 30 min (400 trials) in the fMRI scanner. Two
symbols were displayed randomly left and right on the screen
(Fig. 1). The subject had to press one of two buttons to choose
the target as quickly as possible (maximum response time:
1.5 s). Otherwise, the message BToo slow!^ was presented
and the experiment continued. A white square surrounded
the chosen symbol and simultaneously positive or negative
feedback occurred: Feedback for the correct answer consisted
of the picture of a 10 Cent coin and the message BWin!
+10Cent^, feedback for the wrong answer consisted of the
same picture with a crossed out coin and the message BLoss!
-10Cent^. Feedback was presented for 1 s. During the expo-
nentially jittered intertrial interval (1–6.5 s), a fixation cross Fig. 1 Probabilistic reversal learning task: One trial consists of stimulus
appeared on the screen. One of the stimuli was associated with presentation with a response time window (1.5 s), feedback (1 s) and an
80 % monetary reward and 20 % loss, and vice versa. In the intertrial interval of 1.5 s to 6.5 s. Choosing the currently ‘good’ stimulus
leads with a higher probability (80 %) to a reward than choosing the other
main task, these probability assignments switched after the option (20 %). After achieving the criterion of 5 correct answers out of the
participant had learned them. The criterion was defined as 5 last 6 trials, the chance of a reversal of the probability distribution
correct choices out of a sliding window of the last 6 trials. The becomes 20 % for the following trials
Brain Imaging and Behavior (2017) 11:1862–1872 1865

regressors for the analysis of fMRI data. We used a modified (TR) = 2060 ms, echo time (TE) = 30 ms, slice thickness
Q-learning algorithm, which learns the expected value Q of 2.5 mm, matrix size 64*64 and field of view (FOV)
the chosen option c based on the outcome of previous trials 224*224 mm2, thus yielding an in-plane voxel resolution of
(Sutton and Barto 1998). We adapted this model in order to 3.5 mm2, flip angle = 80°). Fieldmaps used to correct for inho-
update both options during one trial simultaneously (Glascher mogeneities in the magnetic field were acquired with the follow-
et al. 2010; Hauser et al. 2014; Schlagenhauf et al. 2014). At ing parameters: TR = 434 ms, TE = 5.19 ms (first) and 7.65 ms
each trial t, expectation values were adjusted according to the (second), slice thickness = 3.5 mm, matrix size = 64*64 and
received feedback: FOV = 224*224 mm2, voxel size = 3.5*3.5*3.5 mm3, flip an-
gle = 60°. Two 3D anatomical images of the whole brain were
Qc;t ¼ Qc;t−1 þ αδQc;t−1
obtained using a T1-weighted 3D spoiled-gradient echo pulse
sequence with TR = 1900 ms, TE = 2.52 ms, matrix size
Here, α determines the individual learning rate and δ, the
256*256, FOV 256*256 mm2, voxel size 1*1*1 mm3, flip an-
prediction error, is defined as the difference between the ex-
gle = 9°. The first anatomical image was acquired before, the
pected and the actually received reinforcement R:
second after the reversal task. Anatomical images were acquired
δQc;t ¼ Rc;t −c;t Qc;t parallel to the anterior commissure-posterior commissure line,
functional images and fieldmaps were tilted by 25°. fMRI data
Instead of coding reward and punishment simply as 1 and were analyzed using SPM8 (http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm)
−1, we let them vary individually as additional free parameters in Matlab R2010b (The MathWorks, Natick, MA, USA).
for reward and punishment sensitivity. For preprocessing, the following steps were performed:
The double-update algorithm learns values of the unchosen correction for differences in slice time acquisition and
option u, which was additionally modulated by the weighing motion correction including unwarping by using the ac-
parameter κ: quired fieldmaps, co-registration of the mean EPI and the
mean of the two anatomical images, spatial normalization
Qu;t ¼ Qu;t−1 −καδQu;t−1
and segmentation into tissue classes of the T1 image
A softmax estimated the probability of choices based on using the unified segmentation approach as implemented
the model-derived values. The softmax equation calculates the in SPM8 (Ashburner and Friston 2005). Normalization
likelihood of a subject choosing action a over b on trial t, parameters were applied to all functional images. Data
which is assumed to be proportional to the expected value of were spatially smoothed with an isotropic Gaussian kernel
this option. of 8 mm full width at half maximum kernel. A voxel-
based morphometry analysis was used to estimate gray
expðQa ðt ÞÞ matter density on the basis of tissue probability maps
pa ðt Þ ¼
expðQa ðt ÞÞ þ expðQb ðt ÞÞ implemented in SPM8. Unmodulated maps from the seg-
mentation step were compared between the two groups
The set of 4 free parameters Θ were fitted individually for using a two-sample t-test.
each participant by maximizing the sum of the negative log For statistical analysis of the fMRI data, the preprocessed
likelihood of the sequence of choice c made in trial t: images were analyzed using the general linear model ap-
∑ logPðct jΘÞ: proach as implemented in SPM8. Data analysis was per-
t
Choice behavior of each participant was explained at least formed in an event-related manner by modeling feedback on-
better than chance (based on the likelihood that the observed set events and adding the prediction errors derived from the
data is given by the parameters). Thus, time-series derived learning model as a parametric modulator. These regressors
from these parameters reflect important aspects of the exper- were convolved with the canonical hemodynamic response
imental data and can therefore be regressed against imaging function provided by SPM8.
data in a meaningful way. For between-group comparison, all No significant difference was found between movement
four parameters were entered into a multivariate analysis of parameters of HC and HD patients in an ANOVA analysis
variance (MANOVA). of translational movement (all F < 3 for p < 0.05). In addition,
in order to account for movement-associated variance, the six
realignment parameters as well as their first temporal deriva-
MRI data analysis tive and a regressor censoring scans with more than 1 mm
scan-to-scan movement were included in the model as addi-
Functional MRI was conducted using a 3.0 Tesla Siemens trio tional regressors of no interest. The individual contrast images
scanner with a 12-channel head coil to acquire gradient echo were then taken to a random effects group-level analysis (one-
T2*-weighted echo-planar images. 905 EPI volumes (approx. sample t-test for within group and two-sample t-test for be-
31 min) containing 38 slices were measured (repetition time tween group analyses).
1866 Brain Imaging and Behavior (2017) 11:1862–1872

For correction of multiple comparisons, statistics are re- Computational modeling of behavior
ported using family wise error (FWE) small volume correction
(SVC) at the peak level for a priori region of interest (ROI), With respect to the modeling analysis, the parameter x group
the ventral striatum. A mask for the nucleus accumbens de- MANOVA revealed a significant group difference for reward
rived from the WFUPickAtlas tool using IBASPM 71 sensitivity Rrew (mean Rrew: HC = 13.4 ± 11.9, HD = 5.7 ± 3.8,
(Maldjian et al. 2003) was applied. Whole brain results are F(1,43) = 7.62, P < 0.01) and for learning rate α (mean α:
reported FWE-corrected at peak level and cluster level HC = 0.72 ± 0.1, HD = 0.78 ± 0.1, F(1,43) = 4.16, P < 0.05). In
(assessed at an uncorrected threshold of p < 0.001). addition, UHDRS motor score values of HD patients correlated
Parameter estimates were extracted from the ventral-striatal negatively with κ (R = −0.5, P < 0.05), indicating that more
ROI by averaging across all voxels of this ROI. Since we advanced HD patients tended to use a simpler single-update
had a strong a priori hypothesis about the direction of associ- strategy instead of a double-update model, where expectations
ations between the data and measures of disease severity, cor- for both stimuli are updated during each trial.
relational analysis on these data were performed using one-
sided two-sample t-tests. Gray matter density values from the Structural imaging
VBM analysis were extracted using the same ROI, then aver-
aged over both hemispheres to provide a covariate for RPE- Voxel-based morphometry (VBM) analysis revealed a signif-
analysis in the patient group. icant reduction in gray matter density for HD patients com-
pared to HC in the whole striatum (Fig. 3a and Table 3) in-
cluding its ventral part (left [−8 8–12], t = 3.98, right [6 8–10],
t = 3.87, both P FWE SVC nucleus accumbens < 0.01). There was no
Results difference in gray matter density between the two hemispheres
in the HD group (p = 0.2). The reduction in ventral striatum
Behavior gray matter density correlated negatively with patients’ motor
scores, a measure for the severity of motor symptoms with a
During probabilistic reversal learning, HD patients displayed higher score indicating more motor symptoms (right nucleus
impaired performance compared to HC. HD patients made accumbens r = −0.548, P < 0.05, trend in left nucleus accum-
less correct choices (mean ± sd: HC = 0.72 ± 0.04, HD = bens r = −0.425 P = 0.062; Figs. 3b, c).
0.69 ± 0.05, t = 2.4, P < 0.05), and achieved less reversal In line with the group differences on modeling parameters,
stages (mean ± sd: HC = 26.13 ± 5.3, HD = 21.65 ± 5.4, t = gray matter density correlated positively with reward sensitiv-
2.8, P < 0.001; Fig. 2). A closer inspection of the learning ity Rrew (nucleus accumbens left: R = 0.047, P < 0.01, right:
curve averaged over all reversal stages suggested that HD R = 0.04, P < 0.01), and negatively with learning rate α (left
patients were able to adapt their behavior after a reversal, but and right: R = −0.41, P < 0.01) in the HD group. In addition to
were impaired in maintaining the advantageous choice behav- this analysis, which was focused on the ventral striatum as our
ior (group by trial interaction F = 4.13, P = 0.005; Fig. 2). region of interest, we also conducted an exploratory correla-
In order to control for differences in reaction times and tional analysis on the whole brain level. Here, we found no
impulsive behavior, subjects performed several subtests of significant association between modeling parameters and gray
the TAP. HD patients showed slower reaction times than matter density. Using a less stringent threshold (p < 0.001,
HC in the subtests Alertness without warning tone and uncorrected for multiple comparisons, cluster size > 40
Alertness with warning tone as well as in the Divided voxels), we found a negative correlation between learning rate
attention task (Table 2). However, the number of errors and bilateral gray matter density in the cerebellum (left: [−36
in the Go/NoGo task did not differ between both groups −60 −42], cluster size = 93, t = 6.52, p (uncorr.) < 0.001; right
(Table 2), suggesting that the ability of HD patients to [34–62 −42], cluster size = 42, t = 4.9, p (uncorr.) < 0.001).
control impulsive behavior was not significantly reduced
in this test. Functional imaging
When taking into account slower reaction times of the HD
group, the behavioral differences did not remain significant The probabilistic reversal learning task elicited the expected
(ANCOVA with reaction times as covariate: % correct effect of a RPE signal bilaterally in the ventral striatum in HC
choices: F = 2.3, p = 0.14; number of reversals: F = 2.7, p = (left [−12 5–11], t = 3.82, right [12 11–11], t = 3.61, both P FWE
0.1; % switches: F = 2.1, p = 0.15). However, since reaction SVC nucleus accumbens < 0.01; Fig. 4a), and activation in a RPE
time (here: Alertness without warning tone) is highly related network including insular cortex, cingulate cortex and frontal
to motor symptoms (correlation with UHDRS motor score: and parietal regions (cluster level FWE corrected; Table 4). In
r = 0.56, p = 0.01), this measure could be understood as an- HD patients, RPE-associated activation approached signifi-
other indicator of disease stage. cance only in the left ventral striatum ([−12 5–8], t = 2.2, P
Brain Imaging and Behavior (2017) 11:1862–1872 1867

Fig. 2 HD patients are impaired in the probabilistic reversal learning the same performance level as HC as depicted by the learning curves
task. a HD patients achieve significantly less reversal stages than HC. b averaged over the first ten trials after each reversal. Error bars indicate
After a reversal, patients are able to adapt their behavior, but do not reach standard deviation

FWE SVC nucleus accumbens = 0.091; Fig. 4b). Here, RPE parameter symptom group. CAG-repeat lengths ranged from 40 to 45 in
estimates correlated negatively with UHDRS motor score the low symptom group (median = 40), and from 41 to 50 in
values (R = −0.4, P < 0.05). No significant group difference the high symptom group (median = 46.5). The median TFC
for the RPE between HC and HD was obtained. score was 12 (range = 10–13) in the low symptom group, and
Based on the observed correlation of symptom severity 10 (range = 4–13) in the high symptom group. MMSE scores
with structural and functional changes, we expected an asso- ranged from 26 to 30 in the low symptom group (median =
ciation between gray matter density and the RPE-related func- 29), and from 22 to 30 in the high symptom group (median =
tional activation in patients. Indeed, RPE signal in HD patients 27). Total cognitive scores ranged from 213 to 319 in the low
in the left ventral striatum correlated with the degree of gray symptom group (median = 278), and from 120 to 255 in the
matter reduction in this area (R = 0.42, P < 0.05). Given the high symptom group (median = 183). Patients in the low
apparent reduction in gray matter in the striatum, we used the symptom group had a median of 14 years of education
individual average of gray matter density from nucleus ac- (range = 13–18), patients in the high symptom group also
cumbens bilaterally as a covariate. This recovered the PE- had a median of 14 years of education (range = 8–18)
related activation in the HD group in the left ventral striatum (Table 5).
([−12 8–8], t = 3.38, P FWE SVC nucleus accumbens = 0.002). Comparison of these two subgroups revealed that the sig-
Given the heterogeneity in clinical severity in our HD co- nificant RPE signal in the left ventral striatum of HD patients
hort, we explored possible subgroup effects by using the was driven by the less symptomatic group with lower motor
UHDRS motor score as a measure of disease severity for scores. In this group, the RPE signal was significant in the left
creating two subgroups of patients by median split. Motor nucleus accumbens ([−12 8–11], t = 4.64, P FWE SVC nucleus
score values ranged from 1 to 11 in the low symptom group accumbens < 0.01), but not on the right. In the high motor score
(mean = 6.7 ± 3.5), and from 18 to 58 in the high symptom group (moderate to severe motor impairment), no significant
group (mean = 28 ± 12). Seven males and two females were in activation was found (P FWE SVC nucleus accumbens > 0.3).
the low symptom group, seven males and four females were in This result was confirmed in a VOI analysis comparing the
the high symptom group. The mean age was 43.4 ± 9.6 years parameter estimates in the left ventral striatum, which revealed
in the low symptom group, and 50.8 ± 10.0 years in the high a significant effect of group (F = 3.2, p = 0.049). This was

Table 2 TAP results


HD (n = 20) HC (n = 24) HD vs. HC

Mean SD Mean SD P (t-test)

Alertness without warning tone 338.9 ms 84.7 ms 235.6 ms 34.8 ms P < 0.0001
Alertness with warning tone 335.9 ms 92.9 ms 233.4 ms 41.9 ms P < 0.0001
Divided attention (visual) 1031.4 ms 273.6 ms 837.8 ms 136.8 ms P < 0.01
Divided attention (audible) 759.5 ms 210.9 ms 573.0 ms 86.8 ms P < 0.001
Go/NoGo (mistakes) 2.2 1.9 1.6 2.3 P = 0.345
1868 Brain Imaging and Behavior (2017) 11:1862–1872

Fig. 3 a Voxel-based morphometry (VBM) results reveal a significant main effect as depicted in a. c Overlay of the correlation, here depicted in
reduction in gray matter density in HD patients in the striatum compared blue, over the contrast between controls and patients as seen in a, for
to healthy controls. b Individual UHDRS motor score values of HD illustrative purposes thresholded at P = 0.001 uncorr., y = 9, color bar
patients correlate with gray matter density in the striatum (masked with indicates t-values

driven by a difference between the two HD subgroups reward sensitivity and an increased learning rate in the patient
(P < 0.05; Fig. 5). The HC group was not different from any group. Interestingly, these parameters were associated with
of the two HD groups, but was descriptively found in between gray matter reductions in the ventral striatum. On the neural
(mean HC = 0.22 ± 0.35, mean low symptom HD = 3.5 ± level, we found RPE encoding in the ventral striatum in the
0.23, mean high symptom HD = 0.006 ± 0.26). patient group when taking into account the gray matter al-
terations. An exploratory analysis with two subgroups in-
dicated that there might be an overactivation during early
Discussion disease stage.
Notably, we observed that HD patients (except three of
This is the first study to examine reversal learning in symp- them) were able to adapt their behavior after a reversal had
tomatic HD patients without CNS-active medication using occurred, but were impaired in maintaining the advantageous
functional and structural MRI. We found that HD patients choice behavior, i.e. they switched more often between sym-
were able to perform the reversal learning task, even though bols before the next reversal occurred. This effect is captured
they reached less reversals than healthy controls. by the higher learning rate in HD patients. Our observation is
Computational modeling of behavior revealed a reduced in accordance with the results of a neuropsychological study

Table 3 Regions showing a


significant (P < 0.05) difference p cluster k p peak t x y z
between HC and HD in VBM
analysis at cluster level FEW- Caudate 0.000 2392 0.01 6.14 20 12 14
corrected (assessed at P < 0.001) Putamen 0.451 4.55 22 5 5
Thalamus 0.599 4.4 −20 −22 20
Subcallosal Gyrus 0.981 3.9 5 8 −12
Anterior Cingulate 1 3.5 −4 18 −10
Inferior Temporal Gyrus 0.000 1323 0.099 5.32 52 −76 −8
Middle Occipital Gyrus 0.314 4.85 35 −90 −8
Middle Temporal Gyrus 0.528 4.58 54 −55 0
Cuneus 0.748 4.34 20 −100 2
Middle Temporal Gyrus 0.006 387 0.182 5.08 68 −42 −10
Middle Temporal Gyrus 0.001 510 0.192 5.06 −64 −46 −14
Insula 0.002 459 0.309 4.85 −40 2 −4
Claustrum 0.555 4.43 −35 8 4
Inferior Semi-Lunar Lobule 0.068 219 0.464 4.65 40 −72 −52
Middle Temporal Gyrus 0.037 260 0.62 4.48 62 4 −36
Middle Temporal Gyrus 0.079 210 0.778 4.3 −60 −64 4
Transverse Temporal Gyrus 0.001 564 0.805 4.27 46 −26 14
Insula 0.827 4.24 42 −4 4
Precuneus 0.031 272 0.839 4.22 −2 −52 36
Brain Imaging and Behavior (2017) 11:1862–1872 1869

Fig. 4 a Prediction error signal in


24 HC, for illustrative purposes at
t = 3, and b 20 HD patients,
P = 0.05 uncorrected at y = 11,
color bar indicates t-values

Table 4 Regions with significant


(P < 0.05) and trendwise (P < 0.1) p cluster k p peak t x y z
prediction error activation for HC
at cluster level FEW-corrected Middle Frontal Gyrus 0.000 6039 0.000 8.963 42 47 −11
(assessed at P < 0.001) Insula 0.000 8.887 −33 −1 13
Inferior Frontal Gyrus 0.001 8.280 48 41 −11
Superior Frontal Gyrus 0.002 7.844 15 41 46
Medial Frontal Gyrus 0.004 7.623 9 47 7
Claustrum 0.016 7.010 −36 −1 −2
Middle Frontal Gyrus 0.017 6.968 −33 47 −8
Medial Frontal Gyrus 0.025 6.768 −6 47 7
Anterior Cingulate 0.026 6.738 −3 41 7
Putamen 0.031 6.645 −24 −7 1
Middle Temporal Gyrus 0.000 1968 0.001 8.366 57 −37 −11
Postcentral Gyrus 0.001 8.167 60 −25 40
Inferior Parietal Lobule 0.019 6.919 60 −37 34
Superior Parietal Lobule 0.042 6.487 42 −58 58
Supramarginal Gyrus 0.324 5.330 60 −55 34
Angular Gyrus 0.532 4.984 48 −64 31
Middle Temporal Gyrus 0.552 4.954 39 −55 25
Inferior Parietal Lobule 0.000 1687 0.002 7.882 −60 −40 40
Supramarginal Gyrus 0.002 7.855 −63 −43 37
Middle Temporal Gyrus 0.051 6.382 −66 −49 −2
Postcentral Gyrus 0.061 6.289 −51 −28 19
Superior Parietal Lobule 0.535 4.979 −30 −58 67
Inferior Parietal Lobule 0.575 4.921 −51 −43 58
Angular Gyrus 0.631 4.841 −60 −64 40
Pyramis 0.000 1751 0.052 6.368 −15 −82 −32
Inferior Semi-Lunar Lobule 0.191 5.649 −33 −79 −44
Inferior Semi-Lunar Lobule 0.591 4.897 36 −76 −44
Pyramis 0.664 4.792 27 −73 −29
Cerebellar Tonsil 0.936 4.296 −48 −55 −41
Superior Frontal Gyrus 0.053 76 0.265 5.457 −36 26 49
Cingulate Gyrus 0.000 323 0.320 5.338 3 −34 37
Precuneus 0.452 5.106 −6 −49 31
Posterior Cingulate 0.463 5.089 0 −40 22
Precuneus 0.722 4.708 9 −46 34
Cuneus 0.012 110 0.834 4.527 −15 −106 −5
Inferior Occipital Gyrus 0.957 4.223 −24 −97 −11
1870 Brain Imaging and Behavior (2017) 11:1862–1872

Table 5 Sample characteristics


of the two subgroups of HD HD low symptom group HD high symptom group P (t-test)
patients
Age (mean ± SD) 43.4 ± 9.6 50.8 ± 10.0 P = 0.1
Sex (male/female) 7/2 7/4
Educational years (median) 14, range 13–18 14, range 8–18 P = 0.8
CAG repeats (median) 42, range 40–45 46.5, range 41–50 P < 0.005
UHDRS Motor score (median) 7, range 1–11 23, range 18–58 P < 7 × 10−5
TFC (median) 12, range 10–13 10, range 4–13 P < 0.1
UHDRS total cognitive score (median) 278, range 213–319 183, range 120–255 P < 5 × 10−6
MMSE (median) 29, range 26–30 27, range 22–30 P = 0.07

using a similar probabilistic discrimination and reversal ventral striatum. In a closer analysis, this effect was prob-
task in HD patients and HC (Lawrence et al. 1999). ably driven by the low motor score subgroup (UHDRS
However, we did not observe the perseverative responding motor score ≤11). Interestingly, several morphometry stud-
in HD patients described in this study. Reduced reaction ies recently detected a leftward-biased striatal loss of gray
times as a possible confounding factor need to be consid- matter in HD (Kipps et al. 2005; Muhlau et al. 2007;
ered when interpreting the behavioral data. It remains to be Thieben et al. 2002). Given this leftward-biased atrophy
determined whether reduced reaction times or cognitive in striatal structures in HD patients, the RPE signal in the
deficits are the cause of decreased performance, or wether low motor score subgroup could be interpreted as a func-
these measures simply covary because they are both related tional hypercompensation by remaining neurons in the ven-
to disease stage. To this end, a different task design is re- tral striatum in early disease stages. This has been
quired, which allows to disentangle the effects of cognitive discussed before (Andre et al. 2010), and would be in line
and motor impairments. with fMRI studies in patients with premanifest and early
The finding of a significant bilateral reduction of gray mat- Alzheimer’s disease, which revealed similar functional
ter density in the ventral striatum is in accordance with the hypercompensation in hippocampal and frontoparietal
results of numerous other MRI-based studies in HD patients structures (Quiroz et al. 2010; Wishart et al. 2006).
(Aylward et al. 2012; Kipps et al. 2005; Muhlau et al. 2007; However, our findings need to be interpreted with caution
Weir et al. 2011). considering the small group sizes and the exploratory char-
Findings on RPE-related activation in a network includ- acter of the analysis. In contrast to our results, Saft and
ing ventral striatum, insular cortex, cingulate cortex and colleagues found a rightward lateralization of hemispheric
frontal and parietal regions in HC have been reported pre- activation during auditory processing in response to pre-
viously (D’Ardenne et al. 2008; Haber and Knutson 2010; sentation of sine tones in manifest HD patients (Saft et al.
O’Doherty et al. 2003). Importantly, we found that HD 2008). The studies are difficult to compare, but it is inter-
patients showed significant RPE signals only in the left esting to note that both observed lateralization effects with
disease progression in HD.
We observed a higher learning rate in HD patients, which
was associated with the reduction in gray matter density. A
higher learning rate is not in general an indicator for better task
performance. In fact, due to the occurrence of probabilistic
events, an immediate reaction to negative feedback does not
improve performance level. Our findings are in line with re-
cent evidence for functional compensation in prodromal HD
using a Monetary Incentive task (Malejko et al. 2014).
In an instrumental learning task, Palminteri and colleagues
observed decreased performance of presymptomatic and
symptomatic HD patients during the loss condition, while
Fig. 5 Parameter estimates from the left ventral striatum in HC and two only symptomatic patients showed impairment in the gain
subgroups of HD patients differing in the severity of their motor condition (Palminteri et al. 2012). Enzi and colleagues found
symptoms. While the group with less severe motor symptoms (HD an impairment of ventral striatal blood-oxygen-level-
low) displays an activation related to reward prediction error, prediction
error encoding of the group of patients with advanced motor stage (HD dependent (BOLD) signal during punishment anticipation in
high) was significantly lower. Error bars indicate standard deviation presymptomatic HD patients near disease onset compared to
Brain Imaging and Behavior (2017) 11:1862–1872 1871

HC, but no group difference between the patients groups near References
and far from disease onset (Enzi et al. 2012). Our analysis
focused on the RPE-related activation during the feedback
phase and our task was not designed to differentiate between Andre, V. M., Cepeda, C., & Levine, M. S. (2010). Dopamine and glutamate
in Huntington’s disease: a balancing act. CNS Neuroscience and
reward and punishment contributions, which limits direct Therapeutics, 16(3), 163–178. doi:10.1111/j.1755-5949.2010.00134.x.
comparability. However, we did find differences in reward sen- Ashburner, J., & Friston, K. J. (2005). Unified segmentation. NeuroImage,
sitivity and learning rate in the behavioral model analysis. These 26(3), 839–851. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.02.018.
findings differ from the modeling results reported by Palminteri Aylward, E. H., Nopoulos, P. C., Ross, C. A., Langbehn, D. R., Pierson,
R. K., Mills, J. A., … Paulsen, J. S. (2011). Longitudinal change in
and colleagues, who only observed a difference in choice ran- regional brain volumes in prodromal Huntington disease. Journal of
domness (Palminteri et al. 2012). It is important to note here that Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 82(4), 405–410.
our algorithm contained an additional free parameter, which doi:10.1136/jnnp.2010.208264.
alters the estimation of all free parameters. This additional pa- Aylward, E. H., Liu, D., Nopoulos, P. C., Ross, C. A., Pierson, R. K., Mills,
J. A., … Paulsen, J. S. (2012). Striatal volume contributes to the pre-
rameter κ describes whether participants tend to use more simple
diction of onset of Huntington disease in incident cases. Biological
single-update strategies, or more complex double-update strate- Psychiatry, 71(9), 822–828. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.07.030.
gies. κ was found to be associated with symptom severity, indi- Barker, R., & Priller, J. (2013). Huntington’s disease. In D. Burn (Ed.),
cating that HD patients in an advanced stage of disease might Oxford textbook of movement disorders. Oxford: Oxford University
Press.
use a less complex strategy to solve the task.
D’Ardenne, K., McClure, S. M., Nystrom, L. E., & Cohen, J. D. (2008).
In conclusion, our data suggest that symptomatic HD BOLD responses reflecting dopaminergic signals in the human ven-
patients can be examined by fMRI even in moderate to tral tegmental area. Science, 319(5867), 1264–1267. doi:10.1126
advanced disease stages without any potentially interfer- /science.1150605.
ing CNS-active medication. Our results portray how be- Enzi, B., Edel, M. A., Lissek, S., Peters, S., Hoffmann, R., Nicolas, V., …
Saft, C. (2012). Altered ventral striatal activation during reward and
havior and RPE-related brain activation during a reversal punishment processing in premanifest Huntington’s disease: a func-
learning task are related to disease stages in HD. The tional magnetic resonance study. Experimental Neurology, 235(1),
exploratory analysis of two subgroups suggest a possible 256–264. doi:10.1016/j.expneurol.2012.02.003.
hypercompensation in the degenerating left ventral stria- Glascher, J., Daw, N., Dayan, P., & O’Doherty, J. P. (2010). States versus
rewards: dissociable neural prediction error signals underlying
tum. This effect is apparently lost with disease progres- model-based and model-free reinforcement learning. Neuron,
sion. Small sample sizes and disease-related differences in 66(4), 585–595. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2010.04.016.
cognitive capacities between the HD and the HC groups Haber, S. N., & Knutson, B. (2010). The reward circuit: linking primate
have to be considered as possible confounding factors and anatomy and human imaging. Neuropsychopharmacology, 35(1),
4–26. doi:10.1038/npp.2009.129.
thereby as limitations of our study. Larger longitudinal Hauser, T. U., Iannaccone, R., Ball, J., Mathys, C., Brandeis, D., Walitza,
trials are needed to assess whether performance in fMRI S., & Brem, S. (2014). Role of the medial prefrontal cortex in im-
reversal learning tasks could be used as a non-invasive paired decision making in juvenile attention-deficit/hyperactivity
marker of disease progression in HD. disorder. JAMA Psychiatry, 71(10), 1165–1173. doi:10.1001
/jamapsychiatry.2014.1093.
Kipps, C. M., Duggins, A. J., Mahant, N., Gomes, L., Ashburner, J., &
Acknowledgements We wish to thank Dirk Lang for help with pilot McCusker, E. A. (2005). Progression of structural neuropathology
experiments. This work was supported by grants from the Cluster of in preclinical Huntington’s disease: a tensor based morphometry
Excellence NeuroCure (to J.P.). R.B. received funding by the Deutsche study. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 76(5),
Forschungsgemeinschaft (GRK-1123). The funding sources were not in- 650–655. doi:10.1136/jnnp.2004.047993.
volved in study design; in the collection, analysis and interpretation of Lawrence, A. D., Sahakian, B. J., Rogers, R. D., Hodge, J. R., & Robbins,
data; in the writing of the report; or in the decision to submit the article for T. W. (1999). Discrimination, reversal, and shift learning in
publication. Huntington’s disease: mechanisms of impaired response selection.
Neuropsychologia, 37(12), 1359–1374.
Maldjian, J. A., Laurienti, P. J., Kraft, R. A., & Burdette, J. H. (2003). An
Compliance with ethical standards
automated method for neuroanatomic and cytoarchitectonic atlas-
based interrogation of fMRI data sets. NeuroImage, 19(3), 1233–
Conflict of interest Katharina Nickchen, Rebecca Boehme, Maria del 1239.
Mar Amador, Thomas D. Hälbig, Katharina Dehnicke, Patricia Panneck, Malejko, K., Weydt, P., Sussmuth, S. D., Gron, G., Landwehrmeyer, B.
Joachim Behr, Konstantin Prass, Andreas Heinz, Lorenz Deserno, Florian G., & Abler, B. (2014). Prodromal huntington disease as a model for
Schlagenhauf and Josef Priller declare that they have no conflict of functional compensation of early neurodegeneration. PLoS One,
interest. 9(12), e114569. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0114569.
Montague, P. R., Dayan, P., & Sejnowski, T. J. (1996). A framework for
Informed consent All procedures followed were in accordance with mesencephalic dopamine systems based on predictive Hebbian
the ethical standards of the responsible committee on human experimen- learning. The Journal of Neuroscience, 16(5), 1936–1947.
tation (institutional and national) and with the Helsinki Declaration of Muhlau, M., Gaser, C., Wohlschlager, A. M., Weindl, A., Stadtler, M.,
1975, and the applicable revisions at the time of the investigation. Valet, M., … Peinemann, A. (2007). Striatal gray matter loss in
Informed consent was obtained from all participants for being included Huntington’s disease is leftward biased. Movement Disorders,
in the study. 22(8), 1169–1173. doi:10.1002/mds.21137.
1872 Brain Imaging and Behavior (2017) 11:1862–1872

O’Doherty, J. P., Dayan, P., Friston, K., Critchley, H., & Dolan, R. J. Schultz, W., Dayan, P., & Montague, P. R. (1997). A neural substrate of
(2003). Temporal difference models and reward-related learning in prediction and reward. Science, 275(5306), 1593–1599. doi:10.1126
the human brain. Neuron, 38(2), 329–337. /science.275.5306.1593.
Palminteri, S., Justo, D., Jauffret, C., Pavlicek, B., Dauta, A., Delmaire, Sutton, R., & Barto, A. (1998). Reinforcement learning: an introduction.
C., … Pessiglione, M. (2012). Critical roles for anterior insula and Cambridge: MIT Press.
dorsal striatum in punishment-based avoidance learning. Neuron, Thieben, M. J., Duggins, A. J., Good, C. D., Gomes, L., Mahant, N.,
76(5), 998–1009. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2012.10.017. Richards, F., … Frackowiak, R. S. (2002). The distribution of struc-
Quiroz, Y. T., Budson, A. E., Celone, K., Ruiz, A., Newmark, R., tural neuropathology in pre-clinical Huntington’s disease. Brain,
Castrillon, G., … Stern, C. E. (2010). Hippocampal hyperactivation 125(Pt 8), 1815–1828.
in presymptomatic familial Alzheimer’s disease. Annals of Weir, D. W., Sturrock, A., & Leavitt, B. R. (2011). Development of
Neurology, 68(6), 865–875. doi:10.1002/ana.22105. biomarkers for Huntington’s disease. Lancet Neurology, 10(6),
Saft, C., Schuttke, A., Beste, C., Andrich, J., Heindel, W., & Pfleiderer, B. 573–590. doi:10.1016/s1474-4422(11)70070-9.
(2008). fMRI reveals altered auditory processing in manifest and Wishart, H. A., Saykin, A. J., Rabin, L. A., Santulli, R. B., Flashman, L.
premanifest Huntington’s disease. Neuropsychologia, 46(5), 1279– A., Guerin, S. J., … McAllister, T. W. (2006). Increased brain acti-
1289. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.12.002. vation during working memory in cognitively intact adults with the
Schlagenhauf, F., Huys, Q. J., Deserno, L., Rapp, M. A., Beck, A., APOE epsilon4 allele. American Journal of Psychiatry, 163(9),
Heinze, H. J., … Heinz, A. (2014). Striatal dysfunction during re- 1603–1610. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.163.9.1603.
versal learning in unmedicated schizophrenia patients. NeuroImage,
89, 171–180. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.11.034.

Você também pode gostar