Você está na página 1de 4

Improving children’s diet

The Food Dudes


to the rescue!
KATY TAPPER, PAULINE J. HORNE and C. FERGUS
LOWE describe an innovative scheme to get children
to eat their fruit and veg.

HILDREN don’t like fresh fruit everyone were to eat at least five portions taste a new food may result in his or her

C and vegetables. Visit any primary


school canteen and you will
probably see them tucking into chips,
of fruit and vegetables a day, death from
these diseases could be reduced by up to 20
per cent (see Department of Health, 2000b).
learning to like the flavour.

Modelling Research shows that certain


sausages, baked beans and sponge pudding. Research also suggests that one of the factors make modelling (imitation and
The school cook will tell you that they turn strongest determinants of fruit and vegetable observational learning) more effective. For
their noses up at peas and broccoli and consumption amongst adults is the extent example, a child is more likely to imitate
always ignore the few lonely looking to which they ate fruit and vegetables as another person if that person is liked or
apples and oranges. The aversion of some children (Krebs-Smith et al., 1995). admired by the child (Bandura, 1977), is
children is so great that they will even cry But how can we get children to eat fruit the same age or slightly older (Brody &
if anything green touches their chips or and vegetables? The traditional approach Stoneman, 1981) and has their behaviour
chicken nuggets. has been through health education rewarded (Flanders, 1968). Observing
Such anecdotes are borne out by campaigns. We inform children and parents multiple models has also been shown to be
government statistics. A recent survey of what they should and shouldn’t be eating more effective than observing single models
(Department of Health, 2000a) showed that in the hope that they will alter their habits (Fehrenbach et al., 1979). A number of
4- to 6-year-olds consume an average of accordingly. However, research suggests studies have found an impact of modelling
just two portions of fruit and vegetables that this approach has limited success on children’s food preferences and
a day, with less than 4 per cent of children (Contento et al., 1992; Peterson et al., 1984; consumption patterns (e.g. Birch, 1980;
in this age group eating the recommended Shannon & Chen, 1988). Unfortunately, Harris & Baudin, 1972; Hendy &
five or more portions a day. One in ten of ‘knowing’ does not always mean ‘doing’ – Raudenbush; 2000; Jansen & Tenney, 2001).
these children eat no fruit, half drink no a concept no doubt familiar to anyone who
fruit juice and three in five eat no leafy has ever attempted to quit smoking, lose Rewards This area is more controversial.
green vegetables. weight or embark on a new exercise regime. It has been claimed that rewarding an
So why should they? Because fruit and individual for engaging in a particular task
vegetables are essential for health. They Our approach to the problem undermines his or her intrinsic motivation
contain high levels of vitamins, minerals, At the Bangor Food Research Unit (BFRU) for that task (see Deci et al., 1999). However,
fibre and antioxidants, which are vital for we have approached the problem from a large body of research indicates that, when
normal growth and development and for a different angle. Instead of attempting to used appropriately, rewards can be very
protection against many common illnesses, change knowledge about healthy eating, effective at altering behaviour (e.g. Cameron
such as coughs, colds and flu. They have or attitudes towards particular foods, we et al., 2001; Dickinson, 1989). Rewards are
also been shown to significantly reduce the tackle food consumption itself. In our most effective when they are highly
risk of heart disease, stroke and cancer (see efforts to change children’s eating habits desirable, achievable, their delivery is
Gillman, 1996), the biggest killers in the we have employed three main techniques: contingent upon performance, and when
UK today. According to experts, if taste exposure, modelling and rewards. they convey the message that they are for
behaviour that is both enjoyable and high
Taste exposure The more you taste status (Dickinson, 1989; Lowe et al., 1998).
WEBLINKS a novel food the more you learn to like it It is easy to overlook this latter point,
The Food Dudes site: www.fooddudes.co.uk (Birch & Marlin, 1982; Birch et al., 1987; but it is perhaps one of the most important.
Pliner, 1982). For example, most people For example, certificates and trophies for
British Nutrition Foundation: www.nutrition.org.uk
can probably think of things they didn’t hard work and sporting achievements are
National Cancer Institute campaign: www.5aday.gov like as a child that they have learnt to like accepted practice in most schools and are no
Produce for Better Health Foundation: www.5aday.com as an adult, such as olives, Stilton cheese or doubt a good way of motivating children.
spicy foods. So getting a child to repeatedly The message here is clear: ‘Well done! You

18

The Psychologist Vol 16 No 1 January 2003


Improving children’s diet

should be proud of yourself.’ The Dudes’, a group of four slightly older children’s consumption. Likewise, the
certificates and trophies are marks of children who gain superpowers from eating effects of the peer-modelling video without
achievement and hopefully the child will fruit and vegetables. The Food Dudes do the rewards were minimal. There were
work just as hard next time. battle against the evil ‘Junk Punks’ who some effects when the rewards were used
However, tell a child ‘Eat all your threaten to take over the planet by without the video (especially with fruit),
vegetables and then you can have pudding’, destroying all the fruit and vegetables, but by far the greatest increases in
and the message is entirely different. You thereby depriving humans of their ‘Life consumption were achieved when the video
are telling the child that pudding is better Force’ foods. Throughout the video the and rewards were combined. We believe
than vegetables. In addition, the child is Food Dudes eat and enjoy a variety of fruit that because the rewards are labelled as
likely to feel that their behaviour is being and vegetables. The rewards consisted of ‘Food Dude’ items, they acquire
controlled and may also conclude ‘If I have items such as Food Dude stickers, pens and considerable potency through their
to eat vegetables to get pudding, then erasers, awarded to the children for eating association with the characters on the
vegetables must be really nasty!’ Although target amounts of fruit and vegetables. video, and that for this reason the effects
the child may eat the vegetables on this The results showed that the combination of the combined elements are greater.
occasion, it is unlikely that they will do of peer modelling and rewards was very However, to be of real practical use
so in the future in the absence of pudding. effective at increasing children’s to agencies with an interest in improving
consumption of both fruit and vegetables. children’s diets, the programme would have
The ‘Food Dudes’ at home Prior to the introduction of the intervention, to prove that it was capable of effecting big
Our studies into increasing children’s fruit the children were consuming an average of increases in fruit and vegetable
and veg consumption were first carried out 4 per cent of the fruit presented to them at consumption in large groups of children.
in the home environment with a small home by their parents, and just 1 per cent So we set about establishing whether the
group of 5- to 6-year-old children of the vegetables. However, upon the programme could be adapted for use with
large numbers of children in school and
nursery settings.

Jarvis and Jess in the nursery


In the nursery setting participants were
26 two- to four-year-olds attending the
university’s daycare nursery and Centre for
Child Development (see Woolner, 2000).
The study was largely implemented by the
nursery nurses and the video-modelling
component of the programme used
animated characters called ‘Jarvis and
Jess’, specifically tailored to the nursery
school age range.
The intervention was introduced at
‘snacktime’, (a period just before mid-
morning break) first for fruit, then later for
vegetables. Levels of fruit and vegetable
consumption at lunchtime in the dining
room were also recorded. There were never
(identified by their parents as ‘fussy introduction by their parents of the video any rewards or video at lunchtime, but
eaters’) who ate little fruit and vegetables and rewards, fruit consumption increased to measures were taken to determine whether
(see Dowey, 1996; Horne et al., 1995, Lowe 100 per cent and vegetable consumption to any of the snacktime effects would carry
et al., 1998). The studies employed a 83 per cent. over to the lunchtime context.
multiple baseline research design (see Follow-up measures taken six months The results showed that following
Kazdin, 1982), in which, following later showed that not only were the increases the introduction of the intervention, fruit
baselines of varying duration, the start of large, they were also maintained over time. consumption at snacktime rose from 30
the intervention was staggered over time The children were still eating 100 per cent per cent to 71 per cent. The intervention
across foods, being introduced first for fruit of the fruit presented to them and 58 per was followed by a maintenance phase
and then for vegetable consumption. The cent of the vegetables, even though they during which there were no videos and
studies evaluated the effects of four different were no longer receiving the rewards or the rewards became more intermittent
procedures on children’s consumption of watching or the video. In addition, there was (a procedure known to be effective in
a range of fruit and vegetables presented to evidence to show that the effects were not maintaining behaviour – see Kazdin, 1994).
them. The procedures were as follows: fruit simply restricted to the fruit and vegetables At follow-up, 15 months later, consumption
and vegetable presentation only; rewarded that children had been rewarded for eating, levels were 79 per cent. The effects at
taste exposure; peer modelling; and rewarded but also occurred for other items children lunchtime mirrored those at snacktime,
taste exposure combined with peer were able to name as fruit or vegetables. rising from a baseline of 17 per cent to
modelling. By way of contrast, the results also 76 per cent at the 15-month follow-up.
The peer-modelling element consisted showed that continued presentation of fruit Similarly, in the case of vegetables,
of a video featuring the heroic ‘Food and vegetables alone had no effect on consumption during snacktime rose from

19

January 2003 The Psychologist Vol 16 No 1


Improving children’s diet

34 per cent at baseline to 87 per cent made to generalise from the school context The studies showed that the programme
following the intervention, and was still at to the home. resulted in large, statistically significant
86 per cent at the follow-up nine months increases in fruit and vegetable consumption
later. The increases for lunchtime The ‘whole-school’ programme in all three schools at both snacktime and
consumption of vegetables were even We have recently completed the at lunchtime. The increases occurred for
larger, rising from 20 per cent at baseline to development and evaluation of a ‘whole both boys and girls in infant and junior
89 per cent at follow-up. Three years later, school’ Food Dude programme for use classes (4–7 and 7–11 years respectively).
with very intermittent maintenance across the entire primary age range (4–11 Data collected from a subset of parents
procedures, the culture of the nursery years). The programme is designed to be in the Salford school also showed a
school has altered so that children are now implemented entirely by school staff and significant increase in the number of
consuming approximately 80 per cent of contains the following elements: portions of fruit and vegetables consumed
the fruit and vegetables presented to them ● A Food Dude video containing six on weekdays. (The number of portions
in the snacktime and lunchtime settings. 6-minute adventure episodes. consumed on weekend days showed an
● A set of Food Dude rewards. increase but this failed to reach statistical
Into the classroom ● A set of letters from the Food Dudes. significance. Since most of the programme
Studies were also carried out with classes These provide praise and was delivered at school during the
of primary school children in schools in encouragement and remind children of weekdays, the absence of change at the
Bangor in North Wales (see Horne et al., the reward contingencies. weekend may have occurred due to a lack
1998). The first of these was conducted ● A Food Dude homepack to encourage of appropriate cues, e.g. being reminded at
with a class of 26 children aged 5–6 years. children to eat fruit and vegetables in home of the positive consequences of
It is characteristic of many children’s diets the home context as well as at school. eating fruit and vegetables.)
that though they fail to eat sufficient fruit ● A staff manual and staff briefing video Further evaluation of the programme
and vegetables, they do habitually consume to help teachers implement the was carried out in two schools in Lambeth
snack foods that are high in saturated fat programme correctly. in south London (see Lowe et al., 2002;
and sugar. Therefore an important aspect ● A set of education support materials to Tapper et al., 2002). One of these acted as
of this study was that it tested whether the help teachers meet curriculum targets an experimental school, receiving the full
programme was potent enough to increase using the Food Dude theme. Food Dude programme, whilst the other
children’s consumption of fruit and acted as a control and simply received the
vegetables even in the face of the additional fruit and vegetables for the
competing availability of popular sweet duration of the study. Again, the results
and savoury snacks. Fruit and vegetables showed significant increases in fruit and
were presented at breaktime side by side vegetable consumption at snacktime and at
with sweet and savoury snacks such as lunchtime in the experimental school, but
chocolate bars, cakes and crisps. The not in the control school. Follow-up
children were free to choose whatever measures, conducted four months after the
foods they wanted. end of the intervention, also showed that
Even under these conditions fruit children in the experimental school were
consumption more than doubled, from still eating significantly more fruit and
28 per cent at baseline to 59 per cent at vegetables at lunchtime than they had been
the six-month follow-up. Consumption The main intervention phase of the prior to the introduction of the programme.
of vegetables increased fourfold, from just programme lasts for a period of 16 days Teachers and parents also responded
8 per cent at baseline to 32 per cent at during which children watch the Food very positively. As well as commenting on
follow-up. Accompanying the rise in fruit Dude video episodes and listen to their how much children had enjoyed the
and vegetable consumption was a teacher read out the Food Dude letters. programme, teachers reported additional
substantial fall in the consumption of the Children also receive rewards when they benefits such as enthusiasm for curriculum
sweet and savoury snack foods. These eat the fruit and vegetables that are work using the Food Dude theme,
results show that it is possible to shift presented to them. They receive a Food improved school attendance and an
children’s food choices away from sweet Dude sticker for tasting a food, or a sticker increased confidence amongst children
and fatty snacks towards more healthy and a small prize for eating a whole portion. who were not normally big achievers.
alternatives. The intervention phase is followed by Likewise, almost all of the parents who
The second study was conducted with a maintenance phase during which there returned a questionnaire sent to them at
a class of 28 children, also aged 5–6 years. are no videos and the letters and rewards the end of the study felt that their child had
In addition, a subset of these children become more intermittent. It is possible enjoyed and benefited from taking part.
participated in a concurrent home-based to implement the programme either at
study during which fruit and vegetable snacktime or lunchtime or at both. Why does it work?
consumption was monitored and a small We believe the programme works in three
home intervention was introduced. The Evaluating the programme main ways. Firstly, children discover the
results showed large and long-lasting Initial evaluation of the new whole-school intrinsically rewarding properties of fruit
increases in fruit and vegetable programme was carried out in three schools, and vegetables and develop a liking for
consumption in both the classroom and in Bangor in North Wales, Harwell in them. As we have seen, there is evidence to
home contexts. These results are important, Oxfordshire and Salford in Manchester show that if we taste a food enough times
since they indicate that the effects can be (see Lowe et al., 2001; Lowe et al., 2002). we may actually learn to enjoy it. The

20

The Psychologist Vol 16 No 1 January 2003


Improving children’s diet

intervention gets children to taste fruit and help bring about other forms of behaviour
vegetables repeatedly; eventually they eat change in health domains. For example, in
them for the taste, even when they are no an effort to combat the growing problem of
longer receiving the rewards. obesity, the BFRU team are currently
Secondly, we believe that the programme investigating how similar interventions
changes the culture within the school to might be used to help children and older
one that strongly supports the eating of adults not only to eat more healthily, but
fruit and vegetables. So children get social also to become more physically active.
reinforcement from their peer group for Whether it be eating sensibly, being active,
eating fruit and vegetables: it becomes a not smoking, or indeed any other course of
cool thing to do rather than something for action that can improve health and well-
which they are ostracised. piloted by the Department of Health in being, we believe that great gains can be
And thirdly, children come to see schools in London and Plymouth. And in made for people – not simply by informing
themselves as ‘fruit and vegetable eaters’ Scotland a modified version of the them of what they should and shouldn’t be
and are guided by this self-concept. So for programme has already been rolled out to doing, but by applying known behaviour
example, on being presented with a piece 210 schools in Glasgow and is also being principles in a systematic and coherent
of fruit they may say to themselves ‘I like piloted in schools in Forth Valley. position that is well grounded in the basic
fruit’ or ‘I always eat my fruit’, and these We hope that these pilots will lead research literature on human learning.
rules influence their behaviour. to government agencies’ support for the
programme in order to make it available ■ Katy Tapper is Project Manager at the
The future of the programme to all schools in the UK. The results of our Bangor Food Research Unit, School of
We have had a great deal of interest in the studies to date clearly show that the Psychology, University of Wales, Bangor.
programme from government agencies. The programme can bring about large and long- E-mail: k.tapper@bangor.ac.uk.
Food Standards Agency has commissioned lasting increases in children’s consumption ■ Pauline Horne is a Director at the
us to coordinate and evaluate the programme of fruit and vegetables. If implemented Bangor Food Research Unit. E-mail:
in schools in Wales. This evaluation will nationally, this could result in major health p.j.horne@bangor.ac.uk.
include follow-up measures taken at six- benefits for millions of schoolchildren. ■ C. Fergus Lowe is a Director at the
and twelve-month intervals. A modified The peer-modelling and rewards-based Bangor Food Research Unit. E-mail:
version of the programme is also being approach used here could be adapted to c.f.lowe@bangor.ac.uk.

References
Bandura.A. (1977). Social learning theory. Diet and Nutrition Survey:Young people preschool children. Appetite, 34, 61–76. Lowe, C.F., Horne, P.J., Bowdery, M.A.,
New Jersey: Prentice-Hall. aged 4 to 18 years.Volume 1: Report of Horne, P.J., Lowe, C.F., Bowdery, M. & Egerton, C. & Tapper, K. (2001).
Birch, L.L. (1980). Effects of peer models’ the diet and nutrition survey. London:The Egerton, C. (1998).The way to healthy Increasing children’s consumption of
food choices and eating behaviors on Stationery Office. eating for children. British Food Journal, fruit and vegetables [Abstract]. Public
preschoolers’ food preferences. Child Department of Health (2000b). The NHS 100, 133–140. Health Nutrition, 4, 387.
Development, 51, 489–496. Plan: A plan for investment, a plan for Horne, P.J., Lowe, C.F., Fleming, P.F. & Lowe, C.F., Horne, P.J.,Tapper, K., Jackson,
Birch, L.L. & Marlin, D.W. (1982).‘I don’t like reform. London:The Stationery Office. Dowey,A.J. (1995).An effective M., Hardman, C.,Woolner, J. et al.
it, I never tried it’: Effects of exposure Dickinson,A.M. (1989).The detrimental procedure for changing food (2002). Changing the nation’s diet: A
on two-year-old children’s food. effects of extrinsic reinforcement on preferences in 5–7 year-old children. programme to increase children’s
Appetite, 3, 353–360. ‘intrinsic motivation’. The Behavior Proceedings of the Nutrition Society, 54, consumption of fruit and vegetables
Birch, L.L., McPhee, L., Shoba, B.C., Pirok, E. Analyst, 12, 1–15. 441–452. (Technical report). University of Wales,
& Steinberg, L. (1987).What kind of Dowey,A.J. (1996). Psychological determinants Jansen,A. & Tenney, N. (2001). Seeing mum Bangor, Bangor Food Research Unit.
exposure reduces children’s food of children’s food preferences. drinking a ‘light’ product: Is social Peterson, P.E., Jeffrey, D.B., Bridgwater, C.A.
neophobia? Looking vs. tasting. Appetite, Unpublished doctoral dissertation, learning a stronger determinant of
& Dawson, B. (1984). How pronutrition
9, 171–178. University of Wales, Bangor. taste preference acquisition than
television programming affects
Brody, G.H. & Stoneman, Z. (1981). Fehrenbach, P.A., Miller, D.J. & Thelen, M.H. caloric conditioning? European Journal
children’s dietary habits. Developmental
Selective imitation of same-age, older (1979).The importance of consistency of Clinical Nutrition, 55, 418–422.
Psychology, 20, 55–63.
and younger peer models. Child of modeling behavior upon imitation:A Kazdin,A.E. (1982). Single-case research
Pliner, P. (1982).The effects of mere
Development, 52, 717–720. comparison of single and multiple designs: Methods for clinical and applied
exposure on liking for edible
Cameron, J., Banko, K.M. & Pierce,W.D. models. Journal of Personality and Social settings. Oxford: Oxford University
substances. Appetite, 3, 283–290.
(2001). Pervasive negative effects of Psychology, 37, 1412–1417. Press.
Shannon, B. & Chen,A.N. (1988).A three-
rewards on intrinsic motivation:The Flanders, J.P. (1968).A review of research on Kazdin,A.E. (1994). Behavior modification in
myth continues. The Behavior Analyst, imitative behavior. Psychological Bulletin, applied settings. Pacific Grove, CA: year school based nutrition education
24, 1–44. 69, 316–337. Brooks/Cole. study. Journal of Nutrition Education, 20,
Contento, I.R., Manning,A.D. & Shannon, B. Gillman, M.W. (1996). Enjoy your fruits and Krebs-Smith, S.M., Heimendinger, J., 114–124.
(1992). Research perspective on vegetables: Eating fruit and vegetables Patterson, B.H., Subar,A.F., Kessler, R. & Tapper, K., Lowe, C.F., Horne, P.J., Jackson,
school-based nutrition education. protects against the common chronic Pivonka, E. (1995). Psychosocial factors M.C., Hardman, C.A. & Woolner, J.
Journal of Nutrition Education, 24, diseases of adulthood. British Medical associated with fruit and vegetable (2002).An intervention to increase
247–260. Journal, 313, 765–766. consumption. American Journal of Health children’s consumption of fruit and
Deci, E.L., Koestner, R. & Ryan, R.M. (1999). Harris, M.B. & Baudin, H. (1972). Models Promotion, 10, 98–104. vegetables [Abstract]. Proceedings of The
A meta-analytic review of experiments and vegetable eating:The power of Lowe, C.F., Dowey,A.J. & Horne, P.J. (1998). British Psychological Society, 10, 102.
examining the effects of extrinsic Popeye. Psychological Reports, 31, 570. Changing what children eat. In A. Woolner, J. (2000). Children’s food preferences
rewards on intrinsic motivation. Hendy, H.M. & Raudenbush, B. (2000). Murcott (Ed.) The nation’s diet:The social – A behavioural analysis. Unpublished
Psychological Bulletin, 125, 627–668. Effectiveness of teacher modelling to science of food choice. (pp.57–80). doctoral dissertation, University of
Department of Health (2000a). National encourage food acceptance in London: Longman. Wales, Bangor.

21

January 2003 The Psychologist Vol 16 No 1

Você também pode gostar