Você está na página 1de 16

Reprinted from AMERICANP~YCEOLOCI~T,

Vol. 35, No. 7, July 1980


Printed in U. S. A.

The Totalitarian Ego


Fabrication and Revision of Personal History

ANTHONY G. GREENWALD Ohio State University

ABSTRACT: This article argues that ( a ) ego, or self, totalitarian, was chosen only with substantial reser-
is an organization of knowledge, ( b ) ego is character- vation because of this label's pejorative connota-
ized by cognitive biases strikingly analogous to totali- tions. Interestingly, characteristics that seem un-
tarian information-control strategies, and ( c ) these desirable in a political system can nonetheless serve
totalitarian-ego biases junction to preserve organization adaptively in a personal organization of knowledge.
in cognitive structures. Ego's cognitive biases are ego-
The conception of ego as an organization of
centricity (self as the focus of knowledge), "beneffec-
tance" (perception of responsibility for desired, but knowledge synthesizes influences from three sources
not undesired, outcomes), and cognitive conservatism --empirical, literary, and theoretical. First, recent
(resistance to cognitive change). In addition to being empirical demonstrations of self-relevant cognitive
pervasively evident in recent studies of normal human biases suggest that the biases play a role in some
cognition, these three biases are found in actively func- fundamental aspect of personality. Second, George
tioning, higher level organizations of knowledge, per- Orwell's 1984 suggests the analogy between ego's
haps best exemplified by theoretical paradigms in sci- biases and totalitarian information con&ol. Last,
ence. The thesis that egocentricity, beneffectance, and the theories of Loevinger (1976) and Epstein
conservatism act to preserve knowledge organizations ( 1973) suggest the additional analogy between ego's
leads to the proposal of an intrapsychic analog of organization and theoretical organizations of sci-
genetic evolution, which in turn provides an alternative entific knowledge.
to prevalent motivational and informational interpreta-
The first part of this article surveys evidence
tions of cognitive biases.
indicating that ego's cognitive biases are pervasive
The ego rejects the unbearable idea together in and characteristic of normal personalities. The
with its associated affect and behaves as if the second part sets forth arguments for interpreting
idea had never occurred to the person a t all.
(Freud, 1894/1959, p. 72) the biases as manifestations of an effectively func-
tioning organization of knowledge. The last section
Alike with the individual and the group, the
past is being continually re-made, reconstructed develops an explanation for the totalitarian-ego
in the interests of the present. (Bartlett, 1932, biases by analyzing their role in maintaining cog-
p. 309) nitive organization and in supporting effective
As historians of our own lives we seem to be, behavior.
on the one hand, very inattentive and, on the
other, revisionists who will justify the present
by changing the past. (Wixon & Laird, 1976, I . Three Cognitive Biases: Fabrication and
p. 384)
Revision of Personal History
"Who controls the past," ran the Party slogan,
"controls the future: who controls the present Ego, as an organization of knowledge (a. conclusion
controls the past." (Orwell, 1949, p. 32)
to be developed later), serves the functions of
What follows is a portrait of self (or ego-the terms observing (perceiving) and recording (remember-
are used interchangeably) constructed by inter- ing) personal experience; it can be characterized,
weaving strands drawn from several areas of recent therefore, as a perssnal historian. Many findings
research. The most striking features of the portrait
are three cognitive biases, which correspond dis-
turbingly to thought control and propaganda devices Acknowledgments are given at the end of the article.
Requests for reprints should be sent to Anthony G.
that are to be defining characteristics of Greenwald, Department of Psychology, Ohio State Univer-
a totalitarian political system. The epithet for ego, sity, 404C West 17th Avenue, Columbus, Ohio 43210.

Copyright 1980 by the American Psychological Association, Inc.


0003466X/80/3S07-0603$00.75
from recent research in personality, cognitive, and itated when experimentally encountered information
social psychology demonstrate that ego fabricates was relevant to traits that were part of a person's
and revises history, thereby engaging in practices self-concept.
not ordinarily admired in historians. These lapses
in personal scholarship, or cognitive biases, are SELF AS THE AXIS O F CAUSE AND EFFECT
discussed below in three categories: egocentricity
(self perceived as more central to events than it is), I n a chapter entitled "Overestimating One's Impor-
"beneffectance" l (self perceived as selectively tance as Influence or Target," Jervis (1976), a
responsible for desired, but not undesired, out- political scientist, presents an analysis of egocentric
comes), and conservatism (resistance to cognitive misperceptions in international politics. These
change). involve a decision maker unreasonably perceiving
the act of a foreign nation as being either ( a ) made
Egocentricity: Ego as Self-Focused Historian in response to (i.e., having been caused by) a prior
act of the decision maker or ( b ) made with the
ORGANIZATION OF MEMORY I N RELATION TO SELF
intent of eliciting some response from the decision
maker. The first of these categories corresponds
The past is remembered as if it were a drama in
somewhat to the "illusion of control," which has
which self was the leading player. In part, this
been demonstrated in a series of experiments by
observation refers to the autobiographical or epi-
Langer (1975). This illusion takes the form of
sodic character of much of memory-the tendency
people seeing their behavior as capable of influenc-
for events to be encoded and recorded in terms of
ing outcomes that are, objectively, determined by
the person's location a t the time of original experi-
chance, such as the probability of a lottery ticket
ence (cf. Tulving, 1972). I n describing episodic
they selected being a winner. Ross and Sicoly
memory, Norman ( 1976) wrote,
( 1979), investigating- egocentric processes in 'group
-

My memory for the University of Toronto campus in settings, in addition to confirming Brenner's finding
Canada, where Tulving resides, cannot be separated from of people remembering best their own contributions
my memory of my last visit to Toronto. Trying to recall
how one goes to the Psychology Department automatically to a group effort, also found that "individuals
recreates the last visit there-the snow, the heavy traffic, accepted more responsibility for a group product
the various people I met, and the restaurants at which I than other participants attributed to them" (p.
ate. (p. 189)
322).
Although it is easy to demonstrate the autobio- The other side of Jervis's thesis--overperception
graphical character of memory to ourselves by of self as an intended target of another's action-
means of such personal retrievals, empirical confir- has yet to be documented as a pervasive bias in
mation of the importance of self-reference in experimental research ~ e t t i n g s . ~This bias is a
perception and memory is only a very recent defining characteristic of paranoia, in which one
development. Rogers, Kuiper, and Kirker ( 1977) sees oneself as the intended victim of actually
provided the first of several demonstrations that benign others. Milder versions of this phenomenon
information is especially well remembered if the may also surface in the behavior of normal subjects,
person considers the relation of information to self as soon' as it is sought in the psychological labora-
at the time of initial experience (see also, Bower & tory.
Gilligan, 1979; Kuiper & Rogers, 1979; Lord,
1980). Brenner (1973, 1976) found that in a
group setting, subjects focused on their own per- 1Beneffectance is introduced as a new coinage in this
formance a t the expense of retaining information article. I t is a compound of beneficence (achieving desir-
from the just preceding or just following perform- able outcomes) and effectance (motivation to act com-
petently; cf. White, 1959). The concept is developed in
ances of others. Very likely related to the foregoing detail in the discussion that follows. (See also Footnote 7.)
is a set of robust findings showing that information Jervis's review of evidence for overperception of self
is better remembered the more the person plays an as target focuses on instances of perceiving others as re-
sponsible for one's undesired outcomes, a bias that is
active, rather than passive, role in generating the regarded as important in this article and that is included
information (Erdelyi, Buschke, & Finkelstein, under the heading "Beneffectance." In the context of the
1977; Greenwald & Albert, 1968; Slamecka & Graf, egocentricity bias, overperception of self as target should
be interpreted literally as the opposite of overperception of
1978). Markus (1977) uncovered a variety of self as influence, without regard to the personal desirability
indications that judgment and memory were facil- of the outcome being explained.
Beneflectance: Ego as Self-Aggrandizing Historian as a solvable learning task; the learner-gambler may
invest a substantial sum before (if ever) abandon-
One of the best established recent findings in social ing this beneffectance illusion.
psychology is that people perceive themselves
readily as the origin of good effects and reluctantly SOME EXPERIMENTAL EVIDENCE
as the origin of ill effects (see reviews in support of
this conclusion by Bowerman, 1978; Bradley, 1978; Selective recall of success. Some of the earliest
Jervis, 1976, chap. 9 ; Miller & Ross, 1975; Myers relevant experimental evidence comes from vari-
& Ridl, 1979; Snyder, Stephan, & Rosenfield, 1978; ations of Zeigarnik's (1927) finding that people
Wicklund, 1978 ; Wortman, 1976; Tetlock & Levi, typically recall interrupted tasks better than they
Note 1 ) . The finding has variously been labeled recall completed ones. Zeigarnik observed some
ego-def ensive, self -serving, egocentric, or egotistic apparent exceptions to her general finding among
attribution in these reviews. Because the first two subjects who felt that they had performed poorly
of these terms include more than just the result on the interrupted tasks (see summary by de
presently being considered and the latter two lend Rivera, 1976, pp. 133-146). Rosenzweig (1943)
themselves to confusim with the egocentricity bias, and Glixman (1949) established the importance of
a new designation is suggested here: benefiectance, this exception to the usual Zeigarnik effect by show-
which is a compound of beneficence (doing good) ing that when interruption of performance was a
and effectance (competence; see White, 1959). signal of personal failure and completion was a
Beneffectance is thus the tendency to take credit for signal of success, the effect was reversed-successful
success while denying responsibility for failure. (completed) tasks were remembered better than
were failed (interrupted) ones; that is, memory
INFORMAL OBSERVATIONS supported an association of self with success more
than with failure.
I n automobile driving, beneffectance takes the form Bene8ectance in group settings. When a task is
of reluctance to acknowledge responsibility for performed collectively by members of a group,
various mishaps. The following quotations from a individual-ability feedback may not be available.
collection of drivers' explanations of accidents to This provides free reign for people to believe that
police give some amusing illustrations (San Fran- they have contributed more than their equal share
cisco Sunday Examiner and Chronicle, April 22, toward a group success but less than an equal share
1979, p. 35) : toward a failure. Johnston (1967) demonstrated
As I approached the intersection, a sign suddenly appeared just this effect by having subjects believe them-
in a place where a stop sign had never appeared before. I selves to be members of two-person teams perform-
was unable to stop in time to avoid an accident. ing a skilled task (compensatory tracking). Sub-
The telephone pole was approaching. I was attempting jects received only team feedback, which indicated
to swerve out of its way when it struck my front end. that they and their partner, as a team, were per-
I n asking students to judge an examination's forming below average, average, or above average
quality as a measure of their ability to master at the tracking skill. Subjects accepted credit for
course material, I have repeatedly found a strong the good scores, but assigned most of the blame for
correlation between obtained grade and belief that the poor scores to their assumed partners. Inter-
the exam was a proper measure. Students who do estingly, when team feedback was "average,"
well are willing to accept credit for success; those subjects were inclined to assume that this must
who do poorly, however, are unwilling to accept have resulted from a combination of their own
responsibility for failure, instead seeing the exam better-than-average performance with the partner's
(or the instructor) as being insensitive to their worse-than-average performance. Schlenker and
abilities (cf. Arkin & Maruyama, 1979 ; Schlenker Miller (1977) demonstrated a similarly strong bias
& Miller, 1977). in a knowledge-test group task, even when using a
Beneffectance can lead to problems when random form of group feedback that provided enough in-
events are perceived as more likely to be personally formation for subjects to have made more accurate
caused the more favorable the outcome is. For inferences about their individual performances.
example, when a gambler perceives slot machine Denial of responsibility for harming. I n a vari-
payoffs, but not losses, as being personally caused, ation of Milgram's ( 1963) well-known procedure
a solutionless gambling task may be misconstrued for demonstrating obedience, Harvey, Harris, and
Barnes (1975) induced some subjects, who were ments but also in domains of (presumably noncon-
playing the role of teachers, to administer (appar- troversial) factual knowledge (cf. Nisbett & Ross's
ently) severe shocks, while others believed them- 1980, recent discussion of belief perseverance).
selves to be administering only mild shocks. Sub- Confirmation bias in information search. Snyder
ject-teachers accorded themselves less responsibility and Swam (1978) showed that when asked to de-
for their learners' apparent distress when the shocks termine if an interviewee was, say, an introvert,
appeared to be severe than when they were mild. subject-interviewers selected questions that were
Additionally, third-person observers saw the teach- biased toward the introvert hypothesis. Mischel,
ers as more responsible for the learners' severe Ebbesen, and Zeiss (1973) found that subjects se-
distress than did the subject-teachers themselves. lectively examined available information to confirm
Vicarious beneflectance. A study conducted experimentally established positive or negative self-
simultaneously at several universities by Cialdini expectations. Kuhn ( 1970) and Lakatos (1970)
et al. (197,6) found evidence for a sympathetic form have proposed that the predisposition to confirm
of beneffectance: Students showed more evidence existing theoretical beliefs is pervasively charac-
of identifying with their university, in the form of teristic of the research behavior of scientists, and I
wearing clothing that displayed the university affili- ( Greenwald, 1975a) have reported data showing
ation, in Monday classes after a Saturday football that psychologists are strongly inclined to dis-
victory than in ones following a loss. This exten- regard research results inconsistent with their theo-
sion of personal identity to encompass a victor more retical hypotheses (caveat lector! ) .
than a loser also manifested itself in a second study: Confirmation bias in memory search. In a study
Subjects were more likely to use a first-person parallel to their 1973 study, Mischel, Ebbesen, and
grammatical form when describing their university's Zeiss (1976) showed that subjects selectively re-
victory (e.g., "we won") than when describing a called information that confirmed experimentally
loss (e.g., "they lost"). established positive or negative self-expectations.
Snyder and Uranowitz (1978) found a similar
Cognitive Conservatism: Ego as Self-Justifying memory selectivity in their subjects' retrieving
Historian information about a target person so as to confirm
a recently established belief about that person's
The secret of rulership is to combine a belief in one's own sexual orientation (heterosexual vs. homosexual).
infallibility with the power to learn from past mistakes. Fischhoff, Slovic, and Lichtenstein ( 1977) asked
(Orwell, 1949, p. 1 7 7 )
people to give answers to difficult general informa-
Conservatism is the disposition to preserve that tion questions and then asked them to estimate the
which is already established. Cognitive conserva- probability of correctness of their answers. Under
tism is therefore the disposition to preserve existing these circumstances people tend to be overly con-
knowledge structures, such as percepts, schemata fident in estimating their correctness; Koriat, Lich-
(categories), and memories. Object conservation tenstein, and Fischhoff ( 1980) have suggested that
(permanency or perceptual constancy) is the fun- this overconfidence in memory is due at least in
damental cognitive achievement of a conservative part to a selective search of memory for evidence
nature, followed somewhat later by assimilation that confirms what has been recalled. The demon-
(the fitting of new events into existing cognitive stration of overconfidence in memory recently re-
classifications, or category conservation). Two ad- ported by Trope ( 1978)-subjects treating weak
ditional cognitive processes of a conservative nature, (error-prone) memories as if they were valid-may
both documented in research only quite recently, have a similar explanation.
are referred to here as confirmation bias and re- Confirmation bias in responding to persuasion.
writing of memory. The persuasive impact of a communication on a
target audience member is, puzzlingly, not readily
CONFIRMATION BIAS predictable from knowledge either of the position
advocated in the message or of what the target
Several recent studies have shown that people man- remembers of its content (Anderson & Hubert,
age knowledge in a variety of ways to promote the 1963; Hovland, Janis, & Kelley, 1953). On the
selective availability of information that confirms other hand, persuasive impact is readily predictable
judgments already arrived at. This bias occurs not from knowledge of the target's prior opinion-
only in the domain of (controversial) opinion judg- people tend to reject messages that disagree with
their prior opinions, while being accepting of mes- which each question was to be answered by assign-
sages that reinforce existing opinions (Cullen, 1968; ing a probability of correctness to one of two al-
Greenwald, 1968; Janis & Terwilliger, 1962; Sherif ternative answers. Some of the subjects were first
& Hovland, 1961 ). This potency of prior opinion, informed of the correct answers and then asked
relative to communication content, as a predictor to indicate the probability judgments they would
of response to persuasion reflects a cognitive re- have given had they not first been told these an-
sponse process (Greenwald, 1968; Petty, Ostrom, swers. These subjects substantially overestimated
& Brock, in press) that can be viewed as a com- their prior knowledge of correct answers, as indi-
plex form of confirmation bias. I t is complex in cated by comparison of their judgments with cor-
that it involves not only selective retrieval from rectness-probability judgments of naive subjects.
memory of information that supports existing opin- This fabrication of memory seemed to be accom-
ion but also active construction of new arguments plished with the same ease with which we unthink-
required to refute novel, opinion-opposing arguments. ingly nod agreement in response to conversational
A related confirmation bias, primacy in person references to past events (e.g., "You remember
impression formation, is the relative potency of in- last year, when we were talking at the conven-
formation received early in a description. A well- tion . . .") whether or not the referred-to event
known example is Luchins's (1957) finding that the ever took place.
impression resulting from two somewhat contradic- Cultivating memory with leading questions. Lof-
tory person-descriptive paragraphs varies sharply tus (1979) has assembled evidence showing the
as a function of the order in which they are pre- vulnerability of eyewitness testimony to distortions
sented, being guided more by the first of the two. induced by events that intervene between a wit-
Presumably, this happens because the first para- nessed episode and the provision of testimony. As
graph establishes an impression of the target person, one example, asking the question, Did another car
and the subject then interprets the second paragraph pass the red Datsun while it was stopped at the
with a confirmation bias that tends to negate its stop sign? can result in a subject's later asserting
independent, opposing effect. (Both the primacy that there was a stop sign at an intersection that
phenomenon and systematic exceptions to it have the subject (in fact) had previously seen with a
recently been reviewed by Schneider, Hastorf, & yield sign (Loftus, Miller, & Burns, 1978). As an-
Ellsworth, 1979.) other example, asking the question, How fast were
the cars going when they smashed into each other?
REWRITING O F MEMORY results in a subject's "remembering" a greater speed
than when the word hit is used instead of smashed
I n 1932 Bartlett suggested the existence of con- (Loftus & Palmer, 1974). This sort of question-
structive processes in human memory: induced fabrication of memory could be responsible
for occasional instances of witnesses "recognizing"
The construction that is effected is [onel that would justify
the .
. . [which isl venr. lareelv-.a as a criminal perpetrator a person who was first
matter of feeling or affect. . . . When a subject is being seen at a much later time. but under circumstances
asked to remember, very often the first thing that emerges of guilt.
is something of the nature of an attitude. The recall is
then a construction. made lareelv on the basis of this at- liapid aging of new opinions. A result first ob-
titude, and its effect that of justification of the tained by Bem and McConnell (1970) is of major
attitude. (pp. 206-207) significance in demonstrating the disposition to
Bartlett's initial evidence for constructive pro- fabricate in human memory. One week after having
cesses in memory came from experiments on the given their opinions, which (expectably) favored
repeated reproduction of stories and drawings (cf. student control over university curriculum, Bem
Cofer, Chmielewski, & Brockway, 1976). Almost and McConnellls subjects were induced to choose
50 years later, Bartlett might be among those who to write an essay opposing that opinion. This
would be surprised by the power and extent of counterattitudinal role playing reliably produces
constructive processes demonstrated in three recent opinion change in the direction of the role-played
lines of experiments. These experiments show that position-in this case averaging about 10 points
people rapidly rewrite, or fabricate, memory in situ- on a 60-point single-item rating scale. Immediately
ations for which this seems dubiously appropriate. after the role playing, some of the subjects were
The knew-it-all-along efect . Fischhoff ( 1975, asked to recall the opinion they had expressed one
1977) gave subjects a general knowledge test in week earlier. Remarkably, these subjects showed
a recall error that averaged about 10 points, in product that differs from its predecessor mainly in
effect "remembering" opinions that agreed with packaging design, or the corporation annual report
what their post-role-playing opinions should have in which accountants reorganize the previous year's
been, rather than with what their pretest opinions data so as to show the present year as an improve-
actually were. Bern and McConnell also showed ment, or the politician who campaigns on a portion
that subjects in this role-playing procedure usually of the data ( e g , that policies have resulted in in-
had no awareness of the opinion change that had creased exports of domestic goods) while trusting
occurred. These results, which have been confirmed voters not to be aware of the remainder ( e g , that
by Wixon and Laird (1976), can be viewed as a imports have increased even more). I t is likely that
laboratory model of the behavior of politicians who intrapsychic analogs of these phenomena of repack-
declare themselves as having always supported a aging (buying new clothing?) and flexible account-
previously unpopular policy that has just recently ing or selection of favorable indicators (" [Even
proven wise. though I lost], my backhand was much better than
last time") are common occurrences. However,
COGNITIVE CONSERVATISM : DISCUSSION there has yet to be much empirical attention to
such processes. Among the relevant findings are
The quotation from Orwell's 1984 that opened this ones showing that ( a ) people sometimes arrange
section mentioned a combination of "belief in one's excuses or hedges ("self-handicapping") so as to
own infallibility" and "the power to learn from past create a situation in which actual performance is
mistakes." Although this seems an unlikely com- likely to improve upon expectation (Jones & Berg-
bination, it is indeed the consequence of the con- las, 1978); (b) we are especially attracted to, and
servatism biases just reviewed. Specifically, a form therefore may seek out, people who provide us a
of belief in personal infallibility is evident in the \
pattern of improving evaluations (e.g., the esteem-
confirmation biases that operate in information and
gain finding of Aronson & Linder, 1965); (c) on
memory search and in response to persuasion at- tasks that are personally important, we expect to
tempts; at the same time, people's readiness to re- improve on successive performances (Frank, 193 5 ) ;
write memory permits new information to be re- and (d) most people believe that the present is
ceived and incorporated into the cognitive system better than the past and that the future will be
without the system's registering the occurrence of better still (Brickman, Coates, & Janoff-Bulman,
change. This sort of correction or updating of 1978; cf. Albert, 1977).
memory (i.e., learning) thus does not disrupt the
sense of infallibility. Fischhoff's knew-it-all-along Relation of Totalitarian-Ego Biases to Self-Esteem
effect and Bem and McConnell's misrecall-of-prior-
opinion finding are perfect illustrations of the para- The three totalitarian-ego biases are interrelated in
doxical combination that Orwell had in mind. their bearing on the positiveness of self-evaluations,
The special case of perceiving improvement. In in other words, on self-esteem. Self-enhancement is
contrast with the cognitively conservative strategy by definition a component of beneffectance and is
of failing to perceive change when actual change just slightly less obviously so for cognitive con-
occurs, there is an important class of situations in servatism, a bias that incorporates a sense of per-
which just the reverse may be true-that is, change sonal infallibility. Egocentricity has the least ob-
being perceived when none has actually occurred. vious self-enhancing component, but has one none-
This phenomenon of fabricated change takes place theless, in encompassing the notion that events are
when people compare their past versus present only important to the extent that one's self is in-
selves on evaluative dimensions. A passage in 1983 volved. Because all three biases contribute to self-
suggests the function of this cognitive distortion: esteem, it is tempting to ignore their details in
favor of this major feature of similarity. The de-
The Party member . . . tolerates present-day conditions
partly because he has no standards of comparison. He tails will prove important, however, in analyzing
must be cut off from the past . . . because it is necessary the biases' functions and their relation to processes
for him to believe that he is better off than his ancestors that occur in other knowledge domains.
and that the average level of material comfort is con-
stantly rising. (orwell, 1949, p. 175)
ZZ. Ego as Oyganization of Knowledge
We are familiar with the occurrence of fabricated
changes of this sort in the daily practice of business The assertion that a collection of elements consti-
and politics, for instance, the "new, improved" tutes an organization requires the demonstration of
~nterdependenceamong those elements; accordingly, comes a perfect citizen of the totalitarian society:
the thesis that ego is an organization of knowledge
The final, indispensable, healing change had never hap-
demands a demonstration of interdependence within pened, until this moment. . . . He was back in the Min-
a knowledge domain that is plausibly identified as istry of Love, with everything forgiven, his soul as white
ego. The argument proceeds by showing that ( a ) as snow. He was in the public dock, confessing everything,
implicating everybody. He was walking down the white-
egocentricity, beneffectance, and conservatism are tiled corridor, with the feeling of walking in sunlight, and
indications of organization in extrahuman knowledge an armed guard at his back. The long-hoped-for bullet
domains, ( b ) these biases are enhanced by pro- was entering his brain. . . . Eut it was all right, everything
was all right, the struggle was finished. He had won the
cedures believed to increase ego's role in cognition victory over himself. He loved Big Brother. (Orwell,
( i.e., by ego-involvement ) , and (c) ego-involvement 1949, pp. 244-245)
reveals knowledge interdependencies in the form of
cognitive consistency. Winston Smith's "victory over himself" is his loss
of individuality, his complete subservience to the
state, a condition in which his life (or death) as an
Properties of Large-Scale Knowledge Organizations
individual no longer has special significance. In her
analysis of totalitarianism, Arendt (1966) similarly
Totalitarian societies and scientific theories are cer- describes the totalitarian society's citizens in terms
tainly strange bedfellows with each other, let alone of their loss of individuality:
with ego as a knowledge system. Nevertheless,
analysis of their properties contributes to the under- Total terror 3 . . . substitutes for the boundaries and
channels of communication between individual men a band
standing of ego as organization in two ways: ( a ) of iron which holds them so tightly together that it is as
The cognitive biases identified in the first section though their plurality had disappeared into One Man of
of this article can also be found in the operation of gigantic dimensions. (pp. 465-466, italics added)
these two types of system (totalitarian society and Total domination, which strives to organize the infinite
scientific theory); and ( b ) the sense in which ego plurality and differentiation of human beings as if all of
humanity were just one individual, is possible only if each
is conceived as an organization of knowledge can and every person can be reduced to a never-changing
then be considered by way of analogy to the cog- identity of reactions, so that each of these bundles of
nitive-organizational aspects of totalitarianism and reactions can be exchanged at random for any other. (p.
438, italics added)
scientific theory.
Orwell and Arendt both imply that the totalitar-
KNOWLEDGE ORGANIZATION IN
ian state, as a center of cognitive organization, sub-
TOTALITARIAN SOCIETY
verts and preempts knowledge organization a t the
individual-person level. Arendt ( 1966) also com-
[The] reason for the readjustment of the past is the need ments on the cognitive biases that operate a t the
to safeguard the infallibility of the Party. . . . No change leadership level of the totalitarian state; her obser-
of doctrine or in political alignment can ever be admitted. vations agree with those of Orwell, and they recapit-
For to change one's mind, or even one's policy, is a con-
fession of weakness. (Orwell, 1949, p. 175) ulate the totalitarian-ego themes:
The chief qualification of a mass leader has become un-
The control of the past depends above all on the training
ending infallibility; he can never admit an error. (pp. 348-
of memory. . . . [ I t is] necessary to remember that events
349)
happened in the desired manner. And if it is necessary to
rearrange one's memories or to tamper with written rec- Mass leaders in power have one concern which overrules
ords, then it is necessary to forget that one has done so. all utilitarian considerations: to make their predictions
The trick of doing this can be learned like any other mental come true. (p. 349)
technique. . . . I t is called doublethink. (Orwell, 1949,
p. 176) In a totally fictitious world [i.e., that of the totalitarian
society], failures need not be recorded, admitted, and
With modest rewriting, Orwell's characterizations remembered. (p. 388)
of thought control a t the totalitarian-society level
could stand as a summary of cognitive biases at the
individual-person level. One last quotation from 3The use of terror as a device for social control is a
fundamental part of Arendt's conception of totalitarianism,
1984 illustrates the function of cognitive biases in yet it obviously has no analog in the functioning of ego.
the totalitarian system about which Orwell wrote. This breakdown of the analogy between ego and totalitar-
At the end of 1984, Winston Smith (the protago- ianism is of interest in contemplating the possibility that
knowledge organization at the interindividual (social) level
nist-whose occupation was the daily revision of might become dominant over organization at the individual
history in the ofice of the Ministry of Truth) be- level.
THE SCIENTIFIC PARADIGM AS AN ORGANIZATION OF However, even when one grants that cognitive
KNOWLEDGE biases are signs of cognitive organization, referring
to the organization that possesses these biases as
Contemporary views of, the development of scientific ego remains to be justified. I t is to fill this gap in
knowledge give considerable attention to the pos- the argument that an examination of phenomena of
sibility that successfully developing theories incor- ego-involvement is in order. Ego-involvement is a
porate biased evaluations of research evidence. term traditionally given to procedures that are
Consider the following quotations from Kuhn assumed to activate ego processes, usually by
( 1970) as suggesting, respectively, egocentricity, attaching a sense of personal importance on the
beneffectance, and conservatism biases in the activ- part of a subject to an experimental task. To
ities of "normal science." create a high level of ego-involvement, for example,
subjects may be informed that a task yields a
T o scientists . . . the results gained in normal research are reliable measure of intelligence (or of some other
significant because they add to the scope and precision
with which the paradigm can be applied. (p. 36) highly valued skill). I will show that egocentricity,
beneffectance, and conservatism are magnified by
[Normal science's] object is to solve a puzzle for whose ego-involvement, providing reason to conclude that
very existence the validity of the paradigm must be
assumed. Failure to achieve a solution discredits .only the these biases reflect the operation of an organization
scientist and not the theory. (p. 80) that should be identified as ego.
[Normal science] seems an attempt to force nature into the
preformed and relatively inflexible box that the paradigm EGO-INVOLVEMENT ENHANCES COGNITIVE BIASES
supplies. (p. 24)
Few studies have observed egocentricity, beneffect-
Thus, a successful paradigm accounts for an ance, or conservatism in conjunction with a varia-
increasing range of phenomena ("paradigm-centric- tion of ego-invohement. However, the available
ity"), credits itself with confirmed, rather than with research is consistent in showing that these biases
disconfirmed, hypotheses, and preserves the integ- are enhanced by the presence of ego-involvement.
rity of its theoretical constructs. Egocentricity and ego-involvement. The mani-
The thesis of an analogy between the cognitive festations of egocentricity-self-reference and self-
operations of the human personality and scientific- generation as facilitators of memory and the illusion
theory construction was developed by Kelly (1955) of control-are conceptually close to the notion of
in The Psychology of Personal Constructs. Epstein ego-involvement. The phenomena of memory, for
( 1973 ) extended Kelly's analysis in presenting the example, might have been described as showing that
view that what we call self is, in effect, a theory when there is ego-involvement, there is better
about oneself. Loevinger ( 1976) connected her memory; the incorporation of self into the causal
conception of ego explicitly to Kuhn's analysis of structure of events reveals a tendency to impose
paradigms in comparing the progression of ego ego-involvement onto event perception. While it
through sequential stages of cognitive development may not be meaningful to ask if egocentricity
with the establishment and overthrow of successive increases with ego-involvement-because egocen-
theoretical paradigms. To these previous treat- tricity is, in effect, ego-involvement-it is still of
ments must now be added the observation that self interest to determine if egocentricity is more marked
and scientific theory also resemble each other in in a context of personal importance. There is
sharing cognitive biases-the same ones that char- much evidence that memory improves with the
acterize totalitarian thought control. importance of the material being studied (e.g.,
Nuttin & Greenwald, 1968), but that this improve-
Consequences of Ego-Involvement ment is associated with increased self-reference in
encoding has not been demonstrated. Studies to
Totalitarian propaganda and scientific theory are determine the effect of personal importance on the
assumed to be unarguably classified as organiza- incorporation of ego into causal sequences have not
tions of knowledge. Consequently, the parallels yet been done.
between these knowledge systems and ego suggest Beneffectance and ego-involvement. I t is well
both that ego has comparable organizational status established that beneffectance-the disposition to
and that the totalitarian-ego biases generally signal take credit for desired outcomes and to deny respon-
the existence of an actively functioning organization. sibility for undesired ones-is enhanced to the
extent that outcomes are personally important. likely to be aroused to the extent that the poten-
This was evident in the reversed Zeigarnik effect tially dissonant cognitions were important to the
(see first section), when subjects were ego-involved person. As a result, much early dissonance research
and interruption was the equivalent of failure, an was done under conditions that amounted to high
undesired outcome. Additionally, Nicholls ( 1975 ) , ego-involvement. Deutsch, Krauss, and Rosenau
Miller (1976), and Snyder et al. (1978) have ( 1962 ) deliberately varied ego-involvement and
recently reported studies showing that the proclivity found that "dissonance did not seem to occur"
to take credit more for success than for failure is (p. 27) in the low-involvement condition. More
greater the more important these outcomes are to recently, there has been much debate about the
the subject. necessity of ego-involvement for dissonance arousal.
Conservatism and ego-involvement. Evidence Zajonc (1968, p. 367) suggested that the emphasis
for ego-involvement's enhancement of cognitive on volition in the Brehm and Cohen (1962) pre-
conservatism-in the form of resistance to change sentation of dissonance theory is equivalent to an
of prior judgments-comes particularly from studies assumption that involvement of self is important
of persuasion. The tendency to resist persuasion is to dissonance arousal. Aronson ( 1968) presented
especially strong when the topic is important to a version of dissonance theory in which the central-
the person and there is some commitment to (ego- ity of self-relevant cognitions was explicitly sug-
involvement in) the prior position (Freedman, 1964 ; gested. In the latest comprehensive review of
Rhine & Severance, 1970; Sherif & Hovland, 1961 ; dissonance theory, Wicklund and Brehm ( 1976)
see Petty & Cacioppo, 1979, for a recent summary). accorded "personal responsibility" a central role in
On the other hand, importance of the topic can dissonance theory, and Greenwald and Ronis
sometimes facilitate change (Petty & Cacioppo, (1978) suggested that this was equivalent to an
1979; Zimbardo, 1960), but on these occasions assertion that dissonance reduction occurs only
involvement may be associated more with a post- when there is some cognitive involvement of self.
persuasion judgment than with the pretest. In Self-awareness and ego-involvement. An inter-
the latter studies, it would be interesting to deter- esting recent development in the psychological
mine whether subjects perceived change as actually study of self is Duval and Wicklund's (1972) con-
having occurred. (Goethals & Reckman, 1973, cept of self-awareness. Self-awareness is defined as
demonstrated a failure to perceive communication- a state in which one's attention is focused on one-
induced change on an important issue, but the self, such that discrepancies between actual and
experiment included no contrast with a less impor- ideal self become salient. Duval and Wicklund
tant topic.) Many unresearched questions about proposed that self-awareness is induced by self-
the potential role of ego-involvement as an enhancer focusing manipulations, which include the presence
of conservatism are of practical and theoretical of a mirror, a camera, a tape recorder, an audience,
interest. For example, are people more biased to or the sound of one's own voice. Although the pro-
confirm hypotheses in searching memory when the cedures used to induce self-awareness do not
hypothesis is personally important (one's pet resemble those typically used to generate ego-
theory)? I s the likelihood of postevent modifica- involvement, the similarity of these mental states
tion of memory greater for testimony in a case in is nonetheless suggested by ( a ) their similar cogni-
which one is personally involved than in a case in tive consequences-self-awareness, like ego-involve-
which one is a chance bystander? Are we increas- ment, increases both cognitive consistency (Carver,
ingly overconfident in the accuracy of memory as 1975 ; Gibbons, 1978) and beneffectance (Federoff
a function of personal importance of the content? & Harvey, 1976)-and (b) their similar hypothe-
Are we more dominated by first impressions of sized mediating processes-Sherif and Cantril
people when they will play important roles in our ( 1947) have conjectured that ego-involvement
lives? entails attention to discrepancies between perceived
self and social standards, while Hull and Levy
EGO-INVOLVEMENT INCREASES COGNITIVE ( 1979) have proposed that self-awareness can be
CONSISTENCY redefined as "encoding of information in terms of
its relevance for the self'' (p. 757). The close
Dissonance arousal and ego-involvement. In the relation between self-awareness and ego-involvement
original statement of cognitive dissonance theory, is developed further in Greenwald (in press).
Festinger (195 7) asserted that dissonance was Trait organization and ego-involvement. I n
1943, Allport suggested the following hypothesis: (e.g., Festinger, 1957), self-esteem (Schneider et
"When there is ego-involvement there are general al., 1979; Snyder et al., 1978), belief in a just world
traits; when there is no ego-involvement there are (Lerner & Miller, 1978), effective control (Kelley,
no general traits" (p. 461). This hypothesis might 1971), subjective competence (Bowerman, 1978),
be taken as an adjunct to the recent idiographic and social approval (Tedeschi, Schlenker, &
approaches to personality consistency exemplified Bonoma, 1971 ; Weary, 1980) .4 Informational
in the work of Bem and Allen (1974) and, Markus explanations suggest that biases are communicated
( 1977). To my knowledge, however, Allport's to the person by the environment. Examples of
hypothesis has never received a strong test. Con- informational interpretations are those stated in
firmation would provide a useful addition to the terms of perceived covariation (Kelley, 1971), cor-
evidence supporting the conception of ego as respondent inference (Jones & Davis, l96S), focus
organization. of attention (Jones & Nisbett, 1971; Taylor &
I s cognitive consistency a cognitive bias? This Fiske, 1978 ) , perception of contingency (Miller &
question is raised only to note that it would have Ross, 1975), and selective transmission of favorable
been possible to subsume the discussion of cognitive information (Tesser & Rosen, 1975).
consistency under the conservatism bias; that is, Although motivational and informational inter-
consistency, particularly when it involves the pro- pretations have sometimes been formulated as
cessing of new information in a fashion consistent antagonists-as in the dissonance theory versus
with existing knowledge, is a form of conservatism. self-perception theory debate (Bem, 1967) and in
Consistency was not grouped with the cognitive the interpretations of cognitive bias by Miller and
biases because maintenance of consistency might Ross (1975) and Bradley (1978)-the two classes
be regarded, in many cases, as unbiased information of explanation can coexist (see Greenwald, 1975b,
processing. There is no present need to arrive a t a for a more complete discussion of this point). The
conclusion regarding the appropriateness of refer- present analysis complements motivational and in-
ring to cognitive consistency as a bias. Consider- formational explanations of cognitive bias by focus-
ation of the question, however, does provide an ing on the role of the totalitarian-ego organization
introduction to considering the adaptiveness of ego's in facilitating its own continued existence."
cognitive biases, which is a major topic of the third
section. PARALLEL W I T H GENETIC EVOLUTION :
INTRAPSYCHIC EVOLUTION

I I I . Toward Explanation
In biology, evolutionary explanations look to the
A Tralfamadorian test pilot presses a starter button, and role of fortuitous variations in species characteris-
the whole Universe disappears. (Vonnegut, 1969/1971, p.
117)
The explanation offered by Orwell for the totalitarian-
Existing theoretical interpretations of cognitive ism of 1984 is a motivational one and is different from
the ones just mentioned. He appealed to a need for power
biases attribute causal efficacy either to motivational (Orwell, 1949, p. 217).
or to informational forces. These causal forces are Motivational and informational interpretations of cog-
organism-internal and organism-external, respec- nitive bias have been compared in detail in a recent review
by Tetlock and Levi (Note 1 ) . In a comprehensive
tively, but in either case are external to the analysis of nonveridical inference strategies in human
cognitive organization being explained. A third judgment, Nisbett and Ross (1980) have taken a strong
type of explanation, one that is related to evolution- position against motivational interpretations. They have
classed such interpretations as instances of the "funda-
ary explanations in biology, allows the cognitive mental attribution error" of postulating dispositions to
biases to provide their own explanation. I n this account for observed behavior. Although this is not the
"intrapsychic evolution" type of explanation, cogni- place to develop the point in detail, it is possible t o con-
sider purely informational interpretations equally as
tive-system characteristics such a s the totalitarian- instances of attributional error. The motivation-informa-
ego biases cannot become pervasive unless they tion debate is representative of a pervasive and long-
achieve a criterion of intrapsychic survival. standing paradigm clash between internal-cause and
external-cause explanations in psychology, other instances
Motivational explanations interpret cognitive being instinct versus learning, heredity versus environment,
biases as occurring in the service of motives or nativism versus empiricism, drive theory versus radical
needs. Examples of motives that have been hypoth- behaviorism, and dissonance versus self-perception. By
considering both internal and external causal factors in
esized to account for some portion of the totalitar- interaction, analyses based on evolutionary reasoning offer
ian-ego biases are needs for cognitive consistency a possible escape from this perennial debate.
tics in ensuring the species' survival. Variations isting at the time of shelving the book still be in
that survive are, by definition, successful or adap- existence at the time of searching for it. Recogni-
tive, and surviving species therefore provide living tion, similarly, should be facilitated by stability of
records of adaptive variations. The evolutionary the organization of memory.
interpretation of the totalitarian-ego biases starts To continue this reasoning, the role of totali-
from the observation of their pervasiveness, which tarian-ego biases in facilitating intrapsychic survi-
serves as an index of their survival value; it pro- val (recognition) can be understood by demon-
ceeds to consider why cognitive systems that con- strating their function in maintaining the cognitive
tain these biases may survive better than do ones organization of which they are a part. This demon-
that lack them. stration can start from some existing discussions of
A few comments may elucidate the parallel be- the functions of cognitive biases in scientific theory
tween intrapsychic and genetic evolution. First, and totalitarian information control. For example,
the domain in which the totalitarian-ego organiza- Lakatos (1970), commenting on the behavior of
tion develops is the individual nervous system; the scientists in relation to their theoretical paradigms
potential competitors for this niche are alternative (he used the term research programmes), said,
cognitive forms, which could be organizations lack-
ing the totalitarian-ego biases or possibly even non- All scientific research programmes may be characterized by
their 'hard core.' . . . We must use our ingenuity to articu-
organizations. Second, intrapsychic evolution pro- late or even invent 'auxiliary hypotheses,' which form a
ceeds very rapidly in comparison with genetic evo- protective belt around this core. . . . I t is this protective
lution, occurring within the period of formation of belt of auxiliary hypotheses which has to bear the brunt of
tests and get adjusted and re-adjusted, or even completely
the individual personality. Third, whereas in ge- replaced, to defend the thus-hardened core. (p. 133)
netic evolution reproduction is the survival criterion,
in intrapsychic evolution a cognition (percept, cate- In characterizing totalitarianism, Arendt (1966)
gory, proposition, etc.) survives by being known observed that "the true goal of totalitarian propa-
again, that is, by being recognized. ganda is not persuasion but organization" and that
The process of intrapsychic evolution, although "the organization of the entire texture of life ac-
differing from genetic evolution in the ways just cording to an ideology can be fully carried out only
noted, nonetheless cannot proceed in total indepen- under a totalitarian regime" (pp. 361 ; 363 ; italics
dence of genetic evolution. A cognitive structure added).
that functions well at achieving recognition, but Thus, the cognitive biases of a successful scientific
'
(for whatever reason) interferes with reproductive paradigm or of an established totalitarian system
success, is unlikely to remain characteristic of the presumably function to preserve organization. I t
species for many generations. (The genes that sup- follows that the corresponding biases in ego may
ported it would eventually disappear from the spe- similarly function to protect the integrity of
cies' gene pool.) Accordingly, this analysis must ego's organization of knowledge. In particular, by
consider the role of the totalitarian-ego biases in coding much information in relation to self, the
affecting survival both of cognitions and of the body egocentricity bias ensures that the self-system main-
inhabited by them. tains wide scope; this information-assimilating ac-
tivity preserves organization in the same way that a
INTRAPSYCHIC SURVIVAL : EGOCENTRICITY AND library's maintenance depends on a continuing pro-
CONSERVATISM I N THE SERVICE O F gram of acquisitions. By retaining previously used
MAINTAINING ORGANIZATION cognitive categories, the conservatism bias ensures
that similar information encountered at different
The workings of a library provide a useful analogy points in time is encoded into the same categories;
to the process of recognition. Reexperiencing a as with the library, such consistency of encoding
prior cognition is analogous to finding a book in a over time preserves access to already stored in-
library. In the library, book finding is possible be- formation in a growing organization of knowledge.
cause of the existence of an organized system for
shelving books and recording the locations of
shelved books. By this analogy, the existence of 6 T h e reader is asked to accept without further laboring
organization in human memory should be an aid to of the argument that organization within a knowledge
system facilitates location of information contained within
recognition.= Further, in a library, success in lo- that system. The argument might be made in rigorous
cating a book requires that the organization ex- fashion, but that is beyond the scope of this article.
To return to Lakatos's (1970) observation, the Some recent findings from the study of depres-
egocentricity and conservatism biases may provide a sion suggest that normals differ from depressives
"protective belt" that preserves the "hard core" along the beneffectance dimension in a way con-
belief that all of one's memory is the interrelated sistent with the hypothesis of a relationship be-
experience of a single entity-the one called myself. tween beneffectance and effective performance.
The protection of this belief may be a very im- Lewinsohn, Mischel, Chaplin, and Barton (1980)
portant matter indeed; its breakdown is considered found that depressives' self-evaluations agreed
to be a pathological condition when it occurs in the more with observers' ratings than did those of nor-
forms of multiple personality, amnesia, fugue, or mals; normals gave relatively inflated self-evalu-
depersonalization (cf . Hilgard, 1977, chap. 2 ) .7 ations. Alloy and Abramson (1979) found that
depressives appraised their degree of control over
GENETIC SURVIVAL : BENEFFECTANCE IN THE probabilistic outcomes more accurately than did
SERVICE O F BEHAVIORAL PERSEVERANCE normals, with normals frequently overestimating
their extent of control. These interesting, but un-
I t must be conceded that the totalitarian-ego biases expected, findings should receive the support of
are, in the long run, disadvantageous. In scientific additional studies before strong conclusions are
theory, for example, since any theory is certain based on them. Tentatively, however, it appears
eventually to be found inadequate and to be re- that the reduced level of effective action associ-
placed, a bias that resists change in knowledge (the ated with depression could be a consequence of
conservatism bias) prolongs the life of an incor- depressives' lack of a beneffectance bias.
rect theory. In the case of self, it can be argued Bandura ( 1977) has recently developed a theory
similarly that ego's biases will produce cognitive of self-efficacy that offers a systematic account of
stagnation in a person who is capable of greater the involvement of beliefs about being able to per-
developmental achievement. Despite these con- form appropriate actions (efficacy expectations) in
siderations, available speculations about scientific effective performance. I t is consistent with Ban-
theory indicate that the behavioral perseverance dura's theory to conclude that the likelihood of
characteristic of an organization possessing the effective performance may be greater for a person
beneffectance bias is a survival asset. whose efficacy expectations are generally inflated
The dogmatic attitude of sticking to a theory a~ long as (i.e., a person with a beneffectance bias) than for
possible is of considerable significance. Without it we one whose expectations may be more objectively
could never find out what is in a theory-we should give accurate.
the theory up before we had a real opportunity of finding
out its strength; and in consequence no theory would
ever be able to play its role of bringing order into the Toward Explanation: Conclusions
world, of preparing us for future events, of drawing our
attention to events we should otherwise never observe. Through continued development of the parallels
(Popper, 1963, p. 312) among self, scientific theory, and totalitarian society
By ensuring that the paradigm will not be too easily that were introduced in the second section of this
surrendered resistance guarantees that scientists will not be article, I suggested that the totalitarian-ego biases
lightly distracted and that the anomalies that lead to para-
digm change will penetrate existing knowledge to the succeed intrapsychically because they preserve the
core. (Kuhn, 1970, p. 65). cognitive organization in which they exist and that
I n the psychological research literature, there are they succeed behaviorally because they facilitate
several indications that a beneffectance bias is as- goal attainment via perseverance in goal seeking.
sociated with effective performance in situations in
which perseverance might be the critical determi- 7 In recognition of these virtues of cognitive conserva-

nant of effectiveness. I n Johnston's (1967) track- tism, I had an attack of caution before deciding to intro-
duce a new label (beneffectance) for one of the cognitive
ing experiment (described earlier), subjects who re- biases with which this article is concerned. The dilemma
ceived feedback indicative of team success not only was resolved in part by taking care t o cross-reference the
felt responsible for the success but also performed labels (ego-defensive attribution, self-serving attribution,
egocentric attribution, and attributional egotism) that
better on the tracking task than did subjects who readers may have used for some of the relevant prior
received feedback of repeated poor performance. literature. More generally, this illustrates that cognitive
Johnston attributed the performance deterioration conservatism is not being put forth as an absolute virtue.
The gains of a new conceptualization (cognitive change)
of the latter subjects to a decline in morale associ- are to be set off against the costs of repairing the oraaniza-
ated with their declining self-evaluation (p. 326). tion that is damaged by the change.
This intrapsychic evolution analysis offers an al- The knowledge system must sometimes operate with
ternative to existing motivational and informational out-of-date or inaccurate information. More than
interpretations of cognitive biases8 compensating for these costs, presumably, are ad-
The reasoning developed in characterizing the vantages: The biases help to preserve the knowledge
functions of the conservatism bias is capable of ex- system's organization and allow it to link effectively
plaining a variety of resistances to cognitive change, with behavior.
such as unwillingness to learn new languages (e.g.,
the reluctance of English- and Spanish-speaking New
Yorkers to learn the other's language), resistance Preparation of this article was facilitated by grants from
to adopting a new standard (e.g., the resistance of the National Science Foundation (BNS 76 11175) and the
the US. population to adopting metric units for National Institute of Mental Health (MH-20527, MH-
31762, MH-32317, and MH-07669). The reader of this
weight, distance, and temperature), and reluctance article should appreciate that the author is prepared to
of users of a functioning computer system to "up- take full responsibility only for the good ideas that are to
grade" to a new generation of equipment. I t is ap- be found in it. I am nonetheless grateful to the following
people, who commented on earlier drafts: Daryl J. Bem,
parent that such resistances interfere with rapid Eugene Borgida, Nancy Cantor, Robert B. Cialdini,
diffusion of improvements in knowledge; however, Baruch Fischhoff, Kenneth J. Gergen, Alan E. Gross,
this liability may be more than balanced by the Reid S. Hastie, John H . Harvey, Ernest R. Hilgard, Irving
L. Janis, Barbara Kellerman, Ellen Langer, Elizabeth F.
asset of allowing the cognitive system to allocate Loftus, Hazel Markus, Walter Mischel, T. B. Rogers, David
its resources to storage and retrieval of information L. Ronis, Ralph L. Rosnow, Michael Ross, Silvan S. Tom-
(rather than continual revision of its indexing or kins, Gifford Weary, Gary L. Wells, and Kipling D.
Williams. If this disguise of gratitude is itself seen as inept,
coding scheme), thereby permitting access to a then the reader should know that it was the suggestion of
large amount of information within a single system. Robert B. Zajonc, modified with the help of Robert Trivers.
To return to the library analogy, once a commitment
has been made to a specific cataloging scheme, it REFERENCE NOTE
may be more efficient to maintain consistency with
1. Tetlock, P. E., & Levi, A. Attribution bias: On the in-
that scheme than to allocate librarian effort to re- conclusiveness of the cognition-motivation debate. Man-
vising it (recataloging and reshelving the existing uscript submitted for publication, 19.79.
collection) every time an improved indexing or re-
trieval system becomes available. REFERENCES

Albert, S. Temporal comparison theory. Psychological


Conclusion Review, 1977, 84,485-503.
Alloy, L. B., & Abramson, L. Y. Judgment of contingency
A variety of cognitive biases can be grouped into in depressed and nondepressed students: Sadder but
wiser? Journal of Experimental Psychology: General,
three categories: egocentricity, beneffectance, and 1979, 108,441-485.
conservatism. There is much evidence for the op- Allport, G. W. The ego in contemporary psychology.
eration of these biases in an organization that is of Psychological Review, 1943, 50, 451-478.
Anderson, N. H., & Hubert, S. Effects of concomitant verbal
special interest to psychologists-the human self recall on order effects in personality impression forma-
or ego-as well as in two extrapersonal knowledge tion. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior,
domains-paradigmatic scientific theory and totali- 1963, 2 , 379-391.
Arendt, H. The origins of totalitarianism (3rd ed.). New
tarian propaganda. The biases' predilection for York: Harcourt, Brace & World, 1966.
fabrication a n d revision of history entails costs: Arkin, R. M., & Maruyama, G. M. Attribution, affect, and
college exam performance. Journal of Educational Psy-
chology, 1979, 71, 85-93.
sNisbett and Ross (1980) temper their plea for use of Aronson, E. Dissonance theory: Progress and problems.
rationally correct inferential strategies by noting the po- In R. P. Abelson et al. (Eds.), Theories of cognitive con-
tential virtues of normatively inferior heuristics (cf. Tversky sistency: A sourcebook. Chicago: Rand McNally, 1968.
& Kahneman, 1974). Two of Nisbett and Ross's observa- Aronson, E., & Linder, D. Gain and loss of esteem as de-
tions fit particularly well with the intrapsychic evolution terminants of interpersonal attractiveness. Journal oj
analysis: "The behavior of subjects, inappropriate as it Experimental Social Psychology, 1965, 1, 156-171.
is from the standpoint of rationality in the inferential Bandura, A. Self-efficacy: Toward a unifying theory of be-
contexts studied, may arise from pursuit of important, havioral change. Psychological Review, 1977,84,191-215.
higher order epistemic goals [such as1 (a) the importance Bartlett, F. C. Remembering: A study in experimental and
of stability to beliefs and belief-systems . . . and (b) social psychology. London: Cambridge University Press,
real-world constraints on time" (p. 191) ; and "People some- 1932.
times may require overly optimistic or overly pessimistic Bem, D. J. Self-perception: An alternative interpretation
subjective probabilities to goad them into effective action of cognitive dissonance phenomena. Psychological Re-
or prevent them from taking dangerous actions" (p. 271). view, 1967, 74, 183-200.
Bem, D. J., & Allen, A. On predicting some of the people Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception
some of the time: The search for cross-situational con- and Performance, 1977, 3, 552-564.
sistencies in behavior. Psychological Review, 1974, 81, Frank, J. D. Some psychological determinants of the level
506-520. of aspiration. American Journal of Psychology, 1935, 47,
Bem, D. J., & McConnell, H . K. Testing the self-percep- 285-293.
tion explanation of dissonance phenomena: On the sali- Freedman, J. L. Involvement, discrepancy, and change.
ence of premanipulation attitudes. Journal of Personality Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1964, 69,
and Social Psychology, 1970, 14, 23-31. 290-295.
Bower, G. H., & Gilligan, S. G. Remembering information Freud, S. The defence neuro-psychoses. In E. Jones
related to one's self. Journal of Research in Personality, (Ed.) and J . Rickman (Trans.), Sigmund Freud: Col-
1979, 13, 420-432. lected papers (Vol. 1 ) . New York: Basic Books, 1959.
Bowerman, W. R. Subjective competence: The structure, (Originally published, 1894.)
process and function of self-referent causal attributions Gibbons, F. X. Sexual standards and reactions to pornog-
Journal for the Theory of Social Behaviour, 1978, 8, raphy: Enhancing behavioral consistency through self-
45-57. focused attention. Journal of Personality and Social
Bradley, G. W. Self-serving biases in the attribution pro- Psychology, 1978, 36, 976-987.
cess: A reexamination of the fact or fiction question. Glixman, A. F. Recall of completed and uncompleted
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1978, 36, activities under varying degrees of stress. Journal of
56-71. Experimental Psychology, 1949, 39, 281-296.
Brehm, J. W., & Cohen, A. R. Explorations in cognitive Goethals, G. R., & Reckman, R. F . The perception of
dissonance. New York: Wiley, 1962. consistency in attitudes. Journal o f Experimental Social
Brenner, M. The next-in-line effect. Journal of Verbal P ~ y c h o l ~ g 1973,
y, 9, 491-501.
Learning and Verbal Behavior, 1973, 12, 320-323. Greenwald, A. G. Cognitive learning, cognitive response t o
Brenner, M. W. Memory and interpersonal relations. Un- persuasion and attitude change. In A. G. Greenwald, T.
published doctoral dissertation, University of Michigan, C. Brock, & T . M. Ostrom (Eds.), Psychological founda-
1976. tions of attitudes. New York: Academic Press, 1968.
Brickman, P., Coates, D., & Janoff-Bulman, R. Lottery Greenwald, A. G. Consequences of prejudice against the
winners and accident victims: Is happiness relative? null hypothesis. Psychological Bulletin, 1975, 82,
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1978, 36, 1-20. (a)
917-927. Greenwald, A. G. On the inconclusiveness of "crucial" cog-
Carver, C. S. Physical aggression as a function of objec- nitive tests of dissonance versus self-perception theories.
tive self-awareness and attitudes toward punishment. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 1975, 11,
Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 1975, 11, 490499. (b)
510-519. Greenwald, A. G. Ego task analysis: An integration of
Cialdini, R. B., et al. Basking in reflected glory: Three research on ego-involvement and self-awareness. In A.
(football) field studies. Journal of Personality and So- Hastorf & A. Isen (Eds.), Cognitive social psychology.
cial Psychology, 1976, 34, 366-375. Amsterdam: Elsevier/North-Holland, in press. ,
Cofer, C. N., Chmielewski, D. L., & Brockway, J. F. Greenwald, A. G., & Albert, R. D. Acceptance and; recall
Constructive processes and the structure of human mem- of improvised arguments. Journal of Personalit,y and
ory. In C. N. Cofer (Ed.), The structure of human mem- Social Psychology, 1968, 8, 31-34. I
ory. San Francisco: Freeman, 1976. Greenwald, A .G., & Ronis, D. L. Twenty years 6f cog-
Cullen, D. M. Attitude measurement by cognitive sam- nitive dissonance: Case study of the evolution of aitheory.
pling (Doctoral dissertation, Ohio State University, 1968). Psychological Review, 1978, 85, 53-57.
Dissertation Abstracts, 1968, 29, 1597A. Harvey, J. H., Harris, B., & Barnes, R. D. Actor-~bserver
de Rivera, J . H . Field theory as human-science. New differences in the perceptions of responsibility and free-
York: Gardner Press, 1976. dom. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1975,
Deutsch, M., Krauss, R., & Rosenau, N. Dissonance or 32, 22-28.
defensiveness ? Journal of Personality, 1962, 30, 16-28, Hilgard, E. R. Divided consciousness: Multiple controls in
Duval, S., & Wicklund, R. A. A theory of objective self- human thought and action. New York: Wiley, 1977.
awareness. New York: Academic Press, 1972. Hovland, C. I., Janis, I . L., & Kelley, H. H. Communica-
Epstein, S. The self-concept revisited: Or a theory of a tion and persuasion. New Haven: Yale University Press,
theory. American Psychologist, 1973, 28, 404-416. 1953.
Erdelyi, M., Buschke, H., & Finkelstein, S. Hypermnesia Hull, J. G., & Levy, A. S. The organizational functions of
for Socratic stimuli: The growth of recall for an in- the self: An alternative to the Duval and Wicklund
ternally generated memory list abstracted from a series model of self-awareness. Journal of Personality and
of riddles. Memory 6. Cognition, 1977, 5, 283-286. Social Psychology, 19798,37, 756-768.
Federoff, N. A., & Harvey, J . H . Focus of attention, self- Janis, I. L., & Terwilliger, R. F. An experimental study
esteem, and the attribution of causality. Journal of Re- of psychological resistances to fear arousing communica-
search in Personality, 1976, 10, 336-345. tions. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1962,
Festinger, L. A theory of cognitive dissonance. Evanston, 65, 403-410.
111.: Row, Peterson, 1957. Jervis, R. Perception and misperception in international
Fischhoff, B. Hindsight # foresight: The effect of out- politics. Princeton, N.J.: Princeton University Press,
come knowledge on judgment under uncertainty. Jour- 1976.
nal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Johnston, W. A. Individual performance and self-evaluation
Performance, 1975, 1 , 288-299. in a simulated team. Organizational Behavior and
Fischhoff, B. Perceived informativeness of facts. Journal Human Performance, 1967, 2 , 309-328.
of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Jones, E. E., & Berglas, S. Control of attributions about
Performance, 1977, 3, 349-358. the self through self-handicapping strategies: The appeal
Fischhoff, B., Slovic, P., & Lichtenstein, S. Knowing with of alcohol and the role of underachievement. Personality
certainty: The appropriateness of extreme confidence. and Social Psychology Bulletin, 1978, 4, 200-206.
Jones, E . E., & Davis, K. E . From acts to dispositions: of selective memory about the self. Journal of Consult-
The attribution process in person perception. I n L. ing and Clinical Psychology, 1976, 44, 92-103.
Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances i n experimental social psy- Myers, D. G., & Ridl, J. R. A better than average insight
chotogy (Vol. 2). New York: Academic Press, 1965. into pride. Psychology Today, August 1979, pp. 89; 95-
Jones, E . E., & Nisbett, R. E. The actor and the observer: 96; 98.
Divergent perceptions of the causes of behavior. I n E. E. Nicholls, J. G. Causal attributions and other achievement-
Jones et al. (Eds.), Attribution: Perceiving the causes o f related cognitions: Effects of task outcome, attainment
behavior. Morristown, N.J.: General Learning Press, value, and sex. Journal of Personality and Social Psy-
1971. chology, 1975, 31, 379-389.
Kelley, H. H . Attribution in social interaction. I n E. E . Nisbett, R. E., & Ross, L. H u m a n inference: Strategies and
Jones et al. (Eds.), Attribution: Perceiving the causes o f shortcomings. Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice-Hall,
behavior. Morristown, N.J.: General Learning Press, 1980.
1971. Norman, D. A. Memory and attention (2nd ed.). New
Kelly, G. A. T h e psychology of personal constructs (Vol. York: Wiley, 1976.
1). New York: Norton, 1955. Nuttin, J. R., & Greenwald, A. G. Reward and punishment
Koriat, A,, Lichtenstein, S., & Fischhoff, B. Reasons for i n human learning. New York: Academic Press, 1968.
confidence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Orwell, G. 1984. New York: Harcourt, Brace, 1949.
Learning and Memory, 1980, 6, 107-118. Petty, R. E., & Cacioppo, J. T . Issue involvement can
Kuhn, T. S. T h e structure of scientific revolutions (2nd increase or decrease persuasion by enhancing message-
ed.). Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1970. relevant cognitive responses. Journal of Personality and
Kuiper, N. A,, & Rogers, T. B. Encoding of personal Social Psychology, 1979, 37, 1915-1926.
information: Self-other differences. Journal of Person- Petty, R. E., Ostrom, T. M., & Brock, T. C. (Eds.). Cog-
ality and Social Psychology, 1979, 37, 499-514. nitive responses i n persuasion. Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum,
Lakatos, I. Falsification and the methodology of scientific in press.
research programmes. In I . Lakatos & A. Musgrave Popper, K. Conjectures and refutations. London: Rout-
(Eds.), Criticism and the growth of knowledge. London: ledge & Kegan Paul, 1963.
Cambridge University Press, 1970. Rhine, R. J., & Severance, L. J. Ego-involvement, dis-
Langer, E . J. The illusion of control. Journal o f Person- crepancy, source credibility, and attitude change. Jour-
ality and Social Psychology, 1975, 32, 311-329. nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1970, 16, 175-
Lerner, M. J., & Miller, D. T . Just world research and the 190.
attribution process: Looking back and ahead. Psycho- Rogers, T. B., Kuiper, N. A,, & Kirker, W. S. Self-reference
logical Bulletin, 1978, 85, 1030-1051. and the encoding of personal information. Journal o f
Lewinsohn, P. M., Mischel, W., Chaplin, W., & Barton, R. Personality and Social Psychology, 1977, 35, 677-688.
Social competence and depression: The role of illusory Rosenzweig, S. An experimental study of "repression" with
self-perceptions. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 1980, special reference to need-persistive and ego-defensive
89, 203-212. reactions to frustration. Journal of Experimental Psy-
Loevinger, J. Ego development. San Francisco: Jossey- chology, 1943, 32, 64-74.
Bass, 1976.-
Ross, M., & Sicoly, F. Egocentric biases in availability and
Loftus, E. F. Eyewitness testimony. Cambridge, Mass.:
attribution. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol-
Harvard University Press, 1979.
ogy, 1979, 37, 322-336.
Loftus, E. F., Miller, D. G., & Burns, H. J. Semantic
Schlenker, B. R., & Miller, R. S. Egocentrism in groups:
integration of verbal information into a visual memory.
Journal o f Experimental Psychology: Human Learning Self-serving biases or logical information processing?
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1977, 35,
and Memory, 1978, 4, 19-31.
755-764.
Loftus, E . F., & Palmer, J. C. Reconstruction of auto-
mobile destruction: An example of the interaction between Schneider, D. A,, Hastorf, A. H., & Ellsworth, P. C. Per-
language and memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and son perception (2nd ed.) . Reading, Mass.: Addison-
Verbal Behavior, 1974, 13, 585-589. Wesley, 1979.
Lord, C. G. Schemas and images as memory aids: Two Sherif, M., & Cantril, H. The psychology of ego-involve-
modes of processing social information. Journal of Per- ments. New York: Wiley, 1947.
sonality and Social Psychology, 1980, 38, 257-269. Sherif, M., & Hovland, C. I . Social judgment. New
Luchins, A. S. Primacy-recency in impression formation. Haven: Yale University Press, 1961.
I n C. I. Hovland (Ed.), The order of presentation i n Slamecka, N. J., & Graf, P. The generation effect: Delin-
persuasion. New Haven: Yale University Press, 1957. eation of a phenomenon. Journal of Experimental Psy-
Markus, H . Self-schemata and processing information chology: H u m a n Learning and Memory, 1978, 4, 592-604.
about the self. Journal of Personality and Social Psy- Snyder, M., & Swann, W. B. Hypothesis-testing processes
chology, 1977, 35, 63-78. in social interaction. Journal o f Personality and Social
Milgram, S. Behavioral study of obedience. Journal o f Psychology, 1978, 36, 1202-1212.
Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1963, 67, 371-378. Snyder, M., & Uranowitz, S. W. Reconstructing the past:
Miller, D. T . Ego involvement and attributions for suc- Some cognitive consequences of person perception. Jour-
cess and failure. Journal of Personality and Social Psy- nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1978, 36, 941-
chology, 1976, 34, 901-906 950.
Miller, D. T., & Ross, M. Self-serving biases in the attri- Snyder, M. L., Stephan, W. G., & Rosenfield, C. Attribu-
bution of causality: Fact or fiction? Psychological tional egotism. In J . H . Harvey, W. J. Ickes, & R. F.
Bulletin, 1975, 82, 213-225. Kidd (Eds.), N e w directions in attribution research (Vol.
Mischel, W., Ebbesen, E. B., & Zeiss, A. R. Selective atten- 2). Hillsdale, N. J.: Erlbaum, 1978.
tion to the self: Situational and dispositional determin- Taylor, S. E., & Fiske, S. T . Salience, attention, and attri-
ants. Journal o f Personality and Social Psychology, bution: Top of the head phenomena. In L. Berkowitz
1973, 27, 129-142. (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol.
Mischel, W., Ebbesen, E . B., & Zeiss, A. M. Determinants 11). New York: Academic Press, 1978.
Tedeschi, J. T., Schlenker, B. R., & Bonoma, T. V. Cogni- Wicklund, R. A. Three years later. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.),
tive dissonance: Private ratiocination or public spectacle? Cognitive theories in social psychology. New York:
American Psychologist, 1971, 26, 685-695. academic Press, 1978.
Tesser, A., & Rosen, S. The reluctance to transmit bad Wicklund, R. A,, & Brehm, J. W. Perspectives on cognitive
news. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental dissonance. Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum, 1976.
social psychology (Vol. 8 ) . New York: Academic Press, Wixon, D. R., & Laird, J. D. Awareness and attitude
1975. change in the forced-compliance paradigm: The impor-
Trope, Y. Inferences of personal characteristics on the tance of when. Journal of Personality and Social Psy-
basis of information retrieved from one's memory. Jour- chology, 1976, 34, 376-384.
nal o f Personality and Social Psychology, 1978, 36, 93-106. Wortman, C. B. Causal attributions and personal control.
Tulving, E. Episodic and semantic memory. In E. Tul- In J. H. Harvey, W. J. Ickes, & R. F. Kidd (Eds.), New
ving & W. Donaldson (Eds.), Organization oj memory. directions in attribution research (Vol. 1 ) . New York:
New York: Academic Press, 1972. Erlbaum, 1976.
Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. Judgment under uncer- Zajonc, R. B. Cognitive theories in social psychology. In
tainty: Heuristics and biases. Science, 1974, 185, 1124- G. Lindzey & E . Aronson (Eds.), Handbook of social
1131. psychology (Vol. 1, 2nd ed.). Reading, Mass.: .4ddison-
Vonnegut, K., Jr. Slaughterhouse-fzve. New York: Dell, Wesley, 1968.
1971. (Originally published, 1969.) Zeigarnik, B. Ueber das Behalten von erledigten und
Weary, G . Examination of affect and egotism as medi- underledigten Handlungen. Psychologische Forschung,
ators of bias in causal attributions. Journal of Personal- 1927, 9, 1-85.
i t y and Social Psychology, 1980, 38, 348-357. Zimbardo, P. G. Involvement and communication discrep-
White, R. W. Motivation reconsidered: The concept of ancy as determinants of opinion conformity. Journal oj
competence. Psychological Review, 1959, 66, 297-333. Abnovmal and Social Psychology, 1960, 60, 86-94.

Você também pode gostar